Skip to main content
The BMJ logoLink to The BMJ
. 2019 Jun 26;365:l2327. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l2327

Investigating causal relations between sleep traits and risk of breast cancer in women: mendelian randomisation study

Rebecca C Richmond 1,2,, Emma L Anderson 1,2, Hassan S Dashti 3,4, Samuel E Jones 5, Jacqueline M Lane 3,4, Linn Beate Strand 6, Ben Brumpton 6,7, Martin K Rutter 8,9, Andrew R Wood 5, Kurt Straif 10, Caroline L Relton 1,2, Marcus Munafò 1,11, Timothy M Frayling 5, Richard M Martin 1,2,12, Richa Saxena 3,4,13,14, Michael N Weedon 5, Debbie A Lawlor 1,2,12, George Davey Smith 1,2,12
PMCID: PMC6592406  PMID: 31243001

Abstract

Objective

To examine whether sleep traits have a causal effect on risk of breast cancer.

Design

Mendelian randomisation study.

Setting

UK Biobank prospective cohort study and Breast Cancer Association Consortium (BCAC) case-control genome-wide association study.

Participants

156 848 women in the multivariable regression and one sample mendelian randomisation (MR) analysis in UK Biobank (7784 with a breast cancer diagnosis) and 122 977 breast cancer cases and 105 974 controls from BCAC in the two sample MR analysis.

Exposures

Self reported chronotype (morning or evening preference), insomnia symptoms, and sleep duration in multivariable regression, and genetic variants robustly associated with these sleep traits.

Main outcome measure

Breast cancer diagnosis.

Results

In multivariable regression analysis using UK Biobank data on breast cancer incidence, morning preference was inversely associated with breast cancer (hazard ratio 0.95, 95% confidence interval 0.93 to 0.98 per category increase), whereas there was little evidence for an association between sleep duration and insomnia symptoms. Using 341 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) associated with chronotype, 91 SNPs associated with sleep duration, and 57 SNPs associated with insomnia symptoms, one sample MR analysis in UK Biobank provided some supportive evidence for a protective effect of morning preference on breast cancer risk (0.85, 0.70, 1.03 per category increase) but imprecise estimates for sleep duration and insomnia symptoms. Two sample MR using data from BCAC supported findings for a protective effect of morning preference (inverse variance weighted odds ratio 0.88, 95% confidence interval 0.82 to 0.93 per category increase) and adverse effect of increased sleep duration (1.19, 1.02 to 1.39 per hour increase) on breast cancer risk (both oestrogen receptor positive and oestrogen receptor negative), whereas evidence for insomnia symptoms was inconsistent. Results were largely robust to sensitivity analyses accounting for horizontal pleiotropy.

Conclusions

Findings showed consistent evidence for a protective effect of morning preference and suggestive evidence for an adverse effect of increased sleep duration on breast cancer risk.

Introduction

In 2007 the World Health Organization’s International Agency for Research on Cancer classified shift work that involves circadian disruption as being probably carcinogenic to humans.1 Disturbed sleep, exposure to light at night, and exposure to other lifestyle factors have been proposed as possible underlying mechanisms.2 3 4 Although much of the literature on breast cancer risk has focused on the potentially adverse effects of night shift work and exposure to light at night, less investigation has been done into the potential adverse effects of sleep disruption and traits such as chronotype (morning or evening preference), sleep duration, and insomnia.5

In a meta-analysis of 28 studies, strong evidence suggested a positive association between circadian disruption and breast cancer risk (relative risk 1.14, 95% confidence interval 1.08 to 1.21). However, the association with short sleep duration (<7 hours a night) in seven contributing studies was much less conclusive (0.96, 0.86 to 1.06), and no dose-response association with sleep deficiency was observed.6 Findings from other meta-analyses have been conflicting, with two showing no conclusive evidence that sleep duration is associated with breast cancer risk7 8 and one showing evidence of an adverse effect of increased sleep duration (>7 hours a night).9 Most studies in the meta-analyses, however, have been case-control designs, vulnerable to reverse causation, or cohort studies with a small number of cases. Fewer studies have investigated associations between chronotype and insomnia with breast cancer risk. The Nurses’ Health Study cohort of 72 517 women (1834 breast cancer cases) found no strong evidence of an association with chronotype,10 and a prospective study of 33 332 women (862 incident breast cancer cases) in the Nord-Trøndelag Health Study (HUNT) found no strong evidence of an association with individual insomnia symptoms, although there was evidence of some excess risk among participants with multiple insomnia problems.11 Studies have tended to rely on self report of sleep exposures, meaning associations could be biased by measurement error and by residual or unmeasured confounding, making causal inference challenging.

Mendelian randomisation (MR) uses genetic variants that are robustly associated with potentially modifiable risk factors to explore causal effects on outcomes.12 13 14 This method is less susceptible to measurement error, confounding, and reverse causation than conventional multivariable regression approaches, provided certain assumptions are met. These are that the genetic variants are robustly associated with the exposure of interest, are not associated with confounders of the exposure-outcome relation, and do not influence the outcome through pathways other than the exposure of interest. Genetic variants robustly associated with chronotype, sleep duration, and insomnia symptoms have recently been identified in large genome-wide association studies (GWAS) with sample sizes of around 50 000 to more than one million.15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 Findings from those GWAS have confirmed the role of several core circadian genes influencing sleep traits, and identified genetic variants with no previously known circadian role.24 These genetic variants have been used in two sample MR and provided some evidence that longer sleep has a causal effect on schizophrenia risk,16 whereas being a “morning person” is causally associated with a reduced risk of schizophrenia and depression,15 and insomnia is causally associated with an increased risk of type 2 diabetes, higher body mass index (BMI), coronary heart disease, and several psychiatric traits.17 23 In our study we used MR to explore the causal effect of sleep traits on breast cancer risk.

We used genetic variants robustly associated with chronotype, sleep duration, and insomnia symptoms identified in three recent UK Biobank GWAS15 16 17 to investigate whether these sleep traits have a causal effect on breast cancer risk. To do this, we performed a one sample MR analysis using data from UK Biobank, from which estimates were compared with conventional observational multivariable regression results in the same study, as well as a two sample MR analysis using data from the Breast Cancer Association Consortium (BCAC).25 Furthermore, we aimed to assess the extent to which findings were robust to potential pleiotropy and supported by genetic variants associated with accelerometer derived measures of chronotype (sleep midpoint timing of the least active five hours of the day), sleep duration, and sleep fragmentation (number of nocturnal sleep episodes).

Methods

Multivariable regression and one sample MR analysis

Study participants

We used data on women from the UK Biobank, which recruited more than 500 000 participants (55% women) out of 9.2 million eligible adults aged between 40 and 70 years in the UK who were invited to participate (5.5% response rate).26 The study protocol is available online (www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/wp-content/uploads/2011/11/UK-Biobank-Protocol.pdf) and more details are published elsewhere.27 At recruitment the participants gave informed consent to participate and be followed-up. Overall, 503 317 participants consented to join the study cohort and visited an assessment centre. Information on sleep traits (chronotype, sleep duration, and insomnia symptoms), breast cancer status (prevalent and incident cases with up to nine years of follow-up), relevant confounding factors, and genetic variants are available in UK Biobank.

Sleep traits

At baseline assessment, conducted in one of 22 UK Biobank assessment centres between 2006 and 2010, participants completed a touchscreen questionnaire, which included questions about sociodemographic status, lifestyle and environment, early life and family history, health and medical history, and psychosocial factors. Participants were asked about their chronotype (morning or evening preference), average sleep duration, and insomnia symptoms.

Chronotype (morning or evening preference) was assessed in the question “Do you consider yourself to be?” with one of six possible answers: “Definitely a ‘morning’ person,” “More a ‘morning’ than ‘evening’ person,” “More an ‘evening’ than a ‘morning’ person,” “Definitely an ‘evening’ person,” “Do not know,” or “Prefer not to answer.” We derived a five level ordinal variable for chronotype where “Definitely a ‘morning’ person,” “More a ‘morning’ than ‘evening’ person,” “More an ‘evening’ than a ‘morning’ person,” “Definitely an ‘evening’ person,” “Do not know,” or “Prefer not to answer” were coded as 2, 1, −1, −2, 0, and missing, respectively. Sleep duration was assessed by asking: “About how many hours sleep do you get in every 24 hours? (please include naps).” The answer could only contain integer values. Binary variables for short sleep duration (<7 hours v 7-8 hours) and long sleep duration (>8 hours v 7-8 hours) were also derived. To assess insomnia symptoms, participants were asked: “Do you have trouble falling asleep at night or do you wake up in the middle of the night?” with responses “Never/rarely,” “Sometimes,” “Usually,” or “Prefer not to answer.” Those who responded “Prefer not to answer” were set to missing. We derived a three level ordinal variable for insomnia symptoms where “Never/rarely,” “Sometimes,” and “Usually” were coded as 0, 1, and 2, respectively.

Breast cancer

Participants were followed through record linkage to the National Health Service central registers, which provide information on cancer registrations, using ICD-9 and ICD-10 (international classification of diseases, ninth and 10th revisions, respectively) codes and cancer deaths. The endpoints in these analyses were first diagnosis of invasive breast cancer (ICD-10 C50, ICD-9 174), or breast cancer listed as the underlying cause of death on the death certificate for women who died during follow-up but were not captured by the cancer registers. We excluded all women with any other cancer diagnosis from the analysis. At the time of analysis, mortality data were available up to February 2016 and cancer registry data up to April 2015. Prevalent cases were defined as women with a diagnosis of breast cancer before date of recruitment to the UK Biobank. Incident cases were defined as women with a diagnosis of breast cancer or dying from it during follow-up.

Confounders

We considered several factors to be potential confounders of the association between sleep traits and breast cancer risk: education, body mass index (BMI), alcohol intake, smoking, strenuous physical activity, family history of breast cancer, age at menarche, parity, use of oral contraceptives, menopause status, and hormone replacement therapy.

BMI was derived from weight and height measured when participants attended the initial assessment centre, whereas information on other potential confounders was obtained from questionnaire responses completed at baseline (see methods in supplementary file). Additional information extracted from the initial assessment visit included centre of initial assessment visit, age at recruitment derived from date of birth, and date of attending assessment centre. Participants who were employed were also asked whether their current job involved night shifts: never/rarely, sometimes, usually, or always.

Genetic variants

The full data release in UK Biobank contains the cohort of successfully genotyped people (n=488 377). A total of 49 979 people were genotyped using the UK BiLEVE genotyping chip and 438 398 using the UK Biobank axiom genotyping chip. Pre-imputation quality control, phasing, and imputation of the UK Biobank genetic data have been described elsewhere.28

In the MR analysis, we used a total of 341 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) associated with chronotype,15 91 SNPs associated with continuous sleep duration,16 and 57 SNPs associated with insomnia symptoms17 (see supplementary file, tables 1-3). These genetic variants were derived from self report and confirmed with objective sleep assessment and in independent cohorts.15 16 17

Multivariable regression analysis

We carried out separate multivariable Cox regression between chronotype, insomnia symptoms, and sleep duration and incident breast cancer to investigate prospective associations between these sleep traits and to minimise the likelihood of reverse causality in observational associations. To minimise the role of confounding, we adjusted analyses for age, assessment centre, and the top 40 genetic principal components (obtained from principal components analysis (PCA) to detect and quantify the genetic structure of populations). A second model additionally adjusted for education, BMI, alcohol intake, smoking, strenuous physical activity, family history of breast cancer, age at menarche, parity, menopause status, use of oral contraceptives, and hormone replacement therapy.

One sample MR analysis

For one sample MR, the genetic variants used were extracted genotypes from the UK Biobank imputation dataset (imputed to the Haplotype Reference Consortium reference panel), which performed extensive quality control including exclusion of most third degree or closer relatives from a genetic kinship analysis, as well as those who were not classified as white British based on questionnaire and PCA29 (see methods in supplementary file). Unweighted allele scores were generated as the total number of sleep trait increasing alleles (morning preference alleles from chronotype) present in the genotype of each participant.

A two stage method was implemented to give a population average causal hazard ratio. The first stage model consisted of a regression of the sleep trait (chronotype, sleep duration, and insomnia symptoms) on the allele score and the second stage model consisted of a Cox regression of breast cancer status on the fitted values from the first stage regression, with adjustment for age at recruitment, assessment centre, 40 genetic principal components, and genotyping chip in both stages.

Sensitivity analyses

To check the proportional hazards assumption, we used Pearson correlations to test Schoenfeld residuals from both multivariable Cox regression and one sample MR Cox regression models for an association with follow-up time.

To assess the specificity of our findings to breast cancer, we performed multivariable regression and one sample MR analysis to assess the causal effect of the sleep traits on other cancer diagnoses and on all cause mortality.

We also performed MR analysis using only those genetic variants that replicated at Bonferroni significance in a large independent dataset for chronotype15 (242 variants in 23andMe, n=240 098, highlighted in supplementary file, table 1) to evaluate the potential impact of winner’s curse (ie, overestimation of genetic effects in the initial study), which can bias causal estimates in MR analysis. Given the relatively small sample size of replication datasets for sleep duration (CHARGE Consortium, n=47 180)16 and insomnia (HUNT, n=62 533),17 few SNPs independently replicated at Bonferroni significance to serve as sufficiently strong instruments for this sensitivity analysis.

To test the MR assumption that genetic variants should not be associated with confounders of the exposure-outcome relation, we investigated associations between the allele scores and potential confounders in UK Biobank. We then performed one sample MR analysis adjusted for any potential confounders found to be strongly associated with the allele scores (beyond a Bonferroni significance threshold of P<1.39×10−3) as a further sensitivity analysis.

We also conducted both multivariable regression and one sample MR using all breast cancer cases (incident and prevalent) in a logistic regression analysis in UK Biobank, and performed sensitivity analysis removing participants who reported currently working night shifts (sometimes, usually, or always).

Two sample MR analysis

We conducted a two sample MR analysis of sleep traits on breast cancer risk using female specific estimates of the associations between the genetic instruments and sleep traits identified in the respective GWAS15 16 17 in UK Biobank (sample 1) (see supplementary file, tables 1-3), and estimates of the associations between the genetic instruments and breast cancer from a large scale GWAS of breast cancer (BCAC) (sample 2).

GWAS of chronotype (five level ordinal variable), sleep duration (continuous variable), and insomnia symptoms (three level ordinal variable) were performed among women of European ancestry (n=241 350 - 245 767) in the UK Biobank. This was done using BOLT-LMM30 linear mixed models and an additive genetic model adjusted for age, sex, 10 genetic principal components, genotyping array, and genetic correlation matrix, as was done previously.15 16 17

The GWAS of breast cancer involved 122 977 women with the disease (oestrogen receptor positive and oestrogen receptor negative combined) and 105 974 controls of European ancestry from BCAC.25 BCAC summary data were based on imputation to the 1000 Genomes Project Phase 3 reference panel. To explore potential heterogeneity by breast cancer subtype, we also investigated the causal effect of the sleep traits on breast cancer stratified by oestrogen receptor status, using genetic association data from 69 501 oestrogen positive and 21 468 oestrogen negative cases within BCAC.25

Two sample MR analyses were conducted using “TwoSampleMR,” an R package for such analyses,31 which was first used to extract the SNPs being used to instrument the exposure (here the sleep trait of interest) from the outcome GWAS (here breast cancer in BCAC). If a SNP was unavailable in the breast cancer GWAS summary statistics, we identified proxy SNPs with a minimum linkage disequilibrium (LD) r2=0.8. We then performed harmonisation of the direction of effects between exposure and outcome associations, where palindromic SNPs were aligned when minor allele frequencies were less than 0.3, or they were otherwise excluded. We then used an inverse variance weighted method to meta-analyse the SNP specific Wald estimates (SNP outcome estimate divided by SNP exposure estimate) using random effects, to obtain an estimate for the causal effect of the sleep trait on breast cancer risk.

Sensitivity analyses

The inverse variance weighted random effects method will return an unbiased estimate in the absence of horizontal pleiotropy, or when horizontal pleiotropy is balanced.32 To account for directional pleiotropy, we compared results with three other MR methods, which each makes different assumptions about this: MR Egger,33 weighted median,34 and weighted mode,35 and therefore a consistent effect across multiple methods strengthens causal evidence.

To further detect and correct obtained causal estimates for potential violation of the MR assumptions,32 we performed RadialMR36 in the two sample analyses to identify outliers with the most weight in the MR analysis and the largest contribution to Cochran’s Q statistic for heterogeneity, which may then be removed and the data reanalysed. Radial MR analysis was conducted using modified second order weights and an α level of 0.05 divided by the number of SNPs being used to instrument the exposure. For the outliers identified, we also assessed their potential pleiotropic role by performing a phenome-wide association study (PheWAS) approach37 to investigate the associations between the SNPs and all available traits in the MR-Base PheWAS database (http://phewas.mrbase.org/).

To evaluate the potential impact of winner’s curse, we performed two sample MR analysis using 242 genetic variants that replicated at Bonferroni significance in a large independent dataset for chronotype15 (23andMe, n=240 098, highlighted in supplementary file, table 1). We also carried out further MR analysis using robust adjusted profile scores, which provide an unbiased causal estimate in the presence of weak instruments.38

Given potential non-linear associations between sleep duration and breast cancer risk,9 we also used data on 27 SNPs associated with short sleep (<7 hours v 7-8 hours) and eight SNPs associated with long sleep (>8 hours v 7-8 hours)16 in two sample MR analysis (see supplementary file, tables 4 and 5). Causal effect estimates (ie, odds ratios for breast cancer) were rescaled to be interpreted for each doubling of genetic liability for short or long sleep, as recommended elsewhere.39

Finally, we performed two sample MR using genetic variants robustly associated with accelerometer derived sleep traits in UK Biobank, to be compared with causal estimates obtained using genetic variants associated with self reported traits. For this we used genetic variants identified in GWAS in relation to three accelerometer based measures: timing of the least active five hours (L5 timing) (6 SNPs), nocturnal sleep duration (11 SNPs), and number of nocturnal sleep episodes (21 SNPs) in up to 85 205 participants, as previously described40 (see supplementary file, tables 6-8). Also see the methods section in the supplementary file for more details about how accelerometer sleep traits were derived. Effect estimates represented an hour earlier L5 timing (correlated positively with and to be compared with the self reported chronotype measure of increased morning preference), an hour increase of nocturnal sleep duration (to be compared with self reported sleep duration), and a unit increase in the number of nocturnal sleep episodes (to be compared with self reported insomnia symptoms).

All analyses were conducted using Stata (version 15) and R (version 3.4.1).

Patient and public involvement

The current research was not informed by patient and public involvement because it used secondary data. However, future research following on from our findings should be guided by patient and public opinions.

No patients were involved in setting the research question or the outcome measures, nor were they involved in developing plans for design or implementation of the study. No patients were asked to advise on interpretation or writing up of results. The results of the research will be disseminated to study participants on request, and to stakeholders and the broader public as relevant.

Results

Baseline characteristics

Of the 180 216 women in the UK Biobank who had been successfully genotyped and passed the genetic quality control, and after excluding 23 368 who had a diagnosis of other types of cancer, 7784/156 868 (4.9%) had received an exclusive diagnosis of breast cancer. Of these, 5036/156 868 (3.2%) were defined as prevalent cases and 2740/156 868 (1.7%) developed incident breast cancer over a median follow-up of 2.98 years.

Women with a breast cancer diagnosis (prevalent or incident) were more likely to be older, have a higher BMI, be less physically active, have had an earlier age at menarche, be postmenopausal, have ever used hormone replacement therapy, have a family history of breast cancer, and be nulliparous. They were less likely to be never smokers, work night shifts, and have ever used oral contraceptives (table 1) compared with women without a breast cancer diagnosis. No strong difference was found in education level between women with and without breast cancer, in line with previous findings,41 as well as no clear difference in relation to alcohol intake.

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics of women who had and had not developed breast cancer by date of censoring in UK Biobank. Values are numbers (percentages) unless stated otherwise

Characteristic No breast cancer diagnosis (n=149 064) Breast cancer diagnosis (n=7784)
Mean (SD) age at recruitment (years); No 56.2 (7.9); n=149 064 58.9 (7.0); n=7784
Mean (SD) body mass index; No 27.0 (5.1); n=148 617 27.2 (4.9); n=7764
Mean (SD) age at menarche; No 13.0 (1.6); n=144 845 12.8 (1.6); n=7576
Mean (SD) days/week strenuous physical activity; No 1.7 (1.8); n=141 387 1.5 (1.8); n=7325
Education:
 Degree 65 381 (44.3) 3337 (43.3)
 No degree 89 383 (55.8) 4376 (56.7)
Smoking:
 Never 90 072 (60.6) 4445 (57.4)
 Former 46 374 (31.2) 2700 (34.8)
 Current 12 131 (8.2) 604 (7.8)
Alcohol use:
 Never 6254 (4.2) 343 (4.4)
 Former 5208 (3.5) 287 (3.7)
 Current 137 465 (92.3) 7145 (91.9)
Family history of breast cancer:
 Yes 9221 (6.2) 526 (6.8)
 No 139 843 (93.8) 7258 (93.2)
Parity:
 0 27 508 (18.5) 1489 (19.1)
 ≥1 121 479 (82.5) 6290 (80.9)
Oral contraceptive use:
 Yes 123 688 (83.1) 6193 (79.7)
 No 25 117 (16.9) 1580 (20.3)
Menopause:
 Yes 89 397 (60.0) 5721 (73.6)
 No 36 272 (24.4) 787 (10.1)
 Not sure 23 277 (15.6) 1266 (16.3)
Hormone replacement therapy:
 Yes 57 038 (38.4) 3142 (40.5)
 No 91 682 (61.7) 4620 (59.5)
Shift work:
 Night 4815 (5.8) 158 (4.6)
 Other 6763 (8.1) 282 (8.2)
 None 71 790 (86.1) 3014 (87.3)

Multivariable analysis

In multivariable Cox regression analysis, an inverse association was observed between morning preference and risk of breast cancer, which remained similar in the fully adjusted model (hazard ratio 0.95, 95% confidence interval 0.93 to 0.98 per category increase) but there was no clear association between sleep duration and insomnia symptoms with risk of breast cancer (table 2). The proportional hazards assumption held for all the multivariable Cox regression analyses (see supplementary file, table 9). The inverse association with morning preference was not observed for other cancer diagnoses (hazard ratio 1.00, 95% confidence interval 0.99 to 1.02 per category increase) (see supplementary file, table 10), although it was evident in multivariable Cox regression analysis of all cause mortality (0.95, 0.93 to 0.97 per category increase) (see supplementary file, table 11). Associations with sleep duration and insomnia were also observed in relation to these other outcomes (see supplementary file, tables 10 and 11).

Table 2.

Multivariable and mendelian randomisation Cox regression analysis for risk of breast cancer associated with sleep traits

Sleep trait Basic model* Fully adjusted model† Mendelian randomisation analysis‡
No (incident cases) Hazard ratio (95% CI) P value No (incident cases) Hazard ratio (95% CI) P value No (incident cases) Hazard ratio (95% CI) P value
Chronotype (per category increase§) 151 421 (2732) 0.94 (0.92 to 0.97) <0.001 138 529 (2500) 0.95 (0.93 to 0.98) 0.002 151 421 (2732) 0.85 (0.70 to 1.03) 0.10
Sleep duration (per hour increase) 150 845 (2723) 1.01 (0.98 to 1.05) 0.55 138 228 (2495) 1.00 (0.96 to 1.04) 0.98 150 845 (2723) 1.06 (0.70 to 1.59) 0.78
Insomnia symptoms (per category increase¶) 149 005 (2740) 1.02 (0.97 to 1.08) 0.44 138 771 (2505) 1.02 (0.97 to 1.08) 0.44 149 005 (2740) 1.37 (0.59 to 3.20) 0.47
*

Adjusted for age, assessment centre, and top 40 genetic principal components.

Adjusted for age, assessment centre, top 40 genetic principal components, degree status, body mass index, alcohol intake, smoking, strenuous physical activity, family history of breast cancer, parity, age at menarche, menopause status, use of oral contraceptives, and hormone replacement therapy.

Adjusted for age, assessment centre, top 40 genetic principal components, and genotyping chip.

§

From definite evening, intermediate evening, don’t know, intermediate morning, and definite morning.

From none, some, and frequent.

When incident and prevalent cases were combined and associations investigated in a logistic regression framework, evidence was consistent for an inverse association between morning preference and breast cancer risk (odds ratio 0.96, 95% confidence interval 0.94 to 0.98), as well a positive association between both sleep duration (1.02, 1.00 to 1.05 per hour increase) and insomnia symptoms (1.11, 1.07 to 1.15 per category increase) with breast cancer risk, potentially reflecting reverse causation (see supplementary file, table 12). Cox regression estimates were similar after excluding participants who reported working night shifts (see supplementary file, table 13).

One sample MR analysis

Among UK Biobank female participants, allele scores explained 2.3% of the variance in chronotype, 0.7% of the variance in sleep duration, and 0.4% of the variance in insomnia symptoms (table 3). Some evidence suggested a protective effect of morning preference on breast cancer risk (hazard ratio 0.85, 95% confidence interval 0.70 to 1.03 per category increase) and weaker evidence for an adverse effect of increased sleep duration (1.06, 0.70 to 1.59 per hour increase) and insomnia symptoms (1.37, 0.59 to 3.20 per category increase) (table 2), albeit imprecisely estimated (wide confidence intervals). The proportional hazards assumption held for all the one sample MR Cox regression analyses (see supplementary file, table 9). The protective effect of morning preference was not supported by MR analysis for other cancer diagnoses (1.05, 0.93 to 1.17 per category increase) (see supplementary file, table 10) or all cause mortality (1.15, 0.97 to 1.35 per category increase) (see supplementary file, table 11), although evidence suggested an adverse effect of insomnia on risk of other cancers (1.55, 0.94 to 2.55 per category increase) (see supplementary file, table 10).

Table 3.

Allele scores for sleep traits in UK Biobank

Sleep trait No (incident cases) Mean (SD) No of increasing alleles Association of allele score with sleep trait*
Coefficient (SE) P value R2 F statistic
Chronotype (morning preference) 151 421 (2732) 336 (11.6) 0.017 (0.0003) <0.001 0.0229 3666
Sleep duration 150 845 (2723) 90 (5.9) 0.015 (0.0004) <0.001 0.0072 1127
Insomnia symptoms 149 005 (2740) 56 (4.9) 0.012 (0.0002) <0.001 0.0041 639
*

Adjusted for age, assessment centre, 40 principal components, and genotyping chip.

When using only the genetic variants that replicated in an independent dataset (242 variants in 23andMe) for chronotype, estimates of effect on breast cancer risk were similar (0.89, 0.71 to 1.12 per category increase); although with wider confidence intervals given that the replicated variants explained less of the variance in chronotype (1.6%) (see supplementary file, table 14).

Although most of the confounding factors were not associated with the sleep trait allele scores in UK Biobank, after accounting for multiple testing the chronotype allele score was associated with parity and vigorous activity; the sleep duration allele score was associated with age at menarche and BMI, and the insomnia allele score was associated with using hormone replacement therapy and age at menarche (see supplementary file, table 15). Further sensitivity analysis was undertaken adjusting for these potential confounders in the one sample MR analysis, and effect estimates were consistent (see supplementary file, table 16).

Findings of a protective effect of morning preference were supported in analysis of all breast cancer cases (incident and prevalent) in logistic regression. Evidence for sleep duration and insomnia symptoms was weaker, although both had effect estimates in the positive direction (see supplementary file, table 12). In analyses excluding women who reported working night shifts, findings were also consistent with the main results from Cox regression (see supplementary file, table 13).

Two sample MR analysis

After harmonisation of the SNP effects in the two summary datasets (UK Biobank and BCAC), 305 SNPs were used to instrument chronotype, 82 SNPs were used to instrument sleep duration, and 50 SNPs were used to instrument insomnia symptoms. This included three proxy SNPs (r2≥0.8) for chronotype (rs376957969 for rs111867612, rs1871516 for rs4550782, and rs6583802 for rs61875203). Two sample MR supported the findings of a protective effect of morning preference (inverse variance weighted odds ratio 0.88, 95% confidence interval 0.82 to 0.93 per category increase) (see supplementary file, table 17 and figure 1) as well as an adverse effect of increased sleep duration (1.19, 1.02, 1.39 per hour increase) on breast cancer risk (see supplementary file, table 17 and figure 2). Little evidence for a causal effect of insomnia symptoms was observed (0.93, 0.49, 1.76 per category increase) (see supplementary file, table 17 and figure 3). Figure 1 shows the inverse variance weighted estimates for chronotype, sleep duration, and insomnia symptoms from two sample MR compared with multivariable and one sample MR approaches in UK Biobank. Findings were similar when stratified by oestrogen receptor positive and oestrogen receptor negative breast cancer (see supplementary file, table 17).

Fig 1.

Fig 1

Forest plot of multivariable and mendelian randomisation (MR) estimates for association between sleep traits and breast cancer risk. Odds ratios are per category increase in chronotype (from definite evening, intermediate evening, neither, intermediate morning, definite morning), per hour increase in sleep duration, and per category increase in insomnia risk (from no, some, and frequent insomnia symptoms). Odds ratios rather than hazard ratios for incident breast cancer are shown for multivariable and one sample MR analysis to compare estimates across methods

Effect estimates were broadly consistent between the inverse variance weighted method and the pleiotropy robust methods applied (MR Egger, weighted median, and weighted mode) in two sample MR (see supplementary file, table 17 and figures 1-3). Furthermore, the MR Egger test of directional pleiotropy was consistent with the null for all analyses (see supplementary file, table 18).

Evidence for heterogeneity in causal effects for most of the models (see supplementary file, table 19) could still indicate potential violations of the MR assumptions. We used radial plots to aid in the detection of outlying variants. Radial MR analysis identified six outliers for chronotype, three for sleep duration, and two for insomnia symptoms in both inverse variance weighted and MR Egger (see supplementary file, table 20 and figures 4-6). The pleiotropic effect of many of these outliers was indicated in a PheWAS of the SNPs on all existing traits in the MR-Base database (see supplementary file, figure 7). With removal of outliers, inverse variance weighted and MR Egger effect estimates were largely unchanged (see supplementary file, table 21).

Effect estimates for the causal effect of chronotype on breast cancer risk were consistent when using the 242 genetic variants associated with chronotype, which replicated at Bonferroni significance in 23andMe,15 indicating that winner’s curse is unlikely to have substantially biased effect estimates (see supplementary file, table 22). MR robust adjusted profile scores, which provide unbiased estimates in the presence of weak instruments, provided similar causal estimates to the main MR analysis (see supplementary file, table 23).

Findings of an adverse effect of increased sleep duration on breast cancer risk were supported using genetic variants specifically associated with short and long sleep duration, with evidence for a protective effect of short sleep duration on breast cancer (inverse variance weighted odds ratio 0.92, 95% confidence interval 0.86 to 0.99 per doubling of genetic liability for short sleep duration) and adverse effect of long sleep duration (1.24, 0.96 to 1.60 per doubling of genetic liability for long sleep duration) (see supplementary file, table 24).

Finally, we performed two sample MR using genetic variants robustly associated with accelerometer derived sleep traits in UK Biobank, to be compared with causal estimates obtained using genetic variants associated with self reported traits. Supplementary table 25 shows the genetic correlations between these traits. Using genetic variants robustly associated with accelerometer derived sleep traits in UK Biobank, we found no clear evidence of association with L5 timing measured objectively (1.04, 0.78 to 1.38 per hour decrease) (see supplementary file, table 26 and figure 8). However, an adverse effect of increased sleep duration was supported using estimates from objectively measured sleep duration (1.16, 1.02 to 1.32 per hour increase) (see supplementary file, table 26 and figure 9) and there was some evidence for a causal effect of increased fragmentation on breast cancer risk (1.14, 1.00 to 1.30 per sleep episode) (see supplementary file, table 26 and figure 10). Given the limited availability of SNPs being used to proxy for L5 timing to evaluate its causal role on breast cancer, and given the strong association found between chronotype and L5 timing (see supplementary file, table 25),15 we performed a further MR analysis using the 305 chronotype variants with SNP exposure effect estimates taken from the GWAS of L5 timing, to also evaluate the causal effect of L5 timing (see supplementary file, table 27). This analysis revealed some evidence for an association with L5 timing and risk of breast cancer in the inverse variance weighted analysis (0.86, 0.78 to 0.95), although this estimate was not consistent across the pleiotropy robust methods, which were more consistent with the null.

Discussion

Mendelian randomisation (MR) uses genetic variation to investigate causal relations between potentially modifiable risk factors and health outcomes. In this study we compared observational estimates from multivariable regression with those from MR analyses to make inferences about the likely causal effects of three sleep traits on breast cancer risk.

In multivariable regression analysis using data on breast cancer incidence in the UK Biobank study, morning preference was inversely associated with breast cancer, whereas there was little evidence for an association with sleep duration and insomnia. Using genetic variants associated with chronotype, sleep duration, and insomnia symptoms, one sample MR analysis in UK Biobank provided some evidence for a protective effect of morning preference but imprecise estimates for sleep duration and insomnia. Findings for a protective effect of morning preference and adverse effect of increased sleep duration on breast cancer (both oestrogen receptor positive and oestrogen receptor negative) were supported by two sample MR using data from the Breast Cancer Association Consortium (BCAC), whereas there was inconsistent evidence for insomnia symptoms. Results were largely robust to sensitivity analyses accounting for horizontal pleiotropy.

Comparison with other studies

Previous studies have found an enrichment of circadian pathway genetic variants in breast cancer.25 42 Nonetheless, these studies did not directly implicate modifiable sleep traits by which risk of breast cancer could be minimised and did not attempt to separate the effects of the genetic variants on breast cancer risk through circadian disruption from pleiotropic pathways.

Findings of an adverse effect of evening preference on breast cancer risk in all analyses performed go some way to supporting hypotheses around carcinogenic light-at-night 4 and findings of increased risk among night shift workers who might be exposed to artificial light at night.1 In particular, the specificity of the causal effect of chronotype on breast cancer, which was not observed in relation to other cancers or all cause mortality, is consistent with the hormonal mechanisms implicated in the light-at-night hypothesis. However, findings when using an objective measure of chronotype (the least active five hours (L5 timing)) did not reveal the same adverse effect. Although this last analysis might be limited by the number and strength of the genetic variants used to instrument L5 timing, the lack of consistency in estimates draws to question the mechanisms by which morning or evening preference (rather than actual activity) influences breast cancer risk. Further analysis using the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) identified in relation to chronotype as instruments for L5 timing were consistent with a protective effect of morning preference, suggesting a protective effect of activity as well as reported preference, but as the pleiotropy robust tests were not consistent, more work is needed to distinguish the causal effect of morning preference from activity—for example, with the use of multivariable MR methods.43

Evidence for an adverse effect of increased sleep duration on breast cancer risk contrasts with the observational findings in UK Biobank as well as much of the literature on circadian disruption and breast cancer risk,6 and unlike our findings for chronotype are not aligned with the light at night hypothesis. However, recent studies implicate longer sleep duration as a risk factor for breast cancer.9 Given previous reports of a J-shaped relation between sleep duration and breast cancer risk,9 as well as investigating sleep duration as a continuous variable, we also investigated the causal effects of both short and long sleep duration to investigate non-linear effects. In line with our main findings, we found evidence for a protective effect of short sleep duration and adverse effect of long sleep duration on breast cancer risk. Furthermore, using genetic variants associated with accelerometer derived nocturnal sleep duration, we found evidence for an adverse effect of sleep duration with a similar magnitude of effect.

Overall, we found inconsistent evidence about the causal effect of insomnia symptoms on breast cancer risk in multivariable and MR analyses. A previous study of incident breast cancer in the Nord-Trøndelag Health Study (HUNT) revealed no strong evidence of an association with individual insomnia symptoms,11 although people with multiple insomnia problems were found to be at increased risk. In our analysis, insomnia was defined based on self report of either difficulty initiating sleep or waking in the night. Further work is therefore required to investigate individual symptoms of insomnia on breast cancer risk, and the potential cumulative effect. Interestingly, MR analysis provided some evidence for adverse causal effect of accelerometer derived number of nocturnal sleep episodes on breast cancer risk.

Strengths and limitations of this study

Key strengths of the study are the integration of multiple approaches to assess the causal effect of sleep traits on breast cancer, the inclusion of data from two large epidemiological resources—UK Biobank and BCAC—as well as use of data derived from both self reported and objectively assessed measures of sleep. Furthermore, for MR analysis we used the largest number of SNPs identified in the genome-wide association studies (GWAS) literature, with full summary statistics available to obtain strong genetic instruments for MR analysis and to explore potential pleiotropic pathways.

The approaches of multivariable Cox regression of incident cases, multivariable logistic regression of prevalent and incident cases, one and two sample MR, each have different strengths and limitations in terms of key sources of bias (see supplementary file, table 28). In multivariable analysis, attempts were made to mitigate key sources of bias, including confounding and reverse causation, with the use of multivariable Cox regression analysis of incident cases of breast cancer and adjustment for several hypothesised confounders. Nonetheless, residual or unmeasured confounding, selection bias, and measurement error could also have distorted effect estimates. We used MR analysis to minimise the likelihood of bias due to measurement error, confounding, and reverse causation. In addition, we conducted a series of sensitivity analyses to assess the core assumptions that the genetic instruments are strongly associated with the exposures of interest, are not influenced by confounding factors, and do not directly influence the outcome other than through the exposure.

One limitation of this study related to the self reported measures used in multivariable regression analyses and used to identify genetic variants for MR analysis. In particular, the measure of sleep duration might capture time spent napping and the any insomnia variable is really a measure of insomnia symptoms and not necessarily clinical insomnia. However, both these measures have been validated with the use of objective measures from accelerometer data in the UK Biobank and concordance is good, particularly for the effects of the genetic variants identified.15 16 17

Another limitation relates to the selection of participants. Analysis in the two large epidemiological studies included here (UK Biobank and BCAC) was restricted to women of European ancestry. Further work is required to investigate whether these findings translate to women in other ancestry groups. Although the UK Biobank represents a large and well characterised epidemiological resource, it is not representative of the UK population owing to low participation.27 As well as influencing the generalisability of findings, selection into the study can lead to biased estimates of association through “collider bias.”44 To minimise the influence of this, we also used genetic data from a large case-control study of breast cancer (BCAC), and we compared MR effect estimates across these datasets.

In all MR analyses, SNP exposure estimates were obtained from the UK Biobank as this has formed a major component of the GWAS of sleep traits conducted to date.15 16 17 20 21 23 This could lead to winner’s curse, when the magnitude of the effect sizes for genetic variants identified within a discovery sample are likely to be larger than in the overall population. In a one sample MR analysis, the impact of winner’s curse of the SNP exposure association can bias causal estimates towards the confounded observational estimate, whereas in two sample MR, winner’s curse can result in bias of the causal estimate towards the null. To minimise the impact of winner’s curse in one sample MR analysis we derived an additional allele score for chronotype composed of SNPs that replicated beyond a Bonferroni correction threshold in an independent study (23andMe).15 Similarly, for two sample MR analysis, we used SNP exposure estimates from this replication analysis in sensitivity analyses, and findings were consistent with the main analysis (see supplementary file, tables 14 and 22).

We were unable to apply the same approach to investigate the impact of winner’s curse in the sleep duration and insomnia analysis owing to the relatively small sample size of the replication datasets in those studies, meaning genetic associations could be imprecise. Although we are aware of a large GWAS for insomnia that was conducted using data from both UK Biobank and 23andMe, full summary data for the top SNPs in the replication analysis are not freely available.23 We used unweighted allele scores to minimise the contribution of potential weak instruments in the one sample MR analysis. We also applied a robust adjusted profile score method in the two sample MR analysis, which provides unbiased estimates in the presence of weak instruments, and this revealed similar causal estimates for chronotype, sleep duration, and insomnia as in the main analysis.

Although associations between the allele scores and confounders in UK Biobank imply violation of the MR assumption that genetic variants should not be associated with confounding factors, there are several explanations for these findings. Previous MR studies have identified causal effects of sleep traits on reproductive traits, body mass index, and activity levels,15 16 17 23 suggesting that these factors might be mediators of the association between sleep traits and breast cancer rather than confounders. Furthermore, some of the genetic variants associated with chronotype and insomnia have been found to be adiposity related loci,15 16 implying potential pleiotropic pathways. Nonetheless, we also applied a series of pleiotropy robust MR methods and outlier detection to rigorously explore the possibility that findings of a causal effect of chronotype and sleep duration were not biased as a result of pleiotropy.

As well as attempting to mitigate key sources of bias for each epidemiological approach applied, we also assessed the consistency in estimates between the approaches to provide the best inference about the causal effect of sleep traits on breast cancer. This is aligned with the practice of triangulation, which aims to obtain more reliable answers to research questions through the integration of results from different approaches, where each approach has different sources of potential bias that are unrelated to each other.45 46 We also compared estimates based on self reported sleep with the use of genetic variants associated with accelerometer derived measures of sleep,40 although we did not use female specific SNP estimates here given the smaller number of participants in UK Biobank with these data.

Implications of findings

Findings of a protective effect of morning preference on breast cancer risk add to other evidence from MR supporting a possible beneficial effect of morning preference on decreased risk of schizophrenia and depression.15 However, whether it is the actual behaviour that poses the health risk or the preference for morning versus evening requires further evaluation. Further work is also required to investigate the impact of circadian misalignment, which can be determined by genetic risk, self reported chronotype, and objectively measured L5 timing. In addition, suggestive evidence for a causal effect of increased sleep duration on breast cancer risk should be investigated further.

Conclusions

In this study, both multivariable regression and MR analysis were used to provide strong evidence for a causal effect of chronotype on breast cancer risk. Furthermore, some evidence suggested a causal effect of sleep duration on risk of breast cancer, although findings for these traits were less consistent across the different methods applied. However, the biological role of many of the genetic variants used to instrument these traits in MR and mechanistic pathways underlying the observed effects are not well understood. Previously reported pathways between sleep disruption and mammary oncogenesis include immunological, molecular, cellular, neuroendocrine, and metabolic processes.5 Further work to uncover these possible mediating processes is required. Nonetheless, these findings have potential implications for influencing sleep habits of the general population to improve health.

What is already known on this topic

  • The World Health Organization’s International Agency for Research on Cancer classifies shift work involving circadian disruption as probably carcinogenic to humans

  • Much of the literature on breast cancer risk has focused on the potentially adverse effects of night shift work and exposure to light at night, and less into the potential adverse effects of traits such as chronotype (morning or evening preference), sleep duration, and insomnia

  • Genetic variants robustly associated with chronotype, sleep duration, and insomnia symptoms have recently been identified in large genome-wide association studies

What this study adds

  • This study found consistent evidence for a protective effect of morning preference and suggestive evidence for an adverse effect of increased sleep duration on breast cancer risk

  • The evidence for insomnia symptoms was inconclusive

  • These findings have potential implications for influencing sleep habits of the general population to improve health

Acknowledgments

This research was conducted using the UK Biobank Resource under application numbers 9072, 6818, 15825, and 16391. We thank the participants and researchers from the UK Biobank who contributed or collected data; Ruth Mitchell, Gibran Hemani, Tom Dudding, and Lavinia Paternoster for conducting the quality control filtering of UK Biobank data; and Wes Spiller, Jie Zheng, Gibran Hemani, Philip Haycock, and Kaitlin Wade for help with data acquisition and statistical analysis. This study was made possible with the financial support of Jonathan de Pass and Georgina de Pass.

Web extra.

Extra material supplied by authors

Supplementary information: additional methods, tables, and figures

ricr048387.ww1.pdf (2.3MB, pdf)

Contributors: RCR conceived the study and conducted the main analysis. HSD, SEJ, and JML conducted the female specific genome-wide association studies and assisted with sensitivity analyses. RCR, ELA, and GDS drafted the initial manuscript. All authors assisted with interpretation, commended on drafts of the manuscript, and approved the final version. RCR is the guarantor and attests that all listed authors meet authorship criteria and that no others meeting the criteria have been omitted.

Funding: The breast cancer genome-wide association analyses were supported by the Government of Canada through Genome Canada and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, the Ministère de l’Économie, de la Science et de l’Innovation du Québec through Genome Québec and grant PSR-SIIRI-701, the National Institutes of Health (U19 CA148065, X01HG007492), Cancer Research UK (C1287/A10118, C1287/A16563, C1287/A10710), and the European Union (HEALTH-F2-2009-223175 and H2020 633784 and 634935). All studies and funders are listed in Michailidou et al.25 RCR, ELA, BMB, CLR, RMM, MM, DAL, and GDS are members of the MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit at the University of Bristol funded by the Medical Research Council (grant Nos MM_UU_00011/1, MC_UU_00011/2, MC_UU_00011/5, MC_UU_00011/6, and MC_UU_00011/7). RCR is a de Pass VC research fellow at the University of Bristol. This study was supported by the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre at the University Hospitals Bristol NHS Foundation Trust and the University of Bristol. The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the National Health Service, National Institute for Health Research, or Department of Health and Social Care. This work was also supported by Cancer Research UK (grant No C18281/A19169) and the Economic and Social Research Council (grant No ES/N000498/1). SEJ is funded by the Medical Research Council (grant No MR/M005070/1). TMF is supported by the European Research Council (grant No 323195:GLUCOSEGENES-FP7-IDEAS-ERC). MNW is supported by the Wellcome Trust Institutional Strategic Support Award (grant No WT097835MF).

Competing interests: All authors have completed the ICMJE uniform disclosure form at http://www.icmje.org/coi_disclosure.pdf. MKR reports receiving research funding from Novo Nordisk, consultancy fees from Novo Nordisk and Roche Diabetes Care, and modest owning of shares in GlaxoSmithKline, outside the submitted work. DAL reports receiving research support from Medtronic and Roche Diagnostics for research outside the submitted work. All other authors declare no support from any organisation for the submitted work, no financial relationships with any organisations that might have an interest in the submitted work in the previous three years, no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work.

Ethical approval: UK Biobank has received ethical approval from the UK National Health Service’s National Research Ethics Service (ref 11/ NW/0382).

Data sharing: Scripts for the two sample mendelian randomisation analysis are available on GitHub at: https://github.com/rcrichmond/sleep_breastcancer_mr/. For statistical code relating to the individual level data analysis in UK Biobank, please contact the corresponding author at rebecca.richmond@bristol.ac.uk.

Transparency: The lead author (RCR) affirms that this manuscript is an honest, accurate, and transparent account of the study being reported; that no important aspects of the study have been omitted; and that any discrepancies from the study as planned (and, if relevant, registered) have been explained.

References

  • 1. Straif K, Baan R, Grosse Y, et al. WHO International Agency For Research on Cancer Monograph Working Group Carcinogenicity of shift-work, painting, and fire-fighting. Lancet Oncol 2007;8:1065-6. 10.1016/S1470-2045(07)70373-X  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Fritschi L, Glass DC, Heyworth JS, et al. Hypotheses for mechanisms linking shiftwork and cancer. Med Hypotheses 2011;77:430-6. 10.1016/j.mehy.2011.06.002  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Haus EL, Smolensky MH. Shift work and cancer risk: potential mechanistic roles of circadian disruption, light at night, and sleep deprivation. Sleep Med Rev 2013;17:273-84. 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.08.003  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Stevens RG. Light-at-night, circadian disruption and breast cancer: assessment of existing evidence. Int J Epidemiol 2009;38:963-70. 10.1093/ije/dyp178  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Samuelsson LB, Bovbjerg DH, Roecklein KA, Hall MH. Sleep and circadian disruption and incident breast cancer risk: An evidence-based and theoretical review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2018;84:35-48. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.011  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. He C, Anand ST, Ebell MH, Vena JE, Robb SW. Circadian disrupting exposures and breast cancer risk: a meta-analysis. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 2015;88:533-47. 10.1007/s00420-014-0986-x  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Yang WS, Deng Q, Fan WY, Wang WY, Wang X. Light exposure at night, sleep duration, melatonin, and breast cancer: a dose-response analysis of observational studies. Eur J Cancer Prev 2014;23:269-76. 10.1097/CEJ.0000000000000030  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Qin Y, Zhou Y, Zhang X, Wei X, He J. Sleep duration and breast cancer risk: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Int J Cancer 2014;134:1166-73. 10.1002/ijc.28452  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Lu C, Sun H, Huang J, et al. Long-Term Sleep Duration as a Risk Factor for Breast Cancer: Evidence from a Systematic Review and Dose-Response Meta-Analysis. Biomed Res Int 2017;2017:4845059. 10.1155/2017/4845059  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Ramin C, Devore EE, Pierre-Paul J, Duffy JF, Hankinson SE, Schernhammer ES. Chronotype and breast cancer risk in a cohort of US nurses. Chronobiol Int 2013;30:1181-6. 10.3109/07420528.2013.809359  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Sen A, Opdahl S, Strand LB, Vatten LJ, Laugsand LE, Janszky I. Insomnia and the Risk of Breast Cancer: The HUNT Study. Psychosom Med 2017;79:461-8. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000417  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Smith GD, Ebrahim S. ‘Mendelian randomization’: can genetic epidemiology contribute to understanding environmental determinants of disease? Int J Epidemiol 2003;32:1-22. 10.1093/ije/dyg070  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Davey Smith G, Hemani G. Mendelian randomization: genetic anchors for causal inference in epidemiological studies. Hum Mol Genet 2014;23(R1):R89-98. 10.1093/hmg/ddu328  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Davies NM, Holmes MV, Davey Smith G. Reading Mendelian randomisation studies: a guide, glossary, and checklist for clinicians. BMJ 2018;362:k601. 10.1136/bmj.k601  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Jones SE, Lane JM, Wood AR, et al. Genome-wide association analyses of chronotype in 697,828 individuals provides insights into circadian rhythms. Nat Commun 2019;10:343. 10.1038/s41467-018-08259-7  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Dashti H, Jones SE, Wood AR, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies genetic loci for self-reported habitual sleep duration supported by accelerometer-derived estimates. Nat Commun 2019;10:1100. 10.1038/s41467-019-08917-4  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Lane JM, Jones SE, Dashti HS, et al. HUNT All In Sleep Biological and clinical insights from genetics of insomnia symptoms. Nat Genet 2019;51:387-93. 10.1038/s41588-019-0361-7  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Gottlieb DJ, Hek K, Chen TH, et al. Novel loci associated with usual sleep duration: the CHARGE Consortium Genome-Wide Association Study. Mol Psychiatry 2015;20:1232-9. 10.1038/mp.2014.133  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Hu Y, Shmygelska A, Tran D, Eriksson N, Tung JY, Hinds DA. GWAS of 89,283 individuals identifies genetic variants associated with self-reporting of being a morning person. Nat Commun 2016;7:10448. 10.1038/ncomms10448  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Jones SE, Tyrrell J, Wood AR, et al. Genome-Wide Association Analyses in 128,266 Individuals Identifies New Morningness and Sleep Duration Loci. PLoS Genet 2016;12:e1006125. 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006125  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Lane JM, Liang J, Vlasac I, et al. Genome-wide association analyses of sleep disturbance traits identify new loci and highlight shared genetics with neuropsychiatric and metabolic traits. Nat Genet 2017;49:274-81. 10.1038/ng.3749  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Hammerschlag AR, Stringer S, de Leeuw CA, et al. Genome-wide association analysis of insomnia complaints identifies risk genes and genetic overlap with psychiatric and metabolic traits. Nat Genet 2017;49:1584-92. 10.1038/ng.3888  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Jansen PR, Watanabe K, Stringer S, et al. Genome-wide analysis of insomnia in 1 331 010 individuals identifies new risk loci and functional pathways. Nat Genet 2019;51:394-403. 10.1038/s41588-018-0333-3  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Kalmbach DA, Schneider LD, Cheung J, et al. Genetic Basis of Chronotype in Humans: Insights From Three Landmark GWAS. Sleep 2017;40. 10.1093/sleep/zsw048  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Michailidou K, Lindström S, Dennis J, et al. NBCS Collaborators. ABCTB Investigators. ConFab/AOCS Investigators Association analysis identifies 65 new breast cancer risk loci. Nature 2017;551:92-4. 10.1038/nature24284  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Fry A, Littlejohns TJ, Sudlow C, et al. Comparison of Sociodemographic and Health-Related Characteristics of UK Biobank Participants With Those of the General Population. Am J Epidemiol 2017;186:1026-34. 10.1093/aje/kwx246  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Sudlow C, Gallacher J, Allen N, et al. UK biobank: an open access resource for identifying the causes of a wide range of complex diseases of middle and old age. PLoS Med 2015;12:e1001779. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001779  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Bycroft C, Freeman C, Petkova D, et al. The UK Biobank resource with deep phenotyping and genomic data. Nature 2018;562:203-9. 10.1038/s41586-018-0579-z  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Mitchell R, Hemani G, Dudding T, Paternoster L. UK Biobank Genetic Data: MRC-IEU Quality Control, Version 1. University of Bristol, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Loh PR, Tucker G, Bulik-Sullivan BK, et al. Efficient Bayesian mixed-model analysis increases association power in large cohorts. Nat Genet 2015;47:284-90. 10.1038/ng.3190  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Hemani G, Zheng J, Elsworth B, et al. The MR-Base platform supports systematic causal inference across the human phenome. Elife 2018;7:7. 10.7554/eLife.34408  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Bowden J, Del Greco M F, Minelli C, Davey Smith G, Sheehan N, Thompson J. A framework for the investigation of pleiotropy in two-sample summary data Mendelian randomization. Stat Med 2017;36:1783-802. 10.1002/sim.7221  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Bowden J, Davey Smith G, Burgess S. Mendelian randomization with invalid instruments: effect estimation and bias detection through Egger regression. Int J Epidemiol 2015;44:512-25. 10.1093/ije/dyv080  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Bowden J, Davey Smith G, Haycock PC, Burgess S. Consistent Estimation in Mendelian Randomization with Some Invalid Instruments Using a Weighted Median Estimator. Genet Epidemiol 2016;40:304-14. 10.1002/gepi.21965  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Hartwig FP, Davey Smith G, Bowden J. Robust inference in summary data Mendelian randomization via the zero modal pleiotropy assumption. Int J Epidemiol 2017;46:1985-98. 10.1093/ije/dyx102  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Bowden J, Spiller W, Del Greco M F, et al. Improving the visualization, interpretation and analysis of two-sample summary data Mendelian randomization via the Radial plot and Radial regression. Int J Epidemiol 2018;47:1264-78. 10.1093/ije/dyy101  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Millard LAC, Davies NM, Timpson NJ, Tilling K, Flach PA, Davey Smith G. MR-PheWAS: hypothesis prioritization among potential causal effects of body mass index on many outcomes, using Mendelian randomization. Scientific Reports, 2015: 5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Zhao Q, Wang J, Hemani G, Bowden J, Small D. Statistical inference in two-sample summary data Mendelian randomization using a robust adjusted profile score. arXiv. 2018;https://arxiv.org/abs/1801.09652.
  • 39.Burgess S, Labrecque JA. Mendelian randomization with a binary exposure variable: interpretation and presentation of causal estimates. arXiv. 2018;https://arxiv.org/abs/1804.05545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 40. Jones SE, van Hees VT, Mazzotti DR, et al. Genetic studies of accelerometer-based sleep measures yield new insights into human sleep behaviour. Nat Commun 2019;10:1585. 10.1038/s41467-019-09576-1  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Braaten T, Weiderpass E, Kumle M, Adami HO, Lund E. Education and risk of breast cancer in the Norwegian-Swedish women’s lifestyle and health cohort study. Int J Cancer 2004;110:579-83. 10.1002/ijc.20141  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Mocellin S, Tropea S, Benna C, Rossi CR. Circadian pathway genetic variation and cancer risk: evidence from genome-wide association studies. BMC Med 2018;16:20. 10.1186/s12916-018-1010-1  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Burgess S, Thompson SG. Multivariable Mendelian randomization: the use of pleiotropic genetic variants to estimate causal effects. Am J Epidemiol 2015;181:251-60. 10.1093/aje/kwu283  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Munafò MR, Tilling K, Taylor AE, Evans DM, Davey Smith G. Collider scope: when selection bias can substantially influence observed associations. Int J Epidemiol 2018;47:226-35. 10.1093/ije/dyx206  [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Lawlor DA, Tilling K, Davey Smith G. Triangulation in aetiological epidemiology. Int J Epidemiol 2016;45:1866-86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Munafò MR, Davey Smith G. Robust research needs many lines of evidence. Nature 2018;553:399-401. 10.1038/d41586-018-01023-3  [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary information: additional methods, tables, and figures

ricr048387.ww1.pdf (2.3MB, pdf)

Articles from The BMJ are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES