Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2017 Aug 2;37(31):7294–7296. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1373-17.2017

Sleepy Circuits in Vigilant Mice? A Slow Cortical Oscillation Occurring during Multiple Arousal States

Garrett T Neske 1,
PMCID: PMC6596704  PMID: 28768793

Slow, synchronized neuronal oscillations in thalamocortical systems are hallmarks of sleep and inattentiveness. Sleep rhythms, such as slow (<1 Hz), delta (1–4 Hz), and spindle (7–14) oscillations, are associated with alternating periods of neuronal activation and quiescence that are synchronously enforced across disparate cortical and thalamic regions (Steriade et al., 1993a, b, c; Neske, 2016). This slow synchronized activity is thought to be well suited for synaptic plasticity and internally generated signaling (Steriade and Timofeev, 2003). In contrast, faster, more desynchronized activity characterizes active wakefulness, where it is thought to promote faithful encoding of complex sensory signals (Steriade, 2000). Slow rhythms do occur occasionally in the waking brain, but they usually occur more locally during wakefulness (Vyazovskiy et al., 2011), and they are abolished by active behavior or sensory stimulation (Poulet and Petersen, 2008; Tan et al., 2014).

A recent study in The Journal of Neuroscience (Einstein et al., 2017) presents several intriguing findings that challenge the notion that slow rhythms in the awake brain occur only during behavioral quiescence. Using whole-cell intracellular recordings from several neuron types in the primary visual cortex (V1) of awake, behaving mice, Einstein et al. (2017) found 3–5 Hz oscillations in neuronal membrane potential (Vm) that occurred with surprisingly high frequency (∼1–7 oscillations per minute). While slow Vm oscillations in the delta frequency range have previously been reported from the cortex of awake mice, particularly during behavioral quiescence (Poulet and Petersen, 2008; Bennett et al., 2013; Reimer et al., 2014; Schneider et al., 2014; McGinley et al., 2015a), as discussed below, the oscillations reported by Einstein et al. (2017) exhibited several noteworthy features that have not been previously described or analyzed.

An immediately notable characteristic of the 3–5 Hz oscillations reported by Einstein et al. (2017) was their rhythmicity, curiously reminiscent of the clocklike delta oscillations of slow-wave sleep. Both within a recording from a single neuron and comparing across recordings of multiple neurons, the oscillations exhibited a stereotyped Vm signature: a sudden hyperpolarization, followed invariably within 250 ms by short (∼100 ms), high-amplitude (∼20 mV) depolarizing potentials occurring at 3–5 Hz, with a sharp peak in Vm power at 4 Hz. These oscillations lasted for ∼2 s before the Vm returned to the low-amplitude, high-frequency activity classically associated with waking.

Another interesting feature of the 3–5 Hz oscillations was their dependence on visual stimulation. While the responses of V1 neurons to drifting sine-wave gratings diminished during the 3–5 Hz oscillations, the probability of occurrence of these oscillations strongly depended on the presence of visual stimuli. Specifically, oscillation probability rapidly increased within 2 s of stimulus onset. The authors probed the visual-stimulus dependence of the oscillations in two contexts: one in which the stimulus carried no behavioral relevance (passive viewing) and one in which the stimulus predicted reward during a Go-NoGo task in which mice had to discriminate the stimulus from a stimulus of an orthogonal orientation. While both passive viewing of the stimulus and presentation of the stimulus in the context of the task enhanced oscillation probability, the timing of the enhancement differed between these two conditions. During passive viewing, increased oscillation probability was locked to stimulus offset, whereas during presentation within the task, enhancement was locked to stimulus onset. Interestingly, the presence of oscillations did not clearly affect behavioral performance when the rewarded stimulus was presented: oscillation probability was comparable between hit and miss responses. Rather, oscillations often coincided with a suppression of a licking response, particularly during NoGo trials; oscillation probability was significantly higher during correct rejection compared with false alarm responses. In the language of signal detection theory (Green and Swets, 1966), 3–5 Hz oscillations seemed predominately associated with changes in decision bias rather than perceptual discriminability.

The most remarkable feature of the 3–5 Hz oscillations described by Einstein et al. (2017) was their apparent lack of dependence on behavioral or brain state. Several recent publications have combined pupillometry and locomotion monitoring with electrophysiological recordings in multiple cortical areas of waking mice, demonstrating that slow (<10 Hz) cortical activity is associated with low arousal (constricted pupil) and lack of locomotion, whereas this slow activity is consistently abolished during high arousal (dilated pupil) and locomotion (Reimer et al., 2014; Vinck et al., 2015; McGinley et al., 2015a,b). Yet, Einstein et al. (2017) found that 3–5 Hz oscillations were equally compatible with low and high arousal; oscillations occurred with comparable probability across a range of pupil diameters and during both locomotion and stillness. While the oscillations occurred regardless of baseline arousal level (as indexed by baseline pupil diameter), the onset of oscillations coincided with pupillary constriction and a reduction in walking speed, perhaps suggesting that the oscillations are associated with phasic decreases in arousal rather than low tonic arousal levels.

The observation of slow, rhythmic Vm oscillations in awake V1 is a significant finding, especially in light of the high probability with which these oscillations occur, even during high arousal states. Several questions emerge from this work. First, to what degree are 3–5 Hz oscillations spatially synchronized across cortex? Are they synchronized to a similar degree as sleep delta oscillations, or do they occur more locally? Einstein et al. (2017) performed electrocorticographic recordings in V1 simultaneously with their whole-cell recordings, and the results suggested that the oscillations are synchronized network phenomena, at least within V1. It would be informative to perform dual intracellular recordings in V1 and a higher-order visual area, or even in cortex of a different sensory modality, to gauge the spatial synchrony of these waking 3–5 Hz oscillations.

Related to the issue of spatial synchronization, do waking 3–5 Hz oscillations share similar rhythmogenic mechanisms with sleep delta oscillations? Sleep delta rhythms are thought to be generated by thalamocortical (TC) neurons, which are synchronized in their rhythmic output by corticothalamic projections (Timofeev and Steriade, 1996). During deep slow-wave sleep, TC cells are at their most hyperpolarized Vm, giving rise to an oscillatory interaction between the h-current (Ih) and T-current (IT) in these cells (McCormick and Pape, 1990), which leads to burst firing at delta frequencies. This interaction is abolished when TC cells depolarize upon enhanced neuromodulatory tone (Steriade et al., 1991). Could the highly rhythmic delta-frequency activity Einstein et al. (2017) recorded in awake V1 originate from the same thalamic mechanisms as sleep delta oscillations? This would be an unusual scenario because TC cells would need to be hyperpolarized to a degree typically only seen during deep slow-wave sleep. Another sleep rhythm, the spindle oscillation, results from the synaptic interplay between bursting TC cells and GABAergic cells in the thalamic reticular nucleus. During waking periods, it is conceivable that corticothalamic projections could excite thalamic reticular nucleus cells strongly enough such that TC cells are sufficiently hyperpolarized to fire IT-dependent bursts. Indeed, this mechanism may be responsible for a rhythmic 7–12 Hz corticothalamic oscillation occurring during whisker twitching in awake, immobile rodents (Fanselow et al., 2001). Yet, this waking spindle-frequency oscillation is faster than the delta-frequency oscillation observed by Einstein et al. (2017). Intracellular recordings from TC and thalamic reticular nucleus cells in awake mice would be ideal to reveal the behavior of these cells during the 3–5 Hz oscillations, and determine whether they exhibit the characteristic Vm dynamics of either sleep delta or spindle oscillations.

If the waking 3–5 Hz oscillations observed by Einstein et al. (2017) are not thalamically generated, another possibility is that they are generated within local cortical circuits. Certain pyramidal neurons in cortical layer 5 generate rhythmic oscillations at delta frequencies under different activating conditions and at a variety of membrane potentials (Silva et al., 1991). The less restricted set of conditions under which layer 5 pyramidal cells exhibit delta oscillations seems to make layer 5 pyramidal cells a more likely candidate for the generation of a synchronized 3–5 Hz rhythm during wakefulness. Optogenetic inactivation of thalamus or cortical layer 5 triggered by real-time detection of 3–5 Hz oscillations might allow for a more definitive resolution regarding the necessity of thalamus or cortex for these oscillations.

What conditions are necessary for triggering waking 3–5 Hz oscillations in the first place? While Einstein et al. (2017) observed the oscillations in the absence of sensory stimulation, the oscillations were far more frequent when mice viewed drifting sine-wave gratings. Furthermore, the onset of oscillations was generally time-locked to visual stimulus onset, though with a rather long latency (∼2 s). What is the mechanistic basis for the visual-stimulus dependence of the 3–5 Hz oscillations? It is possible that the spatially and temporally rhythmic nature of the repeatedly presented sine-wave gratings evokes a comparatively rhythmic response in the thalamocortical network. Yet, the notion that the 3–5 Hz oscillation is simply a visually evoked response is inconsistent with their onset latency, which is much longer than that of typical evoked responses in mouse V1 (Gao et al., 2010). It will be important to determine whether 3–5 Hz oscillations in V1 also occur under presentation of visual stimuli with broader spatial and temporal frequency spectra, such as Gaussian white noise movies.

Although Einstein et al. (2017) did not find a relationship between baseline arousal and 3–5 Hz oscillation probability, they did report that oscillation onset coincided with pupillary constriction. It has recently been shown that, whereas activity of ascending cholinergic projections tracks baseline pupil diameter, activity in noradrenergic projections tracks rapid changes in pupil diameter (Reimer et al., 2016). Thus, 3–5 Hz oscillations might be triggered by rapid decreases in noradrenergic tone, although the connection between such decreases and visual stimulation would need to be elucidated.

Last, how do 3–5 Hz oscillations influence visual encoding and visually guided behavior? Several pieces of data in Einstein et al. (2017) begin to provide an answer. When 3–5 Hz oscillations coincided with the presentation of a sine-wave grating, the evoked Vm response was reduced, compared with periods without oscillations. This result is consistent with previous observations of reduced neuronal responsiveness to sensory stimulation during the slow oscillatory cortical and thalamic activity of sleep (Steriade and Paré, 2007). Interestingly, however, in the context of a visual discrimination task, 3–5 Hz oscillations did not appear to impair visual behavioral performance. Rather, high oscillation probability was primarily associated with a suppression of a licking response, especially when NoGo stimuli were presented. It will be important to determine whether 3–5 Hz oscillations perhaps affect perceptual decision-making performance when changes in stimulus parameters are closer to perceptual threshold; in their task, Einstein et al. (2017) used sine-wave gratings of orthogonal orientations, a difference that is likely far above perceptual threshold. The notion that 3–5 Hz oscillations might be used for goal-directed visual behavior is suggested by the difference in oscillation onset time when stimuli were presented passively versus when they were presented in the context of a task. Einstein et al. (2017) discuss the possibility that 3–5 Hz oscillations might serve as a mechanism for filtering out task-irrelevant visual information. The design of future behavioral tasks and causal interventions on waking 3–5 Hz oscillations will hopefully clarify whether these oscillations are a bug the cortex must tolerate, or a feature under adaptive control during active behavior.

Footnotes

Editor's Note: These short reviews of recent JNeurosci articles, written exclusively by students or postdoctoral fellows, summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. If the authors of the highlighted article have written a response to the Journal Club, the response can be found by viewing the Journal Club at www.jneurosci.org. For more information on the format, review process, and purpose of Journal Club articles, please see http://jneurosci.org/content/preparing-manuscript#journalclub.

G.T.N. was supported by Yale School of Medicine Brown-Coxe Postdoctoral Fellowship. I thank David A. McCormick for helpful comments on the manuscript.

References

  1. Bennett C, Arroyo S, Hestrin S (2013) Subthreshold mechanisms underlying state-dependent modulation of visual responses. Neuron 80:350–357. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Einstein MC, Polack PO, Tran DT, Golshani P (2017) Visually evoked 3–5 Hz membrane potential oscillations reduce the responsiveness of visual cortex neurons in awake behaving mice. J Neurosci 37:5084–5098. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3868-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Fanselow EE, Sameshima K, Baccala LA, Nicolelis MA (2001) Thalamic bursting in rats during different awake behavioral states. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:15330–15335. 10.1073/pnas.261273898 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Gao E, DeAngelis GC, Burkhalter A (2010) Parallel input channels to mouse primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 30:5912–5926. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6456-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Green DM, Swets JA (1966) Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  6. McCormick DA, Pape HC (1990) Properties of a hyperpolarization-activated cation current and its role in rhythmic oscillation in thalamic relay neurones. J Physiol 431:291–318. 10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. McGinley MJ, David SV, McCormick DA (2015a) Cortical membrane potential signature of optimal states for sensory signal detection. Neuron 87:179–192. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. McGinley MJ, Vinck M, Reimer J, Batista-Brito R, Zagha E, Cadwell CR, Tolias AS, Cardin JA, McCormick DA (2015b) Waking state: rapid variations modulate neural and behavioral responses. Neuron 87:1143–1161. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Neske GT. (2016) The slow oscillation in cortical and thalamic networks: mechanisms and functions. Front Neural Circuits 9:88. 10.3389/fncir.2015.00088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Poulet JF, Petersen CC (2008) Internal brain state regulates membrane potential synchrony in barrel cortex of behaving mice. Nature 454:881–885. 10.1038/nature07150 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Reimer J, Froudarakis E, Cadwell CR, Yatsenko D, Denfield GH, Tolias AS (2014) Pupil fluctuations track fast switching of cortical states during quiet wakefulness. Neuron 84:355–362. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Reimer J, McGinley MJ, Liu Y, Rodenkirch C, Wang Q, McCormick DA, Tolias AS (2016) Pupil fluctuations track rapid changes in adrenergic and cholinergic activity in cortex. Nat Commun 7:13289. 10.1038/ncomms13289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Schneider DM, Nelson A, Mooney R (2014) A synaptic and circuit basis for corollary discharge in the auditory cortex. Nature 513:189–194. 10.1038/nature13724 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Silva LR, Amitai Y, Connors BW (1991) Intrinsic oscillations of neocortex generated by layer 5 pyramidal neurons. Science 251:432–435. 10.1126/science.1824881 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Steriade M. (2000) Corticothalamic resonance, states of vigilance and mentation. Neuroscience 101:243–276. 10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00353-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Steriade M, Paré D (2007) Gating in cerebral networks. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. [Google Scholar]
  17. Steriade M, Timofeev I (2003) Neuronal plasticity in thalamocortical networks during sleep and waking oscillations. Neuron 37:563–576. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00065-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Steriade M, Dossi RC, Nuñez A (1991) Network modulation of a slow intrinsic oscillation of cat thalamocortical neurons implicated in sleep delta waves: cortical potentiation and brainstem cholinergic suppression. J Neurosci 11:3200–3217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Steriade M, Contreras D, Curró Dossi R, Nuñez A (1993a) The slow (<1 Hz) oscillation in reticular thalamic and thalamocortical neurons: scenario of sleep rhythm generation in interacting thalamic and neocortical networks. J Neurosci 13:3284–3299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Steriade M, Nuñez A, Amzica F (1993b) A novel (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J Neurosci 13:3252–3265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Steriade M, Nuñez A, Amzica F (1993c) Intracellular analysis of relations between the slow (<1 Hz) neocortical oscillation and other sleep rhythms of the electroencephalogram. J Neurosci 13:3266–3283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Tan AY, Chen Y, Scholl B, Seidemann E, Priebe NJ (2014) Sensory stimulation shifts visual cortex from synchronous to asynchronous states. Nature 509:226–229. 10.1038/nature13159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Timofeev I, Steriade M (1996) Low-frequency rhythms in the thalamus of intact-cortex and decorticated cats. 76:4152–4168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Vinck M, Batista-Brito R, Knoblich U, Cardin JA (2015) Arousal and locomotion make distinct contributions to cortical activity patterns and visual encoding. Neuron 86:740–754. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Vyazovskiy VV, Olcese U, Hanlon EC, Nir Y, Cirelli C, Tononi G (2011) Local sleep in awake rats. Nature 472:443–447. 10.1038/nature10009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES