Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Psychol. 2018 Dec 27;28:143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.015

Measuring Cognitive Outcomes in Mindfulness-based Intervention Research: A Reflection on Confounding Factors and Methodological Limitations

David R Vago a,*, Resh S Gupta a, Sara W Lazar b
PMCID: PMC6597333  NIHMSID: NIHMS1519343  PMID: 30682701

Abstract

One potential pathway by which mindfulness-based meditation improves health outcomes is through changes in cognitive functioning. Here, we summarize and comment upon three systematic reviews conducted over the last 7 years that have had the goal of identifying the impact of mindfulness on cognitive outcomes. In our analysis, we identified a number of methodological limitations and potential confounding factors that interfere with and limit our ability to interpret the results. In order to gain a granular view of the relationship between mindfulness training and cognition, we report on the following: 1) What do we know? How does mindfulness affect cognition? 2) variable criteria that define an MBI; 2) limitations of assays used to measure cognition; and 3) methodological quality of an MBI trial and reporting of findings. Finally, we offer constructive means for interpretation and recommendations for moving the field of mindfulness research forward regarding effects on cognition.

Keywords: meditation, mindfulness, cognition, mindfulness-based intervention

Introduction

As the field of mindfulness research continues to grow, there is a need to rigorously interrogate the existing data to better understand the advantages and effectiveness of the 8- week Mindfulness-based Intervention (MBI) model and its variations for specific outcomes while also acknowledging its limitations. One potential pathway by which mindfulness-based meditation training may improve health outcomes and promote adaptive behavioral change is through changes in cognitive functioning. Here, we summarize and comment upon three systematic reviews conducted over the last seven years, two of which are currently published [1,2] and one has recently been completed by the authors, all of which had the goal of identifying effects of MBIs on cognitive outcomes. During our systematic review of the existing studies, we identified a number of methodological limitations and potential confounds (e.g., non-specific effects, ceiling effects) that interfere with and limit our ability to make rigorous interpretations about the (actual) effects of mindfulness on cognition. Moreover, confounding factors may also influence both independent (training effects) and dependent variables (measures of cognition) leading to spurious claims. Furthermore, many of these methodological limitations can be generalized to other mindfulness research and psychological research in general. In order to gain a granular view of the relationship between mindfulness training and cognition, we review these issues here and suggest ways to surmount such limitations in the future. The paper is organized into the following sections:1) What do we know? How does mindfulness affect cognition? 2) Variable criteria that define an MBI; 3) Limitations of assays used to measure cognition; and 4) Methodological quality of an MBI trial and reporting of findings.

What do We Know? How does Mindfulness Affect Cognition?

Mindfulness meditation is commonly characterized as a “systematic form of mental training” which may impact certain aspects of cognition. Several theoretical models have been developed to describe the cognitive processes which may underlie meditation or may be impacted by an MBI [37]. Two highly cited models [6,7] suggest that mindfulness involves: (1) attention regulation (improvement in stability, alerting, orienting, bias, meta-awareness/monitoring), (2) memory extinction and reconsolidation (memory habits shifting toward more adaptive trajectories), and (3) inhibition/switching [de-centering from ongoing cognitions, inhibitory control (from distraction), flexible engagement on relevant objects of attention and rapid disengagement from objects that are not (or no longer) relevant]. Measuring these cognitive mechanisms and determining that any changes associated with an 8-week intervention can be isolated from non-specific factors have proven challenging.

Chiesa et al. (2011) identified 23 cross-sectional and intervention studies that covered different forms of meditation, ranging from mantra and visualization to Buddhist practitioners of Zen, Shamatha-Vipassana and secularized forms such as Acceptance & Commitment Therapy (ACT), Mindfulness-based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) and Mindfulness-based Stress Reduction (MBSR). Programs also varied in duration and intensity from a brief 20 min induction (Wenk- Sormaz, 2005) to 3-month intensive meditation retreats requiring 10–12 h of daily practice. This review provides a mix of evidence that mindfulness-based practices improve some aspects of cognition [attention, memory, executive functioning, and miscellaneous]. Out of the 15 prospective controlled studies, the mindfulness-trained groups were found to out-perform a control group in only six of the 17 attention-related outcomes. Many of the non-significant studies were limited by very short duration of training [8,9] or had a clinical population that may be capacity- limited (traumatic brain injury) [10]. Out of the eight cross-sectional studies [1118], expert meditators (in comparison to controls) were found to have greater attentional capacity in 11 of the 12 assays. In two cases, there was a linear relationship between formal meditation experience and attention-related outcomes [11,18]. Groups of individuals trained in mindfulness also out- performed a control group in six of eight assays of memory (e.g., working memory, memory specificity, retrieval) [1924], in all three assays of executive functioning [20,24,25], and in 3 further assays of miscellaneous cognition [22,26,27].

Lao et. al (2016) focused on 18 controlled trials of MBCT/MBSR - two of the most extensively employed and evaluated MBIs, published between January 2000 and February 2015. This review found no significant evidence for improvement in attention and executive functions. Some evidence was apparent for improvements in working memory, autobiographical memory specificity, cognitive flexibility and meta-awareness. A total of 15/30 assays were found to reach a threshold Cohen’s d of 0.8 (generally considered a large effect), only 1 of which was related to attention outcomes. The small number of studies and various methodological challenges prevented firm conclusions from being drawn. The authors conclude, “Results call into question the theoretical underpinnings of mindfulness, further highlighting the need for a comprehensive theoretical framework”.

Our recent systematic review currently in preparation was intended to build upon what has already been done and get a more nuanced view of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of MBIs that included behavioral measures of cognition, either in patient populations or the general public. The review initially aimed to include only those studies that conformed to the standard 8-week MBI format, also referred to as “first-generation” MBIs [28] found in MBSR [29] and MBCT [30]. Using Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines, 10,802 potentially relevant studies were screened. Yet, in the process of screening for eligibility criteria with the requisite MBI structure and content and distilling the constructs that were relevant for cognitive outcomes, a total of only eight MBI RCT trials (MBSR n = 6, MBCT n = 2) were identified. Several highly regarded and methodologically sound studies that did not conform to the standard MBI curricula were not included in the review despite the fact that they demonstrated significant impacts of mindfulness on cognition. Given the authors’ expertise in this area, modified inclusion criteria were created by consensus to ensure inclusion of 20 additional studies that used modified MBI formats and investigated cognitive outcomes. Detailed results from this review will be presented elsewhere. In 28 studies from our review, MBIs out-performed the control intervention in 19% of all the cognitive assays.

Although there appears to be some evidence cognition may indeed be impacted by mindfulness, definitive evidence appears to be lacking (see also [31]). One must really understand the nuanced context-specific view and limitations for making broad claims about effects of mindfulness on cognition. The following sections explicate methodological issues and potential confounds we observed in our review that are necessary for consideration before making any claims.

Criteria that Define a Mindfulness-based Intervention

Although there are specific pedagogical differences, the standard MBSR and MBCT protocols contain/include: (1) 26 hours of in-class training through 8 weekly group classes (1.5– 2.5 hours/class) plus one 6 hr. class during the 6th week; (2) Home practice (45 min/day 6 days/wk or 36 hours over 8 wks); (3) Dyad/triad/whole group reflection/sharing about practice experiences inside and outside of class; (4) Four core meditation practices in which a state of mindfulness is maintained [Focused Attention (mindful body scan, mindfulness of breath); Open Monitoring/open awareness (mindfulness of present moment experience); and mindful Hatha yoga] Loving-kindness meditation is sometimes added (see [28]). The standard 8-week model waschosen for the review given its extensive use in clinical centers, thereby facilitating comparison across studies. A large number of initially excluded studies in our review used modified MBI curricula tailored for the general population or specific cohorts (e.g., military, classrooms) in order to test for cognitive changes in healthy individuals not seeking stress reduction. Many of these programs were less than 8 weeks, and most did not use a day-long retreat. There is a common assumption that meditation practice is the core active ingredient of MBIs; therefore, we decided to also include any and all models of curricula delivery that utilized at least two of the core mindfulness-based meditation practices, regardless of their length or composition.

While some studies of MBIs have reported that cognitive outcomes are dependent upon dosage (i.e., practice time) [13,16,21,3234], others have failed to find an association [15,3537]. As reported above, cross-sectional studies of long-term meditators with variable amounts of practice time have demonstrated mixed results in cognitive differences from control subjects [13,15,17,19,3841], suggesting that a linear relationship between dosage and cognitive improvements is unclear. Interestingly, some studies have demonstrated short (e.g., 1 week) mindfulness interventions produce significant cognitive changes [8,20,25,4244]. For example, Zeidan et al., 2010 demonstrated that 4 × 20 minute mindfulness training sessions (in comparison to an active control condition) promoted significant changes on cognitive tasks that require sustained attention and executive processing efficiency. In contrast, other studies evaluating cognition before and after a single mindfulness state induction (12–15 min) demonstrate mixed results regarding improvement in cognition [45,46], suggesting state inductions may not be sufficient. Therefore, there is no reason to believe/stipulate that the standard 8-week model is adequate or necessary for mindfulness training to achieve effective impact across all individuals. Furthermore, these results also suggest that training-specific effects on different aspects of cognition may exist.

In both contemporary mindfulness and Buddhist pedagogy, repeated meditation practice is thought to increase stable state and trait mindfulness over time [47,48]. Furthermore, much of the scientific literature suggests trait mindfulness plays a mechanistic role in relation to clinical and cognitive outcomes [4,6,7,49]. Although the scientific literature has reported significant correlations between cognitive or clinical outcomes and self-reported trait mindfulness [50,51], the scientific literature for MBIs has not adequately examined the causal relationship between practice time, state and trait mindfulness, and cognitive (or clinical) outcomes. Thus, the act of sitting formally in meditation may not be sufficient for shifting outcomes. Quality, depth of practice, or other non-specific effects encountered in the MBI should also be considered. Other contextual elements of an MBI may also be critical for cognitive outcomes, including reduction of stress or other clinical symptoms as well as enhancement of emotional wellbeing or emotion regulation. Furthermore, there is a strong body of literature demonstrating the power of group process (e.g., interpersonal learning, group cohesiveness) in addressing psychopathology [52], which could also theoretically influence cognitive outcomes. Other elements include the quality of the teacher and his/her ability to embody the teachings (see Crane and Kuyken, this Issue), the therapeutic alliance between teacher and participant, expectations, and reward systems. There is also a paucity of evidence regarding the impact of “informal practice” during normal daily activities (e.g., while washing dishes or when faced with a real-life stressor). Future studies investigating the relationship between formal and informal practice may use techniques such as experience sampling to better inform the field on the factors influencing dosage.

There is growing evidence that different types of meditation engage different brain areas [5355] and differentially influence psychological and cognitive outcomes [5658] (also see Singer and Engbert, this issue). For example, Marzetti et al. (2014) demonstrated unique alpha band connectivity pattern interactions between default and frontoparietal brain networks during focused attention (FA) vs. open monitoring (OM) meditation with implications for differential patterns of executive and monitoring functions [57]. Britton et al., 2018 found that an 8-week MBI focused on training FA only engaged mechanistic targets related to attentional control, while a structurally- equivalent MBI focused on OM training only engaged targets related to emotional non-reactivity. Because MBIs contain multiple training techniques and have been demonstrated to influence a wide variety of cognitive functions, it is not feasible to determine the individual influence of each component on any particular cognitive function. Future studies using a single training modality are needed to disambiguate the effects.

In summary, active ingredients of an MBI must be better isolated and evaluated in future studies along with contextual factors in order to determine the specific effects of mindfulness training on cognition. Future research should also consider that modifications to the first- generation 8-week MBI prescription that include shorter classes or more emphasis on movement (i.e., yoga) may be just as efficacious as standard MBIs and may be more relevant to non-clinical populations, especially if there are logistical or population-specific barriers to longer in-class time commitments. For example, Carmody and Baer (2009) observed that clinical outcomes were more strongly correlated with the amount of home yoga practice than with the amount of home sitting meditation practice.

Limitations of Assays used to Measure Cognition

The field of cognitive neuroscience is limited in its existing tools to measure specific cognitive processes and also other more complex or subtle phenomenological changes to one’s general cognition as a result of mindfulness training. An extensive literature on cognitive science has emphasized behavioral measures as preferred assays of cognition due to their objectivity, relative simplicity, and ease of administration, often requiring the participant to only make verbal responses or button presses. Behavioral tasks typically measure differences in reaction times (RTs) between tasks or groups of subjects in an attempt to infer which cognitive processes are occurring [59]. Although there is an extensive literature on methodological work designed to parse apart cognitive processes underlying RT as well as relatively more recent work on computational models of RT tasks (e.g., decision-making tasks), and work on diffusion and accumulator models of RT data, there remains a great deal of variability in rigor of various “cognitive tasks”. For example, two individuals demonstrating similar RTs may be using vastly different strategies to perform the task. Many RT experiments provide little means of determining the processing that occurs between stimulus and response [60], and therefore are not necessarily sufficient to elucidate the exact mechanisms by which the brain is processing information [61].

Complementary use of multi-modal neuroimaging techniques, such as event-related potentials (ERPs) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), provide more information about specific neural mechanisms that underlie each cognitive process, can be measured in the absence of overt responses, and provide a means for cross-validation [60,62]. Some behavioral measures have also displayed questionable psychometric properties [63]. In our systematic review, one of the most frequently used tasks was the Stroop test which assesses cognitive interference and inhibition [64]. Studies have demonstrated a lack of convergent validity between different Stroop formats [65], low test-retest reliability [65], and poor internal reliability [66]. The sensitivity and specificity of this measure in identifying deficits in disorders has also been questionable under certain circumstances [67]. Additionally, cognitive tasks including the Stroop may be confounded by intelligence and educational levels [67]. No study in our review reported significant effects on the Stroop task; however, given the poor psychometric properties of the test, the lack of significant effects could be due either to inefficacious training or to the test itself. Most cognitive tests have multiple variations (e.g., variable/fixed ITIs, multiple outcomes, length of task, variable stimuli) with widely varying psychometrics. Therefore, variability in task design and associated findings must be interpreted with respect to the granularity afforded by such methodological attributes of these tasks.

Another issue of concern is the wide variety of cognitive tasks used across studies. Whereas psychopathology has a handful of gold standard measures (e.g., Hamilton and Beck Depression scales), cognitive neuroscience has multiple tests for each theoretical construct. Furthermore, each cognitive test often has multiple variants, and the data from any particular test can be analyzed in a variety of ways which can generate different results. This problem facing the field of cognitive neuroscience prompted NIH to develop a Toolbox of tasks to aid with standardization. Few, if any, studies in our review utilized this toolbox. We suggest inclusion of these tasks in the future to aid reproducibility and facilitate the comparison of results between mindfulness studies as well as in the wider field of cognitive neuroscience.

Some of the existing literature examining effects of mindfulness on cognition suggests that mindfulness training does not exert its largest effects on tasks assessing speed and accuracy [2,68,69]. Rather, mindfulness instructions typically include taking time to observe present- moment experience in a non-judging and decentered way, and therefore may have greater effects on meta-cognitive abilities. Repeated practice of mindfulness skills is likely to influence other qualities of attention, such as interoception [the ability to observe and report upon internal experiences (cognitions, sensations, emotions)], and would function well as a primary outcome in RCTs [30,70,71]. Objective measures of cognition (e.g., EEG, fMRI) have the potential to cross- validate behavioral changes in attentional allocation, efficiency, inhibition, and bias. Further work may be necessary to employ objective measures and identify better metrics for cognitive mechanism engagement and change as a result of practice.

Methodological Quality of an MBI Trial and Reporting of Findings

In our review, several studies were initially excluded because they did not conform to the standard clinical trial methodology. For example, some of the studies did not use a standard randomization strategy. Studies with students in classrooms or military platoons required participants to be randomized by cohort rather than individually. Although this is an acceptable randomization method, it introduces additional confounds which need to be considered when interpreting the data. Other methodological design issues that influenced interpretation of the data included: (1) Clinical studies sometimes include cognitive tasks as secondary outcome measures and were not adequately powered to detect group differences; and (2) Control groups do not match the non-specific contextual elements of the MBI (e.g., group effects, expectation effects, teacher competency, demographics, and education level).

Most MBI studies of cognition to date have been small (n=12–30 subjects/arm) and are likely underpowered to detect real differences in cognitive outcomes unless there is a pre-existing cognitive deficit. When studying cognitively normal populations, there are likely ceiling effects for any significant change in these processes, especially in comparison to an active control intervention that may also confer benefits. Indeed, many studies found no significant between- group differences. Some observed within-group improvements, so it is possible that larger sample sizes may reveal between-group effects. Furthermore, it may take more than 8 weeks for certain cognitive processes to significantly develop, especially in a healthy population. Follow-up studies of cognition are rare. Therefore, future studies should examine how continued practice may contribute to sustained benefits or further improvements longitudinally. Care should be taken when digesting reviews or meta-analyses that do not distinguish between trials focusing on healthy individuals and those focusing on clinical patients.

Similarly, the context of the subjects’ condition or environment needs to be interpreted carefully. For instance, several studies reporting group differences have found that the performance of the controls declined over time, while the scores of the MBI participants remained stable (e.g., [21]). These results are interpreted as the protective effect of MBIs against stress- related cognitive lapses. More studies with non-stressed individuals are needed in order to disambiguate the stress reduction effects of MBIs from actual enhancement of cognition. How and where people practice and learn mindfulness (e.g., consecrated spaces, monastic settings, secular classrooms) may very likely influence outcomes. Yet, such contextual elements have yet to be explored. To further methodological rigor, clear systems are required for assessing and reporting the integrity/fidelity of the intervention (Crane, this issue). It needs to be clear what the subjects are practicing (including informal practice demands), and there must be standard qualifications of the teacher.

How one advertises for a study may influence expectation or placebo effects. For example, one provocative study demonstrated that subjects recruited for a “Brain training and cognitive enhancement study” outperformed individuals recruited to “Participate in a Study” for course credit, despite the fact that all subjects underwent the same training and testing protocols [72]. One possibility is a selection bias in which different types of people might respond to specific “languaging” used in recruitment materials, an observation that we have observed in our own studies. An alternative interpretation is that expectation/placebo effects impact performance. Therefore, recruiting strategies should be reported and considered when comparing MBI studies of cognition.

Conclusions

Challenges remain for properly assessing the effects of mindfulness on cognition. Cross- sectional studies of advanced meditators have clearly demonstrated some cognitive advantages in comparison to matched healthy control subjects. Yet, without longitudinal studies, it remains unclear whether these observed effects are purely correlational and not causal. The data suggest that cognitive outcomes may vary depending on type of meditation practice emphasized relative to the cognitive outcome being measured (see [56]). Because many of the findings in MBI studies are equivocal at best, improved assays and methodological rigor are encouraged to improve cross- validation and clearly estimate the performance edge that mindfulness training offers (if any) in comparison to interventions that include all of the non-specific contextual elements, but not the active ingredient – mindful awareness. We encourage investigators to state a priori hypotheses how cognition may be enhanced, returned to baseline, or preserved from degradation so these studies can be more fairly compared. MBI methodology, including variations in content, context (contemporary MBIs vs. Dharma teachings), delivery format (e.g., direct teacher transmission or mobile apps), or dosage (in formal and informal practice), need to be considered along with the first-generation 8-week model and with respect to the limitations of the population (e.g., interest/motivation, accessibility, baseline cognitive ability). A focus on isolating the active ingredients may reveal the need to measure changes outside the classroom or off the cushion. The challenge is to better isolate the specific mechanisms supporting mindful awareness over non- specific psycho-education support groups, wait-list, and treatment-as-usual groups. Indeed, some of the non-specific effects (e.g., placebo, group effects) may be more specific to the therapeutic model of an MBI and could be incorporated into the model instead of controlled and disregarded as non-specific. Observable cognitive changes may be also be limited by the assays available to measure such change. Novel tasks or variations of cognitive measures need to be constructed (see Hadash and Bernstein, this issue; Van Vugt and Sivakumar, this issue) that target mechanisms specific to mindfulness practice. Finally, replications of data are needed in high-powered studies using appropriately sensitive measures. Given all these recommendations, we feel that measurement of cognition in mindfulness research will likely benefit greatly.

Highlights.

  • The effects of MBIs on cognition remain variable and depend on context

  • Methodological issues in MBI research must be considered for proper interpretation

  • Informed readers should consider variability in criteria that define an MBI

  • Informed readers should consider limitations of assays used to measure cognition

  • Informed readers should consider variability in methodological quality of MBI trials

Acknowledgments.

The authors would like to acknowledge the Mindfulness Research Collective (MRC), a group of multi-disciplinary scientists and scholars from multiple institutions across the US with the common goal to advance the scientific investigation of mindfulness and contemplative science. The MRC has contributed to systematic reviews of MBIs referenced herein. We specifically acknowledge the following individuals for their contribution to the systematic review on cognition: Jodie Scharf, Pamela Acero, Willoughby Britton, Ethan Moitra, Kristina Eichel Sarah Peters, Hannah Kimmel, Hadley Rahrig, Travis Dumais, and Jonah Lipsky. This research and related reviews of the MBI literature was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Science of Behavior Change Common Fund Program through an award administered by the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (grant UH2AT009145). The views presented here are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH. No other specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors was involved in writing this paper.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References.

  • **1.Chiesa A, Calati R, Serretti A: Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clin Psychol Rev 2011, 31:449–464.First systematic review of mindfulness literature focused on cognitive outcomes across prospective, case-controlled, and cross-sectional studies. Twenty-three studies were reviewed with some positive findings; however, caution in interpretation is emphasized given a number of methodological issues.
  • **2.Lao SA, Kissane D, Meadows G: Cognitive effects of MBSR/MBCT: A systematic review of neuropsychological outcomes. Conscious Cogn 2016, 45:109–123.Comprehensive systematic review of cognitive outcomes in MBSR/MBCT trials. Only 18 studies met inclusion criteria with little evidence that MBIs improve cognition.
  • 3.Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, Segal ZV, Abbey S, Speca M, Velting D, et al. : Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology-Science and Practice 2004, 11:230–241. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Brown KW, Ryan RM: The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2003, 84:822–848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *5.Shapiro SL, Carlson LE, Astin JA, Freedman B: Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology 2006, 62:373–386.One of three essential papers describing mechanisms of mindfulness. They propose three components (axioms) of mindfulness: (1) intention, (2) attention, and (3) attitude (IAA) and a meta-mechanism of action, “reperceiving”.
  • 6.Holzel B, Lazar S, Gard T, Schuman-Olivier Z, Vago D, Ott U: How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science 2011, 6:537–559.One of three essential papers describing mechanisms of mindfulness. The authors propose four mechanisms: Attention regulation, 2. Body awareness, 3.Emotion regulation including reappraisal and exposure, extinction, and reconsolidation), and 4. Change in perspective on the self.
  • 7.Vago DR, Silbersweig DA: Self-Awareness, Self-Regulation, and Self-Transcendence (S- ART): A Framework for Understanding the Neurobiological Mechanisms of Mindfulness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2012, 6:1–30.One of three essential papers describing mechanisms of mindfulness. The authors propose a framework by which mindfulness functions through systematic mental training that develops meta-awareness (self-awareness), an ability to effectively modulate one’s behavior (self-regulation), and a positive relationship between self and other that transcends self- focused needs and increases prosocial characteristics (self-transcendence).
  • 8.Polak E: Impact of Two Sessions of Mindfulness Training on Attention. In Psychology Edited by. Open Access Dissertations: University of Miami; 2009. vol Ph.D.] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Tang YY, Ma Y, Wang J, Fan Y, Feng S, Lu Q, Yu Q, Sui D, Rothbart MK, Fan M, et al. : Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007, 104:17152–17156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.McMillan T, Robertson IH, Brock D, Chorlton L: Brief mindfulness training for attentional problems after traumatic brain injury: A randomised control treatment trial. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 2002, 12:117–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Chan D, Woollacott M: Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: is the efficiency of executive or orientation networks improved? J Altern Complement Med 2007, 13:651–657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hodgins HS, Adair KC: Attentional processes and meditation. Consciousness and Cognition 2010, 19:872–878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Moore A, Malinowski P: Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Conscious Cogn 2009, 18:176–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.van den Hurk PA, Giommi F, Gielen SC, Speckens AE, Barendregt HP: Greater efficiency in attentional processing related to mindfulness meditation. Q J Exp Psychol (Colchester) 2010, 63:1168–1180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Josefsson T, Broberg A: Meditators and non-meditators on sustained and executive attentional performance. Mental Health, Religion & Culture 2011, 14:291–309. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pagnoni G, Cekic M: Age effects on gray matter volume and attentional performance in Zen meditation. Neurobiol Aging 2007, 28:1623–1627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.van Leeuwen S, Muller NG, Melloni L: Age effects on attentional blink performance in meditation. Conscious Cogn 2009, 18:593–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Valentine ER, Sweet PLG: Meditation and attention: a comparison of the effects of concentrative and mindfulness meditation on sustained attention. Mental Health, Religion & Culture 1999, 2:59–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Chambers R, Lo BCY, Allen NB: The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research 2008, 32:303–322. [Google Scholar]
  • *20.Zeidan F, Johnson SK, Diamond BJ, David Z, Goolkasian P: Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: evidence of brief mental training. Conscious Cogn 2010, 19:597– 605.One of the few studies to show short-term training (4 × 20 min sessions) can have a significant impact on cognitive functioning.
  • 21.Jha AP, Stanley EA, Kiyonaga A, Wong L, Gelfand L: Examining the protective effects of mindfulness training on working memory capacity and affective experience. Emotion 2010, 10:54–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hargus E, Crane C, Barnhofer T, Williams JM: Effects of mindfulness on meta-awareness and specificity of describing prodromal symptoms in suicidal depression. Emotion 2010, 10:34–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Williams JMG, Teasdale JD, Segal ZV, Soulsby J: Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy reduces overgeneral autobiographical memory in formerly depressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2000, 109:150–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Heeren A, Van Broeck N, Philippot P: The effects of mindfulness on executive processes and autobiographical memory specificity. Behav Res Ther 2009, 47:403–409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wenk-Sormaz H: Meditation can reduce habitual responding. Adv Mind Body Med 2005,21:33–49. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ortner CNM, Kilner SJ, Zelazo PD: Mindfulness meditation and reduced emotional interference on a cognitive task. Motivation and Emotion 2007, 31:271–283. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Tang Y-Y, Ma Y, Wang J, Fan Y, Feng S, Lu Q, Yu Q, Sui D, Rothbart MK, Fan M, et al. : Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2007, 104:17152– 17156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Crane RS, Brewer J, Feldman C, Kabat-Zinn J, Santorelli S, Williams JMG, Kuyken W: What defines mindfulness-based programs? The warp and the weft. Psychological Medicine 2017, 47:990–999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kabat-Zinn J: An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1982, 4:33–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Segal ZV, Williams JMG, Teasdale JD: Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: A new approach to preventing relapse New York: Guilford; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Dunning DL, Griffiths K, Kuyken W, Crane C, Foulkes L, Parker J, Dalgleish T: Research Review: The effects of mindfulness-based interventions on cognition and mental health in children and adolescents - a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 32.Allen M, Dietz M, Blair KS, van Beek M, Rees G, Vestergaard-Poulsen P, Lutz A, Roepstorff A: Cognitive-affective neural plasticity following active-controlled mindfulness intervention. J Neurosci 2012, 32:15601–15610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • **33.Jha AP, Krompinger J, Baime MJ: Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2007, 7:109–119.One of the first papers to explore cognitive outcomes in response to an 8-week MBI, suggesting the endogenous orienting of attention is selectively improved in a healthy population as a result of MBSR.
  • 34.Chan D, Woollacott M: Effects of Level of Meditation Experience on Attentional Focus: Is the Efficiency of Executive or Orientation Networks Improved? The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine 2007, 13:651–658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Jensen CG, Vangkilde S, Frokjaer V, Hasselbalch SG: Mindfulness training affects attention--or is it attentional effort? J Exp Psychol Gen 2012, 141:106–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *36.MacCoon DG, MacLean KA, Davidson RJ, Saron CD, Lutz A: No sustained attention differences in a longitudinal randomized trial comparing mindfulness based stress reduction versus active control. PLoS ONE 2014, 9:e97551.A well-matched active control RCT investigating effects of MBSR on sustained attention and discrimination. Although data are difficult to interpret, they report a significant interaction between time and vigilance. They also report no relation between improvements and class practice.
  • 37.Anderson ND, Lau MA, Segal ZV, Bishop SR: Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 2007, 14:449–463. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hodgins HS, Adair KC: Attentional processes and meditation. Conscious Cogn 2010,19:872–878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Slagter HA, Lutz A, Greischar LL, Francis AD, Nieuwenhuis S, Davis JM, Davidson RJ:Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biol 2007, 5:e138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cusens B, Duggan GB, Thorne K, Burch V: Evaluation of the breathworks mindfulness- based pain management programme: effects on well-being and multiple measures of mindfulness. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 2010, 17:63–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lykins ELB, Baer RA, Gottlob LR: Performance-based tests of attention and memory in long-term mindfulness meditators and demographically matched nonmeditators. Cognitive Therapy and Research 2012, 36:103–114. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Pratzlich M, Kossowsky J, Gaab J, Krummenacher P: Impact of short-term meditation and expectation on executive brain functions. Behav Brain Res 2016, 297:268–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Basso JC, McHale A, Ende V, Oberlin DJ, Suzuki WA: Brief, daily meditation enhances attention, memory, mood, and emotional regulation in non-experienced meditators. Behav Brain Res 2019, 356:208–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Andreu CI, Cosmelli D, Slagter HA, Franken IHA: Effects of a brief mindfulness-meditation intervention on neural measures of response inhibition in cigarette smokers. PLoS One 2018, 13:e0191661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Alberts HJ, Thewissen R: The Effect of a Brief Mindfulness Intervention on Memory for Positively and Negatively Valenced Stimuli. Mindfulness (N Y) 2011, 2:73–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Colzato LS, Sellaro R, Samara I, Hommel B: Meditation-induced cognitive-control states regulate response-conflict adaptation: Evidence from trial-to-trial adjustments in the Simon task. Conscious Cogn 2015, 35:110–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kiken LG, Garland EL, Bluth K, Palsson OS, Gaylord SA: From a state to a trait: Trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict changes in trait mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences 2015, 81:41–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bodhi B: What does mindfulness really mean? A canonical perspective. Contemporary Buddhism: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2011, 12:19 – 39. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Garland EL, Froeliger B, Howard MO: Mindfulness training targets neurocognitive mechanisms of addiction at the attention-appraisal-emotion interface. Front Psychiatry 2014, 4:173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Keng S-L, Smoski MJ, Robins CJ: Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review 2011, 31:1041–1056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • **51.Gu J, Strauss C, Bond R, Cavanagh K: How do Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy and Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction Improve Mental Health and Wellbeing? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Mediation Studies (vol 37, pg 1, 2015). Clinical Psychology Review 2016, 49:119–119.Comprehensive systematic review on MBI mechanisms using 2-stage meta-analytic structural equation modeling to examine mediation by mechanisms on clinical outcomes. Review found evidence for rumination, mindfulness and worry as significant mediators of the effects of MBIs on health outcomes.
  • 52.Yalom ID, Leszcz M: The Theory and Practice of Group Psychotherapy New York, NY: Basic Books; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Fox KC, Dixon ML, Nijeboer S, Girn M, Floman JL, Lifshitz M, Ellamil M, Sedlmeier P, Christoff K: Functional neuroanatomy of meditation: A review and meta-analysis of 78 functional neuroimaging investigations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016, 65:208–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Fox KC, Nijeboer S, Dixon ML, Floman JL, Ellamil M, Rumak SP, Sedlmeier P, Christoff K: Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta- analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014, 43:48–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *55.Fucci E, Abdoun O, Caclin A, Francis A, Dunne JD, Ricard M, Davidson RJ, Lutz A: Differential effects of non-dual and focused attention meditations on the formation of automatic perceptual habits in expert practitioners 2018.Study proposes that the state of open presence in experts is characterized by increased sensory monitoring and reduced perceptual inferences compared to focused attention. This study represents a novel attempt to describe the impact of non-dual meditation states on the regulation of automatic brain predictive processes.
  • *56.Britton WB, Davis JH, Loucks EB, Peterson B, Cullen BH, Reuter L, Rando A, Rahrig H, Lipsky J, Lindahl JR: Dismantling Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy: Creation and validation of 8-week focused attention and open monitoring interventions within a 3-armed randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy 2018, 101:92– 107.One of the first studies to show differential effects on cognition associated with specific meditation practices.
  • 57.Marzetti L, Di Lanzo C, Zappasodi F, Chella F, Raffone A, Pizzella V: Magnetoencephalographic alpha band connectivity reveals differential default mode network interactions during focused attention and open monitoring meditation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2014, 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Arch JJ, Craske MG: Mechanisms of mindfulness: Emotion regulation following a focused breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy 2006, 44:1849–1858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Jensen AR: Clocking the Mind: Mental Chronometry and Individual Differences Amtersdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Luck SJ, Woodman GF, Vogel EK: Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends Cogn Sci 2000, 4:432–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Posner MI: Timing the brain: mental chronometry as a tool in neuroscience. PLoS Biol 2005, 3:e51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Schoenberg PL, Hepark S, Kan CC, Barendregt HP, Buitelaar JK, Speckens AE: Effects of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on neurophysiological correlates of performance monitoring in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Neurophysiol 2014, 125:1407–1416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Rodebaugh TL, Scullin RB, Langer JK, Dixon DJ, Huppert JD, Bernstein A, Zvielli A, Lenze EJ: Unreliability as a threat to understanding psychopathology: The cautionary tale of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2016, 125:840–851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Stroop JR: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 1935, 18:643–662. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kindt M, Bierman D, Brosschot JF: Stroop versus Stroop: Comparison of a card format and a single-trial format of the standard color-word Stroop task and the emotional Stroop task. Personality and individual differences 1996, 21:653,661. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Ataya AF, Adams S, Mullings E, Cooper RM, Attwood AS, Munafo MR: Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias. Drug Alcohol Depend 2012, 121:148– 151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Homack S, Riccio CA: A meta-analysis of the sensitivity and specificity of the Stroop Color and Word Test with children. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2004, 19:725–743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Gallant SN: Mindfulness meditation practice and executive functioning: Breaking down the benefit. Conscious Cogn 2016, 40:116–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Fabio RA, Towey GE: Long-term meditation: the relationship between cognitive processes, thinking styles and mindfulness. Cognitive Processing 2018, 19:73–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Fresco DM, Moore MT, van Dulmen MH, Segal ZV, Ma SH, Teasdale JD, Williams JM: Initial psychometric properties of the experiences questionnaire: validation of a self- report measure of decentering. Behavior Therapy 2007, 38:234–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Farb NA, Segal ZV, Anderson AK: Mindfulness meditation training alters cortical representations of interoceptive attention. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • *72.Foroughi CK, Monfort SS, Paczynski M, McKnight PE, Greenwood PM: Placebo effects in cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Individuals who self-selected into the placebo group by responding to a suggestive flyer showed improvements after a single, 1-h session of cognitive training that equates to a 5- to 10-point increase on a standard IQ test. Controls responding to a nonsuggestive flyer showed no improvement.

RESOURCES