Abstract
Although fMRI using the BOLD contrast is widely used for noninvasively mapping hemodynamic brain activity in humans, its exact link to underlying neural processing is poorly understood. Whereas some studies have reported that BOLD signals measured in visual cortex are tightly linked to neural activity in the narrow band γ (NBG) range, others have found a weak correlation between the two. To elucidate the mechanisms behind these conflicting findings, we hypothesized that BOLD reflects the strength of synaptic inputs to cortex, whereas NBG is more dependent on how well these inputs are correlated. To test this, we measured NBG, BOLD, and cerebral blood flow responses to stimuli that either correlate or decorrelate neural activity in human visual cortex. Next, we simulated a recurrent network model of excitatory and inhibitory neurons that reproduced in detail the experimental NBG and BOLD data. Results show that the visually evoked BOLD response was solely predicted by the sum of local inputs, whereas NBG was critically dependent on how well these inputs were correlated. In summary, the NBG-BOLD relationship strongly depends on the nature of sensory input to cortex: stimuli that increase the number of correlated inputs to visual cortex will increase NBG and BOLD in a similar manner, whereas stimuli that increase the number of decorrelated inputs will dissociate the two. The NBG-BOLD relationship is therefore not fixed but is rather highly dependent on input correlations that are both stimulus- and state-dependent.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT It is widely believed that γ oscillations in cortex are tightly linked to local hemodynamic activity. Here, we present experimental evidence showing how a stimulus can increase local blood flow to the brain despite suppressing γ power. Moreover, using a sophisticated model of cortical neurons, it is proposed that this occurs when synaptic input to cortex is strong yet decorrelated. Because input correlations are largely determined by the state of the brain, our results demonstrate that the relationship between γ and local hemodynamics is not fixed, but rather context dependent. This likely explains why certain neurodevelopmental disorders are characterized by weak γ activity despite showing normal blood flow.
Keywords: CBF, EEG, fMRI, synaptic input
Introduction
fMRI using the BOLD contrast has become the de facto imaging modality for measuring human brain function (Friston, 2009), although its exact relationship with underlying neural activity as measured with local field potentials (LFP) and EEG is unclear. A general assumption is that the stimulus-evoked BOLD and EEG response better reflects the input and local processing of cortical neurons rather than their spiking output (Logothetis, 2008; Whittingstall and Logothetis, 2009; Buzsáki et al., 2012). As a result, changes in the magnitude of a stimulus-evoked BOLD response should be approximately proportional to changes observed in the EEG. Support for this comes from studies showing that the amplitude of both BOLD and EEG/LFP narrow band γ band (NBG) potentials increases in a similar fashion with stimulus contrast (Niessing et al., 2005; Koch et al., 2009), possibly due to the high-energy demand of initiating and maintaining γ oscillations (Galow et al., 2014). However, other studies in humans have shown that BOLD responses in visual cortex are insensitive to large changes in NBG (Muthukumaraswamy and Singh, 2009; Swettenham et al., 2013) and that certain types of visual stimuli (e.g., noise patterns) do not elicit NBG (Hermes et al., 2015) despite being clearly perceivable to the subject and yielding a robust hemodynamic response (Grill-Spector et al., 1998; Kayser et al., 2004). This raises the question of why might certain stimuli comodulate NBG and BOLD in a similar fashion while others dissociate them (Bartolo et al., 2011).
EEG integrates the electrical activity over several millions of neurons. As a result, the magnitude of the response is highly dependent on the degree to which inputs to neurons are correlated (Lindén et al., 2011; Reimann et al., 2013; Musall et al., 2014). This is especially important when interpreting NBG given that the frequency and amplitude of γ oscillations depend on the interaction between excitatory (E) and inhibitory (I) neurons (Atallah and Scanziani, 2009). When inputs are spatially decorrelated (i.e., inputs to different cells are of different strengths), the E-I balance becomes locally perturbed yielding a wide range of oscillation frequencies across the population (Wang and Buzsáki, 1996; Buzsáki and Wang, 2012), which, when spatially integrated (as scalp EEG does), results in a broad EEG spectral profile without a prominent γ peak. Therefore, the lack of a stimulus-induced NBG response may not reflect a lack of input to neurons, but rather a lack of correlated input. The BOLD response, on the other hand, is thought to reflect the metabolic burden associated with integrating and processing synaptic inputs (Logothetis, 2008), although whether or not BOLD is altered by changes in input correlations is largely unexplored.
Assuming that BOLD and NBG are separately dependent on input strength and correlation, respectively, we here hypothesized that stimuli that increase the correlated drive to visual cortex will increase NBG and BOLD in a similar manner, whereas stimuli that solely alter input correlations will dissociate the two. To test this, subjects viewed a grating with three different levels of Michelson contrast and spatial randomization, the latter of which has been shown to decorrelate V1 neurons (Zhou et al., 2008) without altering their overall output rates (Jia et al., 2013), thus making it an ideal stimulus for testing this hypothesis. We then combined the stimulus-evoked NBG, BOLD, and cerebral blood flow (CBF) responses with the output from a model E-I circuit fed with the same stimuli. The results show that NBG is more dependent on input correlations, whereas BOLD better represents the net sum of inputs, regardless whether they are correlated or not. This demonstrates that NBG is not an accurate marker of BOLD, and explains why certain stimuli can elicit large BOLD/CBF responses despite yielding little to no NBG.
Materials and Methods
Subjects and stimuli.
The study was conducted over separate EEG, fMRI, and arterial spin labeling (ASL) sessions. A total of 22 subjects participated in the study (9 female, no psychiatric or neurologic symptoms at the time of scanning or in the past, corrected to normal vision) according to the guidelines of the Internal Review Board of the Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Sherbrooke. All stimuli were generated using Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997) and presented on a gray background with luminance equal to the mean luminance of the stimulus (see Fig. 1a, top). The parameters of the unperturbed grating are as follows: a spatial frequency of 3 cycles/degree, a temporal frequency of 6 cycles/s, drifting from right to left within a 7 degree circular aperture placed in the center of the subject's visual field. The Michelson contrast (MC) was adjusted (100%, 33%, and 5%) by narrowing the luminance range of the unperturbed grating while keeping mean luminance constant and equal to the background luminance. Spatial randomization (SR) of the unperturbed grating was accomplished by spatial swapping of grating patches (Zhou et al., 2008). Briefly, grating patches (0.15 × 0.15°) were randomly selected and swapped with other randomly selected regions, until a desired percentage of the grating was randomized (for this study, SR levels were 10% and 60%) without changing MC (Sun et al., 2015).
fMRI acquisition and analysis.
Whole-brain volumes were acquired on a 3T MRI scanner (Ingenia, Philips Healthcare) using a 32 channel head coil for reception and the following parameters: TR/TE = 2000/30 ms, flip angle = 70°, FOV = 224 × 224 × 136.5 mm and voxel size = 3.5 mm isotropic. Stimuli were projected from an MRI-compatible monitor (resolution = 800 × 600 pixels, frame rate = 75 Hz) to a mirror positioned above the subject's face. Stimulus presentation was divided into 9 separate 8.5 min acquisition intervals, with acquisition halting for ∼1 min between intervals to converse with the subject. A T1-weighted 3D gradient-echo image (TR/TE = 7.9/3.5 ms, flip angle = 8°, FOV = 240 × 240 × 150 mm, voxel size = 1 mm isotropic) was acquired following the fMRI acquisition. Stimuli were presented at a rate of one stimulus per 16 s in which the grating was present for 2 s. Each of the five stimuli types was presented 5 times per 8.5 min interval in a pseudo-random order, for a total of 270 presentations (45 per stimulus type). Subjects were instructed to maintain fixation on a central crosshair for the duration of each 8.5 min interval and withhold blinks from periods during or directly following the stimulus presentation, as indicated by the color of the central fixation point (red for periods −4:10 s relative to stimulus onset, black for periods later than 10 s following stimulus onset). All fMRI processing was done using AFNI (Cox, 1996), FSL (Jenkinson et al., 2012), and custom written MATLAB (The MathWorks) scripts. fMRI images were motion corrected (Jenkinson et al., 2002) for both intrascan and interscan movements. Images were then concatenated across the 9 sessions, and independent component analysis (ICA) using MELODIC (Beckmann and Smith, 2004) was used to generate 150 ICs. The time course from each component was correlated with a stimulus-convolved (using default values in spm_hrf.m) response, and components with a robust and statistically significant (r > 0.25, which corresponds to p < 0.00001, Bonferroni corrected) correlation were selected for further analysis and averaged (typically 1–3 components per subject). This correlation threshold was selected as it resulted in ICA weight maps constrained to gray matter near the occipital pole of all subjects. Stimulus-related BOLD magnitude was calculated on a single-trial basis at each time point from 0 to 16 s relative to stimulus onset by subtraction of the baseline (t = 0 s) from the value at that point. To create group-average maps and perform voxel-based analysis, images were nonlinearly warped to a standardized atlas (MNI 152) using ANTS (Avants et al., 2011).
CBF acquisition and analysis.
Eight subjects who participated in the fMRI experiment also participated in an ASL experiment. To measure CBF, a 20 s on-off block design was used, over 3 stimulus types (5% MC, 100% MC, 60% SR) using sequentially interleaved CBF and BOLD acquisitions where three 8.5 min CBF blocks were interleaved with three 8.5 min BOLD acquisitions, for an experiment lasting 50 min. To estimate relative CBF changes, a pCASL sequence with background suppression was used with a labeling slab at the base of the cerebellum. Imaging was performed with a label duration of 1650 ms and postlabel delay of 1600 ms, using a 2D multislice EPI readout with TR/TE of 4000/14 ms. Eighteen contiguous 4-mm-thick slices were acquired with 3 × 3 mm in-plane resolution and an axial FOV of 240 × 240 mm. At each time point, the labeled image was subtracted from the control image, yielding an effective temporal resolution of 8000 ms. Sixty label/control pairs were acquired. Each subject's ASL and BOLD volumes were registered to the first volume from the first BOLD scan after applying motion correction to all BOLD images. Functional ROIs were defined separately for BOLD and ASL experiments by correlation using the standard HRF-convolution approach. To ensure that the BOLD-ASL comparison was not biased by ROI size/location, the two correlation maps (one for ASL, one for BOLD) were averaged, and a single ROI was created for both modalities by thresholding the averaged maps at r > 0.35. BOLD and ASL time series were then averaged across voxels within this ROI, and baseline normalized by the averaged 8 s preceding the stimulus onset. This correlation coefficient threshold was slightly higher than for the BOLD only experiment (r > 0.25) due to the longer stimulation/rest periods used in the CBF acquisition. Stimulus-induced changes were defined as (stimulus − baseline)/baseline for BOLD, and stimulus/baseline for ASL, giving the percentage change and fractional change, respectively. ICA was not used in the analysis of the ASL/BOLD data because BOLD percentage change must be in a prespecified range (typically between 1% and 10%) for realistic cerebral metabolic rate of oxygen (CMRO2) estimation via the Davis model (Davis et al., 1998), and percentage change values from ICA time courses were well outside this range.
EEG acquisition and analysis.
Scalp signals were acquired on a 64 channel EEG system (Brain Products). Subjects were comfortably seated in a standard office chair as stimuli were presented on a CRT monitor (resolution = 800 × 600 pixels, frame rate = 85 Hz). The stimulus presentation sessions were of similar length to the BOLD sessions, with stimulus presentation broken up into nine separate 8.5 min intervals, halting acquisition for ∼1 min between intervals to converse with the subject and verify signal quality. The stimuli were presented at a rate of 1 stimulus every 5 s, and each of the 6 stimulus types was presented 135 times in a pseudo-random fashion, for a total of 810 trials per subject. Each trial began with a fixation point color change from black to red, followed 0.5–1 s later (pseudo-random jitter) by stimulus onset. The drifting grating was visible for 2 s as in the fMRI experiment, and subjects were instructed to maintain fixation for an additional 0.5 s following stimulus offset before the crosshair changed back from red to black, commencing the 2 s rest period. Each 8.5 min acquisition interval contained five 8 s rest periods, spaced every 2 min, during which subjects were permitted to blink repeatedly and make small postural adjustments in an effort to confine large scale myogenic activity to times during which the stimulus was not being presented. All EEG data processing was done using eeglab (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) version 10.0 and custom-built MATLAB scripts. Datasets were down sampled to 256 Hz, and electrodes with poor signal (defined as those where: zscore (sum(abs(diff(channel time series))) >2.5) were interpolated. EEG sessions were concatenated into a continuous 90 min dataset, bandpass filtered between 1 and 120 Hz, and decomposed using ICA. A novel method of automatic ICA component selection was devised to isolate specific neural components: First, a template neuronal response function (NRF) was defined by filtering scalp space posterior electrodes (all PO, P, and O sites) in the 10–25 Hz and 40–110 Hz range and taking the grand average across all subjects (this range was chosen as it encompassed most significant stimulus-induced activity). This yielded two template time courses: one for the lower and one for the higher frequencies. ICA components were then filtered in the same two frequency ranges and rectified. The rectified, filtered time series of each component was epoched and correlated on a single-trial basis with an NRF in the same frequency range. Components were then sorted according to their correlation (Pearson's r) with the NRF, and the top 2 components from each subject were selected. Visual inspection of these components revealed that their weight maps were always restricted to the occipital lobe electrodes, thus reducing the contribution of eye movements to the data (Onton and Makeig, 2009; Hipp and Siegel, 2013). Stimulus trials were then epoched from [−0.85:2.85 s] relative to stimulus onset, and baseline-corrected event-related spectral perturbation was estimated in single trials and averaged across the top two components. EEG modulation at each frequency was averaged over the [500–2000 ms] time range.
Model network.
The model used corresponds to a spiking Wilson-Cowan network of γ activity (Brunel and Wang, 2003; Wallace et al., 2011; Jadi and Sejnowski, 2014). The network consists of interconnected populations of excitatory and inhibitory Poisson neurons with binary states: when a neuron j is active at time t, its spiking activity Xj(t) is set to 1, and set to 0 otherwise. The neurons' individual firing rates obey re,ij = βe,ijf(ue,ij), for some gain β and where f is the neurons response function. The membrane potential proxies ue,ij obey the set of dynamical equations as follows:
The presynaptic excitatory and inhibitory inputs correspond to sums of afferent, recurrent, and feedback contributions as follows:
and
where the wnm are synaptic weights, Io,{e,i}j are bias currents, and ξe,ij(t) are independent, zero-mean white noise processes. Excitatory neurons are further driven by a feedback input Ifeedback,ej = GNe−2∑kXej(t)Xek(t), through which coincident spiking triggers an inhibitory feedback response. The network is sparse with connection probability 0.8; inhibitory synapses can reach out to all neurons, whereas excitatory synapses are only permitted to connect to the 50 closest neighbors. Model parameter values are listed in Table 1. The network global EEG signal was computed using a weighted average of the individual potentials as follows:
To simulate the effects of MC, excitatory neurons were subjected to an additional contrast-dependent input Cj. For a chosen level of spatial randomization SR = [0, 1], a number SR · Ne of excitatory neurons had their contrast dependent input perturbed by some random amount; that is, Cj = Cmean + ηSRj, where ηSRj are zero mean random perturbations representing localized random changes in contrast. Yet, throughout, the network average contrast-dependent input remained constant; that is, < Cj > = Cmean. For clarity, we refer to input contrast-dependent changes (MC) and input correlation-dependent changes (SR) as Im and Isc, respectively. In this respect, a low Isc value reflects the scenario whereby cortical neurons receive different input strengths: spatially decorrelated input (as opposed to different input timings, i.e., desynchronized input, which was not studied here). This approach was meant to reproduce the so-called “dark dominance” of V1 neurons whose response to dark stimuli (on gray background) is relatively larger than to white stimuli compared with corresponding onset latency differences (Yeh et al., 2009; Xing et al., 2010, 2014).
Table 1.
Symbol | Definition | Value |
---|---|---|
Ne | No. of excitatory neurons | 800 |
Ni | No. of inhibitory neurons | 200 |
p | Connection probability | 0.8 |
f | Response function | (1 + exp[−ϵμ])−1 |
ϵ | Response gain | 25 |
βe | Firing rate gain of excitatory cells | 1 s−1 |
βi | Firing rate gain of inhibitory cells | 2 s−1 |
αe | Membrane time scale of excitatory population | 10 ms |
αi | Membrane time scale of inhibitory population | 5 ms |
ge | Excitatory membrane leak constant | −0.65 |
gi | Inhibitory membrane leak constant | −0.65 |
wee | Synaptic gain e → e | 26.0 |
wei | Synaptic gain e → i | 26.0 |
wie | Synaptic gain i → e | −32.0 |
wii | Synaptic gain i → i | −1.5 |
G | Feedback gain | −5 |
Io,e | Bias potential excitatory cells | 0.0 |
Io,i | Bias potential inhibitory cells | −0.2 |
D | Noise intensity | 0.0015 |
Model validation.
In total, we simulated a total of 2450 possible (Im, Isc) combinations, each yielding a distinct EEG signal. From this, we only focused on the simulated signals that best matched the experimental EEG data in each of the 5 stimulus conditions. To do so, a two-step approach was used: First, the power spectrum of each simulated EEG signal was correlated with each of the 5 experimental EEG power spectra shown in Figure 2a, b, and only the top 2% were retained (representing ∼13 [Im, Isc] combinations per stimulus condition). Although this first step ensured that only simulated data with spectral peaks matching the experimental data were kept, it did not ensure that their amplitudes necessarily matched those observed across stimulus conditions. For this, we used a second step whereby the mean power in the NBG (60–70 Hz) range was computed and only the simulated NBG values falling within 1 SD of the experimental data were retained. In summary, this two-step procedure ensured that the only (Im, Isc) combinations kept for further analysis closely matched the experimental data in both spectral shape and amplitude.
GLM analysis.
To quantify the contributions of Im and Isc to the experimental data, both were used as regressors in a GLM as follows:
where NBGexp and BOLDexp represent the group-average experimental values, whereas Im and Isc represent the simulation values. This was performed in two steps: First, the average (Im, Isc) values were used and the goodness of fit (R2) was computed. Next, all possible (Im, Isc) combinations were used, yielding several R2 values per stimulus condition. This process was repeated after randomly shuffling the experimental and simulated data.
Statistical procedures.
All error bars indicate SEM. Statistical significance of differences between stimulus types in single subjects was assessed with the independent-samples two-tailed t test (MATLAB's ttest2). Statistical significance of difference between stimuli types in group-averaged data was assessed with the paired-sample two-tailed t test (MATLAB's ttest).
Results
NBG and BOLD responses to changes in MC and SR
A qualitative overview of the group-average EEG and BOLD results is shown in Figure 1. Presentation of a high-contrast grating (Fig. 1a, middle) induced a sustained increase in NBG power over posterior/occipital electrodes (Fig. 1b, middle) as well as a BOLD signal increase within striate cortex (Fig. 1c, middle). To quantify this, statistical analysis of the EEG spectral profile revealed that only a narrow band of frequencies in the γ range (60–71 Hz) were significantly modulated in both MC and SR stimulus conditions (p < 0.05, FDR). This band is highlighted in Figure 2a, c; and for the remainder of this work, NBG refers to the average power in the 60–71 Hz range. As expected, decreasing the gratings MC significantly decreased both NBG power (Fig. 2a) and BOLD response magnitude in primary visual cortex (Fig. 2b). On the other hand, perturbing the grating spatial structure (SR) decreased NBG power (Fig. 2c) but slightly increased the BOLD response (Fig. 2d). In summary, these results show that NBG and BOLD are associated during changes in MC, although dissociated during changes in SR (Fig. 2e). The latter was also evident in single-subject analysis (Fig. 2f).
Decoupling of NBG and energy metabolism
The above results clearly demonstrate that SR suppresses NBG despite increasing BOLD. This suggests that stimuli that yield weak NBG responses may nonetheless be highly metabolically expensive. To confirm this, we used ASL to measure CBF and estimate cerebral metabolic rate of oxygen (CMRO2) at 5% MC, 100% MC, and 60% SR. These results were in agreement with the BOLD results, demonstrating that SR indeed substantially increased both CBF and CMRO2 (Fig. 3a,b) despite suppressing NBG.
Modeling the NBG-BOLD relationship
When reducing MC, NBG power and peak frequency decreased in a similar fashion (Figs. 1, 2a). On the other hand, SR appeared to suppress NBG while broadening its spectral profile (Figs. 1, 2b). This strongly suggests that the NBG results obtained during MC and SR are mediated by different neural mechanisms. To test this, we simulated the effects of MC and SR on NBG by varying both the magnitude (Im) and spatial correlation (Isc) of synaptic inputs to a network of cortical neurons (see Materials and Methods; Fig. 4a,b; Table 1). As expected (see Materials and Methods), the simulated NBG (NBGsim) matched in detail the experimental NBG (NBGexp; Fig. 4c), demonstrating that the (Im, Isc) values used in the simulation (Fig. 4d) faithfully reproduced the experimental EEG data. With increasing MC, both Im and Isc increased (Fig. 4d). However, when SR was increased, Im continued to increase, whereas Isc was significantly suppressed. This is because NBGexp was not only weak during SR, but also spectrally broad, as is typically observed during periods of strong, asynchronous input (Miller et al., 2009). Across all five stimulus conditions, NBGexp better followed Isc, whereas the experimental BOLD results (BOLDexp) better tracked Im. To quantify this, both Im and Isc were used as regressors in a GLM. The GLM reproduced both NBGexp and BOLDexp (Fig. 4e), although with different contributions from Im and Isc. Again, NBGexp was more dependent on Isc whereas BOLDexp was dominated by Im (Fig. 4f) and F testing further showed that the Isc regressor added significant information to the NBGexp (p = 0.03) but not the BOLDexp (p = 0.71). In summary, these modeling results reveal two important aspects regarding the experimental NBG and BOLD data: First, they confirm that NBG is more sensitive to changes in Isc, whereas BOLD is more sensitive to changes in Im. Second, they indicate that MC and SR are indeed mediated by different neural mechanisms: Increasing MC increases the number of correlated inputs to visual cortex, whereas increasing SR increases the number of decorrelated inputs. As a result, the NBG-BOLD association during MC can be attributed to an increase in correlated drive, whereas their dissociation during SR is due to an increase in decorrelated drive.
Cortical origin of NBG-BOLD dissociation
The modeling results presented thus far suggest that SR boosts the number of decorrelated inputs to primary visual cortex. In an attempt to localize the origin of these additional inputs, we compared whole-brain BOLD responses during MC 100% with MC 5% (Fig. 5a). Voxelwise statistical testing revealed that the BOLD response in voxels covering a large swath of the visual cortex and lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) was stronger during MC 100% (p ≪ 0.01, cluster corrected). When comparing MC 100% and SR 60%, on the other hand, no differences in LGN were detected despite large differences in visual cortex (Fig. 5b). To explore this further, we next examined the temporal profile of the NBG response in the same conditions. Compared with MC 100%, MC 5% was smaller in both the early (0–400 ms) and late (1000–1400 ms) phases of the NBG response (Fig. 5c). Conversely, SR 60% was smaller only in the late phase (Fig. 5d). Together, the spatial and temporal analysis of the BOLD and NBG response, respectively, suggests that the boost of inputs to V1 observed during SR cannot be solely due to increased activation of early thalamocortical circuits.
Discussion
Studies in animal models have reported a tight link between γ-band LFP and BOLD signals (Logothetis, 2002; Shmuel et al., 2006; Goense and Logothetis, 2008; Maier et al., 2008; Magri et al., 2012), particularly in the NBG range (Niessing et al., 2005). In this study, we sought to identify the neural mechanisms underlying NBG and BOLD responses in human visual cortex. The main findings are twofold: First, we show that certain stimuli can substantially increase BOLD and metabolic activity in visual cortex despite almost completely suppressing NBG. Second, we show that NBG is critically dependent on synaptic input correlations, whereas BOLD magnitude better reflects the net sum of these inputs, regardless whether they are correlated or not. These results show that NBG and BOLD are sensitive to entirely different aspects of synaptic input and, as a result, can easily be dissociated.
Experimental results
When increasing stimulus contrast (condition MC), NBG and BOLD responses were tightly linked, although on the other hand, increasing stimulus spatial randomization (condition SR) suppressed NBG despite further increasing BOLD, CBF, and CMRO2. This provides, to our knowledge, the first evidence demonstrating how stimulus manipulations that progressively reduce NBG can also increase regional CBF and energy metabolism in the same subjects. Importantly, the NBG responses (magnitude and peak frequency) described here are similar to those reported in anesthetized macaque V1 using the same stimulus (Jia et al., 2013), indicating that our results cannot be explained by artifacts associated with eye movements (Yuval-Greenberg et al., 2008; Winawer et al., 2015). In summary, the experimental results confirm that neuronal processes that actively suppress NBG are highly metabolically demanding. In the following section, we describe the characteristics of these mechanisms.
Modeling results
The main goal of this study was to explain how a clear and perceivable stimulus can elicit a large vascular response while almost completely suppressing NBG oscillations in visual cortex. For this, we simulated how changes in input strength and spatial correlation to a population of cortical neurons affect NBG and BOLD activity. These results showed that the NBG response across all stimuli could be modeled as a linear combination of both input strength and correlation, with the latter being the better predictor. Specifically, increasing MC increased the number of correlated inputs, whereas increases in SR increased the number of spatially decorrelated inputs to visual cortex. The weak NBG response observed during SR is thus likely due to the fact that strong, heterogeneous drive to neurons induces frequency dispersion across the population (Wang and Buzsáki, 1996; Buzsáki and Wang, 2012), which, when spatially integrated (as scalp EEG does), results in a broad EEG spectral profile without a prominent γ peak (as observed during SR). This provides further evidence that the absence of a strong NBG peak in humans does not necessarily imply a lack of input to visual cortex, but rather a lack of correlated input. This is similar to studies showing that the spatial reach of the LFP is largely determined by the correlation rather than the strength of synaptic input (Lindén et al., 2011) and that NBG is a poor predictor of spiking (Ray and Maunsell, 2011; Jia et al., 2013).
The BOLD results, on the other hand, were almost entirely explained by the overall strength of input to visual cortex, regardless whether it was correlated or not. This is due to the fact that integrating synaptic inputs is an energetically expensive process (Harris et al., 2012; Niven, 2016) requiring subsequent vessel dilation to increase CBF. Moreover, the SR levels used in our study have been shown to have little to no effect on V1 spike rates (Zhou et al., 2008; Jia et al., 2013), reinforcing the notion that BOLD is more dependent on the input and local processing of neurons rather than their output. However, it should be noted that a recent study also showed that SR of natural scenes does not modulate spiking in anesthesized mice, although it strongly increases it in the awake state (Froudarakis et al., 2014), perhaps due to increased input from higher visual areas. As our measurements were made in alert humans, we therefore cannot rule out that the BOLD increase observed during SR is related to both synaptic input and spiking output. Future experiments comparing neural and vascular based measures in anesthetized and behaving animals are needed to further investigate this.
Overall, our study demonstrates that stimuli and/or cognitive tasks that increase the correlated drive to cortex will enhance NBG and BOLD in a relatively similar manner, whereas those that increase the number of decorrelated inputs will dissociate the two. As the level of synaptic input correlations can change depending on the stimulus and state of the brain, we conclude that the NBG-BOLD relationship is not a static or fixed quantity, and we urge caution when interpreting neurovascular coupling in humans (Nunez and Silberstein, 2000).
Decorrelated input to cortex: effects of low- versus high-level processing
A significant finding from our study is that SR increases the decorrelated drive to visual cortex. But what is the source of this additional input? We speculate that it is not purely thalamic in origin because our fMRI results showed that SR had no significant effect on thalamic BOLD responses and NBG differences associated with SR were most prominent in the late phase of the stimulus response. Together, these findings point to SR having a stronger effect on corticocortical rather than early thalamocortical processing, although the reasons for this are unclear. One possibility that SR increases the subject's attentional focus, which in turn recruits higher-visual areas that send strong, desynchronized feedback input to V1 (Murray et al., 2002), thus increasing its local metabolic burden while suppressing NBG. This would agree with studies showing how increasing attentional load reduces NBG and spike correlations while increasing BOLD in V1 (Chalk et al., 2010; Watanabe et al., 2011; Herrero et al., 2013). Given all of these previous results, visual stimuli that amplify the V1 BOLD response while actively suppressing NBG likely reflect changes in neuromodulatory feedback from higher visual areas rather than changes in feedforward thalamic input. This may also in part explain why certain neurodevelopmental disorders are characterized by reduced NBG despite showing essentially normal or even increased hemodynamic responsiveness to basic visual stimulation (Hadjikhani et al., 2004; Snijders et al., 2013; Schwarzkopf et al., 2014).
Distinct role of NBG versus α/β activity
Rather than using a traditional γ range, we focused on a narrow band of frequencies as they were the only ones significantly modulated in both stimulus conditions. Aside from NBG, however, other frequency ranges are likely also involved in regulating BOLD in other brain regions. In our study, α/β oscillations were strongly modulated by MC, though not SR. This is similar to the BOLD responses observed in LGN, suggesting that α/β and NBG may represent distinct channels of thalamocortical signal propagation (Schmiedt et al., 2014; van Kerkoerle et al., 2014; Bastos et al., 2015). Furthermore, our results indicate that SR also increased broadband (40–120 Hz) EEG power in a manner similar to BOLD, although this effect did not reach statistical significance, nor was this observed in our modeling results (data not shown). Further improvements in EEG denoising or modifications to our model (Miller et al., 2009) may help better assess the potential link between broadband EEG and BOLD (Winawer et al., 2013). Our results therefore further support the view that the BOLD response cannot be exclusively tied to a single, narrow EEG frequency range (Kilner et al., 2005; Magri et al., 2012).
In conclusion, in this study, we show that the NBG-BOLD relationship is variable because of their respective sensitivities to vastly different aspects of cortical input: the magnitude of an evoked-BOLD response is best predicted by the sum of incoming synaptic inputs, whereas NBG magnitude is more dependent on the correlation of these inputs. This provides new insights as to why certain stimuli, cognitive tasks, and brain disorders can be characterized by weak NBG yet robust BOLD activity. We suggest that future experiments purposely designed to dissociate EEG and BOLD will be useful in isolating the role input correlations play on cognitive processing.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the National Science and Engineering Council of Canada and the Canada Research Chair in Neurovascular Coupling. We thank Jozien Goense, Jean-Francois Lepage, and Brian Wandell for helpful discussions.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Atallah BV, Scanziani M (2009) Instantaneous modulation of gamma oscillation frequency by balancing excitation with inhibition. Neuron 62:566–577. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avants BB, Tustison NJ, Song G, Cook PA, Klein A, Gee JC (2011) A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage 54:2033–2044. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartolo MJ, Gieselmann MA, Vuksanovic V, Hunter D, Sun L, Chen X, Delicato LS, Thiele A (2011) Stimulus-induced dissociation of neuronal firing rates and local field potential gamma power and its relationship to the resonance blood oxygen level-dependent signal in macaque primary visual cortex. Eur J Neurosci 34:1857–1870. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07877.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastos AM, Vezoli J, Bosman CA, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Dowdall JR, De Weerd P, Kennedy H, Fries P (2015) Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron 85:390–401. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann CF, Smith SM (2004) Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 23:137–152. 10.1109/TMI.2003.822821 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH. (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436. 10.1163/156856897X00357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunel N, Wang XJ (2003) What determines the frequency of fast network oscillations with irregular neural discharges? I. Synaptic dynamics and excitation-inhibition balance. J Neurophysiol 90:415–430. 10.1152/jn.01095.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G, Wang XJ (2012) Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annu Rev Neurosci 35:203–225. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G, Anastassiou CA, Koch C (2012) The origin of extracellular fields and currents: EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:407–420. 10.1038/nrn3241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chalk M, Herrero JL, Gieselmann MA, Delicato LS, Gotthardt S, Thiele A (2010) Attention reduces stimulus-driven gamma frequency oscillations and spike field coherence in V1. Neuron 66:114–125. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox RW. (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 29:162–173. 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis TL, Kwong KK, Weisskoff RM, Rosen BR (1998) Calibrated functional MRI: mapping the dynamics of oxidative metabolism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:1834–1839. 10.1073/pnas.95.4.1834 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ. (2009) Modalities, modes, and models in functional neuroimaging. Science 326:399–403. 10.1126/science.1174521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Froudarakis E, Berens P, Ecker AS, Cotton RJ, Sinz FH, Yatsenko D, Saggau P, Bethge M, Tolias AS (2014) Population code in mouse V1 facilitates readout of natural scenes through increased sparseness. Nat Neurosci 17:851–857. 10.1038/nn.3707 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galow LV, Schneider J, Lewen A, Ta TT, Papageorgiou IE, Kann O (2014) Energy substrates that fuel fast neuronal network oscillations. Front Neurosci 8:398. 10.3389/fnins.2014.00398 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goense JB, Logothetis NK (2008) Neurophysiology of the BOLD fMRI signal in awake monkeys. Curr Biol 18:631–640. 10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Hendler T, Edelman S, Itzchak Y, Malach R (1998) A sequence of object-processing stages revealed by fMRI in the human occipital lobe. Hum Brain Mapp 6:316–328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadjikhani N, Chabris CF, Joseph RM, Clark J, McGrath L, Aharon I, Feczko E, Tager-Flusberg H, Harris GJ (2004) Early visual cortex organization in autism: an fMRI study. Neuroreport 15:267–270. 10.1097/00001756-200402090-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris JJ, Jolivet R, Attwell D (2012) Synaptic energy use and supply. Neuron 75:762–777. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hermes D, Miller KJ, Wandell BA, Winawer J (2015) Stimulus dependence of gamma oscillations in human visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 25:2951–2959. 10.1093/cercor/bhu091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrero JL, Gieselmann MA, Sanayei M, Thiele A (2013) Attention-induced variance and noise correlation reduction in macaque V1 is mediated by NMDA receptors. Neuron 78:729–739. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hipp JF, Siegel M (2013) Dissociating neuronal gamma-band activity from cranial and ocular muscle activity in EEG. Front Hum Neurosci 7:338. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00338 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jadi MP, Sejnowski TJ (2014) Cortical oscillations arise from contextual interactions that regulate sparse coding. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:6780–6785. 10.1073/pnas.1405300111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S (2002) Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17:825–841. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM (2012) FSL. Neuroimage 62:782–790. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jia X, Xing D, Kohn A (2013) No consistent relationship between gamma power and peak frequency in macaque primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 33:17–25. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1687-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser C, Kim M, Ugurbil K, Kim DS, König P (2004) A comparison of hemodynamic and neural responses in cat visual cortex using complex stimuli. Cereb Cortex 14:881–891. 10.1093/cercor/bhh047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kilner JM, Mattout J, Henson R, Friston KJ (2005) Hemodynamic correlates of EEG: a heuristic. Neuroimage 28:280–286. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koch SP, Werner P, Steinbrink J, Fries P, Obrig H (2009) Stimulus-induced and state-dependent sustained gamma activity is tightly coupled to the hemodynamic response in humans. J Neurosci 29:13962–13970. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1402-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindén H, Tetzlaff T, Potjans TC, Pettersen KH, Grün S, Diesmann M, Einevoll GT (2011) Modeling the spatial reach of the LFP. Neuron 72:859–872. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logothetis NK. (2002) On the neural basis of the BOLD fMRI signal. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 357:1003 10.1098/rstb.2002.1114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logothetis NK. (2008) What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature 453:869–878. 10.1038/nature06976 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magri C, Schridde U, Murayama Y, Panzeri S, Logothetis NK (2012) The amplitude and timing of the BOLD signal reflects the relationship between local field potential power at different frequencies. J Neurosci 32:1395–1407. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3985-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier A, Wilke M, Aura C, Zhu C, Ye FQ, Leopold DA (2008) Divergence of fMRI and neural signals in V1 during perceptual suppression in the awake monkey. Nat Neurosci 11:1193–1200. 10.1038/nn.2173 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller KJ, Sorensen LB, Ojemann JG, den Nijs M (2009) Power-law scaling in the brain surface electric potential. PLoS Comput Biol 5:e1000609. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000609 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray SO, Kersten D, Olshausen BA, Schrater P, Woods DL (2002) Shape perception reduces activity in human primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:15164–15169. 10.1073/pnas.192579399 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Musall S, von Pföstl V, Rauch A, Logothetis NK, Whittingstall K (2014) Effects of neural synchrony on surface EEG. Cereb Cortex 24:1045–1053. 10.1093/cercor/bhs389 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthukumaraswamy SD, Singh KD (2009) Functional decoupling of BOLD and gamma-band amplitudes in human primary visual cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2000–2007. 10.1002/hbm.20644 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niessing J, Ebisch B, Schmidt KE, Niessing M, Singer W, Galuske RA (2005) Hemodynamic signals correlate tightly with synchronized gamma oscillations. Science 309:948–951. 10.1126/science.1110948 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niven JE. (2016) Neuronal energy consumption: biophysics, efficiency and evolution. Curr Opin Neurobiol 41:129–135. 10.1016/j.conb.2016.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nunez PL, Silberstein RB (2000) On the relationship of synaptic activity to macroscopic measurements: does co-registration of EEG with fMRI make sense? Brain Topogr 13:79–96. 10.1023/A:1026683200895 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Onton J, Makeig S (2009) High-frequency broadband modulations of electroencephalographic spectra. Front Hum Neurosci 3:61. 10.3389/neuro.09.061.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray S, Maunsell JH (2011) Different origins of gamma rhythm and high-gamma activity in macaque visual cortex. PLoS Biol 9:e1000610. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000610 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reimann MW, Anastassiou CA, Perin R, Hill SL, Markram H, Koch C (2013) A biophysically detailed model of neocortical local field potentials predicts the critical role of active membrane currents. Neuron 79:375–390. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmiedt JT, Maier A, Fries P, Saunders RC, Leopold DA, Schmid MC (2014) Beta oscillation dynamics in extrastriate cortex after removal of primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 34:11857–11864. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0509-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzkopf DS, Anderson EJ, de Haas B, White SJ, Rees G (2014) Larger extrastriate population receptive fields in autism spectrum disorders. J Neurosci 34:2713–2724. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4416-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shmuel A, Augath M, Oeltermann A, Logothetis NK (2006) Negative functional MRI response correlates with decreases in neuronal activity in monkey visual area V1. Nat Neurosci 9:569–577. 10.1038/nn1675 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snijders TM, Milivojevic B, Kemner C (2013) Atypical excitation–inhibition balance in autism captured by the gamma response to contextual modulation. Neuroimage Clin 3:65–72. 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.06.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun HC, Ban H, Di Luca M, Welchman AE (2015) fMRI evidence for areas that process surface gloss in the human visual cortex. Vision Res 109:149–157. 10.1016/j.visres.2014.11.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swettenham JB, Muthukumaraswamy SD, Singh KD (2013) BOLD responses in human primary visual cortex are insensitive to substantial changes in neural activity. Front Hum Neurosci 7:76. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Kerkoerle T, Self MW, Dagnino B, Gariel-Mathis MA, Poort J, van der Togt C, Roelfsema PR (2014) Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:14332–14341. 10.1073/pnas.1402773111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace E, Benayoun M, van Drongelen W, Cowan JD (2011) Emergent oscillations in networks of stochastic spiking neurons. PLoS One 6:e14804. 10.1371/journal.pone.0014804 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang XJ, Buzsáki G (1996) Gamma oscillation by synaptic inhibition in a hippocampal interneuronal network model. J Neurosci 16:6402–6413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe M, Cheng K, Murayama Y, Ueno K, Asamizuya T, Tanaka K, Logothetis N (2011) Attention but not awareness modulates the BOLD signal in the human V1 during binocular suppression. Science 334:829–831. 10.1126/science.1203161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whittingstall K, Logothetis NK (2009) Frequency-band coupling in surface EEG reflects spiking activity in monkey visual cortex. Neuron 64:281–289. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winawer J, Kay KN, Foster BL, Rauschecker AM, Parvizi J, Wandell BA (2013) Asynchronous broadband signals are the principal source of the BOLD response in human visual cortex. Curr Biol 23:1145–1153. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winawer J, Kupers ER, Chua N, Hermes D, Amano K (2015) Stimulus selectivity of gamma oscillations in human visual cortex measured with magnetoencephalography. Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting, Chicago, IL. [Google Scholar]
- Xing D, Yeh CI, Shapley RM (2010) Generation of black-dominant responses in V1 cortex. J Neurosci 30:13504–13512. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2473-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xing D, Yeh CI, Gordon J, Shapley RM (2014) Cortical brightness adaptation when darkness and brightness produce different dynamical states in the visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:1210–1215. 10.1073/pnas.1314690111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeh CI, Xing D, Shapley RM (2009) “Black” responses dominate macaque primary visual cortex V1. J Neurosci 29:11753–11760. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1991-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yuval-Greenberg S, Tomer O, Keren AS, Nelken I, Deouell LY (2008) Transient induced gamma-band response in EEG as a manifestation of miniature saccades. Neuron 58:429–441. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Z, Bernard MR, Bonds AB (2008) Deconstruction of spatial integrity in visual stimulus detected by modulation of synchronized activity in cat visual cortex. J Neurosci 28:3759–3768. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4481-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]