Skip to main content
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
. 2019 Jun 18;13:128. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00128

Gender Differences in Large-Scale and Small-Scale Spatial Ability: A Systematic Review Based on Behavioral and Neuroimaging Research

Li Yuan 1,*, Feng Kong 1, Yangmei Luo 1, Siyao Zeng 1, Jijun Lan 1, Xuqun You 1,*
PMCID: PMC6591491  PMID: 31275121

Abstract

Background: As we human beings are living in a multidimensional space all the time. Therefore, spatial ability is vital for the survival and development of individuals. However, males and females show gender differences in this ability. So, are these gender differences influenced by the scale type of spatial ability? It's not well specified. Therefore, to tackle this issue, we conducted the current research from the behavioral and neural level.

Methods: Study 1 used the general meta-analysis method to explore whether individuals display the same gender differences in large- and small-scale spatial ability. Study 2 used the method of Activation Likelihood Estimation to identify the commonalities and distinctions of the brain activity between males and females on large- and small-scale spatial ability.

Results: Study 1 showed that in behavior performance, males outperformed females in both large-scale and small-scale spatial ability, but the effect size of the gender difference in large-scale spatial ability is significantly greater than that in small-scale spatial ability. In addition, Study 2 showed that in terms of neural activity, males and females exhibited both similarities and differences no matter in large-scale or small-scale spatial ability. Especially, the contrast analysis between females and males demonstrated a stronger activation in the brain regions of bilateral lentiform nucleus and bilateral parahippocampal gyrus in large-scale spatial ability, and correspondence in right sub-gyral, right precuneus, and left middle frontal gyrus in small-scale spatial ability.

Conclusions: The results indicated that the reason why females performed not so well in large-scale spatial ability was that they were more susceptible to emotions and their parahippocampal gyrus worked less efficiently than males; females performed not so well in small-scale spatial ability because they mostly adopted the egocentric strategy and their sub-gyral also worked less efficiently than males. The two different reasons have made for gender differences in favor of males in terms of spatial ability and such gender differences have different manifestations in large-scale and small-scale spatial ability. Possible implications of the results for understanding the issue of gender differences in spatial ability are discussed.

Keywords: gender differences, large-scale spatial ability, small-scale spatial ability, activation likelihood estimation, meta-analysis

Introduction

Gender differences have long been studied in a variety of fields like psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Gender differences in individual spatial ability have also been emphasized in the field of spatial ability. Our study aims to go deeper into gender differences in individual spatial ability through a meta-analysis based on behavioral performance and neural basis.

Spatial ability is one of the most core cognitive abilities of individuals. The tests of spatial ability also make up a significant part of intelligence testing. In general, spatial ability has been defined as the ability to understand the relationships among different positions in space or imagined movements of two- and three-dimensional objects (Clements, 1998; Wang et al., 2014). Specifically, space ability involves two major categories of large-scale spatial ability and small-scale spatial ability. Large-scale spatial ability refers to the ability of individuals to carry out cognitive processing of spatial information in the large-scale environment. And in this process, viewer's perspective changes with respect to the larger environment, but the spatial relationships among individual objects do not change (Hegarty and Waller, 2004; Wang et al., 2014). The most typical representatives of large-scale spatial ability are navigation ability and spatial orientation ability (Jansen, 2009; Tim and Höffler, 2010; Wang et al., 2014). The navigation ability refers to the ability to navigate in a large-scale environment where the spatial relationships among landmarks cannot be fully apprehended from a single vantage point (Wang and Carr, 2014). Spatial orientation is the ability to imagine objects from different perspectives (Yilmaz, 2009; Turgut, 2015). Small-scale spatial ability means the ability to mentally represent and transform two- and three-dimensional images, which can typically be apprehended from a single vantage point (Wang and Carr, 2014). Small-scale spatial ability mainly includes the capabilities of spatial visualization and spatial relations (Jansen, 2009; Tim and Höffler, 2010). Spatial visualization refers to the ability to manipulate complex spatial information involving configurations of shapes, such as image fold and movement, or changing their thinking of a two-dimensional object into three-dimensional one (Linn, 1985; Yang and Chen, 2010). Spatial relations ability means to recognize the relationships among visual components of an object (Bosnyák and Nagy-Kondor, 2008; Turgut, 2015).

It remains unclear in current studies whether individuals display similarities or same gender differences in large-scale and small-scale spatial ability. In fact, studies conflict with one another on gender differences in individual spatial ability; that is to say, there are three types of results whether studies concern large-scale or small-scale spatial ability: males show better spatial ability; females show better spatial ability; there is no gender difference in spatial ability (Rilea et al., 2004; Coluccia et al., 2007; Rilea, 2008; Gabriel et al., 2011; Hoffman et al., 2011; Burke et al., 2012). It is exactly because of these conflicting results that gender differences in spatial ability have long been insufficiently summarized or explained. One of the main purposes of our study is to make a meta-analysis of gender differences in individual spatial ability. On a behavioral level, this meta-analysis provides the following advantages: (1) Results can be generalized to a larger population, (2) The precision and accuracy of estimates can be improved as more data is used. This, in turn, may increase the statistical power to detect an effect. (3) Inconsistency of results across studies can be quantified and analyzed. Moderators can be included to explain variation between studies. The Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) method may focus on resolving the following problems in the current brain imaging research: firstly, the number of subjects in the single brain imaging study is generally less, and the results are not stable enough; secondly, the result of a single neuroimaging are probably affected by certain experimental operations (e.g., scan parameters); thirdly, the interpretation of the function of a certain brain region by a single brain imaging study is often limited to a single or a few tasks used.

It is noteworthy that there are already other meta-analyses of gender differences in individual spatial ability. For instance, Linn (1985) used the method of meta-analysis to investigate the questions on the sex differences in spatial ability. Their results suggested that firstly, sex differences arise on mental rotation and spatial perception, but not spatial visualization; secondly, large sex differences are found only on measures of mental rotation; thirdly, smaller sex differences are found on measures of spatial perception; finally, when sex differences are found, they can be detected across the life span. Voyer et al. (1995)'s meta-analysis found that sex differences, favoring males, are clearly significant and homogeneous on the Cards Rotation Test, the generic mental rotation task, the Spatial Relations task, and the Paper Form Board task. Sex differences on the Spatial Relation task and Paper Folding are homogeneous but not significant. The rod and frame test and the Block Design subtest of the various Wechsler intelligence scales show sex differences in some age groups but not others. Finally, scoring and testing procedures proved to have an important influence on the magnitude of sex differences on the Mental Rotations Test, the Water Level Test, the Identical Blocks Test, and the Embedded Figures Test. The size of the failsafe numbers associated with the different analyses suggests that the file drawer phenomenon is not sufficient to account for the prevalence of significant sex differences. Maeda and Yoon (2013) conducted a meta-analysis to estimate the magnitude of gender difference in mental rotation ability and to investigate how factors related to test administration conditions play a role in varying gender difference effect sizes and threatening validity. The results indicated that male participants outperformed females on the test. And the moderator analysis indicated that male superiority on spatial ability tasks is related to the implementation of time limits. The gender difference became larger when stringent time limits were implemented. Reilly and Neumann (2013)'s results of meta-analysis showed that measuring tools can influence gender differences in the studies of individual mental rotation ability. Specifically, Vandenberg instrument produced the highest gender-role effect size of any mental rotation task.

Overall, these meta-analyses show that men are significantly better than women in spatial ability and that such gender difference is subject to measuring tool, task type, experimental time limit, and other factors. It is a pity that they are mostly focused on gender differences in small-scale spatial ability (particularly mental rotation ability), with little reference to gender differences in spatial ability at different scales and the neural basis of such gender differences that are crucial to a comprehensive grasp of gender differences in individual spatial ability. Our meta-analysis attempts to explore whether individuals display the same gender differences in large-scale and small-scale spatial ability and answer “What are the manifestations of such gender differences?,” “What are the contributing factors?,” and “What is the neural basis?”

Study 1

Study 1 discussed gender differences in individual spatial ability on a behavioral level. A moderation analysis was performed of the region, age and education level of the subjects and the time of research publication (all of which are claimed to be big individual spatial ability-influencing factors by existing studies; Silverman et al., 2007; Hoffman et al., 2011; Techentin et al., 2014; Pietschnig and Gittler, 2015) as well as the type of spatial ability.

Methods

Literature Search

We searched the studies on the subject of “spatial ability” published in the past 20 years (1988.01–2018.06) on the database retrieval platform (Web of Science, PubMed, PsycINFO, Google Scholar, and CNKI). And, in order to collect the target documents to the maximum extent, we classified the search keywords into the following four series according to the concept and structure of spatial ability, a total of 38 groups:

  1. Spatial ability; Spatial cognition; Spatial perception; Spatial information processing.

  2. Large-scale Spatial ability; Small-scale Spatial ability.

  3. Navigation; Spatial orientation; Spatial visualization; Spatial relations.

  4. Navigation task/ test; Draw maps task/ test; Way-finding task/ test; Map learning task/ test; Spatial orientation task/ test; Perspective taking task/ test; Spatial visualization task/ test; Mental rotations task/ test; Paper folding task/ test; Spatial relations task/ test; Water level task/ test; Card rotation task/ test; Figures task/ test; Differential aptitude task/ test.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

After four rounds of the above-mentioned search, a total of 1,714 documents were obtained. Then we examined each of the documents in full and incorporated the documents with the following characteristics into the meta-analysis of this study:

  1. All of the subjects are sample groups of healthy people.

  2. The specific experimental method in the study must be behavioral. And the articles must include reports of the males' and females' performance (Means ± Standard deviation, M ± SD, etc.), or statistical test results (r, t, or effect size, etc.) which related to the gender difference, when they finish their independent experimental task respectively.

  3. If an experimental result is reported many times in multiple papers, but it has been recorded only once in the meta-analysis, the research data cannot be used again.

After the above screening process, there were totally 44 studies (see Table 1;, Figure 1), involving 18,522 participants (male = 8,424), that met the standards, and a total of 98 effect size were incorporated in the meta-analysis of this research. Among them, there were 14 effect size associated with large-scale spatial ability and 84 effect size associated with small-scale spatial ability.

Table 1.

Summary of studies included in the present meta-analyses of study 1.

Study N (male) Age Region Identity Year Indicator Spatial ability type g
Alexander, 2006u1 64 (29) 20 USA Undergraduate 2006 Score Small-scale 1.19
Alexander, 2006u2 64 (29) 20 USA Undergraduate 2006 Score Small-scale −0.98
Allison et al., 2017 72 (35) 21.62 UK Undergraduate 2017 Time Large-scale 4.9
Battista, 1990 126 (53) Non USA Middle school student 1990 Score Small-scale 0.94
Brownlow et al., 2003 48 (32) Non USA Undergraduate 2003 Score Small-scale −0.08
Black, 2005u1 97 (64) 24.42 USA Non 2005 Score Small-scale 0.38
Black, 2005u2 97 (64) 24.42 USA Non 2005 Score Small-scale 0.4
Black, 2005u3 97 (64) 24.42 USA Non 2005 Score Small-scale 0.04
Burke et al., 2012 185 (80) 17 UK Non 2012 Time Large-scale 2.58
Cai and Chen, 2000a 39 (25) Non China Pupil 2000 Score Small-scale 0.29
Cai and Chen, 2000b 35 (15) Non China Pupil 2000 Score Small-scale 0.57
Cai and Chen, 2000c 46 (31) Non China Middle school student 2000 Score Small-scale 0.94
Cai and Chen, 2000d 55 (26) Non China Middle school student 2000 Score Small-scale 0.64
Cai and Chen, 2000e 37 (19) Non China Middle school student 2000 Score Small-scale 0.29
Cai and Chen, 2000f 48 (26) Non China Middle school student 2000 Score Small-scale 1.47
Cai and Chen, 2000g 38 (5) Non China Middle school student 2000 Score Small-scale −0.29
Cai and Chen, 2000h 45 (15) Non China Middle school student 2000 Score Small-scale 0.38
Cai and Chen, 2000i 42 (16) Non China Middle school student 2000 Score Small-scale 0.08
Cai and Chen, 2000j 34 (8) Non China Middle school student 2000 Score Small-scale 0.99
Cai and Chen, 2000k 63 (34) Non China Undergraduate 2000 Score Small-scale 0.77
Colom et al., 2004 1593 (794) Non Spain Graduate 2004 Time Large-scale 1.02
Contreras et al., 2007 2624 (1291) 28 Spain Graduate 2007 Time Large-scale −0.59
Coluccia et al., 2007 96 (48) 22.31 Italy Undergraduate 2007 Score Large-scale 0.35
Evardone and Alexander, 2009 105 (50) 20.2 USA Non 2009 Score Small-scale 1.05
Fields and Shelton, 2006u1 40 (20) Non USA Undergraduate 2006 Score Large-scale 0.52
Fields and Shelton, 2006u2 40 (20) Non USA Undergraduate 2006 Score Small-scale 1.23
Fields and Shelton, 2006u3 40 (20) Non USA Undergraduate 2006 Score Large-scale 1.07
Fields and Shelton, 2006u4 40 (20) Non USA Undergraduate 2006 Score Large-scale 0.03
Feng and Tian, 2009 81 (20) Non China Undergraduate 2009 Score Small-scale 0.26
Gabriel et al., 2011u1 74 (40) 20.5 Canada Undergraduate 2011 Accuracy Small-scale 0.48
Gabriel et al., 2011u2 74 (40) 20.5 Canada Undergraduate 2011 Time Small-scale 0.79
Ganley and Vasilyeva, 2011 114 (67) 14 USA Middle school student 2011 Score Small-scale 0.62
Ganley et al., 2014 113 (66) 14 USA Middle school student 2014 Score Small-scale 0.6
Hou et al., 1998a 30 (15) 3.75 China Kindergarten 1998 Score Small-scale −0.09
Hou et al., 1998b 24 (12) 4.25 China Kindergarten 1998 Score Small-scale −0.47
Hou et al., 1998c 19 (11) 4.75 China Kindergarten 1998 Score Small-scale −0.19
Hou et al., 1998d 22 (10) 5.25 China Kindergarten 1998 Score Small-scale 0.11
Hou et al., 1998e 21 (10) 5.75 China Kindergarten 1998 Score Small-scale 0.24
Hou et al., 1998f 27 (13) 6.25 China Kindergarten 1998 Score Small-scale 0.1
Hegarty et al., 2006u1 218 (135) 22 USA Mixed 2006 Score Small-scale 0.21
Hegarty et al., 2006u2 218 (135) 22 USA Mixed 2006 Score Small-scale 0.74
Hegarty et al., 2006u3 218 (135) 22 USA Mixed 2006 Score Large-scale 0.2
Heyden et al., 2016au1 210 (76) 9.92 Netherlands Pupil 2016 Score Small-scale 0.31
Heyden et al., 2016au2 210 (76) 9.92 Netherlands Pupil 2016 Score Small-scale 0.41
Heyden et al., 2016bu1 152 (49) 12 Netherlands Pupil 2016 Score Small-scale −0.08
Heyden et al., 2016bu2 152 (49) 12 Netherlands Pupil 2016 Score Small-scale −0.03
Hromatko and Butkovic, 2009u1 201 (51) 20.81 Vienna Undergraduate 2009 Score Small-scale 0.74
Hromatko and Butkovic, 2009u2 201 (51) 20.81 Vienna Undergraduate 2009 Score Small-scale 0.64
Hoffman et al., 2011 1,279 (661) 18 India Non 2011 Time Small-scale 0.19
Jansen and Heil, 2009 50 (25) 22.8 Germany Non 2009 Score Small-scale 3.79
Kaufman, 2007u1 100 (50) 17 UK Graduate 2007 Score Small-scale 1.16
Kaufman, 2007u2 100 (50) 17 UK Graduate 2007 Score Small-scale 0.42
Lj and Borst, 2011u1 75 (37) 23 USA Graduate 2011 Score Small-scale 0.43
Lj and Borst, 2011u2 75 (37) 23 USA Graduate 2011 Score Small-scale 0.12
Langlois et al., 2013a 59 (42) Non Canada Graduate 2005 Score Small-scale 0.86
Langlois et al., 2013b 54 (43) Non Canada Graduate 2006 Score Small-scale 1.07
Langlois et al., 2013c 18 (9) Non Canada Graduate 2007 Score Small-scale 0.36
Langlois et al., 2013d 34 (12) Non Canada Graduate 2008 Score Small-scale 0.28
Langlois et al., 2013e 49 (25) Non Canada Graduate 2010 Score Small-scale 0.78
Langlois et al., 2013f 59 (42) Non Canada Graduate 2005 Score Small-scale 0.68
Langlois et al., 2013g 54 (43) Non Canada Graduate 2006 Score Small-scale 1.45
Langlois et al., 2013h 18 (9) Non Canada Graduate 2007 Score Small-scale 0.22
Langlois et al., 2013i 34 (12) Non Canada Graduate 2008 Score Small-scale 0.16
Langlois et al., 2013j 49 (25) Non Canada Graduate 2010 Score Small-scale 0.91
Liu, 2016 80 (35) 5.5 China Kindergarten 2016 Score Small-scale 0.21
Liao and Dong, 2017 20 (10) 23 China Graduate 2017 Time Large-scale 0.69
Maeda and Yoon, 2015 2468 (580) Non USA Undergraduate 2015 Score Small-scale 0.6
Merrill et al., 2016u1 153 (73) 17.53 USA Middle school student 2016 Time Large-scale 2.9
Merrill et al., 2016u2 153 (73) 17.53 USA Middle school student 2016 Time Small-scale 0.14
Merrill et al., 2016u3 153 (73) 17.53 USA Middle school student 2016 Time Small-scale 0.37
Merrill et al., 2016u4 153 (73) 17.53 USA Middle school student 2016 Time Small-scale 0.07
Poulin et al., 2004 218 (132) Non USA Undergraduate 2004 Score Small-scale 7.71
Ritter, 2004 79 (37) 21 UK Non 2004 Score Small-scale 0.73
Rilea et al., 2004u1 105 (53) 19.55 USA Undergraduate 2004 Time Small-scale 1.12
Rilea et al., 2004u2 105 (53) 19.55 USA Undergraduate 2004 Time Small-scale 4.58
Rilea et al., 2004u3 105 (53) 19.55 USA Undergraduate 2004 Time Small-scale 0.77
Ruggiero et al., 2008 60 (30) 23.5 Italy Undergraduate 2008 Score Small-scale 0.56
Signorella et al., 1989a 344 (146) Non USA Non 1989 Score Small-scale 0.53
Signorella et al., 1989b 288 (132) Non USA Non 1989 Score Small-scale 0.59
Samsudin, 2008 33 (13) 15.5 Malaysia Middle school student 2008 Score Small-scale 0.33
Tuttle and Pillard, 1991 150 (49) 30.8 USA Non 1991 Score Small-scale 0.61
Tao et al., 2008 260 (125) 14.66 China Middle school student 2008 Score Small-scale 0.58
Tzuriel and Egozi, 2010u1 116 (58) 6.5 Israeli Kindergarten 2010 Score Small-scale 0.27
Tzuriel and Egozi, 2010u2 116 (58) 6.5 Israeli Kindergarten 2010 Score Small-scale 0.83
Wu and Yang, 2014u1 588 (290) 16.92 China Middle school student 2014 Score Large-scale 0.22
Wu and Yang, 2014u2 588 (290) 16.92 China Middle school student 2014 Score Small-scale 0.43
Wu and Yang, 2014u3 588 (290) 16.92 China Middle school student 2014 Score Small-scale 0.13
Xu et al., 2016a 1173 (570) Non USA Undergraduate 2016 Score Small-scale 0.8
Xu et al., 2016b 1110 (537) Non USA Undergraduate 2016 Score Small-scale 0.61
Xu et al., 2016c 1175 (568) Non USA Undergraduate 2016 Score Small-scale 0.58
Xu et al., 2016d 1089 (528) Non USA Undergraduate 2016 Score Small-scale 0.74
Yonker et al., 2008 128 (78) 69.4 Sweden Mixed 2008 Score Small-scale 4.05
Yu et al., 2008u1 44 (22) 22.6 China Undergraduate 2008 Score Small-scale −1.08
Yu et al., 2008u2 44 (22) 22.6 China Undergraduate 2008 Score Small-scale −0.63
Zacks et al., 2001u1 48 (22) Non USA Undergraduate 2001 Score Small-scale 4.09
Zacks et al., 2001u2 48 (22) Non USA Undergraduate 2001 Score Large-scale 3.37
Zacks et al., 2001u3 48 (22) Non USA Undergraduate 2001 Score Large-scale 1.91
Zhou and Yun, 2011 20 (10) Non China Undergraduate 2011 Score Small-scale −0.27
u1–u4

Studies report more than one result for each independent sample.

a–k

Studies include more than one independent sample.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Procedure of data selection [PRISMA 2009 Flow Diagram; Moher et al. (2009); http://www.prisma-statement.org].

Literature Coding

Literature coding is carried out by two researchers separately. The coding results will be compared and, if there is any inconsistency, will be decided by all researchers through discussion. In our study, two researchers produced highly (96%) consistent coding results. The coding content is as follows: (1) authors of literature; (2) numbers of male and female subjects in the experiment; (3) behavioral experimental scores of males and females; (4) type of indicators and direction of gender advantage; (5) type of spatial ability tested in the experiment; (6) region of the subjects; (7) age of the subjects; (8) education level of the subjects; (9) year of publication. (5)–(9) are the moderator variables to be tested in our study.

Effect Size Computation

Our study adopted CMA2.0 for meta-analysis. For continuous data with different units, Cohen's d is often used as a measure of effect in meta-analysis (Cohen, 1988). In studies with varying sample sizes, Cohen's d may produce biased effect estimates. In a study with a small sample size, for example, the effect estimate may be biased to higher values. To correct this, Hedges proposed Hedges' g for an unbiased estimate of effect size (Borenstein et al., 2009; Card, 2012). The corrected, standardized mean difference Hedges' g was used in our study as an unbiased effect size.

Heterogeneity Test

A heterogeneity test must be conducted before measuring the total effect and the moderator effect to see if there is a statistical difference between studies. Cochran's Q test is often used to identify heterogeneity. If the test yields a p value ≤ to 0.01, it can be taken as evidence of heterogeneity; otherwise, it means homogeneity. In addition, I2 provides a measure of the degree of heterogeneity. A higher I2 value suggests a higher degree of heterogeneity. For example, 25, 50, and 75% mean low, moderate and high heterogeneity, respectively.

Model Selection

There are mainly two models for meta-analysis: fixed effects and random effects models; their biggest difference lies in weight. The fixed effects model assumes that there is only one true effect size behind all studies and that the effect size difference between studies is caused by the sampling error. Its meta-analytical results don't apply to other non-meta-analytical studies. The random effects model holds that the true effect size varies between studies and that the effect size difference between studies is due to the true effect size difference as well as the sampling error. With the sample difference taken into consideration, the random effects model's results apply to a broader scope of studies. A heterogeneity test can be used to help model selection. A random effects model will be more suitable in the case of heterogeneity in total effect sizes (Borenstein et al., 2009).

Evaluation of Publication Bias

Publication bias occurs when statistically significant studies are more likely to be published than statistically insignificant studies, adding to the difficulty of collecting statistically insignificant literature during meta-analysis, leading to a systematic error between the included studies and the actual studies, and finally affecting the results of meta-analysis. A host of methods are available for publication bias detection. In our study, a funnel plot, Rosenthal's Failsafe N and Egger's Regression Intercept were employed to detect publication bias.

Results

Heterogeneity Results

Our study first made a heterogeneity test of total effect sizes to decide whether to use a fixed effects model or a random effects model for data analysis. The results are shown in Table 2. The Q-value was significant (p < 0.001), indicating that the effect sizes were heterogeneous. The I2 value was 95.67, indicating that 95.67% of total variation came from the true difference between effect sizes in the model while only 4.33% came from the random error. As 25, 50, and 75% stand for low, moderate, and high heterogeneity, the I2 value of 95.67 indicates high heterogeneity among effect sizes in our study. In view of this, a random effects model was adopted in the analysis of the total effect and the moderator effect. Meanwhile, the Tau2 value was 0.47, indicating that 47% of total variation across studies can be used for calculating weight when putting weight on various studies in the random effects model.

Table 2.

The results of total effect and heterogeneity test.

k g 95% CI Q df I2 Tau2
101 0.72*** [0.58, 0.86] 2309.14*** 100 95.67 0.47

k is the number of independent effect size; n is the sample size; g is the Hedges's g; CI, confidence interval;

***

p < 0.001.

Publication Bias Results

A funnel plot, Rosenthal's Failsafe N and Egger's Regression Intercept were used to detect publication bias in our study. According to the funnel plot (Figure 2), the studies were not evenly distributed on both sides of the total effect size with more on the right side, suggesting a likely presence of publication bias in current studies on individual spatial ability. The funnel plot was only a preliminary look at publication bias from a subjective perspective. In order to take a comprehensive look, we performed the tests of Rosenthal's Failsafe N and Egger's Regression Intercept. In terms of Rosenthal's Failsafe N, if the Nfs value is <5k+10, it is a reminder of the potential impacts of publication bias on current studies24. In our study, the Nfs value was 7,057, larger than the critical value of 5k+10 (515), suggesting an unlikely presence of publication bias in current studies. As for Egger's Regression Intercept, if the regression intercept is close to 0, it is less likely that there is a publication bias. In our study, the intercept value was 2.06, p < 0.05, 95% confidence interval (CI) is [0.61, 3.52], suggesting that there may still be a slight publication bias in current studies. Two of the three publication bias tests showed evidence of a possible publication bias, and therefore we concluded that there was a slight publication bias in current studies. According to Borenstein et al. (2009), however, the purpose of a publication bias test is not to find if there is a publication bias in a meta-analysis but to assess if the publication bias impacts the reliability of the meta-analysis. To this end, Borenstein et al. (2009) provided three results of publication bias assessment: first, the impact of bias is negligible; second, the impact of bias is non-negligible but the results are still valid; third, the results may be wrong. Considering that publication bias can't be completely avoided in psychology research since studies with negative results are less likely to be published, and given that the publication bias in our study isn't serious, we believe our meta-analytical conclusions still provide a reference point.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Funnel plot of standard error by standard difference in means.

Total Effect

As shown in Table 2, the total effect size g is 0.72, p < 0.001, 95% CI is [0.58, 0.86]. Figure 3 presents the “forest plot,” a graphic description of the results based on the random-effects modeling analysis of the all effect sizes. In this forest plot, each effect size (square dot) and its estimated confidence interval (horizontal lines extending from both sides of the squared dot) was graphically shown (Fan et al., 2017).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Forest plot under random-effects model.

Moderator Analyses Results

The results are shown in Table 3. The large-scale spatial ability group and the small-scale spatial ability group had a significant difference in effect size (Qbetween = 5.25, p < 0.05), indicating that the type of individual spatial ability has a significant moderator effect, or that gender differences in spatial ability are subject to the type of spatial ability. To be specific, the effect size of gender differences in the large-scale spatial ability group reached a high level (g = 1.34, p < 0.001); the small-scale spatial ability group only reached a medium level (g = 0.62, p < 0.001). The heterogeneity in both groups also reached a high level (I2largescale spatial ability = 98.81%, I2smallscale spatial ability = 92.19%).

Table 3.

Moderator analyses.

Moderator k g 95% CI I2 Qbetween
Type of spatial ability 5.25*
Large-scale spatial ability 14 1.34*** [0.74, 1.94] 98.81
Small-scale spatial ability 87 0.62*** [0.49, 0.75] 92.19
Region 35.77***
Europe 17 1.18*** [0.65, 1.70] 98.59
America 50 0.90*** [0.73, 1.08] 93.74
Asia 31 0.29*** [0.16, 0.43] 65.96
Age 36.32***
0–5 6 −0.05 [−0.38, 0.27] 0
6–10 5 0.41*** [0.21, 0.60] 40.65
11–15 5 0.34* [0.03, 0.65] 77.40
16–20 18 0.89*** [0.55, 1.23] 96.41
21–25 20 0.64*** [0.33, 0.95] 90.52
Above 26 3 1.34 [−0.69, 3.38] 99.22
Education level 31.31***
Undergraduate 34 0.96*** [0.7, 1.21] 96.11
Graduate 17 0.6* [0.11, 1.08] 97.63
Middle school student 20 0.59*** [0.36, 0.83] 88.97
Pupils 6 0.21* [0.02, 0.41] 39.88
Kindergarten 9 0.21 [−0.06, 0.48] 44.15
Year (Undergraduate) 18.39**
1989–1995 None
1996–2000 4 2.54** [1.04, 4.03] 93.85
2001–2005 5 2.82* [0.54, 5.09] 98.87
2006–2010 13 0.31 [−0.07, 0.68] 86.36
2011–2015 4 0.56*** [0.32, 0.79] 36.54
2016–2019 5 1.01*** [0.69, 1.32] 95.54
Year (Graduate) 19.80***
1989–1995 None
1996–2000 None
2001–2005 3 1.01*** [0.91, 1.11] 0
2006–2010 11 0.57* [−0.01, 1.15] 94.47
2011–2015 2 0.28 [−0.04, 0.60] 0
2016–2019 1 0.73 [−0.18, 1.63] 0
Year (Middle school student) 9.15*
1989–1995 1 0.95*** [0.58, 1.32] 0
1996–2000 8 0.59** [0.23, 0.96] 58.57
2001–2005 None
2006–2010 2 0.55*** [0.32, 0.79] 0
2011–2015 5 0.35*** [0.18, 0.53] 69.59
2016–2019 4 0.86 [−0.23, 1.95] 97.49

When a g has asterisks, it means a significant sex difference; when a Q has asterisks, it means a significant moderating effect;

*

p <0.05,

**

p <0.01,

***

p <0.001.

In addition, different regional groups had a significant difference in effect size (Qbetween = 35.77, p < 0.001), and so did different age groups (Qbetween = 36.32, p < 0.001), different educational groups (Qbetween = 31.31, p < 0.001), and different research time groups (Qbetween (undergraduate) = 18.39, p < 0.01; Qbetween (graduate) = 19.80, p < 0.001; Qbetween (middle school student) = 9.15, p < 0.05). Arguably, these four factors have a significant moderator effect; that is to say, gender differences in individual spatial ability are subject to the region, age and education of the subjects as well as the time of research. As for other groups, the effect size g and heterogeneity I2 are shown in Table 3. It is important to note here that all studies in the database were included in the data analysis when the current study examined the moderator effects of regional factor, age factor, and educational factor. However, when we tested the moderator effect of research time factor, we conducted independent data analysis for the database of undergraduates, graduates, and middle school students. The reason we do this is because current research wants to control as much as possible the interference of age and educational factors on this test process. The above-mentioned process of comparing the spatial ability differences of the tested participants with similar ages and education levels in different time studies is more effective in explaining this problem.

Correlation Analysis Results

Besides, the current research further explores whether the above factors are more closely related to the gender differences in individual spatial ability based on the above findings. Among them, the type of spatial ability and regional factor are categorical variables, age factor, educational factor, and research time factor are grade variables. Therefore, we only analyzed the correlation between the last three factors and the gender differences in individual spatial ability. The results showed that there were significant positive correlations between age and gender differences in spatial ability (rage = 0.519, p < 0.05) and education level and gender differences in spatial ability (reducation level = 0.949, p < 0.05). There was no significant correlation between research time and gender differences in spatial ability.

Discussion 1

Our study found that males significantly outperformed females in spatial ability overall, which is consistent with the findings of previous meta-analyses. In spite of some conflicting conclusions on gender differences in individual spatial ability, males are obviously at an advantage over females in spatial ability from a macro perspective, and this gender advantage remains obvious and stable, at least from the perspective of meta-analysis. The gender advantage, however, isn't obvious and stable enough to be free from any moderator effects. Our study has provided relevant evidence.

In particular, our study has discussed for the first time the type of spatial ability as a moderator between gender differences in individual spatial ability. Indeed, males outperformed females in both large-scale and small-scale Spatial Ability, but the gender differences were much different—the gender difference in large-scale spatial ability is significantly greater than that in small-scale spatial ability with the former at a high level and the latter at a medium level. Such findings provide a broader insight into gender differences in individual spatial ability.

Our study also found that males and females in different regions performed differently in spatial ability. The results of this study and the findings of Silverman et al. (2007)'s study have shown consistency to some extent. Silverman collected data on spatial ability test of nearly 250,000 participants in 40 countries through a network survey conducted by the BBC. The data show that individuals in different countries show significant gender differences in spatial ability. However, this gender difference did not show significant correlation with the Gross National Income (GNI) and Socioeconomic status (SES) indices. In fact, the biggest difference between the research included in the current research database and the Silverman et al. (2007)'s study is that Silverman adopts a non-laboratory design, while other studies adopt a laboratory research design. Therefore, from a more rigorous level, the effect sizes of these two types of experimental designs are not comparable. However, what is certain is that both the current study and Silverman et al. (2007)'s study indicated a regional difference of gender differences in individual spatial ability. Moreover, the current study also found that the gender differences in individual spatial ability in Europe and the Americas are larger, while in Asia, this gender difference is smaller. This is a very interesting discovery, but the existing data does not support a deeper discussion of the reasons for this phenomenon. Of course, we also believe that that the causes of this regional difference can be multifaceted, interactional, complicated, unstable, and remain to be examined in more studies.

In addition, age was found to be one of the biggest contributing factors to gender differences in individual spatial ability, which is also a good supplement to Techentin et al. (2014)'s meta-analytical findings. Their meta-analysis revealed a large age-related decrease in spatial performance on psychometric tests. Specifically, older adults performed worse on psychometric tests than younger adults. On this basis, we performed a correlation analysis of gender differences and age and found that gender differences in individual spatial ability increased as individuals grew older. Due to the complex interaction of other factors like individual differences, growth environment and experimental conditions, this finding may not apply to all meta-analyses, but the high correlation index represents and explains the development and growth trend in connection to gender differences in individual spatial ability.

Our study also verified Hoffman et al. (2011)'s findings about education as a contributing factor to gender differences in individual spatial ability. Our study found that different educational groups had significant gender differences in individual spatial ability. To be specific, gender differences in individual spatial ability increase with the education level from kindergarten to primary school, middle school, college, and postgraduate education. It is interesting that education seems to have widened the gender gap in individual spatial ability instead of making it up. How and why that happens may be complicated and at least unexplainable in our study since age was included as a factor into our individual education level-related analysis.

Our study finally examined the moderator effect of research time on gender differences in individual spatial ability though no connection was found between them. Some studies claim that spatial ability, a part of individual intelligence, is also subject to the Flynn effect. The Flynn effect is the substantial and long-sustained increase in both fluid and crystallized intelligence test scores measured in many parts of the world over the Twentieth century. And, the test score increases have been continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to the present (Pietschnig and Gittler, 2015). However, after comparing Pietschnig and Gittler (2015)'s findings with ours, we decided the Flynn effect was confined to the repeated test of individual spatial ability and that gender differences in individual spatial ability wouldn't change regularly with research time.

Study 2

Study 1 summed up the different manifestations of individual gender differences in large-scale and small-scale Spatial Ability on the behavioral level. Is there a neural basis of such different manifestations? This is exactly the question explored in Study 2 where the ALE approach was used to discuss gender differences in large-scale and small-scale Spatial Ability on the level of neural basis.

In fact, there have been many studies on the causes of gender differences in individual spatial ability. For example, Lawton (1994), Malinowski and Gillespie (2001), Lawton and Kallai (2002), and Gabriel et al. (2011)'s studies all point out that the main reason for the gender difference in individual spatial ability is that females are more prone to spatial anxiety when performing spatial cognitive tasks. In addition, after a systematic review of all previous studies on gender differences in spatial abilities, Wang and Carr (2014) also proposed that the main reason for gender differences in spatial ability was that females and males used different cognitive strategies in spatial cognitive tasks. From the perspective of behavioral level, whether spatial anxiety or cognitive strategy, they have become the most commonly used arguments for explaining gender differences in spatial tasks. However, this problem has not been verified at the neurological level. Therefore, the current research hopes to verify the above problems through study 2 and put forward the following research hypothesis:

  • H1. The neural basis of gender differences in spatial anxiety exists in individuals with large-scale spatial ability.

  • H2. The neural basis of gender differences in spatial anxiety exists in individuals with small-scale spatial ability.

  • H3. The neural basis of gender differences in cognitive strategies exists in individuals with large-scale spatial ability.

  • H4. The neural basis of gender differences in cognitive strategies exists in individuals with small-scale spatial ability.

Methods

Literature Search and Study Selection

Study 2 followed Study 1's logic in literature search and screening. Of all the literature retrieved and downloaded in the first step of Study 1's literature search, we first incorporated those using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (FMRI) or Positron Emission Tomography (PET) technologies into our long list and then coded those meeting the conditions as follows:

  1. All of the subjects are sample groups of healthy people.

  2. Data analysis must adopt the whole brain analysis instead of the region of interest (ROI) analysis, and the data reported by the institute is standardized data (Montreal Neurological Institute-MNI or Talairach).

  3. The specific experimental method in the study must be behavioral. And the articles must include reports of the males' and/or females' brain imaging data when they finish their independent experimental task, respectively.

  4. If an experimental result is reported many times in multiple papers, but it has been recorded only once in the meta-analysis, the research data cannot be used again.

After the above screening process, there were totally 41 studies (Table 4; Figure 1), involving 677 participants (male = 447), that met the standards, and total of 1,366 foci were incorporated in the meta-analysis of this research. Among them, there were 467 foci associated with males' large-scale spatial ability, 51 foci associated with females' large-scale spatial ability, 517 foci related to males' small-scale space ability, and 331 foci related to females' small-scale space ability.

Table 4.

Summary of studies included in the present meta-analyses of study 2.

Study N Mean age Country Task and contrast Foci
LARGE-SCALE SPATIAL ABILITY
Males
Baumann et al., 2010 17 31.6 Australia Navigation task > control, good navigators > poor navigators 64
Grön et al., 2000 24 26 Germany Navigation task > control 18
Hartley and Maguire, 2003, 16 28.9 UK Good navigation performance > poor navigation performance, wayfinding > trail following, wayfinding > route following 23
Ino et al., 2002 16 32.3 Japan Mental navigation task > control 10
Ino et al., 2007 1 55 Japan Navigation task > control 7
IglóiI et al., 2010 19 24.3 UK Training trials in navigation > control trials, allocentric and egocentric responses trials > control trials, allocentric responses trials > egocentric responses trials 95
Lux et al., 2003 14 26.8 Germany Spatial orientation task > control 10
Lee et al., 2005 10 22–25 Hong Kong Spatial orientation task > control 39
Latini-Corazzini et al., 2010 16 21.2 Italy Route task > control, survey task > control, route task > survey task 31
Maguire et al., 1997 11 45 UK Routes, landmarks, film plots, and film frames tasks > control 35
Rosenbaum et al., 2004 10 26.4 Canadian Mental navigation task > control 35
Wolbers and Büchel, 2005 11 19–28 Germany Learning, performance, and change phase in navigation task > control 17
Xu et al., 2010 20 24.2 Norway The conditions of Normal, Without and Blocked in navigation task > Line following 83
Females
Clemente et al., 2013 14 21.64 Spain Navigation > video, navigation > photographs 10
Grön et al., 2000 12 26 Germany Navigation task > control 18
Pintzka et al., 2016 53 22.5 Norway Successful > failed navigation 23
SMALL-SCALE SPATIAL ABILITY
Males
Corradi-Dell'Acqua et al., 2009 17 28.31 Germany Body schema and body structural rotation > control, stimulus strategy > control 5
De Lange et al., 2006 17 24 Netherlands Mental rotation task > control 7
Halari et al., 2006 9 25.78 UK Mental rotation task > control 27
Jordan et al., 2002 10 23.17 Germany Three mental rotation conditions (3d, abstract, letter) > control, 3D-condition > the ABSTRACT- and LETTER-conditions 36
Kucian et al., 2005 22 25.9 Switzerland Mental rotation task > control 37
Kawamichi et al., 2007 14 18–33 Japan Mental rotation task > control 40
Lamm et al., 2001 13 24.5 Austria Mental rotation task > control 11
Lange et al., 2005 6 25 Netherlands Mental rotation task > control 10
Lamm et al., 2007 13 23–31 Austria Location and orientation condition during mental rotation > control 16
Ng et al., 2001 12 29.25 UK Line orientation experiment > control, mental rotation experiment > control 15
O'Boyle et al., 2005 6 14.3 Australia Mental rotation task > control 7
Prescott et al., 2010 8 14.2 Australia Mental rotation task > control 18
Paschke et al., 2012 10 25 Germany Mental rotation task > control 3
Seurinck et al., 2004a 11 25.4 Belgium Mental rotation task > control 46
Sluming et al., 2007 10 41 UK Mental rotation task > control 14
Schöning et al., 2007 14 32 Germany Mental rotation task > control 95
Seurinck et al., 2011 16 24 Netherlands Mental rotation task > control 16
Stoodley et al., 2012 9 25 USA Rotated letters > upright letters 18
Vingerhoets et al., 2002 13 29 Belgium Rotated hands and figures > control 31
Wolbers et al., 2006 16 19–29 Germany Spatial visualization task > control 6
Weiss et al., 2009 16 20–39 Germany Mental rotation > stimulus categorization, mirrored presentation > non-mirrored presentation 59
Females
Ecker et al., 2006 10 20–30 UK Mental rotation task > control 15
Ebisch et al., 2012 22 20–24 Italy Induction–visualization > induction–spatial relationships, visualization–induction > visualization–spatial relationships 8
Gogos et al., 2010 10 55.4 Australia Mental rotation task > control 16
Halari et al., 2006 10 25.78 UK Mental rotation task > control 27
Jordan et al., 2002 14 23.17 Germany Three mental rotation conditions (3d, abstract, letter) > control, 3D-condition > the ABSTRACT- and LETTER-conditions 36
Kucian et al., 2005 12 25.9 Switzerland Mental rotation task > control 37
Papeo et al., 2012 18 22–28 USA Motor strategy and visuospatial strategy > control 15
Seurinck et al., 2004a 11 25.4 Belgium Mental rotation task > control 46
Seurinck et al., 2004b 24 23 Belgium Mental rotation task > control 36
Schöning et al., 2007 20 32 Germany Mental rotation task > control 95

Activation Likelihood Estimation

ALE is the most common algorithm for coordinate-based meta-analyses (Eickhoff et al., 2012). It treats activation foci reported in neuroimaging studies not as single points but as spatial probability distributions centered at the given coordinates. ALE maps are then obtained by computing the union of activation probabilities for each voxel. The current research is using the revised algorithm for ALE analysis which proposed by Eickhoff et al. (2009). It models the spatial uncertainty—and thus probability distribution—of each focus using an estimation of the inter-subject and inter-laboratory variability typically observed in neuroimaging experiments, rather than using a pre-specified full- width half maximum (FWHM) for all experiments as originally proposed. The modified permutation procedure reflects a null-distribution of a random spatial association between studies (i.e., random-effects analysis) not between foci (i.e., fixed-effects analysis; Eickhoff et al., 2016, 2017).

The ALE method may focus on resolving the following problems in the current brain imaging research: firstly, the number of subjects in the single brain imaging study is generally less, and the results are not stable enough; secondly, the result of a single brain imaging are probably affected by certain experimental operations (e.g., scan parameters); thirdly, the interpretation of the function of a certain brain region by a single brain imaging study is often limited to a single or a few tasks used.

The Ginger ALE software (version 2.3, http://www.brainmap.org/ale; Feng et al., 2015) were used to conducted the current meta-analysis (also include the conversion of Talairach coordinates into MNI). The resulting p-value maps were thresholded using the false discovery rate (FDR) correction at p < 0.05 with 5,000 threshold permutations (Genovese et al., 2002; Laird et al., 2005), and all clusters were set to a minimum volume of 600 mm3 (Lamm et al., 2011). The results were overlaid onto an anatomical template (Colin27 T1 seg MNI.nii; Luo et al., 2018) and displayed using the Mango software (http://rii.uthscsa.edu/mango; Feng et al., 2018).

Results

In terms of the large-scale spatial ability, the results of the ALE analysis identified 25 clusters of consistent activation for males and 4 clusters females. The former clusters of males were mainly in bilateral limbic lobe, bilateral occipital lobe, bilateral sub-lobar, bilateral frontal lobe, left temporal lobe, left parietal lobe, bilateral anterior lobe, and left posterior lobe. The latter clusters of females were concentrated in bilateral limbic lobe and right sub-lobar. The specific areas of the individual focus see Table 5 and Figure 4. Furthermore, to explore common and distinct neural regions of males and females, we compared the above ALE meta-analytic results. The result of conjunction analysis revealed that left limbic lobe was activated in both of them. In addition, females and males contrast demonstrated correspondence in bilateral sub-lobar and bilateral limbic lobe. Conversely, no suprathreshold clusters were revealed by the males and females contrast (Table 6; Figure 4).

Table 5.

ALE meta-analysis results for large-scale spatial ability.

Cluster No. Laterality Brain regions BA x y z ALE (102) Cluster size (mm3)
Males
  1 R Parahippocampal Gyrus 36 26 −40 −10 0.050369 3288
  2 L Parahippocampal Gyrus 36 −22 −44 −12 0.034565 2200
L Parahippocampal Gyrus 19 −32 −42 −4 0.022634
  3 L Posterior Cingulate 30 −14 −56 18 0.02931 1784
L Posterior Cingulate 29 −10 −52 8 0.020769
  4 R Superior Occipital Gyrus 19 44 −78 32 0.030861 1064
  5 R Claustrum 32 24 −4 0.039246 1032
  6 L Medial Frontal Gyrus 32 0 12 48 0.029162 872
  7 L Insula 13 −32 24 −2 0.030118 736
  8 R Posterior Cingulate 30 16 −54 16 0.02585 664
  9 L Superior Temporal Gyrus 38 −42 18 −34 0.023836 496
  10 L Precuneus 7 −4 −66 54 0.023073 496
  11 L Middle Occipital Gyrus 19 −32 −84 24 0.024111 472
  12 L Superior Occipital Gyrus 19 −32 −86 40 0.024569 416
  13 R Superior Frontal Gyrus 10 28 56 −6 0.022647 384
  14 R Culmen 8 −46 4 0.019288 376
  15 R Parahippocampal Gyrus 28 24 −24 −8 0.020488 304
  16 R Caudate 18 28 8 0.017964 304
R Caudate 18 26 0 0.01794
  17 L Culmen −10 −70 −2 0.018203 288
  18 R Thalamus 8 −16 8 0.019477 232
  19 R Lingual Gyrus 18 6 −82 −6 0.01892 168
  20 R Inferior Frontal Gyrus 9 48 12 30 0.018163 168
  21 L Lingual Gyrus 18 −6 −80 −6 0.017503 144
  22 L Cerebellar Tonsil −34 −50 −36 0.01747 136
  23 R Parahippocampal Gyrus 30 −6 −16 0.017945 136
  24 L Inferior Frontal Gyrus 6 −48 6 32 0.018083 136
  25 R Cuneus 19 12 −78 42 0.017716 136
Females
  1 R Parahippocampal Gyrus 22 −12 −16 0.019123 1280
R Lentiform Nucleus 18 −6 −12 0.019109
R Lentiform Nucleus 26 −6 −10 0.01542
R Parahippocampal Gyrus 35 20 −22 −18 0.013516
R Parahippocampal Gyrus 28 28 −20 −14 0.011108
  2 L Parahippocampal Gyrus 28 −15 −6 −14 0.019109 464
  3 L Parahippocampal Gyrus 28 −14 −14 −18 0.01877 336
  4 L Parahippocampal Gyrus 37 −26 −46 −6 0.014915 208

BA, Brodmann area; R, right; L, left.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on males' (A) and females' (B) large-scale spatial ability. And the results of conjunction analysis and contrast analysis: (C) The common regions associated with males' and females' large-scale spatial ability; (D) Brain regions activated more by females than males.

Table 6.

ALE meta-analysis results of conjunction and contrast analysis of large-scale spatial ability of meals and females.

Cluster No. Laterality Brain regions BA x Y z ALE (102) Cluster size (mm3)
Males∩Females
  1 L Parahippocampal Gyrus 37 −26 −46 −6 0.014915 184
Males > Females
  None
Females > Males
  1 R Lentiform Nucleus 16 −8 −10 0.01058 1776
R Lentiform Nucleus 14 −4 −12 0.01028
R Parahippocampal Gyrus 28 20 −16 −16 0.00768
R Parahippocampal Gyrus 28 18 −12 −16 0.0101
R Lentiform Nucleus 23.6 −8.1 −10.2 0.01085
  2 L Parahippocampal Gyrus 34 −14.8 −16 −20 0.0114 968
L Lentiform Nucleus −12 −8 −12 0.01086
L Lentiform Nucleus −16 −10 −12 0.00733
  3 L Parahippocampal Gyrus 19 −24 −46 −2 0.0054 424

Then, as to the small-scale spatial ability, the results of the ALE analysis identified 18 clusters of consistent activation for males and 13 clusters females (Table 7; Figure 5). The previous clusters of males were mainly in bilateral parietal lobe, bilateral frontal lobe, bilateral occipital lobe, bilateral posterior lobe, and left sub-lobar. The next clusters of females were concentrated in bilateral frontal lobe, bilateral parietal lobe, and bilateral occipital lobe. Besides, the result of conjunction analysis revealed that bilateral frontal lobe, left occipital lobe, and bilateral parietal lobe were activated in both of males and females. Finally, females and males contrast demonstrated correspondence in bilateral frontal lobe and right parietal lobe. Similarly, no suprathreshold clusters were revealed by the males and females contrast (Table 8; Figure 5).

Table 7.

ALE meta-analysis results for small-scale spatial ability.

Cluster No. Laterality Brain regions BA X Y z ALE (102) Cluster size (mm3)
Males
  1 R Precuneus 7 24 −68 46 0.034162 10360
R Precuneus 7 26 −60 54 0.033126
R Precuneus 7 30 −52 54 0.031728
R Precuneus 7 34 −40 42 0.026622
R Superior Parietal Lobule 7 42 −52 54 0.023221
R Supramarginal Gyrus 40 40 −40 34 0.017652
  2 L Inferior Parietal Lobule 40 −38 −40 48 0.046883 8664
L Superior Parietal Lobule 7 −22 −62 54 0.035032
L Superior Parietal Lobule 7 −38 −52 62 0.017468
L Precuneus 7 −18 −76 50 0.014879
  3 L Precentral Gyrus 6 −26 −6 54 0.040707 3648
  4 L Medial Frontal Gyrus 32 2 14 48 0.02908 3280
R Medial Frontal Gyrus 6 6 22 46 0.022784
  5 L Occipital Gyrus 19 −40 −80 2 0.026659 2880
L Fusiform Gyrus 19 −46 −72 −6 0.022888
  6 R Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 28 −6 56 0.035374 2872
R Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 30 2 48 0.02338
R Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 34 4 62 0.015455
  7 L Inferior Frontal Gyrus 9 −46 8 26 0.025227 1856
  8 R Tuber 42 −62 −32 0.032616 1368
  9 R Inferior Frontal Gyrus 9 58 14 24 0.022069 1312
R Middle Frontal Gyrus 9 56 10 36 0.019196
  10 R Precuneus 31 34 −72 22 0.024916 1192
  11 L Pyramis −42 −74 −32 0.029963 656
  12 L Insula 13 −32 26 −2 0.023349 656
  13 L Precuneus 19 −28 −72 36 0.020124 544
  14 R Inferior Frontal Gyrus 47 32 30 −8 0.020464 376
  15 R Middle Occipital Gyrus 18 46 −82 −8 0.019319 312
  16 R Middle Frontal Gyrus 46 44 40 14 0.018787 272
  17 R Cuneus 19 10 −86 46 0.018375 240
  18 R Postcentral Gyrus 2 56 −22 38 0.015817 112
Females
  1 L Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 −22 −4 54 0.04258 2464
  2 R Sub-Gyral 6 28 0 54 0.055919 1984
  3 R Precuneus 7 22 −54 60 0.031903 1648
  4 L Inferior Occipital Gyrus 19 −42 −72 −4 0.024078 1360
L Lingual Gyrus 18 −34 −78 −2 0.017991
  5 L Inferior Parietal Lobule 40 −46 −30 40 0.021931 1096
  6 L Inferior Frontal Gyrus 9 −48 8 28 0.024503 984
L Inferior Frontal Gyrus 9 −50 14 20 0.01735
  7 R Inferior Occipital Gyrus 18 34 −82 0 0.024498 920
  8 R Inferior Parietal Lobule 40 38 −42 46 0.01945 904
  9 L Precuneus 7 −26 −68 38 0.025225 856
  10 L Precuneus 7 −24 −56 60 0.018502 808
L Precuneus 7 −20 −50 54 0.017932
  11 R Inferior Frontal Gyrus 9 52 8 24 0.020205 704
R Precentral Gyrus 6 46 6 30 0.01523
  12 R Precuneus 7 32 −60 38 0.018497 312
  13 R Inferior Frontal Gyrus 44 62 14 16 0.018392 264

Figure 5.

Figure 5

ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on males' (A) and females' (B) small-scale spatial ability. And the results of conjunction analysis and contrast analysis: (C) The common regions associated with males' and females' small-scale spatial ability; (D) Brain regions activated more by females than males.

Table 8.

ALE meta-analysis results of conjunction and contrast analysis of small-scale spatial ability of meals and females.

Cluster No. Laterality Brain regions BA x y z ALE (102) Cluster size (mm3)
Males∩females
  1 L Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 −24 −4 54 0.034997 1576
  2 R Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 28 −4 56 0.030599 1080
  3 L Fusiform Gyrus 19 −42 −70 −6 0.020082 888
L Inferior Occipital Gyrus 19 −36 −78 0 0.017696
  4 L Inferior Frontal Gyrus 9 −48 8 28 0.023748 720
  5 R Inferior Parietal Lobule 40 38 −42 44 0.017832 408
  6 R Precuneus 7 26 −54 58 0.021182 408
  7 L Precuneus 19 −26 −72 38 0.018395 272
  8 L Precuneus 7 −24 −56 58 0.017343 208
  9 L Inferior Parietal Lobule 40 −42 −34 42 0.017033 184
  10 R Inferior Frontal Gyrus 9 56 10 24 0.01525 176
Males > Females
  None
Females > Males
  1 R Sub-Gyral 6 24.4 1.9 55.3 0.03247 800
  2 R Precuneus 7 15.9 −53.1 61.8 0.01462 576
  3 L Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 −17.8 −1.1 53.8 0.02327 552

Discussion 2

Firstly, we discovered that when males were accomplishing large-scale spatial tasks, the areas with higher communalities in the activated brain regions included bilateral parahippocampal gyrus, bilateral posterior cingulate, bilateral superior occipital gyrus, right claustrum, left medial frontal gyrus, left insula, left superior temporal gyrus, left precuneus, left middle occipital gyrus, right superior frontal gyrus, bilateral culmen, right caudate, right thalamus, bilateral lingual gyrus, bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, left cerebellar tonsil, and right cuneus. Correspondingly, when females were accomplishing large-scale spatial tasks, the areas with higher consistency in the activated brain regions included bilateral parahippocampal gyrus and right lentiform nucleus. In conclusion, the above-mentioned brain regions are important neural basis for the large-scale spatial ability of males and females.

Besides, the most important work of this research is to compare the similarity and difference of neural activity in the course of cognitive processing of large-scale spatial information of males and females by the method of ALE. As shown by the results, we found that males and females still had a neural basis with higher communalities when completing large-scale spatial tasks. The results of conjunction analysis prove that left parahippocampal gyrus participate in the cognitive processing of large-scale spatial information of both males and females. This result indicates that the parahippocampal gyrus is the core of the neural basis of large-scale spatial ability no matter for males or for females.

What's more, we not only analyzed the neural basis shared by the males' and females' large-scale spatial ability, but we also analyzed and reported the specific neural activities they involved. Specifically, on the one hand, we discovered through the analysis of the contrast of females and males, that when females executed large-scale spatial tasks, the bilateral lentiform nucleus and bilateral parahippocampal gyrus were activated more intensely. Among them, lentiform nucleus was extensively proved to be related to the individual's emotional experience. Moreover, lentiform nucleus had a close link to individual negative emotions, which is also consistent with the findings of previous studies (Goldin et al., 2008; Telzer et al., 2014; Wardle et al., 2014; Touroutoglou et al., 2015). It shows that females experience more negative emotions (spatial anxiety) than males when performing large-scale spatial tasks. This result is consistent with previous studies on many behavioral levels (Lawton, 1994; Malinowski and Gillespie, 2001; Lawton and Kallai, 2002; Gabriel et al., 2011). Gabriel et al. (2011)'s study proposed that the key to gender differences in individual spatial ability was the time limit for experimental tasks. The time limit puts females under great pressure so that they feel fear and anxiety, and such negative emotions are the direct cause of males outperforming females. Our study further verified Gabriel et al. (2011)'s theory with evidence of its neural basis. Meanwhile, our findings support the research hypothesis 1. In detail, in large-scale spatial tasks, female individuals exhibit stronger neural activity related to spatial anxiety than male individuals.

In addition, as mentioned above, parahippocampal gyrus is the main neural basis of individual large-scale spatial ability. The interesting thing to note is that males still outperformed females in large-scale spatial tasks even though females were better activated in parahippocampal gyrus, which probably means their parahippocampal gyrus works less efficiently than males. On the other hand, nevertheless, the analysis of the contrast between males and females found no specific brain regions in males, showing that males have no specific neural basis for cognitive processing of large-scale spatial information. In other words, it can be considered as, the contrast between males and females studies failed to show any suprathreshold cluster, demonstrating that males' spatial ability recruits a subset of areas also by females' spatial ability. In view of this, we believe behavioral gender differences in large-scale spatial ability are mainly due to the specific neural basis of females rather than males.

Secondly, corresponding to the above gender differences in large-scale spatial ability. we also discovered that when males were accomplishing small-scale spatial tasks, the areas with higher communalities in the activated brain regions included bilateral precuneus, bilateral superior parietal lobule, right supramarginal gyrus, left inferior parietal lobule, left precentral gyrus, bilateral medial frontal gyrus, left occipital gyrus, left fusiform gyrus, bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, right tuber, right middle frontal gyrus, left pyramis, left insula, right middle occipital gyrus, right cuneus, and right postcentral gyrus. Correspondingly, when females were accomplishing small-scale spatial tasks, the areas with higher consistency in the activated brain regions included left middle frontal gyrus, right sub-gyral, bilateral precuneus, bilateral inferior occipital gyrus, left lingual gyrus, bilateral inferior parietal lobule, bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, and right precentral gyrus. In general, the above-mentioned brain regions are important neural basis for the small-scale spatial ability of males and females.

Then, similarly, the next most important work of this research is to compare the similarity and difference of neural activity in the course of cognitive processing of small-scale spatial information of males and females. As shown by the results, we found that males and females still had a neural basis with higher communalities when completing small-scale spatial tasks. The results of conjunction analysis prove that bilateral middle frontal gyrus, left fusiform gyrus, left inferior occipital gyrus, bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, bilateral inferior parietal lobule, and bilateral precuneus participate in the cognitive processing of small-scale spatial information of both males and females. This result indicates that the above-mentioned brain regions are important neural basis for individual small-scale spatial ability no matter for males or for females.

In addition, the analysis of the contrast between females and males showed that compared with males, females were stronger activated in right sub-gyral, right precuneus, and left middle frontal gyrus during cognitive processing of small-scale spatial information, which corresponds to the above gender differences in large-scale spatial ability. Right sub-gyral is the key brain region for the individual to perform executive control (Kerstin et al., 2005; Schubotz and von Cramon, 2009; Fan et al., 2012; Dambacher et al., 2013). Butler et al. (2006) said females made more efforts in top-down executive control than males and were thus better activated in brain regions like sub-gyral when performing cognitive processing of spatial information, which is in line with our findings. Besides, precuneus and middle frontal gyrus are also proved to be key brain regions reflecting individual adoption of the egocentric strategy (Galati et al., 2000). This finding also supports our research hypothesis 4. The allocentric and the egocentric are the two most commonly used strategies for individuals to perform cognitive processing of spatial information. A lot of researchers hold that the egocentric strategy is more suitable for individuals to perform large-scale spatial tasks while the allocentric strategy is more suitable in small-scale spatial tasks (Malinowski, 2001; Zacks et al., 2001; Peña et al., 2008). It can therefore be inferred that behavioral gender differences in small-scale spatial ability may also be associated with cognitive strategies adopted by individuals; that is to say, females tend to adopt the egocentric strategy when performing small-scale spatial tasks, whereas the allocentric strategy is the best choice for individuals to perform such spatial tasks. It is precisely the failure to make the best choice that leads to the inferior performance of females. Meanwhile, it is noteworthy that all our discussions are based on the premise of “most females.” In fact, there is no absolute difference and boundary in individual cognitive strategies whether between males and females or between large-scale and small-scale spatial tasks, which makes it possible that both males and females choose either strategy when it comes to large-scale or small-scale spatial tasks. Furthermore, on the other side, in terms of small-scale spatial ability, the analysis of the contrast of males and females saw no specific brain regions in males, which is similar to the result of the large-scale spatial ability analysis. It shows that males have no specific neural basis for cognitive processing of small-scale spatial information. Similarly, we believe behavioral gender differences in small-scale spatial ability are mainly due to the specific neural basis of females rather than males.

General Discussion

We first revealed the different behavioral manifestations of individual gender differences in large-scale and small-scale Spatial Ability through Study 1. We then analyzed the potential neural basis of such behavioral differences through Study 2. On the behavioral level, we found in Study 1 individuals showed a high level of gender differences in large-scale spatial ability and a medium level of gender differences in small-scale spatial ability. Although males outperformed females in both large-scale and small-scale Spatial Ability, this gender gap is significantly smaller in small-scale spatial ability than in large-scale spatial ability.

On the level of neural basis, we found in Study 2 that males and females shared a common neural basis in both large-scale and small-scale Spatial Ability. The different thing is that compared with large-scale spatial ability there are more overlapping brain regions in small-scale spatial ability, which means a broader neural basis shared by males and females. We believe this is also one of the reasons for different behavioral manifestations of gender differences in large-scale and small-scale Spatial Ability. At the same time, no specific brain regions were found in males in both Spatial Ability, while females showed some specific brain activities. It should be noted, however, that the specific brain activities of females manifested completely differently in large-scale and small-scale Spatial Ability. This also suggests that although females performed not so well as males in both Spatial Ability, the reasons for such performance were completely different. That is to say, the reason why females performed not so well in large-scale spatial ability was that they were more susceptible to spatial anxiety and their parahippocampal gyrus worked less efficiently than males; females performed not so well in small-scale spatial ability because they mostly adopted the egocentric strategy and their sub-gyral also worked less efficiently than males. The two different reasons have made for gender differences in favor of males in terms of spatial ability and such gender differences have different manifestations in large-scale and small-scale Spatial Ability.

To sum up, we believe that behavioral gender differences in large-scale and small-scale Spatial Ability are mainly due to different neural bases. But what makes for such difference in the neural basis? The evidence in this regard is still inconclusive. A great many researchers have offered their thoughts, hypotheses, and explanations. Some hypotheses were made from the perspective of evolutionary psychology: (1) Dispersal Hypothesis- Natal dispersal distance varies between sexes. (2) Fertility and parental care Hypothesis- Females reduce mobility to decrease mortality during reproductive periods. (3) Male foraging Hypothesis- ivision of foraging labor: Men use navigation skills for hunting. (4) Range size- Polygynous males have larger ranges to mate with more females. (5) Male warfare- Men travel long distances to kill competitors and capture females. (6) Female choice- Women choose males on the basis of their hunting success (Jones et al., 2003). The most widely recognized one is the Male foraging Hypothesis, also known as the Hunter-Gatherer Theory (HGT; Silverman et al., 2007; Burke et al., 2012). The hunter-gatherer is a human living in a society in which most or all food is obtained by foraging (collecting wild plants and pursuing wild animals). Hunting and gathering was humanity's first and most successful adaptation, occupying at least 90 percent of human history (Little, 2016). Accordingto the HGT, sex-specific patterns of spatial behavior emerged with the appearance of a hunter-gatherer way of life, accompanied by a sexual division of labor. Men in prehistory are assumed to have been predominantly hunters, ranging widely in unfamiliar surroundings (Burke et al., 2012). In a nutshell, such long-term learning, practice, reinforcement, experience, and evolution are responsible for gender differences in individual spatial ability today.

Of course, in addition to the above factors, there are also possible reasons for the current gender differences in individual spatial ability is social factors. For both children and adults, female individuals and male individuals are given different social expectations in social development. This social expectation has a subtle influence on the performance of female and male individuals in many social activities, such as the choice of toys or games (Raag and Rackliff, 1998; Raag, 1999), social division of labor (Kluwer and Mikula, 2003), learning and scientific research (Hirshfield, 2014). Under the influence of social expectations, individuals' continuous adaptation, and long-term practice of the above social activities may cause and exacerbate the existing gender differences in such individual spatial abiliy. And these gender differences will in turn affect the individual's subsequent social activities, and so on. For example, According to the statistics of Hoffman et al. (2011)'s study, In the general science, engineering, and technology industries, the number of male labor is four times that of female. In the more prominent areas of research of math, chemistry, physics, and mechanical engineering, male tenured professors outnumber female tenured professors 8 to 15 times. Of course, there are many similar data. Hirshfield (2014) believes that the reason for this situation is that female individuals face many obstacles that male individuals do not face in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) educational programmes and careers. Such as differential pay, chilly departmental or workplace climates, a greater likelihood of leaving STEM programmes or careers, better to be an adjunct faculty, and family pressures. Hirshfield (2014) also points out that female individuals often face the following three kinds of prejudicial social expectations in STEM educational programmes and careers: science is associated with masculinity, being a professor is also associated with being a man, and leadership is often associated with men and masculinity and as a result. These social expectations may have a negative impact on females' study and work in STEM and other fields. And the lack of adequate STEM training may lead to a gap between females and males in terms of spatial ability. We can't say that such gender differences are mainly because females are inferior to males in spatial ability, but we believe individual spatial ability is at least one of the contributing factors. Of course, the causes of gender difference in individual spatial ability are complex and long-term, and it cannot be effectively solved in one or two aspects in a short time. As far as the current research is concerned, we hope to provide more reference and inspiration for understanding and solving the gender differences in individual spatial ability from the cognitive and neural basis levels. As spatial ability is inextricably linked to the above disciplines and fields, we hope our study can offer a new perspective to enhance women's spatial ability as well as a targeted, viable approach to that end, examples of which can be training women in control and alleviate spatial anxiety or improving cognitive strategy, training individual parahippocampal gyrus by use of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), and so forth. Spatial ability training and transfer as such are exactly what we hope to study in the future.

Several limitations of the current study should be noted. Firstly, the quantity of researches on large-scale spatial ability in study 1 as well as study 2, especially for females, is not abundant. Secondly, unlike meta-analyses used in other fields of research, the ALE calculations based on neuroimaging do not consider the size of an effect; consequently, they cannot include evidence for the absence of an effect, so-called null results. And the ALE also cannot illuminate the temporal dynamics of cognitive processes (Winlove et al., 2018).

Conclusion

To summarize, the main work of this study is that we find that although males outperform females in both large-scale and small-scale spatial ability, individuals show a high level of gender differences in large-scale spatial ability and a medium level of gender differences in small-scale spatial ability within acceptable publication bias.Then, we also discover that campared to meals, females demonstrate a stronger activation in bilateral lentiform nucleus and bilateral parahippocampal gyrus in the large-scale spatial task, and activation in right sub-gyral, right precuneus, and left middle frontal gyrus in small-scale spatial task. We belive that the reason why females perform not so well in large-scale spatial ability is that they are more susceptible to emotions and their parahippocampal gyrus work less efficiently than males; females perform not so well in small-scale spatial ability because they mostly adopt the egocentric strategy and their sub-gyral also work less efficiently than males. This also suggests that the above tow different reasons lead to the different behavioral performance of gender differences in large- and small-scale spatial ability.

Data Availability

All datasets generated for this study are included in the manuscript and/or the Supplementary Files.

Author Contributions

LY, FK, YL, SZ, JL, and XY: Design of the study, data collection, data analysis, paper writing and revising.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Funding. This research was supported by the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (2016CBZ010; 2017TS115), Key research and development programs in Shaanxi (2017SF-271), and the Major Project of Medicine Science and Technology of PLA (AWS17J012).

References

  1. Alexander G. M. (2006). Associations among gender-linked toy preferences, spatial ability, and digit ratio: evidence from eye-tracking analysis. Arch. Sex. Behav. 35, 699–709. 10.1007/s10508-006-9038-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allison C., Redhead E. S., Chan W. (2017). Interaction of task difficulty and gender stereotype threat with a spatial orientation task in a virtual nested environment. Learn. Motiv. 57, 22–35. 10.1016/j.lmot.2017.01.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Battista M. T. (1990). Spatial visualization and gender differences in high school geometry. J. Res. Math. Educ. 21, 47–60. 10.2307/749456 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baumann O., Chan E., Mattingley J. B. (2010). Dissociable neural circuits for encoding and retrieval of object locations during active navigation in humans. Neuroimage 49, 2816–2825. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Black A. A. (2005). Spatial ability and earth science conceptual understanding. J. Geosci. Educ. 53, 402–414. 10.5408/1089-9995-53.4.402 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Borenstein M., Hedges L. V., Higgins J. P. T., Rothstein H. R. (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Chichester: John Wiley. 10.1002/9780470743386 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bosnyák A., Nagy-Kondor R. (2008). The spatial ability and spatial geometrical knowledge of university students majored in mathematics. Acta Didact. Universit. Comenianae. 8, 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  8. Brownlow S., Mcpheron T. K., Acks C. N. (2003). Science background and spatial abilities in men and women. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 12, 371–380. 10.1023/B:JOST.0000006297.90536.7c [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Burke A., Kandler A., Good D. (2012). Women who know their place: sex-based differences in spatial abilities and their evolutionary significance. Hum. Nat. 23, 133–148. 10.1007/s12110-012-9140-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Butler T., Imperato-Mcginley J., Pan H., Voyer D., Cordero J., Zhu Y., et al. (2006). Sex differences in mental rotation: top-down versus bottom-up processing. Neuroimage 32, 445–456. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cai H. J., Chen Q. (2000). A preliminary study on the development of mental rotation ability and its correlation with intelligence. Psychol. Sci. 23, 363–373. [Google Scholar]
  12. Card N. A. (2012). Applied Meta-Analysis for Social Science Research. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  13. Clemente M., Rey B., Rodríguezpujadas A., Barrosloscertales A., Baños R. M., Botella C., et al. (2013). An fmri study to analyze neural correlates of presence during virtual reality experiences. Interact. Comput. 26, 269–284. 10.1093/iwc/iwt037 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Clements D. H. (1998). Geometric and spatial thinking in young children, in Engaging Young Children in Mathematics: Standards for Early Childhood Mathematics Education, ed Copley J. V. (Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics; ), 267–297. 10.4324/9781410609236 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cohen J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Technometrics 31, 499–500. 10.1080/00401706.1989.10488618 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Colom R., Ma J. C., Arend I., Leal O. G., Santacreu J. (2004). Sex differences in verbal reasoning are mediated by sex differences in spatial ability. Psychol. Record 54, 365–372. 10.1007/BF03395479 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Coluccia E., Iosue G., Brandimonte M. A. (2007). The relationship between map drawing and spatial orientation abilities: a study of gender differences. J. Environ. Psychol. 27, 135–144. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.12.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Contreras M. J., Rubio V. J., Peña D., Colom R., Santacreu J. (2007). Sex differences in dynamic spatial ability: the unsolved question of performance factors. Mem. Cogn. 35, 297–303. 10.3758/BF03193450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Corradi-Dell'Acqua C., Tomasino B., Fink G. R. (2009). What is the position of an arm relative to the body? Neural correlates of body schema and body structural description. J. Soc. Neurosci. 29, 4162–4171. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4861-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dambacher F., Sack A. T., Lobbestael J., Arntz A., Brugman S., Schuhmann T. (2013). A network approach to response inhibition: dissociating functional connectivity of neural components involved in action restraint and action cancellation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 39:821. 10.1111/ejn.12425 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. De Lange F. P., Helmich R. C., Toni I. (2006). Posture influences motor imagery: an fmri study. Neuroimage 33, 609–617. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ebisch S. J., Perrucci M. G., Mercuri P., Romanelli R., Mantini D., Romani G. L., et al. (2012). Common and unique neuro-functional basis of induction, visualization, and spatial relationships as cognitive components of fluid intelligence. Neuroimage 62, 331–342. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ecker C., Brammer M. J., David A. S., Williams S. C. (2006). Time-resolved fmri of mental rotation revisited-dissociating visual perception from mental rotation in female subjects. Neuroimage 32, 432–444. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Eickhoff S. B., Bzdok D., Laird A. R., Kurth F., Fox P. T. (2012). Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. Neuroimage 59, 2349–2361. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Eickhoff S. B., Laird A. R., Fox P. M., Lancaster J. L., Fox P. T. (2017). Implementation errors in the gingerale software: description and recommendations. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 1–5. 10.1002/hbm.23342 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Eickhoff S. B., Laird A. R., Grefkes C., Wang L. E., Zilles K., Fox P. T. (2009). Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2907–2926. 10.1002/hbm.20718 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Eickhoff S. B., Nichols T. E., Laird A. R., Hoffstaedter F., Amunts K., Fox P. T. (2016). Behavior, sensitivity, and power of activation likelihood estimation characterized by massive empirical simulation. Neuroimage 137, 70–85. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.072 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Evardone M., Alexander G. M. (2009). Anxiety, sex-linked behaviors, and digit ratios (2d:4d). Arch. Sex. Behav. 38, 442–455. 10.1007/s10508-007-9260-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fan H., Xu J., Cai Z., He J., Fan X. (2017). Homework and students' achievement in math and science: a 30-year meta-analysis, 1986–2015. Educ. Res. Rev. 20, 35–54. 10.1016/j.edurev.2016.11.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fan J., Bernardi S., Dam N. T., Anagnostou E., Gu X., Martin L., et al. (2012). Functional deficits of the attentional networks in autism. Brain Behav. 2, 647–660. 10.1002/brb3.90 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Feng C., Becker B., Huang W., Wu X., Eickhoff S. B., Chen T. (2018). Neural substrates of the emotion-word and emotional counting stroop tasks in healthy and clinical populations: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroimage 173:258. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Feng C., Luo Y. J., Krueger F. (2015). Neural signatures of fairness-related normative decision making in the ultimatum game: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 591–602. 10.1002/hbm.22649 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Feng J., Tian L. (2009). Spatial ability testing and its influence on engineering graphics study. J. Eng. Graphics 6, 142–148. [Google Scholar]
  34. Fields A. W., Shelton A. L. (2006). Individual skill differences and large-scale environmental learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 32:506. 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.506 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gabriel K. I., Hong S. M., Chandra M., Lonborg S. D., Barkley C. L. (2011). Gender differences in the effects of acute stress on spatial ability. Sex Roles 64, 81–89. 10.1007/s11199-010-9877-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Galati G., Lobel E., Vallar G., Berthoz A., Pizzamiglio L., Le Bihan D., et al. (2000). The neural basis of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in humans: a functional magnetic resonance study. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 156–164. 10.1007/s002210000375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ganley C. M., Vasilyeva M. (2011). Sex differences in the relation between math performance, spatial skills, and attitudes. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 32, 235–242. 10.1016/j.appdev.2011.04.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ganley C. M., Vasilyeva M., Dulaney A. (2014). Spatial ability mediates the gender difference in middle school students' science performance. Child Dev. 85,1419–1432. 10.1111/cdev.12230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Genovese C. R., Lazar N. A., Nichols T. (2002). Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15, 870–878. 10.1006/nimg.2001.1037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gogos A., Gavrilescu M., Davison S., Searle K., Adams J., Rossell S. L., et al. (2010). Greater superior than inferior parietal lobule activation with increasing rotation angle during mental rotation: an fmri study. Neuropsychologia 48, 529–535. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Goldin P. R., Mcrae K., Ramel W., Gross J. J. (2008). The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 577–586. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Grön G., Wunderlich A. P., Spitzer M., Tomczak R., Riepe M. W. (2000). Brain activation during human navigation: gender-different neural networks as substrate of performance. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 404–408. 10.1038/73980 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Halari R., Sharma T., Hines M., Andrew C., Simmons A., Kumari V. (2006). Comparable fmri activity with differential behavioural performance on mental rotation and overt verbal fluency tasks in healthy men and women. Exp. Brain Res. 169, 1–14. 10.1007/s00221-005-0118-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hartley T., Maguire E. A. (2003). The well-worn route and the path less traveled: distinct neural bases of route following and wayfinding in humans. Neuron 37, 877–888. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00095-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hegarty M., Montello D. R., Richardson A. E., Ishikawa T., Lovelace K. (2006). Spatial abilities at different scales: individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence 34, 151–176. 10.1016/j.intell.2005.09.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hegarty M., Waller D. (2004). A dissociation between mental rotation and perspective-taking spatial abilities. Intelligence 32, 175–191. 10.1016/j.intell.2003.12.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Heyden K. M. V., Atteveldt N. M. V., Huizinga M., Jolles J. (2016). Implicit and explicit gender beliefs in spatial ability: stronger stereotyping in boys than girls. Front. Psychol. 7:1114 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hirshfield L. E. (2014). ‘she's not good with crying': the effect of gender expectations on graduate students' assessments of their principal investigators. Gender Educ. 26:6 10.1080/09540253.2014.940036 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hoffman M., Gneezy U., List J. A. (2011). Nurture affects gender differences in spatial abilities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 14786–14788. 10.1073/pnas.1015182108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hou G., Miu X., Chen Y., Hu S., Xu H. (1998). Research on the development of children's two-dimensional mental rotation ability. Psychol. Sci. 21, 494–512. [Google Scholar]
  51. Hromatko I., Butkovic A. (2009). Sensation seeking and spatial ability in athletes: an evolutionary account. J. Hum. Kinet. 21, 5–13. 10.2478/v10078-09-0001-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. IglóiI K., Doeller C. F., Berthoz A., Rondi-Reig L., Burgess N. (2010). Lateralized human hippocampal activity predicts navigation based on sequence or place memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 14466–14471. 10.1073/pnas.1004243107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ino T., Doi T., Hirose S., Kimura T., Jin I., Fukuyama H. (2007). Directional disorientation following left retrosplenial hemorrhage: a case report with fmri studies. Cortex 43, 248–254. 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70479-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ino T., Inoue Y., Kage M., Hirose S., Kimura T., Fukuyama H. (2002). Mental navigation in humans is processed in the anterior bank of the parieto-occipital sulcus. Neurosci. Lett. 322, 182–186. 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00019-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Jansen P. (2009). The dissociation of small- and large-scale spatial abilities in school-age children. Percept. Motor Skills 109, 357–361. 10.2466/pms.109.2.357-361 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Jansen P., Heil M. (2009). Gender differences in mental rotation across adulthood. Exp. Aging Res. 36, 94–104. 10.1080/03610730903422762 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Jones C. M., Braithwaite V. A., Healy S. D. (2003). The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability. Behav. Neurosci. 117:403. 10.1037/0735-7044.117.3.403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Jordan K., Wüstenberg T., Heinze H. J., Peters M., Jäncke L. (2002). Women and men exhibit different cortical activation patterns during mental rotation tasks. Neuropsychologia 40, 2397–2408. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00076-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kaufman S. B. (2007). Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity? Intelligence 35, 211–223. 10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kawamichi H., Kikuchi Y., Noriuchi M., Senoo A., Ueno S. (2007). Distinct neural correlates underlying two- and three-dimensional mental rotations using three-dimensional objects. Brain Res. 1144, 117–126. 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kerstin K., Susanne N., Thiel C. M., Karsten S., Charlotte H., Fan J., et al. (2005). Development of attentional networks: an fmri study with children and adults. Neuroimage 28, 429–439. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kluwer E., Mikula G. (2003). Gender-related inequalities in the division of family work in close relationships: a social psychological perspective. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 13, 185–216. 10.1080/10463280240000064 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kucian K., Loenneker T. H., Dietrich T. H., Martin E. (2005). Gender differences in brain activation patterns during mental rotation and number related cognitive tasks. Psychol. Sci. 47, 112–131. [Google Scholar]
  64. Laird A. R., Fox P. M., Price C. J., Glahn D. C., Uecker A. M., Lancaster J., et al. (2005). Ale meta-analysis: controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 155–164. 10.1002/hbm.20136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lamm C., Decety J., Singer T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54, 2492–2502. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Lamm C., Windischberger C., Leodolter U., Moser E., Bauer H. (2001). Evidence for premotor cortex activity during dynamic visuospatial imagery from single-trial functional magnetic resonance imaging and event-related slow cortical potentials. Neuroimage 14, 268–283. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0850 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Lamm C., Windischberger C., Moser E., Bauer H. (2007). The functional role of dorso-lateral premotor cortex during mental rotation: an event-related fmri study separating cognitive processing steps using a novel task paradigm. Neuroimage 36, 1374–1386. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Lange F. P. D., Hagoort P., Toni I. (2005). Neural topography and content of movement representations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 97–112. 10.1162/0898929052880039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Langlois J., Wells G. A., Lecourtois M., Bergeron G., Yetisir E., Martin M. (2013). Sex differences in spatial abilities of medical graduates entering residency programs. Anatomical Sci. Educ. 6, 368–375. 10.1002/ase.1360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Latini-Corazzini L., Nesa M. P., Ceccaldi M., Guedj E., Thinus-Blanc C., Cauda F., et al. (2010). Route and survey processing of topographical memory during navigation. Psychol. Res. 74, 545–559. 10.1007/s00426-010-0276-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lawton C. A. (1994). Gender differences in way-finding strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles 30, 765–779. 10.1007/BF01544230 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lawton C. A., Kallai J. (2002). Gender differences in wayfinding strategies and anxiety about wayfinding: a cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles 47, 389–401. 10.1023/A:1021668724970 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  73. Lee T. M. C., Liu H. L., Hung K. N., Pu J., Ng Y. B., Mak A. K. Y., et al. (2005). The cerebellum's involvement in the judgment of spatial orientation: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuropsychologia 43, 1870–1877. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Liao H., Dong W. (2017). An exploratory study investigating gender effects on using 3d maps for spatial orientation in wayfinding. Int. J. Geo Information 6:60 10.3390/ijgi6030060 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Linn M. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 56, 1479–1498. 10.2307/1130467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Little M. A. (2016). Why forage? hunters and gatherers in the twenty-first century brian f. codding karen l. kramer (eds.) santa fe: school for advanced research press and albuquerque: university of New Mexico press; Am. J. Hum. Biol. 30:e23099 10.1002/ajhb.23099 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Liu Q. (2016). Research on the development of mental rotation ability of large class children. J. Educ. Dev. 10, 21–25. [Google Scholar]
  78. Lj V. D. H., Borst G. (2011). Individual differences in spatial relation processing: effects of strategy, ability, and gender. Brain Cogn. 76, 184–190. 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.01.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Luo Y., Eickhoff S. B., Hétu S., Feng C. (2018). Social comparison in the brain: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies on the downward and upward comparisons. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 440–458. 10.1002/hbm.23854 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Lux S., Marshall J. C., Ritzl A., Zilles K., Fink G. R. (2003). Neural mechanisms associated with attention to temporal synchrony versus spatial orientation: an fmri study. Neuroimage 20, 58–65. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Maeda Y., Yoon S. (2015). Are gender differences in spatial ability real or an artifact? Evaluation of measurement invariance on the revised psvt:r. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 34, 397–403. 10.1177/0734282915609843 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Maeda Y., Yoon S. Y. (2013). A meta-analysis on gender differences in mental rotation ability measured by the purdue spatial visualization tests: visualization of rotations (psvt:r). Educ. Psychol. Rev. 25, 69–94. 10.1007/s10648-012-9215-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. Maguire E. A., Frackowiak R. S. J., Frith C. D. (1997). Recalling routes around london: activation of the right hippocampus in taxi drivers. J. Neurosci. 17, 7103–7110. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-18-07103.1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Malinowski J. C. (2001). Mental rotation and real-world wayfinding. Percept. Mot. Skills 92, 19–30. 10.2466/pms.2001.92.1.19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Malinowski J. C., Gillespie W. T. (2001). Individual differences in performance on a large-scale, real-world wayfinding task. J. Environ. Psychol. 21, 73–82. 10.1006/jevp.2000.0183 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  86. Merrill E. C., Yang Y., Beverly R., Sara S. (2016). Sex differences in using spatial and verbal abilities influence route learning performance in a virtual environment: a comparison of 6- to 12-year old boys and girls. Front. Psychol. 7:258. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Moher D., Liberati A., Tetzlaff J., Altman D. G. The PRISMA Group. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Ng V. W., Bullmore E. T., de Zubicaray G. I., Cooper A., Suckling J., Williams S. C. (2001). Identifying rate-limiting nodes in large-scale cortical networks for visuospatial processing: an illustration using fmri. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13:537. 10.1162/08989290152001943 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. O'Boyle M. W., Cunnington R., Silk T. J., Vaughan D., Jackson G., Syngeniotis A., et al. (2005). Mathematically gifted male adolescents activate a unique brain network during mental rotation. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 583–587. 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Papeo L., Rumiati R. I., Cecchetto C., Tomasino B. (2012). On-line changing of thinking about words: the effect of cognitive context on neural responses to verb reading. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2348–2362. 10.1162/jocn_a_00291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Paschke K., Jordan K., Wüstenberg T. (2012). Mirrored or identical–is the role of visual perception underestimated in the mental rotation process of 3d-objects?: a combined fmri-eye tracking-study. Neuropsychologia 50:1844. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Peña D., Contreras M. J., Pei C. S., Santacreu J. (2008). Solution strategies as possible explanations of individual and sex differences in a dynamic spatial task. Acta Psychol. 128, 1–14. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Pietschnig J., Gittler G. (2015). A reversal of the flynn effect for spatial perception in german-speaking countries: evidence from a cross-temporal irt-based meta-analysis (1977–2014). Intelligence 53, 145–153. 10.1016/j.intell.2015.10.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  94. Pintzka C. W., Evensmoen H. R., Lehn H., Håberg A. K. (2016). Changes in spatial cognition and brain activity after a single dose of testosterone in healthy women. Behav. Brain Res. 298(Pt B), 78–90. 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.10.056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Poulin M., O'Connell R. L., Freemanb L. M. (2004). Picture recall skills correlate with 2d:4d ratio in women but not men. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 174–181. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.03.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  96. Prescott J., Gavrilescu M., Cunnington R., O'Boyle M. W., Egan G. F. (2010). Enhanced brain connectivity in math-gifted adolescents: an fmri study using mental rotation. Cogn. Neurosci. 1:277. 10.1080/17588928.2010.506951 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Raag T. (1999). Influences of social expectations of gender, gender stereotypes, and situational constraints on children's toy choices. Sex Roles 41, 809–831. 10.1023/A:1018828328713 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Raag T., Rackliff C. L. (1998). Preschoolers' awareness of social expectations of gender: relationships to toy choices. Sex Roles 38, 685–700. 10.1023/A:1018890728636 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Reilly D., Neumann D. L. (2013). Gender-role differences in spatial ability: a meta-analytic review. Sex Roles 68, 521–535. 10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  100. Rilea S. L. (2008). A lateralization of function approach to sex differences in spatial ability: a reexamination. Brain Cogn. 67, 168–182. 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Rilea S. L., Roskosewoldsen B., Boles D. (2004). Sex differences in spatial ability: a lateralization of function approach. Brain Cogn. 56, 332–343. 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Ritter D. (2004). Gender role orientation and performance on stereotypically feminine and masculine cognitive tasks. Sex Roles 50, 583–591. 10.1023/B:SERS.0000023077.91248.f7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  103. Rosenbaum R. S., Ziegler M., Winocur G., Grady C. L., Moscovitch M. (2004). “I have often walked down this street before”: fmri studies on the hippocampus and other structures during mental navigation of an old environment. Hippocampus 14, 826–835. 10.1002/hipo.10218 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Ruggiero G., Sergi I., Iachini T. (2008). Gender differences in remembering and inferring spatial distances. Memory 16, 821–835. 10.1080/09658210802307695 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Samsudin K. A. (2008). Training in spatial visualization: the effects of training method and gender. J. Educ. Tech. Soc. 11, 127–140. [Google Scholar]
  106. Schöning S., Engelien A., Kugel H., Schäfer S., Schiffbauer H., Zwitserlood P., et al. (2007). Functional anatomy of visuo-spatial working memory during mental rotation is influenced by sex, menstrual cycle, and sex steroid hormones. Neuropsychologia 45:3203. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Schubotz R. I., von Cramon D. Y. (2009). The case of pretense: observing actions and inferring goals. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 642–653. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Seurinck R., de Lange F. P., Achten E., Vingerhoets G. (2011). Mental rotation meets the motion aftereffect: the role of hv5/mt+ in visual mental imagery. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23:1395. 10.1162/jocn.2010.21525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Seurinck R., Vingerhoets G., de Lange F. P., Achten E. (2004a). Does egocentric mental rotation elicit sex differences? Neuroimage 23, 1440–1449. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Seurinck R., Vingerhoets G., Vandemaele P., Deblaere K., Achten E. (2004b). Trial pacing in mental rotation tasks. Neuroimage 25, 1187–1196. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Signorella M. L., Jamison W., Krupa M. H. (1989). Predicting spatial performance from gender stereotyping in activity preferences and in self-concept. Dev. Psychol. 25, 89–95. 10.1037/0012-1649.25.1.89 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  112. Silverman I., Choi J., Peters M. (2007). The hunter-gatherer theory of sex differences in spatial abilities: data from 40 countries. Arch. Sex. Behav. 36, 261–268. 10.1007/s10508-006-9168-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Sluming V., Brooks J., Howard M., Downes J. J., Roberts N. (2007). Broca's area supports enhanced visuospatial cognition in orchestral musicians. J. Neurosci. 27, 3799–3806. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0147-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Stoodley C. J., Valera E. M., Schmahmann J. D. (2012). Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: an fmri study. Neuroimage 59, 1560–1570. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Tao X. L., Xie C. X., Sun X. Q., Tao W. D. (2008). The influence of personality type on the magnitude of gender difference in mental rotation ability. Prog. Modern Med. 18, 2323–2330. [Google Scholar]
  116. Techentin C., Voyer D., Voyer S. D. (2014). Spatial abilities and aging: a meta-analysis. Exp. Aging Res. 40, 395–425. 10.1080/0361073X.2014.926773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Telzer E. H., Qu Y., Goldenberg D., Fuligni A. J., Galván A., Lieberman M. D., et al. (2014). Adolescents' emotional competence is associated with parents' neural sensitivity to emotions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:558. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00558 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Tim N., Höffler (2010). Spatial ability: its influence on learning with visualizations—a meta-analytic review. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 22, 245–269. 10.1007/s10648-010-9126-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  119. Touroutoglou A., Lindquist K. A., Dickerson B. C., Barrett L. F. (2015). Intrinsic connectivity in the human brain does not reveal networks for ‘basic’ emotions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10:1257 10.1093/scan/nsv013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Turgut M. (2015). Development of the spatial ability self-report scale (sasrs): reliability and validity studies. Quality Quant. 49, 1997–2014. 10.1007/s11135-014-0086-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  121. Tuttle G. E., Pillard R. C. (1991). Sexual orientation and cognitive abilities. Arch. Sex. Behav. 20, 307–318. 10.1007/BF01541849 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Tzuriel D., Egozi G. (2010). Gender differences in spatial ability of young children: the effects of training and processing strategies. Child Dev. 81, 1417–1430. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01482.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Vingerhoets G., De F. L., Vandemaele P., Deblaere K., Achten E. (2002). Motor imagery in mental rotation: an fmri study. Neuroimage 17, 1623–1633. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Voyer D., Voyer S., Bryden M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychol. Bull. 117, 250–270. 10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.250 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Wang L., Carr M. (2014). Working memory and strategy use contribute to gender differences in spatial ability. Educ. Psychol. 49, 261–282. 10.1080/00461520.2014.960568 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  126. Wang L., Cohen A. S., Carr M. (2014). Spatial ability at two scales of representation: a meta-analysis. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 36, 140–144. 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.10.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  127. Wardle M. C., Fitzgerald D. A., Angstadt M., Rabinak C. A., De W. H., Phan K. L., et al. (2014). Effects of oxycodone on brain responses to emotional images. Psychopharmacology 231, 1–13. 10.1007/s00213-014-3592-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Weiss M., Wolbers T. M., Witt K., Marshall L., Buchel C., Siebner H. (2009). Rotated alphanumeric characters do not automatically activate frontoparietal areas subserving mental rotation. Neuroimage 44, 1063. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Winlove C., Milton F., Ranson J., Fulford J., Mackisack M., Macpherson F., et al. (2018). The neural correlates of visual imagery: a co-ordinate-based meta-analysis. Cortex 105, 4–25. 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.12.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Wolbers T., Büchel C. (2005). Dissociable retrosplenial and hippocampal contributions to successful formation of survey representations. J. Neurosci. 25, 3333–3340. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4705-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Wolbers T., Schoell E. D., Büchel C. (2006). The predictive value of white matter organization in posterior parietal cortex for spatial visualization ability. Neuroimage 32, 1450–1455. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Wu R. N., Yang Y. S. (2014). A study on the relationship between senior high school students' cognitive style, spatial ability development and mathematics achievement. J. Inner Mongolia Normal University 27, 46–50. [Google Scholar]
  133. Xu J., Evensmoen H. R., Lehn H., Pintzka C. W., Håberg A. K. (2010). Persistent posterior and transient anterior medial temporal lobe activity during navigation. Neuroimage 52, 1654–1666. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Xu X., Kim E. S., Lewis J. E. (2016). Sex difference in spatial ability for college students and exploration of measurement invariance *. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 45, 176–184. 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.11.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  135. Yang J. C., Chen S. Y. (2010). Effects of gender differences and spatial abilities within a digital pentominoes game. Comput. Educ. 55, 1220–1233. 10.1016/j.compedu.2010.05.019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  136. Yilmaz H. B. (2009). On the development and measurement of spatial ability. Int. Electr. J. Elem. Educ. 1, 83–96. [Google Scholar]
  137. Yonker J. E., Nilsson L. A., Anthenelli R. M. (2008). Sex differences in spatial visualization and episodic memory as a function of alcohol consumption. Alcohol Alcohol. 40, 201–207. 10.1093/alcalc/agh141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Yu Q. B., Lu Q. L., Li J., Sui D. N., Qiu X., Wang Y., et al. (2008). Sex differences in mental rotation affected by working memory load. Prog. Modern Med. 8, 1717–1725. [Google Scholar]
  139. Zacks J. M., Mires J., Tversky B., Hazeltine E. (2001). Mental spatial transformations of objects and perspective. Spatial Cogn. Comput. 2, 315–332. 10.1023/A:1015584100204 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  140. Zhou P., Yun J. (2011). An experimental study on ability of mental rotation among college students of minorities. J. Shanxi Datong University 27, 80–89. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the manuscript and/or the Supplementary Files.


Articles from Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES