Abstract
In higher mammals, orientation tuning of neurons is organized into a quasi-periodic pattern in the primary visual cortex. Our previous model studies suggested that the topography of cortical orientation maps may originate from moiré interference of ON and OFF retinal ganglion cell (RGC) mosaics, but did not account for how the consistent spatial period of maps could be achieved. Here we address this issue with two crucial findings on the development of RGC mosaics: first, homotypic local repulsion between RGCs can develop a long-range hexagonal periodicity. Second, heterotypic interaction restrains the alignment of ON and OFF mosaics, and generates a periodic interference pattern map with consistent spatial frequency. To validate our model, we quantitatively analyzed the RGC mosaics in cat data, and confirmed that the observed retinal mosaics showed evidence of heterotypic interactions, contrary to the previous view that ON and OFF mosaics are developed independently.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Orientation map is one of the most studied functional maps in the brain, but it has remained unanswered how the consistent spatial periodicity of maps could be developed. In the current study, we address this issue with our developmental model for the retinal origin of orientation map. We showed that local repulsive interactions between retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) can develop a hexagonal periodicity in the RGC mosaics and restrict the alignment between ON and OFF mosaics, so that they generate a periodic pattern with consistent spatial frequency for both the RGC mosaics and the cortical orientation maps. Our results demonstrate that the organization of functional maps in visual cortex, including its structural consistency, may be constrained by a retinal blueprint.
Keywords: functional map, moiré interference, orientation map, orientation tuning, retinal ganglion cell, visual cortex
Introduction
In the primary visual cortex (V1) of higher mammals, the spatial distribution of orientation preference across the cortical surface is arranged quasi-periodically, ending in an organized orientation map (Blasdel and Salama, 1986; Ts'o et al., 1990; Bonhoeffer and Grinvald, 1993). It is not completely understood how such periodic organization is initially developed (Purves et al., 1992; Horton and Adams, 2005; Van Hooser, 2007), but clues are found in how the segregation between ON and OFF subregions in the cortical receptive field is developed to originate the orientation tuning in a single V1 neuron (Hubel and Wiesel, 1959; DeAngelis et al., 1995; Reid and Alonso, 1995). In the classical view, it is suggested that a V1 neuron selectively samples feedforward inputs from thalamic neurons to generate an orientation-tuned receptive field (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962).
However, experimental observations have suggested that the orientation tuning in V1 might be determined by the spatial distribution of thalamic afferents. Orientation tuning of V1 neurons was well predicted by the local average of thalamic ON and OFF receptive fields (Jin et al., 2008, 2011; Wang et al., 2015). Also, the topography of functional maps in V1 is strongly correlated to the spatial distribution of thalamic afferents (Kremkow et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2016). These results may reconfirm the earlier observations that the preferred orientation of a V1 neuron remains consistent without recurrent cortical activity, when the thalamic afferents are the only input provided (Ferster et al., 1996; Chung and Ferster, 1998; Lien and Scanziani, 2013). These observations altogether indicate that the topography of cortical orientation maps may be initially seeded by the spatial distribution of ON and OFF thalamic inputs before the postnatal refinement (Chapman et al., 1996; Crair et al., 1998; Fig. 1A).
Given this, how can the spatial distribution of thalamic afferents be organized in a quasi-periodic manner, as observed in the orientation map? Considering that neurons in lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) relay the receptive field of the retinal ganglion cells (RGCs; Usrey et al., 1999), the statistical wiring model (Ringach, 2004, 2007) suggests that V1 receptive field structure may be constrained by the spatial organization of ON- and OFF-center receptive fields of RGCs (Soodak, 1987). Based on this notion, a theoretical model (Paik and Ringach, 2011, 2012) proposes that moiré interference between the hexagonal lattice of ON- and OFF-center RGC generates a quasi-periodic pattern that can seed the topography of an orientation map at an early stage of development (Fig. 1B).
However, the model did not state explicitly how the consistent spatial periodicity of maps within a species could be achieved (Obermayer and Blasdel, 1993; Blasdel et al., 1995). According to the model, the spatial periodicity of an interference pattern depends on two parameters; the lattice distance ratio (α) and the alignment angle (θ) between the two lattice patterns (Fig. 1B). The value of α can be restricted by the cell densities of ON and OFF hexagonal mosaics, but θ also needs to be restricted to a small range to achieve consistent spatial periodicity (Fig. 1C). Thus, it must be explained how θ is restricted during development (Fig. 1D).
Here, we demonstrate that local repulsive interactions between RGCs can restrict θ, and achieve a consistent spatial periodicity in the retinal moiré interference pattern and the cortical orientation map. Using our model simulations, we tested the effect of repulsive interactions between nearby RGCs that induced gradual shifts of cell positions. We first observed that homotypic (ON-to-ON or OFF-to-OFF) interactions could induce a long-range hexagonal organization in each type of RGC mosaic (Wässle et al., 1981a), and that the presence of heterotypic (ON-to-OFF) interactions plays an important role in regulating θ. Then, to validate our model, we quantitatively analyzed the structure of the RGC mosaics observed in cat data, and found evidence that the heterotypic repulsive interaction is in effect during the development, contrary to the notion from previous research (Eglen et al., 2005).
Materials and Methods
Experimental design and statistical analysis.
Local repulsive interaction F between nearby cells was designed as a function of distance r between the centers of dendritic fields to induce a gradual shift of cell position (Fig. 2A). To simulate the development of RGC mosaics, particularly focusing on the effect of local repulsive interactions, we implemented a simplified model (Fig. 2B). Initially, cells are randomly distributed and then a repulsive force, F, between any pair of two RGCs modulates the position of cells in the mosaic (Fig. 2C). We assumed that (1) F increases as two cells approach and (2) that the somas of two cells should not overlap during the developmental process (Ringach, 2007). Considering the physical definition of potential energy, F was assumed to have the form 1/r2 and the diameter of the soma (s) was assumed to be 0.17d (Wässle et al., 1981a; Rapaport and Stone, 1983), where the unit distance d is the theoretically estimated lattice distance of mosaics with ideal hexagonal structure assumed (Fig. 2B). Then, for a given unit distance d, the equation of F is given as follows:
where A is a coefficient for calibration of the strength and the interaction distance R. ε Is a fixed constant set to 0.01 for making the interaction disappear at r = R, so F(R) = 0.
Considering the slight difference between cell densities of ON and OFF RGCs, each homotypic interaction (FON and FOFF) was defined based on the lattice distance (dON and dOFF) as FON = F(r;dON) and FOFF = F(r;dOFF). From the mosaic in the animal data (Zhan and Troy, 2000), the relationship between dON and dOFF was estimated as dON = 1.094 × dOFF, so α = − 1 = 9.4%. Heterotypic interaction (FONOFF) was defined by the average of FON and FOFF for a given cell distance r as FONOFF = F(r; dONOFF), where dONOFF = = 1.05dOFF.
For each iteration of mosaic development simulation, the actual shift of a cell was decided by the net force F⃗net, the summation of all local repulsive interactions that the cell receives. To avoid any unrealistic oscillatory movement, spatial shifts of the cells were modeled to occur by a unit distance, and only when the net force exceeded a threshold at each iteration. If F⃗net exceeded the threshold (10−4), the cell was shifted by 0.01d in the direction of F⃗net. Otherwise, the cell maintained its location. The simulation was terminated when <0.5% of cells moved at each iteration.
All simulations and statistical tests were performed using MATLAB R2016b. Bootstrap and Wilcoxon rank sum test were performed: bootstrap was used to define the significant peaks in 2D autocorrelation of the cell mosaics (see subsections below for details).
Autocorrelation of cell mosaics.
Autocorrelation of a single mosaic was calculated as a smoothed 2D autocorrelogram, which revealed the relative distribution between every pair of cells. Autocorrelations were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of SD 0.15d. The value of correlation was normalized so that the self-correlation peak at the center becomes unity, and the center peak was removed after normalization. The significance of a local peak was verified by bootstrap test: the magnitude of a peak was compared with those in the autocorrelations of control mosaics where the y-coordinates of cell positions were shuffled among the cells. To calculate the averaged autocorrelation, each single autocorrelation was rotated so that the strongest peak was aligned in the +y-axis (Paik and Ringach, 2011). The significance of local peaks was verified by bootstrap test similarly as above, but the control was obtained by calculating the average of randomly rotated autocorrelations.
Estimation of intermosaic distance between ON and OFF RGC mosaics.
Based on the observation that the dendritic fields of ON and OFF RGCs are not developed in the same plane, but in separate layers in the inner plexiform layer (IPL), we estimated the ON and OFF intermosaic distance in two steps: estimating the width of the IPL and the depth difference between ON and OFF RGC mosaics in IPL. The IPL width was estimated to be 45.6 μm in the cross-sectional image of the retina (Kolb, 1979), and the depth difference between ON and OFF layers was 55.6% of the IPL (Balasubramanian and Sterling, 2009). Considering the unit lattice distance in the OFF RGC mosaic (d) was assumed to be 102 μm, the intermosaic distance between ON and OFF RGC mosaics was estimated to be 45.6 μm × 55.6% × (d/102 μm) = 0.248d.
Estimating angle alignment between simulated ON and OFF mosaics.
The angle alignment between simulated ON and OFF mosaics was estimated by matching 2D autocorrelation between the mosaics. Autocorrelation of OFF mosaics was enlarged (increasing α) and rotated (varying θ) to find the condition at which the similarity to autocorrelation of ON mosaics was maximized. Similarity between autocorrelation was measured by the dot product between the 2D correlation plots within 1.25d from the origin. In this step, each single autocorrelation was smoothed by a Gaussian kernel with a SD of 0.26d, for better estimation of peak positions.
Calculation of repulsive energy for a spatial shift of one mosaic.
The energy map was calculated to measure the variation of energy state between heterotypic cell pairs when one mosaic is intentionally shifted. When an ON and OFF cell pair induces repulsive interaction, the repulsive energy (E) is derived from the repulsive force:
First, the heterotypic energy per ON cell was recorded at the origin of the energy map. For all the other points of the map, the OFF mosaic was intentionally shifted by the spatial vector to the point and the energy per cell was measured in the area of overlap between ON and the shifted OFF mosaic.
Results
Homotypic local repulsive interaction as the developmental mechanism of hexagonal RGC mosaics
Regarding the development of orderly retinal mosaics, it has been reported that somas (Wässle et al., 1981a; Zhan and Troy, 2000), dendritic fields (Wässle et al., 1981b), and receptive fields (Gauthier et al., 2009) of one class of RGC are distributed in a quasi-uniform manner across retinal surfaces in adult mammals. This uniform coverage was thought to evolve as a consequence of repulsive interactions between the dendrites of densely located neighboring cells during development to avoid overlap between the functional territory of each cell (Parrish et al., 2007; Grueber and Sagasti, 2010; Zipursky and Grueber, 2013; Lefebvre et al., 2015). This leads their dendritic fields to develop in directions opposite from neighboring cells and results in the shifting of their functional territories to minimize overlap, implying the critical role of the repulsive interaction between nearby cells to develop orderly spatial patterns mosaics. If a local repulsive interaction between neighboring cells can induce gradual shifts of RGC dendritic fields across retinal surfaces (Fig. 2A), this may modulate the spatial organization of RGC mosaics. Here we assumed that this local interaction between dendrites of neighboring cells may develop the quasi-regular hexagonal distribution of dendritic fields observed in RGC mosaics data (Wässle et al., 1981a) and tested the hypothesis by simulating the development of RGC mosaics.
We first investigated whether a local repulsive interaction between cells of the same type (homotypic interaction) could develop a hexagonal pattern in the RGC mosaics (Fig. 2C), which is a necessary condition to achieve moiré interference (Wässle et al., 1981a; Gauthier et al., 2009; Paik and Ringach, 2011). In this case, we simulated an arbitrary monotypic mosaic that could be either an ON or an OFF mosaic. From the initial condition where the dendritic fields were randomly distributed (Fig. 2D), homotypic repulsive interaction gradually rearranged the center of dendritic fields, and could generate a quasi-regular structure of a developed dendritic field mosaic (Fig. 2E,F). When the interaction distance R was smaller than 1d, dendritic fields were spread to some degree but could not tile the mosaic regularly enough to generate a long-range lattice pattern (Fig. 2E,I). However, when the range of interaction was sufficiently large (R > 1d), we could observe a salient pattern of hexagonal lattice in the developed cell mosaics (Fig. 2F,J). Our results suggest that the local interaction range is a critical factor in the development of regularly structured mosaics (Fig. 2G).
To quantify the lattice pattern developed, we examined the distribution of the lattice angle (ϕ) between neighboring cells (Boots, 1974; Muir et al., 2011; Fig. 2F, black dash lines). As the interaction distance R increased, the peaks at 60° became stronger, confirming the hexagonal structure of the mosaics (Fig. 2H). In addition, we examined the distribution of peaks in the autocorrelation of mosaics (Fig. 2I,J, black circles). At R = 0.75d, autocorrelation peaks show weak periodicity in the mosaic lattice (Fig. 2I), but when R = 1.1d, the angular locations of the local peaks relative to the strongest peak (0°) in the individual mosaics were within ±5° of the multiple of 60° in all trials of the simulation (Fig. 2J). We also found that this hexagonal pattern was not limited to local mosaic structure, but also had long-range order (Fig. 2K). When the local repulsive interaction range was sufficiently large (R > 1d), higher-order peaks (up to 6th–7th order) were observed in the autocorrelation of mosaics (Fig. 2K, black circles). In addition, the positions of the peaks up to fourth order were matched to those mathematically calculated from an ideal hexagonal lattice (Fig. 2K, solid white squares). Our results suggest that a local repulsive interaction between RGCs may generate a long-range hexagonal lattice pattern of ON and OFF mosaics.
Repulsive interactions between heterotypic RGCs
The superposition of hexagonal ON and OFF RGC mosaics induced by homotypic interaction could produce a periodic moiré interference, as suggested in previous model studies (Paik and Ringach, 2011, 2012). However, there is no guarantee of a consistent spatial periodicity of an interference pattern (Obermayer and Blasdel, 1993; Blasdel et al., 1995), unless the alignment angle (θ) between the two mosaics is restricted to a small range. This restricted alignment between two mosaics might not be achieved when ON and OFF RGC mosaics are developed independently as suggested previously (Eglen et al., 2005; Hore et al., 2012). Instead, we assumed that heterotypic interaction between the ON and OFF RGC mosaics might also be in effect during retinal development, and that this could restrict the alignment angle.
It has been observed that the dendritic fields of ON and OFF RGCs occupy different tissue planes in the IPL (Balasubramanian and Sterling, 2009; Fig. 3A). The interlayer distance (dm) between the two types of planes guarantees that ON and OFF RGCs are at least dm away from each other. This implies that the actual distance of a heterotypic RGC pair must be greater than the distance previously estimated from the 2D data where ON and OFF RGCs are assumed to be on the same plane (Wässle et al., 1981a; Eglen et al., 2005; Ringach, 2007). Furthermore, the growth of RGC dendritic fields is not isotropic but is largely on the plane of each layer (Famiglietti et al., 1977), indicating that the repulsive interaction of dendritic fields between heterotypic RGCs may have smaller effect than that between homotypic RGCs. For these reasons, the influence of heterotypic repulsive interaction might not have been observed to be as significant as the homotypic ones (Wässle et al., 1981a).
Heterotypic interaction restricts alignment between mosaics
As with the model of single-type RGC mosaic development, we implemented both homotypic (Eglen et al., 2005) and heterotypic repulsive interactions in our model simulations of ON and OFF RGC development (Fig. 3A). We modulated the strength of heterotypic interaction (FONOFF) by introducing the weight term w, to indicate the relative strength compared with that of homotypic interaction. Based on the experimental observations, we set interlayer distance at 0.25d, where d is the expected average lattice distance in simulated OFF mosaics (Famiglietti et al., 1977; Kolb, 1979). The ratio between the expected lattice distances of simulated ON and OFF mosaics (1 + α) was set to 1.094, based on animal data (Zhan and Troy, 2000).
In the mosaics developed in the absence of heterotypic interaction FONOFF (termed simulated control mosaics; Fig. 3B, left), we observed hexagonal patterns from the angular distribution of local peaks in the autocorrelation (Fig. 3C, left), similar to the result in Figure 2J. Here, the alignment between ON and OFF hexagonal patterns appeared independent (Fig. 3C, left, bottom). On the other hand, we observed that a certain level of heterotypic interaction could affect the alignment between the mosaics without interrupting the development of the hexagonal pattern in each mosaic. In the presence of FONOFF (termed simulated model mosaics; Fig. 3B, right), each of the simulated mosaics still showed a hexagonal pattern (Fig. 3C, right), but the alignment between ON and OFF hexagonal patterns appeared restricted, and different from that developed without FONOFF (Fig. 3C, right, bottom).
To further investigate the influence of FONOFF, we measured the angular alignment between simulated ON and OFF mosaics for different w. We estimated the relative ratio of lattice distance (α) and the alignment angle (θ) between ON and OFF mosaics, by searching the amount of rotation and linear expansion of the autocorrelation of the OFF mosaic that maximizes the similarity to the autocorrelation of ON mosaic (Fig. 3D). We found that θ was restricted to small angles as w increased (Fig. 3E). On the other hand, α was maintained at near the expected level in the model (0.094; Fig. 3F). This is because α is mostly constrained by the ratio between the cell densities. Consequently, the scaling factor S, which is the spatial period of the moiré interference pattern, could be computed based on the obtained θ and α (Paik and Ringach, 2011). As w increased, the computed scaling factor was also restricted within a narrow range around the expected level of the scaling factor (S = 10.4; Fig. 3G, black arrow; Paik and Ringach, 2011). When the heterotypic interaction was sufficiently large (w > 0.02), we confirmed that the obtained θ and the estimated scaling factor S were significantly different from those of the control where ON and OFF mosaics were randomly aligned (*p < 10−21, Wilcoxon rank sum test; Fig. 3H,I), whereas they were not distinguishable from the control for w = 0. Considering that the absence of the heterotypic interaction (w = 0) resulted in a failure to restrict the scaling factor to a small range, these results suggest that the heterotypic interaction in RGC mosaics could be the key factor in achieving consistent spatial periodicity in the interference pattern within a species.
Validation of heterotypic interaction in observed RGC mosaics
Next, to validate the central prediction of our model about the presence of the heterotypic interaction, we tried to estimate the influence of the heterotypic interaction during development using experimental data. The typical approaches for measuring interactions between two cell mosaics are 2D cross-correlograms (Ringach, 2007) or 1D density recovery profiles (Rodieck, 1991; Eglen et al., 2005). However, these methods only focus on the spatial distribution relative to neighboring cells, rather than the relationship at whole-mosaic scale. Thus, to investigate the effect of heterotypic interaction at the whole-mosaic scale, we conducted an analysis of the repulsive energy in each pair of ON and OFF RGCs, using simulated mosaics and measured mosaic data from animal experiments (soma: Wässle et al., 1981a, Zhan and Troy, 2000; receptive field: Gauthier et al., 2009; Fig. 4). Although our developmental model is for the spatial distribution of dendritic fields, we assumed that this model could also be applied to the mosaic structure of a cell body and receptive field, because it was reported that all three of them are spatially clustered on the retinal surface (Brown et al., 2000). Thus, the influence of the heterotypic interaction between dendrites can also be estimated in receptive field and soma mosaics data.
As two cells shift away from each other, repulsive energy between the cells would decrease, resulting in a more stable state of the cell pair. Thus, if heterotypic repulsive interaction exists during the development of RGC mosaics, regardless its strength, the structure of ON and OFF mosaics will be modulated in a way that heterotypic repulsive energy is reduced. Therefore, by examining whether the heterotypic repulsive energy of RGC mosaics is minimized in the animal data, we could investigate whether the actual organization of the RGC mosaic is consistent with our model with heterotypic interaction.
Under this condition, if either an ON or an OFF RGC mosaic developed with heterotypic interaction is shifted a bit in any direction, the repulsive energy per cell within the areas of overlap of ON and OFF mosaics will increase compared with the original state (Fig. 4A). On the other hand, ON and OFF mosaics developed without heterotypic repulsive interactions will not show this pattern of energy change (Fig. 4B). To validate this hypothesis, we examined the repulsive energy, while we varied the amount of spatial shift of an OFF mosaic in all directions in both simulated and observed mosaics (Fig. 4C–E).
In the simulated mosaic developed with heterotypic interaction, the energy map had a local minimum at the origin, and a band of a higher energy level around it (Fig. 4F, left). In contrast, this pattern was not observed in the energy map of simulated control mosaics developed with no heterotypic interaction (Fig. 4F, middle). Notably, the energy maps estimated from the RGC mosaics observed in animal studies (Wässle et al., 1981a; Zhan and Troy, 2000; Gauthier et al., 2009) showed a local minimum at the origin and a higher energy band around it (Fig. 4F, right), similar to that from the model simulation. To analyze this energy distribution quantitatively, we investigated the radial distribution of energy by averaging the map in polar coordinates (Fig. 4G). In both the model simulation and the animal data, the energy maps showed local minima at the origin and the energy levels off the origin were higher than those in annuli close to the origin, implying that the original RGC mosaics were in stable energy states in terms of ON-OFF RGC repulsive energy (Fig. 4G). Our results suggest that the observed RGC mosaics in animal data show the vestige of heterotypic interaction during RGC development.
Development of hexagonal pattern does not require a particular form of the repulsive interaction
Although the dendritic maturation of RGC could be affected by various factors (such as stimulus-dependent synaptic activity; Tian, 2011), it has been observed that RGC mosaics can develop without visual experience (Anishchenko et al., 2010). This implies that a simple intercellular interaction in the retina may be sufficient to originate orderly functional structure across retinal surfaces. Our model provides a simple explanation of this retinal interaction between neighboring cells during retinal development. In previous studies, it was argued that molecular mechanisms caused avoidance between dendrites of other cells in the retina (Zipursky and Grueber, 2013; Lefebvre et al., 2015), but it is worth pointing out that our current result is valid as long as both homotypic and heterotypic repulsive interactions exist, regardless of the underlying mechanism. Considering that the exact form of local interaction in retinal mosaics has not been measured, we performed additional simulation for different forms of interaction (Fig. 5A), just in case the form of interaction (∝1/r2 in the main simulation) substantially affected our conclusion. As long as the interaction distance is sufficient to tile the mosaic (R = 1.1d), all the forms of interaction reproduced regular and hexagonal mosaic structures (Fig. 5B,C), consistent with the results in Figure 2. Probably, observations of the retinal mosaic structure during each developmental stage could provide direct evidence of details in our model.
Alignment between mosaics was kept even with noisy mosaics
One might argue that the simulated mosaics developed in our model have a much more regular organization of the lattice pattern than what is observed in the animal data (Wässle et al., 1981a; Zhan and Troy, 2000). Here, we intended to focus on illustration of the mechanism by which local RGC repulsive interaction could develop long-range order in the ON and OFF mosaics, and also regulate the alignment between them (see Discussion). For this reason, the simulation parameters were chosen to best describe the hexagonal lattice structure of mosaics and the alignment angle of mosaics (R = 1.1d). However, we confirmed that the suggested mechanism also worked under noisy conditions of mosaics by modulating the regularity of the mosaic structure with the interaction range R (Fig. 2E,F; Fig. 5D). We observed that the alignment of ON and OFF simulated mosaics and the computed scaling factor were still restricted (Fig. 5E,F) when we reduced interaction range R to produce much less orderly mosaics, consistent with the results in Figure 3E–G. This result suggests that our theoretical model is also applicable to more realistic parameter conditions.
Limitation in observing periodicity at long range in measured mosaics
One may argue that the simplest way to validate our model is to observe the moiré patterns of ON and OFF RGC in the measured mosaics. However, an analytical limitation arises from the size of the measured retinal patches, which is, at most, ∼1 mm2 including only ∼100 cells for a single type of RGC. This may not be sufficiently large to investigate the emergence of interference pattern between mosaics. To address this issue, we reconstructed a larger mosaic with a level of long-range order similar to that of the measured mosaic and examined whether the size of the mosaic could affect the observation of the interference pattern.
As represented in our previous work (Paik and Ringach, 2011), our analysis of the autocorrelation of a measured RGC mosaic revealed the hexagonal arrangement of first-order peaks and many other higher-order peaks (Fig. 6A). For quantitative analysis of the hexagonal arrangement at long range in the measured RGC mosaics, we found that the spatial distribution of these higher-order peaks are well fitted to a hexagonal lattice up to fifth to sixth order, which is the limit of analysis with the given size of measured mosaic (Fig. 6B). However, considering that the expected period of the interference pattern in RGC mosaics is larger than this (Paik and Ringach, 2011), the available range of local peaks may not be large enough to reveal a complete period of the interference pattern.
Thus, for further validation of our prediction, we generated large mosaics with level of long-range periodicity equivalent to that of the measured mosaics from the parameter search (Fig. 6C). We searched model parameters where simulated mosaics and measured mosaics had the same level of error distance from an ideal hexagonal lattice. Note that as the parameter R increases, the developed mosaic becomes more hexagonal (Fig. 2H); so the error distance decreases (Fig. 6C). Then, we observed that the mosaics simulated with repulsion range R = 0.9d represented a level of long-range hexagonal periodicity similar to that of the measured mosaics (Fig. 6C, red arrow). We used this condition to reconstruct large mosaics in Figure 6D and further examined whether the reconstructed large mosaics could reproduce an interference pattern that could seed an orientation map. In the autocorrelation analysis of each mosaic structure, not only the whole reconstructed mosaics but also the cropped mosaics showed a hexagonal arrangement of first-order peaks (Fig. 6E). This implies that both sizes of mosaics show hexagonal lattice patterns, as in Figure 6A.
Next, with reconstructed ON and OFF mosaics in (Fig. 6D), we simulated orientation maps using the statistical wiring model (Fig. 6F; Paik and Ringach, 2011). If ON and OFF mosaics reproduce an interference pattern at long-range, the resulting orientation map must show a hexagonal structure, as predicted by the moiré pattern. As expected, the interference pattern between the largely reconstructed mosaics seeded a hexagonal structure in the orientation map (Fig. 6G, top). However, we also observed that a small part of the reconstructed mosaics that were cropped to the same size as the measured mosaics could not clearly show hexagonal periodicity of a reconstructed orientation map (Fig. 6G, bottom). This implies that among mosaics, sufficient size is necessary for a direct estimation of the long-range periodicity, and that the observation of the interference pattern at long range might not be guaranteed from currently available size of measured mosaics. Thus, our analysis provides a vestige of the long-range periodicity in the measured mosaics (Fig. 6A,B), but it remains to be investigated further when larger patches of retinal mosaics could be obtained experimentally.
Discussion
How is the consistent spatial periodicity of an orientation map retained within a species? In the present study, we proposed a developmental model of RGC mosaics to provide an answer to this fundamental question by addressing a scenario with both homotypic and heterotypic interactions. We first showed that a local repulsive interaction between same-type (homotypic) RGCs developed a long-range hexagonal pattern in ON and OFF simulated mosaics. Then we showed that the relatively weak interaction between ON and OFF (heterotypic) RGCs constrains the alignment angle between the two mosaics, even though it is not strong enough to significantly modulate the structure of the mosaic of the other type. This restricted alignment between mosaics induces a consistent periodicity of retinal moiré interference patterns that will seed a cortical orientation map. Last, in the analysis of the observed RGC mosaics of the experimental data, we found evidence of heterotypic interaction that supports our model prediction. This suggests that both homotypic and heterotypic retinal interactions may be crucial to the organization of functional architecture in the retina, which would initiate the functional architectures in V1 (Soodak, 1987; Ringach, 2004, 2007, Paik and Ringach, 2011, 2012).
Because the quasi-uniform tiling of a retinal cell mosaic has been observed (Wässle et al., 1981a), the developmental origin of this regular spacing has been studied in some molecular-biological and genetic studies (Lin et al., 2004; Zipursky and Grueber, 2013; Lefebvre et al., 2015). In particular, Eglen et al. (2005) suggested a mathematical model for the developmental process of the regularly structured retinal mosaic. The developmental mechanism implemented in this study was based on the pairwise interaction point process (PIPP), where cells were created or removed by conceptual birth-and-death steps. In the PIPP model, each RGC is repeatedly removed and relocated to random positions until all the positions of cells are accepted by a process of stochastic interaction. Consequently, the PIPP model could not generate a hexagonal pattern in mosaics, leading to the conclusion that local interaction between nearby RGCs does not reproduce a hexagonal pattern (Hore et al., 2012). However, the PIPP process did not consider the actual process of development, where cells are not really created and removed every time. The model did not address the possibility that the position of RGCs are gradually modulated by repulsive interactions so that they might generate a well packed organization of dendritic fields and receptive fields as observed (Gauthier et al., 2009). In our present study, instead of the conceptual birth-and-death steps, we introduced a more biologically plausible mechanism based on the observed pattern of dendritic field growth. Our developmental mechanism allowed a continuous change of dendritic field area, which enabled close-packing of cells into a hexagonal lattice pattern. Furthermore, our simulation showed that the closely packed local hexagonal lattice could generate a long-range order of lattice (Wässle et al., 1981a,b; Zhan and Troy, 2000; Gauthier et al., 2009).
In the aspect of the role of heterotypic interaction, in a previous study, it was assumed that ON and OFF RGC mosaics develop independently (Eglen et al., 2005). Based on the analysis whether a density of one type of cell is dependent on the position of the other types of cells, the researchers concluded that the distribution of the cells was uniform, implying that the location of an RGC does not provide any information about the location of other types of cells. However, the authors neglected the chance that relatively weak heterotypic interactions between ON and OFF RGCs exist. Indeed, an analysis in the study above showed that there was a weak spatial dependency between the ON and OFF cells (Eglen et al., 2005, their Fig. 1). Because the dendritic fields of ON and OFF RGCs occupy different tissue planes (Balasubramanian and Sterling, 2009), the actual distance of a heterotypic RGC pair could be larger than what is measured in previous 2D data, where ON and OFF RGCs were assumed to be in the same plane (Wässle et al., 1981a; Eglen et al., 2005; Ringach, 2007). Thus, the repulsive interaction between heterotypic RGCs may not appear as significant as that between homotypic RGCs.
For better realization of a biologically plausible model, we may implement some additional factors in a future model that we did not apply to the current model for the convenience of theoretical argument. For example, variation in the spatial aspect ratio of receptive fields (Suematsu et al., 2013) implies the elliptic and anisotropic development of RGC dendritic fields, and this may develop more realistic and noisy structure of RGC mosaics. Thus, it remains to be determined how much the restricted alignment between the mosaics can be maintained by local repulsion under noisy conditions. Because our model predicts that the orderly structure of RGC mosaics is necessary for the development of orientation maps, it can be also tested whether incomplete tiling of RGCs induces abnormal development of the cortical map. Controlled experiments on the abnormal development of RGC mosaics, such as in mutant animals with the shrinkage of repulsion between RGC dendrites (Parrish et al., 2007; Grueber and Sagasti, 2010; Zipursky and Grueber, 2013; Lefebvre et al., 2015) might give an answer to this argument. To validate our model prediction further, more precise investigation is needed about what is required in the feedforward and recurrent circuits to seed quasi-regular orientation maps from the simulated mosaics. For example, the convergence of retinogeniculate (Usrey et al., 1999) and thalamocortical (Reid and Alonso, 1995; Jin et al., 2011) feedforward connections need to be examined carefully because they significantly affect the receptive field structure and orientation tuning developed in V1 neurons.
Considering the comparable retinal organization between higher mammals and rodents, one might argue why the orientation preference is arranged randomly in rodent V1, termed a salt-and-pepper map, if the structure of the orientation map is already seeded in the retinal mosaics. To address this issue, we can argue two aspects. First, experimental data suggest that the composition of retinal cells in rodents has greater variation than that in higher mammals. In the statistics of RGC spacing, the nearest-neighbor distance in a single type of RGC widely varies across animals and even sometimes the number of ON cells in a retinal patch is greater than that of OFF cells (Anishchenko et al., 2010), although more OFF cells are measured in most experiments in higher mammals (Wässle et al., 1981a; Zhan and Troy, 2000; for review, see Balasubramanian and Sterling, 2009). Second, more importantly, the structure of feedforward projection through the visual pathway seems to be different between rodents and primates. Retinogeniculate connection in higher mammals is almost one-to-one so the structure of a single LGN receptive field has a shape similar to that of a single RGC (Usrey et al., 1999). However, in rodents, recent tracing techniques revealed that the network of retinogeniculate connection is much more complex, so that a single LGN neuron receives input from a number of different types of RGCs (Morgan et al., 2016). We expected that the characteristics of feedforward convergence would be a crucial factor in determining the structure of cortical receptive fields and their spatial organization, so that even different types of orientation map could be seeded even from an identical set of retinal mosaics. Further tracing experiments may reveal the structural difference in visual systems of rodents and higher mammals and explain the origin of the diversity of orientation maps across species.
Note that the present scope of the model is limited to explaining how the cortical map structure is initially organized by the retinal origin (Paik and Ringach, 2011). For the complete comprehension of the visual cortical functions (Lien and Scanziani, 2013; Reinhold et al., 2015), the postnatal activity-dependent development of recurrent cortical circuits should be taken into account to explain observations such as the cortical responses for multiple orientations (Basole et al., 2003; Koch et al., 2016) or the matching process between orientation maps through two eyes (Crair et al., 1998).
It is worth pointing out that the basic concept of our model is connected to other disciplines that also investigate the spatial distribution of particles or their local interaction. In solid-state physics, the hexagonal arrangement of atoms has been widely observed to maximize the volume of the virtual spheres between atoms when they are closely-packed in a limited space (Ashcroft and Mermin, 1976; Kittel, 2005), supporting that the hexagonal arrangement would be an efficient strategy for uniform tiling of cells. In physical chemistry, the role of local interaction between nearby particles were extensively discussed in phase transition (Blundell and Blundell, 2009; Ivancevic and Ivancevic, 2014). These previous studies in various disciplines suggest the crucial role of local interaction in the organization of regular spacing of particles.
Overall, our results suggest that local repulsive interactions in retinal development may be crucial to the development of the orderly structure of retinal mosaics, and also to the consistent organization of functional architectures in the visual cortex.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF), funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT and Future Planning (NRF-2016R1C1B2016039), and the Future Systems Healthcare Project of KAIST (to S.P.). We thank Jose-Manuel Alonso, Matteo Carandini, and Dario Ringach for providing comments on earlier versions of this paper.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Anishchenko A, Greschner M, Elstrott J, Sher A, Litke AM, Feller MB, Chichilnisky EJ (2010) Receptive field mosaics of retinal ganglion cells are established without visual experience. J Neurophysiol 103:1856–1864. 10.1152/jn.00896.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashcroft NW, Mermin ND (1976) Solid state physics. San Diego: Harcourt College. [Google Scholar]
- Balasubramanian V, Sterling P (2009) Receptive fields and functional architecture in the retina. J Physiol 587:2753–2767. 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.170704 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basole A, White LE, Fitzpatrick D (2003) Mapping multiple features in the population response of visual cortex. Nature 423:986–990. 10.1038/nature01721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blasdel GG, Salama G (1986) Voltage-sensitive dyes reveal a modular organization in monkey striate cortex. Nature 321:579–585. 10.1038/321579a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blasdel G, Obermayer K, Kiorpes L (1995) Organization of ocular dominance and orientation columns in the striate cortex of neonatal macaque monkeys. Vis Neurosci 12:589–603. 10.1017/S0952523800008476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blundell SJ, Blundell KM (2009) Concepts in thermal physics. New Yokr: Oxford UP. [Google Scholar]
- Bonhoeffer T, Grinvald A (1993) The layout of iso-orientation domains in area 18 of cat visual cortex: optical imaging reveals a pinwheel-like organization. J Neurosci 13:4157–4180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boots BN. (1974) Delaunay triangles: an alternative approach to point pattern analysis. Proc Assoc Am Geogr 6:26–29. [Google Scholar]
- Brown SP, He S, Masland RH (2000) Receptive field microstructure and dendritic geometry of retinal ganglion cells. Neuron 27:371–383. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00044-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman B, Stryker MP, Bonhoeffer T (1996) Development of orientation preference maps in ferret primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 16:6443–6453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chung S, Ferster D (1998) Strength and orientation tuning of the thalamic input to simple cells revealed by electrically evoked cortical suppression. Neuron 20:1177–1189. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80498-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crair MC, Gillespie DC, Stryker MP (1998) The role of visual experience in the development of columns in cat visual cortex. Science 279:566–570. 10.1126/science.279.5350.566 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeAngelis GC, Ohzawa I, Freeman RD (1995) Receptive-field dynamics in the central visual pathways. Trends Neurosci 18:451–458. 10.1016/0166-2236(95)94496-R [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eglen SJ, Diggle PJ, Troy JB (2005) Homotypic constraints dominate positioning of on- and off-center beta retinal ganglion cells. Vis Neurosci 22:859–871. 10.1017/S0952523805226147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Famiglietti EV, Kaneko A, Tachibana M (1977) Neuronal archictecture of on and off pathways to ganglion cell in carp retina. Science 198:1267–1269. 10.1126/science.73223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferster D, Chung S, Wheat H (1996) Orientation selectivity of thalamic input to simple cells of cat visual cortex. Nature 380:249–252. 10.1038/380249a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gauthier JL, Field GD, Sher A, Greschner M, Shlens J, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ (2009) Receptive fields in primate retina are coordinated to sample visual space more uniformly. PLoS Biol 7:e1000063. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grueber WB, Sagasti A (2010) Self-avoidance and tiling: mechanisms of dendrite and axon spacing. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2:a001750. 10.1101/cshperspect.a001750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hore VR, Troy JB, Eglen SJ (2012) Parasol cell mosaics are unlikely to drive the formation of structured orientation maps in primary visual cortex. Vis Neurosci 29:283–299. 10.1017/S0952523812000338 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horton JC, Adams DL (2005) The cortical column: a structure without a function. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360:837–862. 10.1098/rstb.2005.1623 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1959) Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex. J Physiol 148:574–591. 10.1113/jphysiol.1959.sp006308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol 160:106–154. 10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivancevic VG, Ivancevic TT (2014) Complex nonlinearity: combining it all together. In: Complex Nonlinearity, pp 657–711. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Jin JZ, Weng C, Yeh CI, Gordon JA, Ruthazer ES, Stryker MP, Swadlow HA, Alonso JM (2008) On and off domains of geniculate afferents in cat primary visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 11:88–94. 10.1038/nn2029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jin J, Wang Y, Swadlow HA, Alonso JM (2011) Population receptive fields of ON and OFF thalamic inputs to an orientation column in visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 14:232–238. 10.1038/nn.2729 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kittel C. (2005) Introduction to solid state physics, Ed 8 Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Koch E, Jin J, Alonso JM, Zaidi Q (2016) Functional implications of orientation maps in primary visual cortex. Nat Commun 7:13529. 10.1038/ncomms13529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolb H. (1979) The inner plexiform layer in the retina of the cat: electron microscopic observations. J Neurocytol 8:295–329. 10.1007/BF01236124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kremkow J, Jin J, Wang Y, Alonso JM (2016) Principles underlying sensory map topography in primary visual cortex. Nature 533:52–57. 10.1038/nature17936 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee KS, Huang X, Fitzpatrick D (2016) Topology of ON and OFF inputs in visual cortex enables an invariant columnar architecture. Nature 533:90–94. 10.1038/nature17941 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lefebvre JL, Sanes JR, Kay JN (2015) Development of dendritic form and function. Annu Rev Cell Dev Biol 31:741–777. 10.1146/annurev-cellbio-100913-013020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lien AD, Scanziani M (2013) Tuned thalamic excitation is amplified by visual cortical circuits. Nat Neurosci 16:1315–1323. 10.1038/nn.3488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin B, Wang SW, Masland RH (2004) Retinal ganglion cell type, size, and spacing can be specified independent of homotypic dendritic contacts. Neuron 43:475–485. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan JL, Berger DR, Wetzel AW, Lichtman JW (2016) The fuzzy logic of network connectivity in mouse visual thalamus. Cell 165:192–206. 10.1016/j.cell.2016.02.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muir DR, Da Costa NM, Girardin CC, Naaman S, Omer DB, Ruesch E, Grinvald A, Douglas RJ (2011) Embedding of cortical representations by the superficial patch system. Cereb Cortex 21:2244–2260. 10.1093/cercor/bhq290 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Obermayer K, Blasdel GG (1993) Geometry of orientation and ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. J Neurosci 13:4114–4129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paik SB, Ringach DL (2011) Retinal origin of orientation maps in visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 14:919–925. 10.1038/nn.2824 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paik SB, Ringach DL (2012) Link between orientation and retinotopic maps in primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:7091–7096. 10.1073/pnas.1118926109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parrish JZ, Emoto K, Kim MD, Jan YN (2007) Mechanisms that regulate establishment, maintenance, and remodeling of dendritic fields. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:399–423. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112907 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purves D, Riddle DR, LaMantia AS (1992) Iterated patterns of brain circuitry (or how the cortex gets its spots). Trends Neurosci 15:362–368. 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90180-G [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rapaport DH, Stone J (1983) Time course of morphological differentiation of cat retinal ganglion cells: influences on soma size. J Comp Neurol 221:42–52. 10.1002/cne.902210104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid RC, Alonso JM (1995) Specificity of monosynaptic connections from thalamus to visual cortex. Nature 378:281–284. 10.1038/378281a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinhold K, Lien AD, Scanziani M (2015) Distinct recurrent versus afferent dynamics in cortical visual processing. Nat Neurosci 18:1789–1797. 10.1038/nn.4153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ringach DL. (2004) Haphazard wiring of simple receptive fields and orientation columns in visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 92:468–476. 10.1152/jn.01202.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ringach DL. (2007) On the origin of the functional architecture of the cortex. PLoS One 2:e251. 10.1371/journal.pone.0000251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodieck RW. (1991) The density recovery profile: a method for the analysis of points in the plane applicable to retinal studies. Vis Neurosci 6:95–111. 10.1017/S095252380001049X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soodak RE. (1987) The retinal ganglion cell mosaic defines orientation columns in striate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 84:3936–3940. 10.1073/pnas.84.11.3936 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suematsu N, Naito T, Miyoshi T, Sawai H, Sato H (2013) Spatiotemporal receptive field structures in retinogeniculate connections of cat. Front Syst Neurosci 7:103. 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tian N. (2011) Developmental mechanisms that regulate retinal ganglion cell dendritic morphology. Dev Neurobiol 71:1297–1309. 10.1002/dneu.20900 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ts'o DY, Frostig RD, Lieke EE, Grinvald A (1990) Functional organization of primate visual cortex revealed by high resolution optical imaging. Science 249:417–420. 10.1126/science.2165630 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Usrey WM, Reppas JB, Reid RC (1999) Specificity and strength of retinogeniculate connections. J Neurophysiol 82:3527–3540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Hooser SD. (2007) Similarity and diversity in visual cortex: is there a unifying theory of cortical computation? Neuroscientist 13:639–656. 10.1177/1073858407306597 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Jin J, Kremkow J, Lashgari R, Komban SJ, Alonso JM (2015) Columnar organization of spatial phase in visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 18:97–103. 10.1038/nn.3878 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wässle H, Boycott BB, Illing RB (1981a) Morphology and mosaic of on- and off-beta cells in the cat retina and some functional considerations. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 212:177–195. 10.1098/rspb.1981.0033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wässle H, Peichl L, Boycott BB (1981b) Dendritic territories of cat retinal ganglion cells. Nature 292:344–345. 10.1038/292344a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhan XJ, Troy JB (2000) Modeling cat retinal beta-cell arrays. Vis Neurosci 17:23–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zipursky SL, Grueber WB (2013) The molecular basis of self-avoidance. Annu Rev Neurosci 36:547–568. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150414 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]