Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Jun 27.
Published before final editing as: Aging Ment Health. 2018 Dec 27:1–10. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2018.1544214

Late Life Depressive Symptoms and Cognitive Function among Older Mexican Adults: The Past and the Present

Joseph L Saenz 1, Marc A Garcia 2, Brian Downer 3
PMCID: PMC6597334  NIHMSID: NIHMS1008852  PMID: 30588839

Abstract

Objective:

To evaluate associations between depression and individual cognitive domains and how changes in depressive symptoms relate to cognition three years later in the context of Mexico, a developing country experiencing rapid aging.

Method:

Data comes from waves 3 (2012) and 4 (2015) of the Mexican Health and Aging Study (n=12,898, age 50+). Depression is ascertained using a modified Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression Scale. Cognition is assessed using verbal learning, verbal memory, visual scanning, verbal fluency, visuospatial ability, visual memory, and orientation tasks. Depressive symptoms and cognitive functioning were both measured in 2012 and 2015. Scores across cognitive domains are modeled using ordinary least squares regression, adjusting for demographic, health, and economic covariates.

Results:

When depression and cognition were measured concurrently in 2015, depression exhibited associations with all cognitive domains. When considering a respondent’s history of depression, individuals who had elevated depressive symptoms in 2012 and recovered by 2015 continued to exhibit poorer cognitive function in 2015 in verbal learning, verbal memory, visual scanning, and verbal fluency tasks compared to individuals who were neither depressed in 2012 nor 2015.

Conclusions:

Depression was associated with cognition across cognitive domains among older Mexican adults. Despite improvements in depressive symptomatology, formerly depressed respondents continued to perform worse than their counterparts without a history of depression on several cognitive tasks. In addition to current mental health status, researchers should consider an individual’s history of depression when assessing the cognitive functioning of older adults.

Background:

Rapid population aging in Mexico has made the mental health, including late-life depression and cognitive functioning, of older adults an important public health and health policy priority. An estimated one out of every eight older adults in Mexico have major depressive symptoms (García-Peña et al., 2008), and the prevalence of cognitive impairment with no dementia (CIND) and dementia among older Mexican adults are estimated to be 28.7% and 6.1% respectively (Mejia-Arango & Gutierrez, 2011). While these are unique clinical entities, depression and cognitive impairment often co-occur (Afridi, Hina, Qureshi, & Hussain, 2011; Butters et al., 2004; Korczyn & Halperin, 2009; Potter & Steffens, 2007). This has been observed in several cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses that have noted significant negative correlations between depression and cognition (Diniz, Butters, Albert, Dew, & Reynolds, 2013; González, Bowen, & Fisher, 2008; Goveas et al., 2014; Mourao, Mansur, Malloy-Diniz, Costa, & Diniz, 2016; Richard et al., 2013). In Mexico, the prevalence of cognitive impairment among older adults is estimated to be approximately 38% higher in depressed individuals compared to the non-depressed (García-Peña et al., 2008). To this end, some have suggested late-life depression can be a preventable risk factor for mild cognitive impairment and dementia (Byers & Yaffe, 2011; Diniz et al., 2013; Mourao et al., 2016; Steenland et al., 2012).

Due to their common co-occurrence, previous work has suggested multiple mechanisms that may plausibly connect late-life depression to cognitive ability. For example, various brain regions may be negatively affected by depression which may lead to diminished cognitive ability. The hippocampus may play a role in diminished cognitive function, particularly in memory tasks, among depressed adults (Potter & Steffens, 2007). In addition, depression is associated with activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-axis (HPA) (Nemeroff & Vale, 2005; Sapolsky, 2001) and increases in glucocorticoid hydrocortisone which may contribute to damage in the hippocampus and reduced hippocampal volumes (Sapolsky, 1996). Although depression and hippocampal atrophy do not always co-occur, depression has been found to be associated with decreased hippocampal volume in several studies (Campbell, Marriott, Nahmias, & MacQueen, 2004; Sawyer, Corsentino, Sachs-Ericsson, & Steffens, 2012; Steffens, McQuoid, Payne, & Potter, 2011; Videbech & Ravnkilde, 2004). Further, prolonged activation of the HPA has also been found to be associated with release and deposition of amyloid-beta in animal models (Dong & Csernansky, 2009). Depression is also associated with reduction in the volume of other brain regions important to cognitive functioning, including the prefrontal cortex (Bremner et al., 2002; Drevets et al., 1997) and greater white matter atrophy in the temporal and parietal lobes (Lee et al., 2012).

In addition to these intermediary mechanisms, researchers have hypothesized that depression is the result of early signs of cognitive deterioration as individuals notice cognitive changes (Jorm, 2001) or that both changes in cognition and depression may be driven by shared risk factors such as cerebrovascular diseases (Richard et al., 2013). Other investigators have argued that depression represents a prodromal feature of dementia, thus explaining their co-occurrence (Brommelhoff et al., 2009; Yaffe et al., 1999). Recent findings from the Whitehall II revealed that although older adults with and without dementia experienced similar depressive symptoms in middle age, older adults with dementia exhibited increased depressive symptomology 11-years prior to a dementia diagnosis (Singh-Manoux et al., 2017). These findings provide evidence that increasing depressive symptoms are part of the dementia prodrome (Singh-Manoux et al., 2017; Steffens, 2017).

While prior studies have evaluated the association between depression and cognition in order to identify mechanisms through which they may be related, less research has examined whether the cognitive profiles of individuals with a history of depression differ from individuals without a history of depression. Previous studies have emphasized the importance of considering depression history when modeling cognitive function and decline (Jorm, 2001; Potter & Steffens, 2007) as cognitive deficits may persist after depressed individuals have recovered from past episodes of depression (Bhalla et al., 2006; Neu et al., 2005; Reppermund, Ising, Lucae, & Zihl, 2009; Richard et al., 2013). Persistence of cognitive deficits among the formerly depressed has also been found in meta-analyses (Hasselbalch, Knorr, & Kessing, 2011; Rock, Roiser, Riedel, & Blackwell, 2014). Further, decreased volume of the hippocampus has been observed in formally depressed patients who have been depression-free for months to decades (Sheline, Wang, Gado, Csernansky, & Vannier, 1996). Moreover, persistently elevated depressive symptoms have been associated with poorer executive function and an accelerated decline in memory and attention (Dotson, Resnick, & Zonderman, 2008), greater declines in memory, verbal knowledge, and verbal fluency (Goveas et al., 2014), and incident dementia (Richard et al., 2013). Overall, these studies suggest that depression may lead to an increased risk of cognitive decline independent of other risk factors including vascular disease (Barnes, Alexopoulos, Lopez, Williamson, & Yaffe, 2006; Goveas et al., 2014). Taken together, these findings suggest a greater need to consider how current depression and history of depression may interact to impact cognitive function.

Most work on depression and cognition has focused on countries in later stages of economic development including European countries and the United States. For instance, a meta-analysis using data primarily from higher-income countries has found cross-sectional associations between depression and episodic memory, executive function, as well as processing speed but not semantic or visuospatial memory (McDermott & Ebmeier, 2009). Another meta-analysis found negative associations between depression and executive function, memory, and attention (Rock et al., 2014) when comparing the currently depressed to non-depressed controls. The importance of research outside of high-income countries is accented by the fact that depression is a global phenomenon, affecting both developed and developing contexts (Lépine & Briley, 2011). Indeed, depression is predicted to become the leading cause of loss of disability adjusted life years globally in the coming decades (World Health Organization, 2004) with major impacts in developing countries (Murray & Lopez, 1997).

Despite the expected global burden of depression, few studies have focused on depression in countries in earlier stages of economic development (Guerra et al., 2016; Patel, Araya, & Bolton, 2004). This is an important distinction as factors which influence both depression and cognition may vary significantly across countries and economic context. Overall levels of education are often lower in developing countries, and a meta-analysis found low education to be consistently associated with depression in developing countries (Lima & Soares, 2008). A preponderance of evidence also shows education to be closely associated with cognitive ability (Fors, Lennartsson, & Lundberg, 2009; Jefferson et al., 2011; Lee, Kawachi, Berkman, & Grodstein, 2003; Singh-Manoux, Richards, & Marmot, 2005). Findings from Mexico indicate education is associated with both lower depression (García-Peña et al., 2008; Slone et al., 2006) and better cognitive health (Al Hazzouri, Haan, Galea, & Aiello, 2011). Furthermore, mental health disorders in low- and middle-income countries are less likely to be detected (Lima & Soares, 2008) and treated (World Health Organization [WHO], 2018) which has been the case in Mexico (Guerra et al., 2009). Given the unique contexts of education and mental health in developing countries, more studies examining the association between late-life depression and cognitive ability are needed.

We build upon prior studies by evaluating individual cognitive domains to better elucidate the nuanced relationships between depression and cognition and estimate associations while accounting for confounding variables including demographic, health, and economic characteristics. Our results may guide the development of interventions aimed at ameliorating the effects of late-life depression on the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia, especially in the context of developing countries. Specifically, the aims of the current analysis are: 1) to estimate the association between depression and individual cognitive domains; and 2) to determine whether a respondent’s history of depression is associated with cognitive function in the context of Mexico, a developing country experiencing rapid aging. Regarding our first aim, we hypothesize that depression will be associated with poorer cognitive function across cognitive domains. For our second aim, we hypothesize that respondents with a history of depression will have diminished cognitive function relative to their non-depressed counterparts without a history of depression.

Methods:

Data:

Data came from the 2012 and 2015 waves of the Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS) (MHAS, 2015). The MHAS is a large, nationally representative, sample of older adults (age 50+) and their spouses/partners. The first wave of the MHAS was collected in 2001 and the sample was followed-up in 2003. In 2012, a refresher cohort was added that was born between 1952 and 1962 to maintain representation of the Mexican population aged 50 and over. The original and refresher cohorts were followed up in 2015. The 2012 and 2015 waves were then the baseline and follow-up points for our analysis, respectively, as our analyses were only based on those interviewed 2012 and later (including both the original and refresher cohorts). The MHAS collects data across a variety of domains including demographics, chronic conditions, depression, cognitive function, income, and wealth, among others. The MHAS is highly comparable to the United States Health and Retirement Study (HRS) and has been described in greater detail elsewhere (Wong, Michaels-Obregon, & Palloni, 2017). The MHAS is partly sponsored by the National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Aging (grant number NIH R01AG018016). Data files and documentation are public use and available at www.MHASweb.org. While the 2015 MHAS sample included 14,217 respondents age 50 and older, we omitted proxy interviews as they do not contain the cognitive measures we analyze (n=918), and also individuals missing information on: depression (n=132) all cognitive domains (n=72), and covariates included in the analysis (n=197) resulting in a sample size of 12,898 for our cross-sectional analyses.

Cognitive Function:

Cognitive function was measured using the Cross-Cultural Cognitive Examination (CCCE) (Glosser et al., 1993). The CCCE is useful among populations with limited literacy and mathematical ability (Wolfe et al., 1992). Several domains are captured in the CCCE including verbal learning (the respondent was read a list of eight words and asked to recall them, this was repeated two more times and the total number of words recalled across trials was summed, range 0–24), verbal recall (the eight word list was recalled after a delay, range 0–8), visual scanning (the respondent was asked to identify a visual stimulus in an array of visual stimuli, range 0–60), visuospatial ability (respondent was asked to draw a figure, range 0–6), and visual memory (respondent was asked to recall the figure they drew after a delay, range 0–6). We also analyzed verbal fluency (respondent identified as many animals as he/she was able in one minute, range 0–60), and orientation (respondent identified the day, month, and year, range 0–3). We standardized cognitive scores by calculating the respondent’s z-score using sample means and standard deviations across cognitive tasks. Standardization was done by task due to the large differences in domain ranges so regression parameter estimates represented differences in cognition in terms of standard deviations for all domains. Higher scores indicated better cognitive functioning across domains. Standardized cognitive scores were constructed for both the 2012 and 2015 waves of the MHAS. We used the 2015 cognitive scores as outcome variables in our analyses and used the 2012 cognitive scores to adjust for past differences in cognitive function.

Depressive Symptoms:

Depressive symptoms were assessed in the MHAS using a modified nine-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression (CES-D) scale (Radloff, 1977). Respondents were asked whether they experienced the following symptoms of depression in the previous week: 1) feeling depressed, 2) feeling that everything he/she did was an effort, 3) feeling that his/her sleep was restless, 4) feeling happy (reverse coded), 5) feeling lonely, 6) feeling that he/she enjoyed life (reverse coded), 7) feeling sad, 8) feeling tired, and 9) feeling that he/she had a lot of energy (reverse coded). These items were presented as binary outcomes (yes/no). Following previous research using the MHAS (Agudelo-Botero, Giraldo-Rodríguez, Murillo-González, Mino-León, & Cruz-Arenas, 2018), we classified participants who responded “yes” to five or more items as being depressed and participants who responded affirmatively to four or fewer items as non-depressed. The reliability of the modified CES-D instrument and the validity of this cut-point have been established in this population in prior work (Aguilar-Navarro, Fuentes-Cantú, Ávila-Funes, & García-Mayo, 2007). Respondents were classified as depressed or non-depressed in both the 2012 and 2015 waves of the MHAS. With these classifications, we constructed a four-level variable based on depression status in both 2012 and 2015. Categories included: stable non-depressed (non-depressed in 2012 or 2015), formerly depressed (depressed in 2012 but not in 2015), newly depressed (depressed in 2015 but not in 2012), or stable depressed (depressed in both 2012 and 2015).

Covariates:

We included several covariates in our analysis to capture the respondents’ demographic, economic, and health characteristics. We included the respondent’s age, sex, educational attainment, marital status, and locality size as demographic covariates. Educational attainment was measured as years of formal education. Marital status was categorized as married/partnered, widowed, or other (divorced, separated, or never married). Locality size was categorized as 100,000+ residents, 15,000–99,999 residents, 2,500–14,999 residents, or fewer than 2,500 residents. We measured economic wellbeing through income and wealth. Income was measured at the individual level as the sum of income from various sources while wealth was assessed at the household level as the total value of assets including money in stocks and accounts, businesses, vehicles, real estate, and other assets. Missing values of both income and wealth were imputed by the MHAS (Wong, Orozco-Rocha, Zhang, Michaels-Obregon, & Gonzalez-Gonzalez, 2016). We categorized both income and wealth into deciles which were treated as continuous variables in our models. Parameter estimates then represented the change in cognition associated with increasing one decile of either income or wealth.

We also evaluated the respondents’ health characteristics by including chronic conditions and health behaviors. Chronic conditions were measured using respondents’ self-report of hypertension, diabetes, cancer, stroke, heart attack, and respiratory conditions. We then created a categorical variable representing whether the respondent reported no chronic conditions, one chronic condition, or two or more chronic conditions. Health behaviors included smoking (classified as current smoker, former smoker, or never smoker) and whether the respondent reported binge drinking (having four or more alcoholic beverages in a single occasion) in the past three months. All economic, demographic, and health covariates came from the 2015 wave of the MHAS.

Statistical Analysis:

We presented descriptive results of respondents in the 2015 wave of the MHAS stratified by elevated depressive symptomatology and conducted t-tests to test for differences in continuous variables by depression status and chi-square tests to test for differences in binary and categorical variables by depression status. In descriptive results, cognitive scores were presented before standardization to provide distributional information while regression results used standardized cognitive scores so that depression parameter estimates were in similar scales across cognitive tasks with differing ranges. We conducted two sets of regression analyses. First, we used ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions to model the cross-sectional associations between elevated depressive symptoms and cognitive domains, both determined in 2015. The purpose of our first set of analyses was to determine whether elevated depression and cognition were associated in similar ways across domains of cognitive function. In addition to elevated depressive symptoms, these models included respondents’ age, sex, level of education, marital status, locality size, income decile, wealth decile, smoking behavior, binge drinking, and number of chronic conditions. We also tested interactions between sex and depression as prior studies have noted differences in the associations between depression and cognition by sex (Biringer et al., 2005; Forno et al., 2005; Paterniti, Dufouil, Bisserbe, & Alpérovitch, 1999). In our second set of analyses, we modeled cognitive domains in 2015 as a function of our four level variable combining depression status in 2012 and 2015 to understand how history of depression and current depressive status was related with cognition across domains. In this set of analyses, we modeled cognition in 2015 with and without accounting for differences in cognition in 2012 to determine whether cognitive differences in 2015 were due to persistent cognitive differences over follow-up. All regression models were estimated using Stata SE version 14.2.

Results:

Descriptive Results:

We present our descriptive results for our analytic sample in 2015 (follow-up) by depression status in Table 1. Approximately 31% (3,972) of our sample was classified as depressed based on the count of depressive symptoms. The analytic sample was 57.8% female and had an average age of 66.5. Respondents with elevated depressive symptoms performed significantly worse on all cognitive tasks. Cohen’s d statistics for each cognitive task by depression were 0.31, 0.22, 0.37, 0.34, 0.21, 0.18, and 0.27 for verbal learning, verbal recall, visual scanning, verbal fluency, visuospatial, visual memory, and orientation, respectively. The depressed and non-depressed groups in our sample showed several other significant differences in terms of their demographic, health and health behavior, and economic characteristics. Depressed respondents were more likely to be older, female, less educated, widowed, living in a more rural area, and in a lower decile of income and wealth. Depressed respondents also reported higher levels of never smoking, not binge drinking in the previous three months, and reported more chronic conditions.

TABLE 1.

Sociodemographic, Cognitive, Health, and Economic Characteristics of Older (Age 50+) Mexican Adults by Depression Status in 2015

Non-Depressed (n=8,926) Depressed (n=3,972)
Mean SD Mean SD Sig

Cognitive Function
Verbal Learning 14.6 3.7 13.4 3.8 ***
Verbal Recall 4.3 2.1 3.8 2.2 ***
Visual Scanning 30.6 15.8 24.8 14.8 ***
Verbal Fluency 16.0 5.3 14.2 5.0 ***
Visuospatial 5.6 0.9 5.4 1.2 ***
Visual Memory 4.9 1.6 4.6 1.8 ***
Orientation 2.5 0.8 2.3 1.0 ***
Age 65.9 9.2 67.8 9.9 ***
Years of Education 6.4 4.9 4.4 4.0 ***
Sex
Male (n, %) 4,211 47.2 1,237 31.1 ***
Female (n, %) 4,715 52.8 2,735 68.9
Marital Status
Married/Partnered (n, %) 6,249 70.0 2,356 59.3 ***
Widowed (n, %) 1,431 16.0 994 25.0
Other (n, %) 1,246 14.0 622 15.7
Locality Size
100,000+ (n, %) 5,360 60.0 2,092 52.7 ***
15,000–99,999 (n, %) 1,151 12.9 551 13.9
2,500–14,999 (n, %) 789 8.8 435 11.0
<2,500 (n, %) 1,626 18.2 894 22.5
Economic Well-being (Continuous)
Income Decile 4.6 3.0 4.1 2.7 ***
Wealth Decile 4.7 2.8 4.2 2.8 ***
Smoking
Never (n, %) 5,152 57.7 2,544 64.0 ***
Former (n, %) 2,661 29.8 1,026 25.8
Current (n, %) 1,113 12.5 402 10.1
Binge Drinking (Last 3 Months)
No (n, %) 8,085 90.6 3,749 94.4 ***
Yes (n, %) 841 9.4 223 5.6
Chronic Conditions
0 (n, %) 3,082 34.5 772 19.4 ***
1 (n %) 3,400 38.1 1,512 38.1
2+ (n, %) 2,444 27.4 1,688 42.5

Source: Author’s own calculations using data from the 2015 wave of the Mexican Health and Aging Study. Depression status is determined cross-sectionally using scores from the Center for Epidemiologic Studies - Depression scale. n=12,898.

*

indicates p<0.05

**

indicates p<0.01

***

indicates p<0.001.

Descriptive results are presented as means and standard deviations for continuous variables as indicated by the column headings. Descriptive results for binary/categorical variables are presented as sample size (n) and %. This is indicated in the category labels for each categorical variable. All percentages are column percentages. Sample size used for calculations of mean scores for verbal learning, verbal recall, visual scanning, verbal fluency, visuospatial, visual memory, and orientation tasks were 12,854, 12,834, 12,027, 12,865, 12,048, 11,928, and 12,898 respectively. Descriptive results for all independent variables were based on the full analytic sample (n=12,898).

Cross-Sectional Results:

Regression results are shown in Table 2. Depression was associated with poorer cognitive performance across all cognitive domains. Parameter estimates for depression indicated that, compared to the non-depressed, individuals with elevated depressive symptoms scored between 0.04 and 0.15 standard deviations worse on cognitive tasks. Parameter estimates for depression were: verbal learning (β=−0.15, p<0.001), verbal recall (β=−0.10, p<0.001), visual scanning (β=−0.09, p<0.001), verbal fluency (β=−0.11, p<0.001), visuospatial ability (β=−0.06, p<0.01), visual memory (β=−0.04, p<0.05), and orientation (β=−0.12, p<0.001). Being older, having fewer years of education, living in a more rural area, having less income, and being in a lower decile of wealth were associated with poorer cognitive function across all cognitive domains. Women performed significantly better on verbal learning, verbal recall, and visual scanning tasks, whereas men performed better on the visuospatial task. In ancillary analyses (results not shown, available upon request), we tested whether the association between depression and cognitive domains differed for older males and females by creating an interaction between sex and elevated depressive symptomatology. While the interaction between sex and depression did not reach statistical significance for any domain, the interaction was marginally significant for verbal learning (p=0.0557) suggesting the association between depression and cognition may be stronger among males than females. When we stratified the models by sex, the depression parameter estimates were (β=−0.19, p<0.001) and (β=−0.09, p<0.001) for males and females, respectively, indicating a stronger negative association between depression and verbal learning for males.

TABLE 2.

Ordinary Least Squares Regression of Cognitive Scores by Domain among Older (Age 50+) Mexican Adults

Verbal Learning Verbal Recall Visual Scanning Verbal Fluency Visuospatial Visual Memory Orientation
Depression β SE β SE β SE β SE β SE β SE β SE

Depressed −0.15*** (0.017) −0.10*** (0.017) −0.09*** (0.015) −0.11*** (0.017) −0.06** (0.019) −0.04* (0.019) −0.12*** (0.018)
Demographics
Age −0.03*** (0.001) −0.03*** (0.001) −0.03*** (0.001) −0.02*** (0.001) −0.02*** (0.001) −0.03*** (0.001) −0.02*** (0.001)
Female 0.34*** (0.018) 0.33*** (0.019) 0.08*** (0.016) 0.00 (0.018) −0.05** (0.021) −0.04 (0.021) 0.02 (0.020)
Years of Education 0.06*** (0.002) 0.05*** (0.002) 0.09*** (0.002) 0.07*** (0.002) 0.05*** (0.002) 0.04*** (0.002) 0.05*** (0.002)
Marital Status (Ref: Married)
Widowed −0.03 (0.022) −0.00 (0.023) −0.10*** (0.020) −0.04 (0.022) −0.13*** (0.025) −0.06* (0.025) −0.12*** (0.024)
Other −0.05* (0.022) −0.04 (0.023) −0.08*** (0.020) −0.08*** (0.023) −0.02 (0.026) −0.06* (0.025) −0.05 (0.024)
Locality Size (Ref: 100,000+)
15,000–99,999 −0.03 (0.023) −0.03 (0.024) −0.08*** (0.020) −0.03 (0.023) −0.01 (0.026) −0.03 (0.026) −0.06* (0.025)
2,500–14,999 −0.13*** (0.026) −0.06* (0.028) −0.22*** (0.024) −0.12*** (0.027) −0.04 (0.031) −0.02 (0.030) −0.10*** (0.029)
<2,500 −0.19*** (0.021) −0.10*** (0.022) −0.30*** (0.019) −0.15*** (0.021) −0.14*** (0.024) −0.11*** (0.024) −0.22*** (0.023)
Economic Well-being
Income Decile 0.02*** (0.003) 0.01* (0.003) 0.02*** (0.002) 0.02*** (0.003) 0.01* (0.003) 0.01 (0.003) 0.01*** (0.003)
Wealth Decile 0.01* (0.003) 0.00 (0.003) 0.02*** (0.002) 0.01*** (0.003) 0.01*** (0.003) 0.01* (0.003) 0.01** (0.003)
Smoking (Ref: Never)
Former 0.06** (0.018) 0.03 (0.019) 0.13*** (0.016) 0.14*** (0.019) 0.06** (0.021) 0.06** (0.021) 0.03 (0.020)
Current 0.04 (0.025) 0.05* (0.026) 0.13*** (0.022) 0.11*** (0.025) 0.05 (0.029) 0.05 (0.028) −0.00 (0.027)
Binge Drinking (Last 3 Months)
Yes −0.02 (0.028) −0.01 (0.030) −0.02 (0.025) −0.04 (0.029) −0.05 (0.032) −0.04 (0.032) −0.03 (0.031)
Chronic Conditions (Ref: 0)
1 0.03 (0.018) 0.02 (0.019) −0.01 (0.016) 0.02 (0.019) 0.05* (0.021) 0.04 (0.021) 0.07*** (0.020)
2+ 0.01 (0.020) −0.01 (0.021) −0.11*** (0.018) −0.03 (0.020) 0.02 (0.023) 0.02 (0.023) 0.04* (0.022)
Sample Size: 12854 12834 12027 12865 12048 11928 12898

Source: Authors’ own calculation using data from the 2015 wave of the Mexican Health and Aging Study. Cognition and depression are determined contemporaneously in 2015.

*

indicates p<0.05

**

indicates p<0.01

***

indicates p<0.001.

History of Depression & Current Depression:

We then present our regression results for history of depression in Table 3. As our analysis of history of depression inherently required that respondents be interviewed at baseline and follow-up, our sample size for the second set of analyses was reduced to 11,299 respondents. Based on our four-level variable on depression status at baseline and follow-up, approximately 55% of respondents were classified as stable non-depressed, 14% were classified as formerly depressed, 13% were classified as newly depressed, and 18% were classified as stable depressed. For each cognitive domain, we fit two models. Models in Panel A regressed cognition at follow-up as a function of depression operationalized as the four-level variable on depression status at baseline and follow-up and the control variables used in Table 2. Models in Panel B added standardized domain specific cognitive scores from baseline. While depression was contemporaneously associated with poorer cognition across domains in our cross-sectional results, several differences by domain emerged when considering a respondent’s history of depression.

TABLE 3.

Ordinary Least Squares Regression of Cognitive Scores by Domain among Older (Age 50+) Mexican Adults

PANEL A
Verbal Learning Verbal Recall Visual Scanning Verbal Fluency Visuospatial Visual Memory Orientation
Depression (Ref: Stable Non-Depressed) β SE β SE β SE β SE β SE β SE β SE
Formerly Depressed −0.06* (0.024) −0.06* (0.026) −0.07** (0.022) −0.09*** (0.025) 0.02 (0.027) −0.03 (0.028) −0.03 (0.027)
Newly Depressed −0.16*** (0.024) −0.10*** (0.026) −0.07** (0.023) −0.13*** (0.025) −0.06* (0.028) −0.03 (0.028) −0.10*** (0.027)
Stable Depression −0.17*** (0.022) −0.12*** (0.024) −0.12*** (0.021) −0.12*** (0.023) −0.03 (0.025) −0.03 (0.026) −0.13*** (0.025)
Sample Size 11,217 11,155 10,117 11,195 10,144 9,956 11,255
PANEL B
Verbal Learning Verbal Recall Visual Scanning Verbal Fluency Visuospatial Visual Memory Orientation
Depression (Ref: Stable Non-Depressed) β SE β SE β SE β SE β SE β SE β SE
Formerly Depressed −0.02 (0.023) −0.04 (0.024) −0.03 (0.019) −0.05* (0.023) 0.01 (0.025) −0.03 (0.027) −0.02 (0.025)
Newly Depressed −0.13*** (0.023) −0.09*** (0.025) −0.06** (0.020) −0.10*** (0.023) −0.06* (0.026) −0.02 (0.027) −0.09*** (0.025)
Stable Depression −0.12*** (0.021) −0.06** (0.023) −0.06** (0.018) −0.08*** (0.021) −0.02 (0.024) −0.01 (0.025) −0.10*** (0.023)
Baseline Cognitive Function
2012 Cognitive Score 0.35*** (0.009) 0.32*** (0.009) 0.47*** (0.008) 0.41*** (0.009) 0.34*** (0.009) 0.27*** (0.010) 0.36*** (0.009)
Sample Size 11,217 11,155 10,117 11,195 10,144 9,956 11,255

Source: Authors’ own calculation using data from the 2015 wave of the Mexican Health and Aging Study. Dependent cognition variables comes from 2015, baseline cognitive score comes from 2012. “Stable non-depressed” indicates not depressed in 2012 or 2015, “Formerly depressed” indicates respondent was depressed in 2012 but not depressed in 2015, “Newly depressed” indicates respondent was not depressed in 2012 but was depressed in 2015, “Stable depression” indicates respondent was depressed in both 2012 and 2015. Models are also adjusted for 2015 age, sex, education, marital status, locality size, income, wealth, smoking, binge drinking, and chronic conditions.

*

indicates p<0.05

**

indicates p<0.01

***

indicates p<0.001.

In Panel A, compared to stable non-depressed, formerly depressed respondents still exhibited poorer cognitive performance in verbal learning (β=−0.06, p<0.05), verbal recall (β=−0.06, p<0.05), visual scanning (β=−0.07, p<0.01), and verbal fluency (β=−0.09, p<0.001) at follow-up. No significant differences in cognition were noted for visuospatial ability, visual memory, or orientation between stable non-depressed and formerly depressed. When we adjusted for differences in baseline cognitive function in the models in Panel B, respondents who had elevated depressive symptoms at baseline and had recovered by follow-up only performed significantly worse on the verbal fluency task. This suggests that, rather than having greater cognitive decline between baseline and follow-up, respondents who recovered from elevated depressive symptoms had poorer cognitive function at baseline than the stable non-depressed, and this disparity persisted in several cognitive domains despite improvements in depression symptomatology. In sensitivity analyses (results not shown, available upon request), the associations between former depression and cognitive tasks did not differ significantly by sex.

Incident depression (having elevated depressive symptoms at follow-up but not at baseline) was associated with poorer cognitive outcomes compared to the stable non-depressed across all cognitive domains in Panel A with the exception of visual memory. The significant parameter estimates for onset in Panel A remained statistically significant even after accounting for baseline cognitive function in Panel B. Last, having stable depression (i.e. elevated depressive symptoms in both baseline and follow-up) was associated with poorer cognitive outcomes in all cognitive assessments, with the exceptions of visuospatial ability and visual memory in Panel A. After we adjusted for differences in baseline cognitive function in Panel B, the significant parameter estimates for stable depression in Panel A were reduced but remained statistically significant.

In sensitivity analyses, we changed the reference group to individuals who were newly depressed and found no significant differences in follow-up cognitive function between the newly depressed and the stable depressed group, with or without adjustment for cognitive function at baseline. We then changed the reference group to the formerly depressed and found that, regardless of adjustment for baseline cognitive function, respondents with new depression performed significantly worse on verbal learning, visuospatial ability, and orientation tasks; whereas individuals with stable depression performed significantly worse on verbal learning, visual scanning, and orientation tasks (although the differences in visual scanning performance were no longer significant after adjusting for baseline visual scanning). Results of analyses with newly depressed respondents as the reference group are provided in Supplemental Table 1 and results with formerly depressed as the reference group are provided in Supplemental Table 2.

Conclusions:

Regarding our cross-sectional findings, we found support for our first hypothesis that elevated depressive symptomatology would be associated with poorer cognitive function. Our results were consistent with a large body of research demonstrating negative associations between depression and cognitive function (Diniz et al., 2013; González et al., 2008; Goveas et al., 2014; Mourao et al., 2016; Richard et al., 2013). Elevated depressive symptomatology was associated with poorer performance across all cognitive domains in our cross-sectional analyses. In our regression analyses, elevated depressive symptomatology exhibited the largest negative association with verbal learning performance. However, it should be noted that there was significant overlap in the confidence intervals for depression parameters across cognitive domains. The associations between depression and cognitive domains appeared to be independent of demographic, health, and economic characteristics.

We found marginal evidence suggesting the association between elevated depressive symptomatology and poorer cognitive function differed by sex for the verbal learning task. The marginally significant interaction suggested a stronger association between depression and cognition for males than females, which is consistent with prior studies (Biringer et al., 2005; Forno et al., 2005; Paterniti et al., 1999). One study finding mild depressive symptoms to be associated with incident amnestic mild cognitive impairment among men, but not women (Sundermann, Katz, & Lipton, 2017) posited that sex differences may be due to greater depression related changes in medial frontal lobe volume among males (Taki et al., 2005). Sundermann et al. (2017) and Forno et al. (2005) also attributed their findings to gender based societal expectations which may lead men to be more reluctant to acknowledge depression symptoms due to gender roles of suppressing emotional expression. Thus, depressive symptoms reported by men have the potential to be more severe.

Regarding our findings combining depression status at baseline and follow-up, we also found support for our second hypothesis that the formerly depressed would continue to exhibit poorer cognitive function relative to their non-depressed counterparts without a history of depression, despite improvements in depressive symptomatology. Our results were consistent with previous work suggesting poorer cognitive function persisting among individuals with prior episodes of depression from which they have recovered (Bhalla et al., 2006; Hasselbalch et al., 2011; Neu et al., 2005; Reppermund et al., 2009; Richard et al., 2013; Rock et al., 2014). Although this was not observed for all cognitive tasks, respondents who had elevated depressive symptoms three years prior and recovered still had significantly lower verbal learning, verbal recall, visual scanning, and verbal fluency scores than those who were not depressed three years prior and remained non-depressed. These differentials (with the exception of verbal fluency) were explained by differences in cognitive function three years prior. Our results did not generally suggest that the formerly depressed had steeper cognitive decline over follow up. Rather, it appeared that respondents who recovered from elevated depressive symptoms that were present at baseline had poorer cognitive function at baseline than the stable non-depressed, and this disparity persisted despite improvements in depression symptomatology.

Persistent differences in cognitive function, specifically on our memory tasks including verbal learning and verbal recall performance, despite recovery from depression may be explained by atrophy of the hippocampus during past periods of elevated depressive symptomatology. While some research has shown hippocampal volume recovery following depression remission (Arnone et al., 2013), others have found smaller hippocampal volumes relative to non-depressed controls (Neumeister et al., 2005) even long after depression has remitted (Sheline et al., 1996). In the case of our analysis, the relatively short period between baseline and follow-up may not have given the formerly depressed ample opportunity to recover. We suggest the hippocampus as an area for future work. These results suggest that, in addition to current mental health status, health professionals should also consider a patient’s history of depression when assessing cognitive functioning among older adults (Jorm, 2001; Panza et al., 2010; Potter & Steffens, 2007).

While considering the respondent’s history of depression provided valuable information among non-depressed respondents, a history of depression did not appear to modify associations between current depression and cognition. Among those with elevated depressive symptoms at follow-up, we found no significant differences in cognitive outcomes according to baseline depressive status. The lack of an additive effect is somewhat surprising given that the formerly depressed did not seem to fully recover cognitively after becoming non-depressed. Further, it is somewhat surprising that the stable depressed group did not have significantly worse cognitive function than respondents with an onset of depression as previous work has found number of depressive episodes to be positively related to dementia diagnosis among individuals with depressive disorder (Kessing & Andersen, 2004). Both greater durations of major depression (McKinnon, Yucel, Nazarov, & MacQueen, 2009; Sheline et al., 1996) as well as greater lengths of time with untreated depression (Sheline, Gado, & Kraemer, 2003) are associated with decreased hippocampal volume. Our results are consistent with Richard et al., (2013) who found similar hazard ratios for dementia incidence whether comparing those with persistent depression or an onset of depression to those were stable non-depressed.

Our analysis comes with several limitations. First, while we consider the respondents’ history of depression, our analysis is based on two waves of data which greatly limits our ability to conduct longitudinal analyses of cognitive decline. Future work should consider how trajectories of depressive symptoms are associated with trajectories of cognitive decline across longer follow-up periods. Second, while the CES-D is widely used in studies of depression, the CES-D provides merely a snapshot of the respondent’s level of depression as the items refer to the “last week.” This is an important limitation as prior literature has suggested that depression which has an onset in early versus late life may be associated differently with cognitive health (Herrmann, Goodwin, & Ebmeier, 2007; Sachs-Ericsson et al., 2013). Studies of depression and cognition in developing countries would be enriched by conducting detailed life-histories including questions about prior episodes of depression. Last, many of our variables are based on self-reported data which may result in biased responses.

Despite these limitations, our analyses come with several strengths. First, the MHAS data is rich, consisting of a large sample size with detailed information on a respondent’s cognitive health using tasks representing a variety of cognitive domains. This affords us the opportunity to evaluate the associations between depression and cognition, across time, in the context of a developing country. Second, our analyses were adjusted for a broad array of confounding variables which allowed us to estimate associations between depression and cognitive domains that were independent of demographic, health, and economic confounders.

Depression remains a prevalent issue among older adults in Mexico (García-Peña et al. 2008). However, mental health disorders are infrequently treated in low- and middle-income settings where an estimated 76% to 85% of individuals with mental health problems do not receive treatment (WHO, 2018). In Mexico in particular, between 87% (in urban areas) and 96% (in rural areas) of individuals with a depressive episode do not report having received treatment (Guerra et al., 2009). This clearly points to the need to improve both screening and treatment of depressive disorders in the context of Mexico and other developing countries. Dementia is also costly as it represents the largest contributor to disability in Mexico (Sousa et al., 2009) and had an estimated worldwide cost of US $604 billion in 2010 (WHO & Alzheimer’s Disease International [ADI], 2012) with substantial informal care costs in Latin America (Custodio, Wheelock, Thumala, & Slachevsky, 2017; WHO & ADI, 2012). Given population aging in Mexico, it is becoming increasingly important to understand the nuanced associations between depression and cognition to develop targeted interventions to promote cognitive health among the currently depressed as well as individuals with a history of depression. Successful interventions may aid in reducing burdens on healthcare systems and family care-givers in Mexico.

Supplementary Material

1

Acknowledgments

Funding:

This work was funded by the National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Aging under Grants [R01AG018016]; [T32AG000037]; [T32AG000270]; and [K99AG058799].

References:

  1. Afridi MI, Hina M, Qureshi IS, & Hussain M (2011). Cognitive disturbance comparison among drug-naïve depressed cases and healthy controls. Journal of the College of Physicians and Surgeons--Pakistan: JCPSP, 21(6), 351–355. https://doi.org/07.2011/JCPSP.351355 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agudelo-Botero M, Giraldo-Rodríguez L, Murillo-González JC, Mino-León D, & Cruz-Arenas E (2018). Factors associated with occasional and recurrent falls in Mexican community-dwelling older people. PLOS ONE, 13(2), e0192926 10.1371/journal.pone.0192926 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aguilar-Navarro SG, Fuentes-Cantú A, Ávila-Funes JA, & García-Mayo EJ (2007). Validity and reliability of the screening questionnaire for geriatric depression used in the Mexican Health and Age Study. Salud Pública de México, 49(4), 256–262. 10.1590/S0036-36342007000400005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Al Hazzouri A, Haan MN, Galea S, & Aiello AE (2011). Life-Course Exposure to Early Socioeconomic Environment, Education in Relation to Late-Life Cognitive Function Among Older Mexicans and Mexican Americans. Journal of Aging and Health, 23(7), 1027–1049. 10.1177/0898264311421524 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Arnone D, McKie S, Elliott R, Juhasz G, Thomas EJ, Downey D, … Anderson IM (2013). State-dependent changes in hippocampal grey matter in depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 18(12), 1265–1272. 10.1038/mp.2012.150 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barnes DE, Alexopoulos GS, Lopez OL, Williamson JD, & Yaffe K (2006). Depressive Symptoms, Vascular Disease, and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Findings From the Cardiovascular Health Study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(3), 273–279. 10.1001/archpsyc.63.3.273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bhalla RK, Butters MA, Mulsant BH, Begley AE, Zmuda MD, Schoderbek B, … Becker JT (2006). Persistence of Neuropsychologic Deficits in the Remitted State of Late-Life Depression. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14(5), 419–427. 10.1097/01.JGP.0000203130.45421.69 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Biringer E, Mykletun A, Dahl AA, Smith AD, Engedal K, Nygaard HA, & Lund A (2005). The association between depression, anxiety, and cognitive function in the elderly general population--the Hordaland Health Study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20(10), 989–997. 10.1002/gps.1390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Nazeer A, Adil J, Khan S, … Charney DS (2002). Reduced volume of orbitofrontal cortex in major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 51(4), 273–279. 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01336-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brommelhoff JA, Gatz M, Johansson B, McArdle JJ, Fratiglioni L, & Pedersen NL (2009). Depression as a risk factor or prodromal feature for dementia? Findings in a population-based sample of Swedish twins. Psychology and Aging, 24(2), 373–384. 10.1037/a0015713 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Butters MA, Whyte EM, Nebes RD, Begley AE, Dew MA, Mulsant BH, … Becker JT (2004). The Nature and Determinants of Neuropsychological Functioning in Late-LifeDepression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(6), 587–595. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.6.587 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Byers AL, & Yaffe K (2011). Depression and risk of developing dementia. Nature Reviews Neurology, 7(6), 323–331. 10.1038/nrneurol.2011.60 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Campbell S, Marriott M, Nahmias C, & MacQueen GM (2004). Lower Hippocampal Volume in Patients Suffering From Depression: A Meta-Analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(4), 598–607. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.4.598 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Custodio N, Wheelock A, Thumala D, & Slachevsky A (2017). Dementia in Latin America: Epidemiological Evidence and Implications for Public Policy. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Diniz BS, Butters MA, Albert SM, Dew MA, & Reynolds CF (2013). Late-life depression and risk of vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: systematic review and meta-analysis of community-based cohort studies. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 202(5), 329–335. 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.118307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dong H, & Csernansky JG (2009). Effects of Stress and Stress Hormones on Amyloid-β Protein and Plaque Deposition. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease : JAD, 18(2), 459–469. 10.3233/JAD-2009-1152 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dotson VM, Resnick SM, & Zonderman AB (2008). Differential Association of Concurrent, Baseline, and Average Depressive Symptoms With Cognitive Decline in Older Adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16(4), 318–330. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181662a9c [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Drevets WC, Price JL, Jr JRS, Todd RD, Reich T, Vannier M, & Raichle ME (1997). Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature, 386(6627), 824–827. 10.1038/386824a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Forno GD, Palermo MT, Donohue JE, Karagiozis H, Zonderman AB, & Kawas CH (2005). Depressive symptoms, sex, and risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of Neurology, 57(3), 381–387. 10.1002/ana.20405 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fors S, Lennartsson C, & Lundberg O (2009). Childhood Living Conditions, Socioeconomic Position in Adulthood, and Cognition in Later Life: Exploring the Associations. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, gbp029. 10.1093/geronb/gbp029 [DOI] [PubMed]
  21. García-Peña C, Wagner FA, Sánchez-Garcia S, Juárez-Cedillo T, Espinel-Bermúdez C, García-Gonzalez JJ, … Gallo JJ (2008). Depressive Symptoms Among Older Adults in Mexico City. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 23(12), 1973–1980. 10.1007/s11606-008-0799-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Glosser G, Wolfe N, Albert ML, Lavine L, Steele JC, Calne DB, & Schoenberg BS (1993). Cross-Cultural Cognitive Examination: Validation of a Dementia Screening Instrument for Neuroepidemiological Research. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 41(9), 931–939. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1993.tb06758.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. González HM, Bowen ME, & Fisher GG (2008). Memory Decline and Depressive Symptoms in a Nationally Representative Sample of Older Adults: The Health and Retirement Study (1998–2004). Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 25(3), 266–271. 10.1159/000115976 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Goveas JS, Espeland MA, Hogan PE, Tindle HA, Shih RA, Kotchen JM, … Resnick SM (2014). Depressive Symptoms and Longitudinal Changes in Cognition: Women’s Health Initiative Study of Cognitive Aging. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 27(2), 94–102. 10.1177/0891988714522697 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Guerra M, Prina AM, Ferri CP, Acosta D, Gallardo S, Huang Y, … Prince M (2016). A comparative cross-cultural study of the prevalence of late life depression in low and middle income countries. Journal of Affective Disorders, 190, 362–368. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Guerra Mariella, Ferri CP, Sosa AL, Salas A, Gaona C, Gonzales V, … Prince M (2009). Late-life depression in Peru, Mexico and Venezuela: the 10/66 population-based study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 195(6), 510–515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hasselbalch BJ, Knorr U, & Kessing LV (2011). Cognitive impairment in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder: A systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 134(1), 20–31. 10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Herrmann LL, Goodwin GM, & Ebmeier KP (2007). The cognitive neuropsychology of depression in the elderly. Psychological Medicine, 37(12), 1693–1702. 10.1017/S0033291707001134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jefferson AL, Gibbons LE, Rentz DM, Carvalho JO, Manly J, Bennett DA, & Jones RN (2011). A Life Course Model of Cognitive Activities, Socioeconomic Status, Education, Reading Ability, and Cognition. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 59(8), 1403–1411. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03499.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jorm AF (2001). History of Depression as a Risk Factor for Dementia: An Updated Review. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35(6), 776–781. 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00967.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kessing LV, & Andersen PK (2004). Does the risk of developing dementia increase with the number of episodes in patients with depressive disorder and in patients with bipolar disorder? Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 75(12), 1662–1666. 10.1136/jnnp.2003.031773 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Korczyn AD, & Halperin I (2009). Depression and dementia. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 283(1), 139–142. 10.1016/j.jns.2009.02.346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lee GJ, Lu PH, Hua X, Lee S, Wu S, Nguyen K, … Thompson PM (2012). Depressive Symptoms in Mild Cognitive Impairment Predict Greater Atrophy in Alzheimer’s Disease-Related Regions. Biological Psychiatry, 71(9), 814–821. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lee S, Kawachi I, Berkman LF, & Grodstein F (2003). Education, Other Socioeconomic Indicators, and Cognitive Function. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157(8), 712–720. 10.1093/aje/kwg042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lépine J-P, & Briley M (2011). The increasing burden of depression. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 7(Suppl 1), 3–7. 10.2147/NDT.S19617 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lima M. S. de, & Soares B. G. de O. (2008). Depression in Developing Countries In Biology of Depression (pp. 979–994). Wiley-Blackwell; 10.1002/9783527619672.ch39 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. McDermott LM, & Ebmeier KP (2009). A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. Journal of Affective Disorders, 119(1), 1–8. 10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. McKinnon MC, Yucel K, Nazarov A, & MacQueen GM (2009). A meta-analysis examining clinical predictors of hippocampal volume in patients with major depressive disorder. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN, 34(1), 41–54. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mejia-Arango S, & Gutierrez LM (2011). Prevalence and Incidence Rates of Dementia and Cognitive Impairment No Dementia in the Mexican Population Data From the Mexican Health and Aging Study. Journal of Aging and Health, 23(7), 1050–1074. 10.1177/0898264311421199 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. MHAS Mexican Health and Aging Study. (2015). Data Files and Documentation (public use): Mexican Health and Aging Study Wave 4 (2015) Retrieved from www.MHASweb.org
  41. Mourao RJ, Mansur G, Malloy‐Diniz LF, Costa EC, & Diniz BS (2016). Depressive symptoms increase the risk of progression to dementia in subjects with mild cognitive impairment: systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 31(8), 905–911. 10.1002/gps.4406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Murray CJ, & Lopez AD (1997). Alternative projections of mortality and disability by cause 1990–2020: Global Burden of Disease Study. The Lancet, 349(9064), 1498–1504. 10.1016/S0140-6736(96)07492-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Nemeroff CB, & Vale WW (2005). The neurobiology of depression: inroads to treatment and new drug discovery. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66 Suppl 7, 5–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Neu P, Bajbouj M, Schilling A, Godemann F, Berman RM, & Schlattmann P (2005). Cognitive function over the treatment course of depression in middle-aged patients: correlation with brain MRI signal hyperintensities. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 39(2), 129–135. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Neumeister A, Wood S, Bonne O, Nugent AC, Luckenbaugh DA, Young T, … Drevets WC (2005). Reduced hippocampal volume in unmedicated, remitted patients with major depression versus control subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 57(8), 935–937. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Panza F, Frisardi V, Capurso C, D’Introno A, Colacicco AM, Imbimbo BP, … Solfrizzi V (2010). Late-Life Depression, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Dementia: Possible Continuum? The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(2), 98–116. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181b0fa13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Patel V, Araya R, & Bolton P (2004). Editorial: Treating depression in the developing world. Tropical Medicine & International Health, 9(5), 539–541. 10.1111/j.1365-3156.2004.01243.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Paterniti S, Dufouil C, Bisserbe J-C, & Alpérovitch A (1999). Anxiety, depression, psychotropic drug use and cognitive impairment. Psychological Medicine, 29(2), 421–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Potter GG, & Steffens DC (2007). Contribution of Depression to Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in Older Adults. The Neurologist, 13(3), 105 10.1097/01.nrl.0000252947.15389.a9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Radloff LS (1977). The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401. 10.1177/014662167700100306 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Reppermund S, Ising M, Lucae S, & Zihl J (2009). Cognitive impairment in unipolar depression is persistent and non-specific: further evidence for the final common pathway disorder hypothesis. Psychological Medicine, 39(4), 603–614. 10.1017/S003329170800411X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Richard E, Reitz C, Honig LH, Schupf N, Tang MX, Manly JJ, … Luchsinger JA (2013). Late-Life Depression, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Dementia. JAMA Neurology, 70(3), 383–389. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.603 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rock PL, Roiser JP, Riedel WJ, & Blackwell AD (2014). Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(10), 2029–2040. 10.1017/S0033291713002535 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Sachs-Ericsson N, Corsentino E, Moxley J, Hames JL, Rushing NC, Sawyer K, … Steffens DC (2013). A longitudinal study of differences in late- and early-onset geriatric depression: Depressive symptoms and psychosocial, cognitive, and neurological functioning. Aging & Mental Health, 17(1), 1–11. 10.1080/13607863.2012.717253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sapolsky RM (1996). Why Stress Is Bad for Your Brain. Science, 273(5276), 749–750. 10.1126/science.273.5276.749 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Sapolsky RM (2001). Depression, antidepressants, and the shrinking hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(22), 12320–12322. 10.1073/pnas.231475998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Sawyer K, Corsentino E, Sachs-Ericsson N, & Steffens DC (2012). Depression, hippocampal volume changes, and cognitive decline in a clinical sample of older depressed outpatients and non-depressed controls. Aging & Mental Health, 16(6), 753–762. 10.1080/13607863.2012.678478 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sheline YI, Wang PW, Gado MH, Csernansky JG, & Vannier MW (1996). Hippocampal atrophy in recurrent major depression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93(9), 3908–3913. 10.1073/pnas.93.9.3908 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sheline Yvette I., Gado MH, & Kraemer HC (2003). Untreated Depression and Hippocampal Volume Loss. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(8), 1516–1518. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.8.1516 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Singh-Manoux A, Dugravot A, Fournier A, Abell J, Ebmeier K, Kivimäki M, & Sabia S (2017). Trajectories of Depressive Symptoms Before Diagnosis of Dementia: A 28-Year Follow-up Study. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(7), 712–718. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.0660 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Singh-Manoux A, Richards M, & Marmot M (2005). Socioeconomic Position across the Lifecourse: How Does it Relate to Cognitive Function in Mid-life? Annals of Epidemiology, 15(8), 572–578. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2004.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Slone LB, Norris FH, Murphy AD, Baker CK, Perilla JL, Diaz D, … Rodriguez J. de J. G. (2006). Epidemiology of major depression in four cities in Mexico. Depression and Anxiety, 23(3), 158–167. 10.1002/da.20137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sousa RM, Ferri CP, Acosta D, Albanese E, Guerra M, Huang Y, … Prince M (2009). Contribution of chronic diseases to disability in elderly people in countries with low and middle incomes: a 10/66 Dementia Research Group population-based survey. The Lancet, 374(9704), 1821–1830. 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61829-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Steenland K, Karnes C, Seals R, Carnevale C, Hermida A, & Levey A (2012). Late-Life Depression as a Risk Factor for Mild Cognitive Impairment or Alzheimer’s Disease in 30 US Alzheimer’s Disease Centers. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 31(2), 265–275. 10.3233/JAD-2012-111922 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Steffens David C., McQuoid DR, Payne ME, & Potter GG (2011). Change in Hippocampal Volume on Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Cognitive Decline Among Older Depressed and Nondepressed Subjects in the Neurocognitive Outcomes of Depression in the Elderly Study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 19(1), 4–12. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181d6c245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Steffens David Carl. (2017). Late-Life Depression and the Prodromes of Dementia. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(7), 673–674. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.0658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sundermann EE, Katz MJ, & Lipton RB (2017). Sex Differences in the Relationship between Depressive Symptoms and Risk of Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25(1), 13–22. 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.08.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Taki Y, Kinomura S, Awata S, Inoue K, Sato K, Ito H, … Fukuda H (2005). Male elderly subthreshold depression patients have smaller volume of medial part of prefrontal cortex and precentral gyrus compared with age-matched normal subjects: A voxel-based morphometry. Journal of Affective Disorders, 88(3), 313–320. 10.1016/j.jad.2005.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Videbech P, & Ravnkilde B (2004). Hippocampal Volume and Depression: A Meta-Analysis of MRI Studies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(11), 1957–1966. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.1957 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wolfe N, Imai Y, Otani C, Nagatani H, Hasegawa K, Sugimoto K, … Kuroda Y (1992). Criterion Validity of the Cross-Cultural Cognitive Examination in Japan. Journal of Gerontology, 47(4), P289–P291. 10.1093/geronj/47.4.P289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Wong R, Orozco-Rocha K, Zhang D, Michaels-Obregon A, & Gonzalez-Gonzalez. (2016). Imputation of Non-Response on Economic Variables in the Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS/ENASEM) 2012 Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS) Retrieved from http://mhasweb.org/Resources/DOCUMENTS/2012/Imputations/Imputation_of_Non_Reponse_on_Economic_Variables_in_the_MHAS_ENASEM_2012.pdf
  72. Wong Rebeca, Michaels-Obregon A, & Palloni A (2017). Cohort Profile: The Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS). International Journal of Epidemiology, 46(2), e2–e2. 10.1093/ije/dyu263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. World Health Organization. (2004). The Global Burden of Disease: 2004 Update Retrieved from http://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/GBD_report_2004update_full.pdf
  74. World Health Organization. (2018). Fact Sheets: Mental Disorders Retrieved from http://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/mental-disorders
  75. World Health Organization, & Alzheimer’s Disease International. (2012). Dementia: A Public Health Priority Geneva: World Health Organization (WHO) Retrieved from http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/75263/9789241564458_eng.pdf;jsessionid=7DE1665AA2A6D7F31F5303F0BEA9028E?sequence=1 [Google Scholar]
  76. Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Gore R, Sands L, Reus V, & Browner WS (1999). Depressive Symptoms and Cognitive Decline in Nondemented Elderly Women: A Prospective Study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(5), 425–430. 10.1001/archpsyc.56.5.425 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES