Abstract
Local adaptation is determined by the strength of selection and the level of gene flow within heterogeneous landscapes. The presence of benign habitat can act as an evolutionary stepping stone for local adaptation to challenging environments by providing the necessary genetic variation. At the same time, migration load from benign habitats will hinder adaptation. In a community context, interspecific competition is expected to select against maladapted migrants, hence reducing migration load and facilitating adaptation. As the interplay between competition and spatial heterogeneity on the joint ecological and evolutionary dynamics of populations is poorly understood, we performed an evolutionary experiment using the herbivore spider mite Tetranychus urticae as a model. We studied the species's demography and local adaptation in a challenging environment that consisted of an initial sink (pepper plants) and/or a more benign environment (cucumber plants). Half of the experimental populations were exposed to a competitor, the congeneric T. ludeni. We show that while spider mites only adapted to the challenging pepper environment when it was spatially interspersed with benign cucumber habitat, this adaptation was only temporary and disappeared when the populations in the benign cucumber environment were expanding and spilling-over to the challenging pepper environment. Although the focal species outcompeted the competitor after about two months, a negative effect of competition on the focal species’s performance persisted in the benign environment. Adaptation to challenging habitat in heterogeneous landscapes thus highly depends on demography and source–sink dynamics, but also on competitive interactions with other species, even if they are only present for a short time span.
Keywords: local adaptation, source–sink dynamics, spatial sorting, heterogeneous habitats, spider mites, Tetranychus urticae
1. Background
Local adaptation is a major driver of range expansion and invasion [1]. Species that colonize new areas outside their native range are likely to end up in novel, often heterogeneous environments. The persistence and establishment of populations in these new environments does not only depend on the number of founders but also on the local environmental filter, which may be overcome by adaptation [2,3]. Moreover, persistence will be facilitated by fitness stabilizing mechanisms following resource partitioning where individuals or species specialize in either a genetic or plastic way on different resources in a patch [4,5].
The distribution of species is not restricted to areas where expected fitness is positive. While dispersing, species also come into contact with marginal and even completely unsuitable habitat, but may reach substantial local population sizes there via spill-over effects. Species may therefore be ecologically rescued in marginal habitat by source-sink dynamics [6–8]. Dispersal towards these harsher habitats may be beneficial to escape strong competition with superior individuals or species. This will equalize fitness across habitats and promote regional coexistence [4,9]. These habitats differ not only in their local conditions but also in their connectedness [10], rendering insights from source–sink theories useful to understand demography. The extent of fragmentation as reflected by the number, size and connectedness of habitats will strongly affect dispersal and drift, and therefore local adaptation [11,12]. Connectedness may facilitate this local adaptation if standing genetic variation is maintained in connected but less challenging habitats (i.e. under moderate dispersal, sink populations may undergo evolutionary rescue). However, too much gene flow is known to hamper adaptation (migration load [13,14]). In these situations, persistence is more likely due to ecological rescue alone [6,15]. Intermediate amounts of dispersal are, therefore, thought to be most beneficial for evolutionary rescue [16], depending on the dispersal-selection balance [17].
Ecological rescue by dispersal from source populations is, in the long run, unstable unless further adaptation occurs [18–20]. This evolutionary rescue is more successful when the stressors imposed by the habitat are organized along gradients [21]. Such gradients result in shallower selection clines, and hence, maintain the genetic variation needed to fuel further adaptations along the gradient. The presence of moderately challenging habitat is especially beneficial for persistence if general, rather than specialized stress responses can evolve [22], such as when there is selection for phenotypes that are able to deal with a broad array of the stressor and/or perform well in several habitats (generalism) [23]. An example of a general stress response is fluctuating temperatures leading to thermal generalism [23].
Species usually colonize environments that are inhabited by other species. The presence of heterospecific competitors is known to complicate the processes of local adaptation, species persistence and coexistence [24]. Species interactions will affect the strength of the local selection pressures, thereby accelerating or decelerating adaptation rates [14,25–27]. Higher adaptation rates are expected when selection pressures imposed by competition overcome the negative effects of lower population sizes as long as traits for adaptation to new habitats and for higher competitive fitness are aligned [26]. Even species that are driven to extinction—these are called ghost species—can have a long-term evolutionary impact because they may create habitat modifications or evolutionary changes during their episodes of existence in the community [28–30].
Environmental heterogeneity and competition are thus anticipated to affect the process of local adaptation to novel habitats, but to our knowledge, they have not been simultaneously studied experimentally. Here, we present the results of an evolutionary experiment in which the two-spotted spider mite (Tetranychus urticae) is transferred towards homogeneous or mixed sets of host species with or without the congeneric competitor, T. ludeni. We test the prediction that adaptation to the challenging pepper plants is more likely in the heterogeneous environment by evolutionary and ecological rescue. We furthermore expect interspecific competition to promote the exploitation of challenging resources and consequently to lead to faster adaptation to the challenging environment. We here confirm these predictions but highlight the non-trivial pathways leading to persistence in marginal habitats.
2. Methods
(a). Study species
The species for this study are members of the family Tetranychidae (Acari, Arachnida): the two-spotted spider mite, Tetranychus urticae Koch (focal species), and the red-legged spider mite, Tetranychus ludeni Zacher (congeneric competitor). Large populations can be easily maintained due to their small body sizes (approx. 0.4 mm). Their short generation times, high performance, fast responses to selection and high levels of standing genetic variation [31–36], render spider mites highly suitable for experimental evolution.
(b). Experimental set-up
The experimental set-up is visualized in electronic supplementary material, figure S1. We created 13 inbred lines of T. urticae by mother–son mating. These lines were already highly inbred from previous experiments [37]. The initial population for these inbred lines was the LS-VL line, which started from about 5000 spider mites collected from roses in October 2000. They were afterwards maintained on bean plants, Phaseolus vulgaris ‘Prelude’. We tried to create 13 inbred lines for the competitor (T. ludeni) as well, but we failed because of low fertility and early mortality. Therefore, we created only six inbred lines of the competitor, which were also kept on Phaseolus vulgaris ‘Prelude’. We chose these strongly inbred lines to create similar gene pools for the evolutionary experiment and thereby reduce variability in outcomes simply due to variability in starting genetic variation.
We were interested in the effect of spatial heterogeneity and the impact of an interspecific competitor on adaptation to novel environments. Environmental variation was created by using different host plants, which served as resources to the mites. To investigate the effect of the resources, we created independent experimental units (islands) that each contained several plants among which dispersal was possible. Islands consisted of four three-week-old cucumber plants, Cucumis sativus ‘Tanja’, four five-week-old pepper plants, Capsicum annuum ‘California Wonder’, or a mixture with two cucumber and two pepper plants. All plants within the islands were in direct contact with each other. Cucumber is the more palatable of the two and hence hypothesized to present an ecological source and a possible evolutionary stepping stone to adaptation to pepper. To prevent spider mites from crossing between islands, the bottom of the boxes with plants was covered with yellow sticky paper (Pherobank) and the walls were covered with Vaseline. This method was effective in previous work [14]. To examine the impact of the competitor, 24 islands received only T. urticae (eight replicates for each treatment: homogeneous cucumber, homogeneous pepper and the mixture), and another 24 also received the heterospecific T. ludeni. We initiated each island with 52 adult females. Islands without interspecific competition received four adult females from each of the 13 inbred lines of our focal species, and islands with interspecific competition received two adult females from the same inbred lines, supplemented with 26 adult females of the competitor, T. ludeni. Twelve of the T. ludeni females were from the six successful inbred lines, while the 14 others were taken from the stock population that was kept on bean. The initial population size of 52 adult females was chosen because natural populations colonize plants at low population sizes and it was found that inbreeding does not influence genetic trait variation in T. urticae [38]. We created a control population on bean that was initiated with the same isogenic lines from T. urticae.
Every week, we assessed the deterioration of the plants in the islands. When necessary, the two oldest plants were replaced by two new plants. This way of refreshment guaranteed enough time for the mites to move towards the fresh plants and also for a generation of mites to develop. Although half of the plants per island were removed, this was much less than half of the population as most mites did not prefer to stay on the deteriorated plants. Therefore, this replacement protocol was preferred over fixed intervals regardless of the state of deterioration. The temperature in the climate-controlled room was 25–30°C and the light regime was 16 : 8 LD. For logistical reasons, we performed the experiment in two blocks with four replicates per treatment (hereafter ‘blocks’). The experiment lasted for 10 months which is equivalent to about 25 generations, considered to be sufficiently long to detect local adaptation in this species [33–36,39].
(c). Measurements
For the ecological dynamics, the density of mites per unit surface was tracked. Density of individuals per unit of resource is a better representation of the current competition than total population size. We assessed the density on the islands every two weeks by counting the number of adult females on a 1 cm² square next to the stalk of the highest fully grown leaf of the newest plants of the island. A specific location standardizes the measurements and enables a more reliable comparison in time. Both the abaxial and the adaxial side of the leaf were measured and the numbers summed.
We assessed evolutionary dynamics in performance by measuring fecundity. We chose fecundity as a proxy of adaptation because previous research demonstrated it to be the best predictor of adaptation compared to survival or development [14,35,40]. From here on we refer to ‘fecundity’ and ‘performance’ interchangeably. We sampled five T. urticae females from each plant species per island at 2, 4, 6, 8 and 10 months during the experiment. We placed them on bean leaf discs for two generations to standardize juvenile and maternal effects [41,42]. These bean leaf discs (17 × 27 mm²) were surrounded by paper strip borders in Petri dishes with wet cotton wool. From this last generation, three quiescent deutonymph females were taken and placed separately each with an adult male from the same leaf on, respectively, a bean, cucumber and pepper leaf (same set-up as for common garden) in a climate cabinet at 30°C. The number of eggs and larvae after 6 days was assessed with daily pictures (hereafter referred to as ‘performance’) and females that drowned before the 6th day were excluded from the analysis.
(d). Statistical analysis
We first computed the goodness-of-fit of various parametric distributions to our performance data, separately for each plant species on which the performance was assessed. Based on these results, we chose a Gaussian distribution for the data from bean and cucumber, while the best-fitting distribution for performance on pepper was a zero-inflated negative binomial distribution with log link function, because of overdispersion in these data. The variance was determined as µk in which µ is the mean and k is the overdispersion parameter (linear-variance parameterization). We chose a single zero-inflation parameter because the overall percentage of zeroes per month in the dataset did not change in time. We tested this with a linear model (95% confidence interval = [60.83; 79.47]; Z = 1.268; p = 0.205). These distributions were used in generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs).
To control for changes in performance caused by different leaf qualities between assessed time points, we investigated differences in performance between the control population and the experimental populations through time for each plant species (bean, cucumber and pepper). We expected no differences in performance through time for the control population because they have always been maintained on bean plants. The dependent variable was performance (i.e. fecundity, number of eggs laid after 6 days), and the fixed explanatory variables were time, main treatment (control or experiment, i.e. both pepper and cucumber) and the time-by-treatment interaction. Because the replicates were split into two blocks for logistical reasons, we nested the islands in blocks that were treated as a random variable in the statistical model. Model selection was based on the lowest AICc and the pairwise comparisons of the least square means were adjusted for multiple comparisons based on Tukey's method. For an overview of the importance of the separate independent variables and their interactions, we also performed Wald χ2 tests of the maximal model. Time was treated as a categorical variable (2, 4, 6, 8 and 10 months) rather than a continuous variable, because we cannot assume a linear response of adaptation and because leaf quality for the fecundity tests might change in time.
To determine the effect of interspecific competition and habitat heterogeneity on adaptation through time, we built a GLMM per plant species with performance (number of eggs laid after 6 days) as the dependent variable. The fixed explanatory variables in the full model were time, treated as a categorical variable (2, 4, 6, 8 and 10 months), interspecific competition, also treated as a categorical variable (the presence or absence of T. ludeni), and the combination of the homogeneity or heterogeneity of the island with the plant where the females were sampled from (homogeneous from cucumber, heterogeneous from pepper and heterogeneous from cucumber). The populations on the homogeneous pepper islands were not able to survive, so they were not considered. The random variables were the different islands nested within the two blocks. Model selection was based on the lowest AICc and Wald χ2 tests were performed on the maximal model. The pairwise comparisons of the least square means were adjusted for multiple comparisons based on Tukey's method.
A final test was performed in which we were interested in potential differences between the plant species within the heterogeneous islands. The model included performance as the dependent variable and the fixed effects were the plant species (cucumber or pepper) and the time (2, 4, 6, 8 and 10 months), both treated as categorical variables. The different islands nested within the blocks were random variables. We performed Wald χ2 tests of the independent variables of the maximal model. Least square means between the different plant species were computed in which the p-values were adjusted for multiple comparisons based on Tukey's method.
The estimates provided in the tables are the raw and untransformed estimates for the fixed effects of the final models, of which the one for performance on pepper has a negative binomial error distribution.
All analyses were performed in R (v. 3.5.1) with glmmTMB v. 0.2.2.0 [43], MuMIn v. 1.42.1 [44], emmeans v. 1.2.4 [45], fitdistrplus v. 1.0-11 [46] and tseries v. 0.10-45 [47].
3. Results
Our main findings are fourfold. (i) Mites adapted to the most benign habitat (cucumber) in both homogeneous and heterogeneous environments. (ii) Populations in the sink habitat (pepper) established in heterogeneous environments only. (iii) Temporary local adaptation in the sink habitat (pepper) occurred within heterogeneous environments. (iv) Competition negatively affected performance on the ancestral habitat (bean), but not on the novel habitats (cucumber, pepper). Below we detail these findings.
(a). Ecological dynamics
In the heterogeneous islands, the focal species's population density on cucumber increased immediately, while the density on pepper initially remained stable at about five adult females per cm². After two months—the density on cucumber had already reached about 15 adult females per cm2—a steep increase was observed on both plant species (figure 1a). Mites reached higher densities in the homogeneous islands more rapidly, but the densities levelled off to similar values as on the heterogeneous islands (figure 1).
The populations of our focal species, Tetranychus urticae, and the competitor, T. ludeni, on the homogeneous pepper islands went extinct after a few weeks. Therefore, we were unable to perform fecundity tests on populations from the homogeneous pepper islands. Pepper habitat thus represented a true sink habitat where neither species could persist. On the homogeneous cucumber islands and the heterogeneous islands, the interspecific competitor, T. ludeni, could not survive and went extinct after about two months.
(b). Ancestral versus novel host plants
We detected a signal of local adaptation for the populations on cucumber; populations taken from the novel host plants (cucumber and/or pepper) evolved a 10 per cent higher fecundity on the cucumber leaves than the control population (kept on bean), independent of time (Z = 2.544 and p = 0.011; figure 2b; table 1; electronic supplementary material, tables S1 and S2). When assessed on bean leaves, populations from the novel host plants (cucumber and/or pepper) showed an overall performance similar to the control population, indicating no loss of adaptation to the original host (bean). In fact, they even reached higher fecundity (an increase of 16 per cent) than the control at four months (Z = 2.266 and p = 0.0234; figure 2a; table 1; electronic supplementary material, tables S1 and S2). On pepper, the populations from the novel environments (cucumber and/or pepper) performed as badly as the control population, indicating no local adaptation to the pepper plants (figure 2c; table 1; electronic supplementary material, tables S1 and S2).
Table 1.
estimate | s.e. | Z-value | Pr (>|z|) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(a) bean | (intercept) | 47.5620 | 4.2964 | 11.070 | < 2 × 10−16*** |
four months | −7.1388 | 3.8591 | −1.850 | 0.0643 | |
six months | −4.2116 | 3.8049 | −1.107 | 0.2683 | |
eight months | −0.8783 | 3.8049 | −0.231 | 0.8174 | |
10 months | −18.4544 | 3.9770 | −4.640 | 3.48 × 10−16*** | |
Experiment | −3.5087 | 3.3864 | −1.036 | 0.3001 | |
four months : exp. | 10.9829 | 4.1670 | 2.636 | 0.0084** | |
six months : exp. | 4.1284 | 4.1055 | 1.006 | 0.3146 | |
eight months : exp. | 0.1652 | 4.0926 | 0.040 | 0.9678 | |
10 months : exp. | 8.8113 | 4.2952 | 2.051 | 0.0402* | |
(b) cucumber | (intercept) | 28.869 | 1.211 | 23.846 | <2 × 10−16*** |
experiment | 2.889 | 1.136 | 2.544 | 0.011* | |
(c) pepper | (intercept) | 1.9886 | 0.1101 | 18.063 | < 2 × 10−16*** |
four months | −0.8767 | 0.1926 | −4.552 | 5.32 × 10−6*** | |
six months | −1.4587 | 0.2074 | −7.035 | 1.99 × 10−12*** | |
eight months | −0.6534 | 0.1529 | −4.274 | 1.92 × 10−5*** | |
10 months | −1.5919 | 0.2145 | −7.421 | 1.17 × 10−13*** |
(c). Interspecific competition and time
We found an effect of time and interspecific competition on the performance measured on bean for the experimental populations (figure 3a; table 2; electronic supplementary material, tables S1 and S2). All populations, including the control population, had a lower performance in the last month, which seems to be due to differences in the quality of leaves used in the fecundity tests. Mites under interspecific competition laid significantly fewer eggs on bean, a decrease of eight per cent, than those from the treatment with only intraspecific competition (Z = 2.901; p = 0.00371; table 2; electronic supplementary material, tables S1 and S2). These relative differences in performance are probably due to drift effects caused by a lower number of founders in the islands with interspecific competition than in those with only intraspecific competition. The presence of interspecific competition was the only explanatory variable in the best model on cucumber, but the difference between both treatments was not significant (Z = 1.51; p = 0.132; figure 3b; table 2; electronic supplementary material, table S1) and just not significant in the χ2 test (p = 0.089; electronic supplementary material, table S2).
Table 2.
estimate | s.e. | Z-value | Pr (>|z|) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(a) bean | (intercept) | 42.4354 | 3.4129 | 12.434 | <2 × 10−16*** |
no competition | 3.4794 | 1.1992 | 2.901 | 0.00371** | |
four months | 3.8220 | 1.5859 | 2.410 | 0.01595* | |
six months | −0.1068 | 1.5565 | −0.069 | 0.94531 | |
eight months | −0.6940 | 1.5215 | −0.456 | 0.64831 | |
10 months | −9.8571 | 1.6390 | −6.014 | 1.81 × 10−9*** | |
(b) cucumber | (intercept) | 31.0613 | 0.8459 | 36.72 | <2 × 10−16*** |
no competition | 1.4345 | 0.9531 | 1.51 | 0.132 | |
(c) pepper | (intercept) | 0.9428 | 0.1491 | 6.323 | 2.56 × 10−10*** |
four months | −0.4258 | 0.1771 | −2.404 | 0.0162* | |
six months | −0.9496 | 0.1943 | −4.887 | 1.02 × 10−6*** | |
eight months | −0.1068 | 0.1545 | −0.691 | 0.4896 | |
10 months | −1.1249 | 0.2202 | −5.108 | 3.26 × 10−7*** |
Fecundity on pepper decreased in time and seemed to coincide with population build-up (figure 3c; table 2; electronic supplementary material, tables S1 and S2), which is similar to the control population. The observed performance on pepper was lowest after 10 months. No effect of interspecific competition on pepper was found.
(d). Comparison between mites taken from cucumber and pepper in heterogeneous islands
Overall, we did not find significant differences between mites sampled from the cucumber and pepper plants within the same heterogeneous islands for performance on the initial host plant, bean, and the novel host plant, cucumber. However, we did detect a transient signal of local adaptation. At four months, mites sampled from pepper plants had a significantly higher fecundity on pepper, an increase of 44 per cent, than the mites sampled from cucumber from the same island (Z = −2.546; p = 0.0109; figure 1b). This signal vanished after six months. By contrast, mites sampled from cucumber did not perform better on cucumber than mites sampled from pepper at any point in time.
4. Discussion
We have shown that the establishment of populations in novel environments depends on the tight interplay between transient ecological and evolutionary dynamics in response to the nature and diversity of the local conditions (here host plant identity). Establishment in the most challenging environment (pepper) was impossible when it was the only available one. However, when this marginal habitat was spatially interspersed with a more benign one (cucumber), establishment succeeded because rapidly expanding populations on the neighbouring benign habitat generated sufficient immigration. These source–sink dynamics thus initially created ecological rescue in the challenging environment. Evolutionary rescue (i.e. adaptation to the challenging environment) was, however, only transient. We speculate that this is because a further expansion of the source populations broke down adaptation through an influx of unadapted genes (i.e. genetic load).
Although transient, the signal of adaptation in our heterogeneous habitat was convincing: mites that were sampled from pepper plants reached the highest performance on that host plant. The difference in performance on the two host plants suggests that dispersal was not random. A possible explanation is isolation by environment through biased dispersal [4,48], as it is known that dispersal with habitat choice can favour rapid evolution [49]. Also, non-random dispersal might result from a competition–colonization trade-off, as suggested by earlier dispersal experiments with this model species [50]. The general theory states that competitively inferior individuals may be better at colonization [51], which could be consistent with our observations, as individuals on the more challenging pepper plants could be escaping competition on the less challenging and, therefore, more densely populated cucumber plants. Also, when negative frequency dependence is operating, invading lower population sizes could be beneficial if invaders are ecologically distinct. This non-random dispersal probably vanished after the first months and it was replaced by random spill-over from cucumber to pepper.
Overall, and independently of the exact mechanism, establishment on the less challenging cucumber habitat was essential as homogeneous pepper populations became extinct. Therefore, we argue that the neighbouring populations on cucumber served as evolutionary stepping stones for evolutionary and ecological rescue [18]. The theory behind evolutionary stepping stones is illustrated by Bell & Gonzalez [21]: gradual adaptation maintains enough genetic variation for further adaptation. While probably a common mechanism in range expansions and invasions, empirical evidence is rare: Fitzpatrick et al. [20] performed an experiment with translocations of Trinidadian guppies where they investigated the impact on downstream native populations. They found clear evidence that even low levels of gene flow from different ecotypes can assist small populations through ecological and evolutionary rescue.
The level of dispersal changed during the course of the experiment. This was demonstrated by the steep growth in population density on pepper plants tracking the growth in population density in the source. Source–sink dynamics (sensu [9]) arose, in which the cucumber plants served as sources with a strongly positive ratio of birth and immigration relative to death and emigration, while the pepper plants functioned as sinks. This indicates that the initial adaptation to the challenging host plant through resource partitioning was counteracted by increasing levels of dispersal from the source. The enlarged dispersal occurred at a density of ten to fifteen adult females per square centimetre which is the same threshold found by Bitume et al. [37]. This is in line with both theoretical predictions and some empirical work: local adaptation strongly depends on dispersal rates and limited gene flow is favoured [7,14,40].
Interestingly, while the competing species was driven to extinction after about two months, we still found a negative effect on performance on the initial host plant after 10 months. It is surprising that we found no significant differences in the effects of competition on fecundity assessed on cucumber and pepper plants, as we expected maladaptation to new hosts to increase the effects of interspecific competition. Even though the competitor went extinct, ghost interactions are known to affect performance by a reduction in effective population size, and hence an increase in drift effects [52]. However, the differences in initial population sizes between both treatments could also have caused strong drift effects, thus overriding any signal from competition.
5. Conclusion
Our study demonstrates transient adaptation in colonization processes with episodes of higher potential for adaptation to extreme environments via a combination of ecological and evolutionary rescue. Persistent adaptation was however never achieved, presumably because it was eventually completely overruled by spill-over. While in a homogeneous challenging habitat none of the populations was able to establish, in heterogeneous habitats establishment did occur: less challenging habitat can host source populations that play an important role as evolutionary stepping stones for further adaptation to more challenging habitats, by generating moderate dispersal. Furthermore, we emphasize that too much dispersal is disadvantageous because it counteracts ongoing local adaptation, and that interspecific interactions impact these dynamics even beyond their extinction. To maintain biodiversity in our changing world, it is of utmost importance to understand which factors affect local adaptation. As populations are not able to establish in homogeneous landscapes consisting of marginal habitat, persistence can be ensured in intermediately connected landscapes where some patches of good quality may serve as enhancers of local adaptation to more marginal habitat.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Viki Vandomme, Angelica Alcantara, Pieter Vantieghem, Katrien Van Petegem, Stefano Masier, Matti Pisman, Mike Creutz, Hilde De Nil and Johan Bisschop for making the experiments possible. We also thank Raphaël Scherrer for the comments on a previous version of the manuscript and Sarah Magalhães for providing the strains of T. ludeni. We thank the Terrestrial Ecology department of Ghent University and the Centre for Ecology, Evolution and Environmental Changes of the University of Lisbon for the spider mite populations.
Data accessibility
The data and script can be found at: https://hdl.handle.net/10411/PLHOGQ.
Authors' contributions
The idea and the design of the study were developed by K.B., D.B. and R.S.E. The data were collected by K.B. and F.M. and the statistical analyses were done by K.B. K.B. wrote the draft of the manuscript and all authors contributed to revisions and discussions.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
D.B. and R.S.E. are supported by the FWO network EVENET (W0.003.16N) and FWO research grant no. G018017N. R.S.E. thanks the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) for financial support through a VICI grant (VICI grant no. 865.13.00). K.B. thanks the Special Research Fund (BOF) of Ghent University and the Ubbo Emmius sandwich program of the University of Groningen
References
- 1.Weiss-Lehman C, Tittes S, Kane NC, Hufbauer RA, Melbourne BA. 2019. Stochastic processes drive rapid genomic divergence during experimental range expansions. Proc. R. Soc. B 286, 20190231 ( 10.1098/rspb.2019.0231) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Szűcs M, Vahsen ML, Melbourne BA, Hoover C, Weiss-Lehman C, Hufbauer RA. 2017. Rapid adaptive evolution in novel environments acts as an architect of population range expansion. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 13 501–13 506. ( 10.1073/pnas.1712934114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Renault D, Laparie M, Mccauley SJ, Bonte D. 2017. Environmental adaptations, ecological filtering, and dispersal central to insect invasions. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 63, 345–368. ( 10.1146/annurev-ento-020117) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jeltsch F, et al. 2013. Integrating movement ecology with biodiversity research: exploring new avenues to address spatiotemporal biodiversity dynamics. Mov. Ecol. 1, 1–13. ( 10.1186/2051-3933-1-6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Amarasekare P. 2003. Competitive coexistence in spatially structured environments: a synthesis. Ecol. Lett. 6, 1109–1122. ( 10.1046/j.1461-0248.2003.00530.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kawecki TJ. 2000. Adaptation to marginal habitats: contrasting influence of the dispersal rate on the fate of alleles with small and large effects. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 1315–1320. ( 10.1098/rspb.2000.1144) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kawecki TJ, Ebert D. 2004. Conceptual issues in local adaptation. Ecol. Lett. 7, 1225–1241. ( 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00684.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kawecki TJ. 2008. Adaptation to marginal habitats. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 39, 321–342. ( 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.38.091206.095622) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Furrer RD, Pasinelli G. 2016. Empirical evidence for source–sink populations: a review on occurrence, assessments and implications. Biol. Rev. Camb. Phil. Soc. 91, 782–795. ( 10.1111/brv.12195) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fahrig L. 2002. Effect of habitat fragmentation on the extinction threshold: a synthesis. Ecol. Appl. 12, 346–353. ( 10.2307/3060946) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Legrand D, Cote J, Fronhofer EA, Holt RD, Ronce O, Schtickzelle N, Travis JMJ, Clobert J. 2017. Eco-evolutionary dynamics in fragmented landscapes. Ecography 40, 9–25. ( 10.1111/ecog.02537) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bonte D, Masier S, Mortier F. 2018. Eco-evolutionary feedbacks following changes in spatial connectedness. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 29, 64–70. ( 10.1016/J.COIS.2018.06.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bolnick DI, Nosil P. 2007. Natural selection in populations subject to a migration load. Evolution 61, 2229–2243. ( 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00179.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Alzate A, Bisschop K, Etienne RS, Bonte D. 2017. Interspecific competition counteracts negative effects of dispersal on adaptation of an arthropod herbivore to a new host. J. Evol. Biol. 30, 1966–1977. ( 10.1111/jeb.13123) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Chevin LM, Lande R. 2011. Adaptation to marginal habitats by evolution of increased phenotypic plasticity. J. Evol. Biol. 24, 1462–1476. ( 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02279.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ching J, Musheyev SA, Chowdhury D, Kim JA, Choi Y, Dennehy JJ. 2012. Migration enhances adaptation in bacteriophage populations evolving in ecological sinks. Evolution 67, 10–17. ( 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01742.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Forester BR, Jones MR, Joost S, Landguth EL, Lasky JR. 2016. Detecting spatial genetic signatures of local adaptation in heterogeneous landscapes. Mol. Ecol. 25, 104–120. ( 10.1111/mec.13476) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Brown JH, Kodric-Brown A. 1977. Turnover rates in insular biogeography: effect of immigration on extinction. Ecology 58, 445–449. ( 10.2307/1935620) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Carlson SM, Cunningham CJ, Westley PAH. 2014. Evolutionary rescue in a changing world. Trends Ecol. Evol. 29, 521–530. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2014.06.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Fitzpatrick SW, et al. 2016. Gene flow from an adaptively divergent source causes rescue through genetic and demographic factors in two wild populations of Trinidadian guppies. Evol. Appl. 9, 879–891. ( 10.1111/eva.12356) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bell G, Gonzalez A. 2011. Adaptation and evolutionary rescue in metapopulations experiencing environmental deterioration. Science 332, 1327–1330. ( 10.1126/science.1203105) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Parsons PA. 1990. The metabolic cost of multiple environmental stresses: implications for climatic change and conservation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 5, 315–317. ( 10.1016/0169-5347(90)90089-V) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ketola T, Mikonranta L, Zhang J, Saarinen K, Örmälä AM, Friman VP, Mappes J, Laakso J. 2013. Fluctuating temperature leads to evolution of thermal generalism and preadaptation to novel environments. Evolution 67, 2936–2944. ( 10.1111/evo.12148) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Turcotte MM, Corrin MSC, Johnson MTJ. 2012. Adaptive evolution in ecological communities. PLoS Biol. 10, e1001332 ( 10.1371/journal.-pbio.1001332) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Siepielski AM, Nemirov A, Cattivera M, Nickerson A. 2016. Experimental evidence for an eco-evolutionary coupling between local adaptation and intraspecific competition. Am. Nat. 187, 447–456. ( 10.1086/685295) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Osmond MM, de Mazancourt C. 2013. How competition affects evolutionary rescue. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20120085 ( 10.1098/rstb.2012.0085) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Johansson J. 2008. Evolutionary responses to environmental changes: how does competition affect adaptation? Evolution 62, 421–435. ( 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00301.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Law R, Daniel Morton R. 1996. Permanence and the assembly of ecological communities. Ecology 77, 762–775. ( 10.2307/2265500) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Miller TE, TerHorst CP, Burns JH. 2009. The ghost of competition present. Am. Nat. 173, 347–353. ( 10.1086/596531) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mallon CA, Le Roux X, Van Doorn GS, Dini-Andreote F, Poly F, Salles JF.. 2018. The impact of failure: unsuccessful bacterial invasions steer the soil microbial community away from the invader's niche. ISME J. 12, 728–741. ( 10.1038/s41396-017-0003-y) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zhang ZQ. 2003. Mites of greenhouses: identification, biology and control. Wallingford, UK: CABI Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Belliure B, Montserrat M, Magalhaes S. 2010. Mites as models for experimental evolution studies. Acarologia 50, 513–529. ( 10.1051/acarologia/20101985) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gould F. 1979. Rapid host range evolution in a population of the phytophagous mite Tetranychus urticae Koch. Evolution 33, 791–802. ( 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1979.tb04735.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Fry JD. 1989. Evolutionary adaptation to host plants in a laboratory population of the phytophagous mite Tetranychus urticae Koch. Oecologia 81, 559–565. ( 10.1007/BF00378969) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Magalhães S, Fayard J, Janssen A, Carbonell D, Olivieri I. 2007. Adaptation in a spider mite population after long-term evolution on a single host plant. J. Evol. Biol. 20, 2016–2027. ( 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01365.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bonte D, De Roissart A, Vandegehuchte ML, Ballhorn DJ, van Leeuwen T, de la Peña E.. 2010. Local adaptation of aboveground herbivores towards plant phenotypes induced by soil biota. PLoS ONE 5, e11174 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0011174) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bitume EV, Bonte D, Ronce O, Bach F, Flaven E, Olivieri I, Nieberding CM. 2013. Density and genetic relatedness increase dispersal distance in a subsocial organism. Ecol. Lett. 16, 430–437. ( 10.1111/ele.12057) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Van Petegem K, Moerman F, Dahirel M, Fronhofer EA, Vandegehuchte ML, Van Leeuwen T, Wybouw N, Stoks R, Bonte D.. 2018. Kin competition accelerates experimental range expansion in an arthropod herbivore. Ecol. Lett. 21, 225–234. ( 10.1111/ele.12887) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Magalhães S, Blanchet E, Egas M, Olivieri I. 2009. Are adaptation costs necessary to build up a local adaptation pattern? BMC Evol. Biol. 9, 182 ( 10.1186/1471-2148-9-182) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Alzate A, Etienne RS, Bonte D. 2018. Experimental island biogeography demonstrates the importance of island size and dispersal for the adaptation to novel habitats. Glob. Ecol. Biogeogr. 28, 1–10. ( 10.1111/geb.12846) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Magalhães S, Blanchet E, Egas M, Olivieri I. 2011. Environmental effects on the detection of adaptation. J. Evol. Biol. 24, 2653–2662. ( 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02388.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kawecki TJ, Lenski RE, Ebert D, Hollis B, Olivieri I, Whitlock MC. 2012. Experimental evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 547–560. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2012.06.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Brooks ME, Kristensen K, van Benthem KJ, Magnusson A, Berg CW, Nielsen A, Skaug HJ, Mächler M, Bolker BM.. 2017. glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R J. 9, 378–400. ( 10.3929/ETHZ-B-000240890) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Barton K. 2018. MuMIn. R package version 1.42.1.
- 45.Lenth R. 2019. Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R package version 1.3.2.
- 46.Delignette-Muller ML. 2015. fitdistrplus: an R package for fitting distributions. J. Stat. Softw. 64, 1–34. ( 10.18637/jss.v064.i04) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Trapletti A, Hornik K. 2018. tseries: time series analysis and computational finance. R package version 0.10-45.
- 48.Wang IJ, Bradburd GS. 2014. Isolation by environment. Mol. Ecol. 23, 5649–5662. ( 10.1111/mec.12938) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Jacob S, Legrand D, Chaine AS, Bonte D, Schtickzelle N, Huet M, Clobert J. 2017. Gene flow favours local adaptation under habitat choice in ciliate microcosms. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 1407–1410. ( 10.1038/s41559-017-0269-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bonte D, De Roissart A, Wybouw N, Van Leeuwen T.. 2014. Fitness maximization by dispersal: evidence from an invasion experiment. Ecology 95, 3104–3111. ( 10.1890/13-2269.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Calcagno V, Mouquet N, Jarne P, David P. 2006. Coexistence in a metacommunity: the competition-colonization trade-off is not dead. Ecol. Lett. 9, 897–907. ( 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2006.00930.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bisschop K, Mortier F, Bonte D, Etienne R. 2019. Performance in a novel environment subject to ghost competition. bioRxiv, 515833 ( 10.1101/515833) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data and script can be found at: https://hdl.handle.net/10411/PLHOGQ.