Abstract
Recent evidence suggests that mesoscopic neural oscillations measured via intracranial electroencephalography exhibit spatial representations, which were previously only observed at the micro- and macroscopic level of brain organization. Specifically, theta (and gamma) oscillations correlate with movement, speed, distance, specific locations, and goal proximity to boundaries. In entorhinal cortex, they exhibit hexadirectional modulation, which is putatively linked to grid cell activity. Understanding this mesoscopic neural code is crucial because information represented by oscillatory power and phase may complement the information content at other levels of brain organization. Mesoscopic neural oscillations help bridge the gap between single-neuron and macroscopic brain signals of spatial navigation and may provide a mechanistic basis for novel biomarkers and therapeutic targets to treat diseases causing spatial disorientation.
Keywords: Neural representation, spatial navigation, memory, oscillations, intracranial EEG, grid cells
Towards a multi-level neural code of spatial navigation
Spatial navigation is a core ability of most animals and humans [1]. Successful navigation requires highly specialized neural representations (see Glossary) that encode information about the environment’s shape and content, neural codes that reflect the location, direction, and speed of the navigating organism, and neural mechanisms that underlie goal-directed behavior within the environment. These spatial representations have been identified at various levels of brain organization ranging from place, head direction, border, and grid cells [2–7] up to the macroscopic level of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), which revealed large-scale representations of locations [8], environments [9], directions [10–12], borders [13], and grid-like hexadirectional orientation [14–16].
Traditionally, there has been a gap between studies of spatial representations at the levels of single neurons and fMRI brain networks. This review focuses on the potential of intracranial electroencephalography (iEEG) recordings of local field potentials (LFPs) to bridge this gap, by providing spatial information at the mesoscopic level of relatively small neural assemblies (Box 1). These recordings have provided evidence for oscillatory signatures of multiple key components of spatial navigation, ranging from more general signals of movement, speed, and distance, to representations of specific spatial locations, goal proximity to boundaries, and very recently, to mesoscopic grid-like representations. First, we will review these results and discuss their relationship to the neural representations of space obtained at the microscopic, single-neuron level and the macroscopic level as observed with fMRI. Next, we will elaborate on the basic relationships between single-neuron spiking activity, LFPs, and the fMRI blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signal in order to address the question of how mesoscopic spatial representations relate to their microscopic and macroscopic counterparts. We will finally discuss the functional and clinical relevance of mesoscopic representations during spatial navigation.
Box 1. Detecting and analyzing mesoscopic brain oscillations.
Since the discovery of the scalp electroencephalography (EEG) by Hans Berger in the 1920s it has been known that cognition is accompanied by brain oscillations across a broad range of frequencies. EEG oscillations are classically divided into frequency bands: delta (1–4Hz), theta (4–8Hz), alpha (8–13Hz), beta (13–30Hz), low gamma (30–50Hz), and high gamma (>50Hz). Although it is more common to record these signals from the scalp, in rare circumstances researchers are able to measure these oscillations directly with high spatiotemporal resolution from the human brain when neurosurgical patients are implanted with intracranial depth, strip, or grid electrodes for clinical purposes (Fig. I). Contacts from these intracranial electroencephalographic (iEEG) electrodes record neuronal activity within a radius of several millimeters when electrode contacts are relatively large (macroelectrodes) [138]. Some patients are implanted with so-called microelectrodes which have smaller conductive surfaces and record neural signals from a region within a few hundred micrometers [98]. These microelectrode recordings also allow for the detection of multi- and single-unit spiking activity. During each patient’s implantation period, they can perform experimental tasks in order to identify neural activity related to cognitive functions [139]. Because iEEG patients are usually bedridden, these studies often probe spatial navigation via virtual navigation tasks performed on a bedside computer, although differences to real-world navigation have to be taken into account. During the analysis of iEEG data, both univariate and multivariate approaches (such as multivariate pattern analysis [140] and representational similarity analysis [141]) have been used effectively to identify general and feature-specific neural representations during spatial navigation.
Navigating the landscape of mesoscopic spatial representations
Movement, speed, time, and distance
Theta oscillations (Box 2) reliably appear during movement in the hippocampus of both rodents [17] and humans [18], although some characteristics of this signal differ between species. First, theta oscillations are more stable in rodents than in humans, in which theta oscillations occur in bursts of several cycles. Second, theta frequency may usually appear higher in rodents (4–10Hz) than in humans (1–4Hz) [19,20], possibly because larger anatomical assemblies tend to synchronize at lower frequencies [21]. However, this latter difference may also be attributed to real-world navigation versus virtual navigation [22,23] or to more frequent electrode placement in anterior parts of the human hippocampus (where theta oscillations may have a lower frequency) [24].
Box 2. Basics of theta oscillations.
Despite decades of research, the process of how hippocampal theta oscillations are generated is still under debate. Different models draw a complex picture of contributing brain regions, cell types, transmitters, and receptors, in which the medial septum and the diagonal band of Broca (MS-DBB) occupy major roles [17].
The classic model proposes that theta oscillations are generated by hippocampal pyramidal cells that receive inhibitory and excitatory input from the MS-DBB and the EC/perforant path, respectively (Fig. I). While the inhibitory input is induced by glutamatergic [143] and cholinergic [144] MS-DBB neurons and, via hippocampal interneurons, leads to inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs) at the soma of the hippocampal pyramidal cells, the excitatory input results in excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) at their distal dendritic region. The summed activity of somatic IPSPs and distal dendritic EPSPs then generates theta oscillations with specific amplitudes and phases [17].
Later work suggested multiple modifications of this model [17], of which we will describe three. First, different types of theta oscillations presumably emerge from different mechanisms. For example, atropine-sensitive theta oscillations, which appear during consummatory behaviors, may reflect the CA3 recurrent collateral system, whereas atropine-resistant theta oscillations, which correlate with movement, may be conveyed by entorhinal input. Second, interneuron activity driving the somatic IPSPs may not only depend on MS-DBB inputs, but also on CA1 recurrent collaterals, CA3 afferents, entorhinal afferents, and other interneurons with intrinsic oscillatory properties. These interneurons may consist of multiple classes [145] that target different parts of hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Third, the recurrent circuit of CA3 may form an intrinsic intrahippocampal theta generator and pyramidal neurons endowed with intrinsic properties to oscillate at theta frequency may produce single-cell theta oscillations.
Various potential functions have been proposed for hippocampal theta oscillations. Amongst others, these include information transfer between regions [17] and gating of information flow [146]. Furthermore, theta oscillations may enable the grouping and segregation of neuronal assemblies that were activated at different phases of the theta cycle, for example, during working memory [147]. Theta oscillations may also support mnemonic processes by inducing synaptic modifications of intrahippocampal associational pathways [148] and may allow the creation of temporally ordered memories [149]. Finally, theta oscillations may underlie phase coding by allowing single neurons to represent information by varying the timing of their spiking relative to theta oscillations during perception, navigation, and memory [29].
Another oscillatory signal that is involved in spatial navigation is the hippocampal gamma oscillation (30–100Hz), which increases in power during movement [25,26] and interacts with theta oscillations, such that gamma power varies across the theta cycle [25,27]. This phenomenon of cross-frequency coupling between gamma power and theta phase generalizes to other cognitive domains [28] and is thought to reflect phase coding [29], in order to, e.g., bind multiple stimuli into sequences.
In rodents, faster movement through an environment is associated with increases in both power [30] and frequency [31] of theta oscillations. Similar results have been obtained in human iEEG recordings, during virtual navigation [19] and during active walking [22,23]. This mesoscopic representation of speed is paralleled by single neurons in the hippocampus and medial EC that linearly increase their firing rate with speed [32,33]. A glutamatergic septal-hippocampal circuit that controls the initiation and velocity of locomotion [34] presumably occupies a key role in coordinating these single-neuron and mesoscopic representations of speed, which may nevertheless be dissociable from each other [35]. Functionally, increased theta frequencies during faster movements may serve to normalize the phase resolution within the theta cycle with respect to higher sampling rates of sensory information during faster movements [34].
In order to calculate travelled distance, speed must be integrated over time. Therefore, neural representations of time are likely important partners of spatial representations – particularly for accomplishing path integration [36]. In rodents, so-called “time cells”, which activate at particular time points in task intervals, are present in both the hippocampus [37] and in lateral EC [38]. At the macroscopic level, hippocampal BOLD patterns reflect the processing of temporal intervals within sequences [39] and precise temporal memories are associated with increased fMRI activity in lateral EC [40]. Interestingly, a neural signature of time may also be visible at the mesoscopic level, since rodent hippocampal theta oscillations exhibit increased power in a temporal discrimination task [41].
Since theta oscillations are related to the processing of movement, speed, and time, it is not surprising that they also contain information about distance travelled: theta power increases during long- versus short-distance “teleportation” during virtual navigation [42]. Similarly, theta oscillations are stronger before and during long versus short paths [43]. Increased theta power may serve to avoid cognitive overload due to increased sensory input during longer paths – for example by shortening the fraction of the theta cycle available for information processing and, thus, excluding less relevant information from being processed [44]. More broadly, these two studies indicate that theta oscillations are involved in the planning and initiation of movement, a finding that is paralleled by results in rodents [34]. Furthermore, an internal sense of movement seems to be sufficient for evoking hippocampal theta oscillations since they occur during virtual teleportation [42]. At the macroscopic level, hippocampal fMRI activity reflects distances between places [45] and path distance to goal-locations [46], but (to our knowledge) no fMRI study directly examined the BOLD correlates of previously travelled distance so far.
Locations, borders, and grids
Place cells fire whenever animals or humans are at a specific location within a spatial environment [2,3]. Place cells are mainly observed in the hippocampus [2,3], but they may also exist in extrahippocampal regions [47]. In addition, particularly during sharp-wave ripples, place cell activity often reflects future positions [48], and place-cell sequences “preplay” future paths to goal-locations [49]. Furthermore, place cells activate sequentially within theta cycles, allowing theta cycles to simulate time-compressed trajectories through space [50]. Interestingly, mesoscopic oscillations provide additional information about self-location when place cell firing phases are taken into account: Adding information about the theta phase at the moment of place-cell firing increases one’s ability to reconstruct the animal’s location [51,52]. As an extension of these findings, a rat’s position can be accurately decoded from the spatiotemporal structure of the theta rhythm obtained from multi-site, high-density hippocampal LFP recordings, without utilizing information from place-cell spiking activity [53]. These mesoscopic studies in rodents demonstrate that (multisite) oscillatory theta power and phase contain information regarding self-location. They are complemented by fMRI studies, which suggest that current and future locations can be decoded from human hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex [8,54]. However, the possibility of finding reliable macroscopic representations of location in the hippocampus has been challenged [55] and their relationship to single place cells is unclear, because the hippocampus presumably employs sparse coding and hippocampal place cells lack a clear topographical organization [56–58].
Complementing place cells, border cells in the medial EC [5] and boundary-vector cells in the subiculum [59] activate adjacent to or at specific distances from a border, respectively. These cells may be instrumental in anchoring place [60] and grid fields [61] to an allocentric reference frame. At the macroscopic level, border representations were identified in the parahippocampal place area and retrosplenial cortex [13], and transcranial magnetic stimulation demonstrated a causal role of the occipital place area in representing environmental boundaries [62]. While a direct mesoscopic signal for borders has yet to be found, a closely related representation was recently identified in human subiculum using iEEG: when subjects encoded goal-locations near boundaries, power in the theta frequency range (4–10Hz) was stronger as compared to the encoding of goal-locations in the center of the virtual environment [63]. Of note, this study examined the distance between borders and the subject’s goal-locations (i.e., not the person’s own locations), and thus provide further evidence that the brain’s representations of space generalize from the self to others and also to inanimate objects for which an exact localization is important [64].
Grid cells fire at the vertices of an internally generated grid tiling the entire environment into equilateral triangles [6,7]. They were mainly observed in medial EC so far, where about 10–20% of all neurons are grid cells [33,65]. Grid cells may help to compute vector relationships and distances between spatial locations [66] – presumably constituting the neural basis for path integration [16,67]. In fMRI, there is evidence for macroscopic grid-like representations (i.e., hexadirectional modulation of the BOLD signal as a function of movement direction) in healthy participants who were scanned as they moved through a virtual environment [14–16]. FMRI grid-like representations were observed in EC, but also in extrahippocampal regions such as medial prefrontal cortex and parietal cortex ([14]; Supplemental Information, Text S1 and Table S1), warranting a brain-wide examination of grid cell density.
Crucially, two recent iEEG studies revealed grid-like representations at the mesoscopic level of brain organization, i.e., a hexadirectional modulation of entorhinal theta power by movement direction in epilepsy patients who performed a virtual navigation task [68,69]. Both studies found that this hexadirectional modulation was restricted to the EC and not present in neighboring similarly sized regions (hippocampus, amygdala) and other control regions, consistent with single-neuron [7] and fMRI recordings [14,15]. Furthermore, both studies showed that the effect was specific to six-fold rotational symmetry and specific to the theta frequency range. Importantly, both studies found a link between grid-like representations and spatial memory performance: Stronger hexadirectional modulation correlated positively with better spatial memory performance [69], conceptually in agreement with previous fMRI studies [14–16,70], and stronger grid-like representations were demonstrated in later as compared to earlier parts of the experiment after significant learning had occurred [68]. Furthermore, stronger hexadirectional modulation was revealed in parts of the environment closer to the environmental boundary of the virtual arena [68], potentially related to stabilized grid cell firing via error-correcting border cell input after encounters with environmental boundaries [61]. Moreover, mesoscopic grid-like representations were particularly observed during fast movements [68], in line with prior fMRI studies [14,15] and potentially related to speed modulation of grid cells and conjunctive grid by head-direction cells [14,71].
Mesoscopic grid-like representations were also observed in broad-band gamma activity: Building upon evidence for grid cell activity [72] and fMRI grid-like representations [73,74] during visual exploration in monkeys and humans, respectively, a recent study searched for modulations of oscillatory power as a function of gaze direction [75]. Indeed, hexadirectionally modulated signals were evident in broadband gamma activity (60–120Hz), both in MEG and in one epilepsy patient with iEEG electrodes in the EC.
Taken together, unique patterns of brain oscillations emerge when animals and humans navigate spatial environments, with theta [17–20,22–24,30,31,34,35,42,43,50,76] and gamma oscillations [25–27] being associated most often with characteristics of spatial navigation such as movement, speed, and distance. Crucially, recent evidence demonstrates that mesoscale neural networks oscillating in these frequency bands provide representations of locations, proximity of goals to boundaries, and grid-like hexadirectional orientation that were previously only observed at the microscopic or macroscopic level (Fig. 1, Key Figure). It is notable that, in particular, theta [51–53,63,68,69] and gamma oscillations [75] stand out as carriers of this feature-specific information. Although several of these findings are preliminary, and have not yet been demonstrated across species, their possible functional significance merits further investigation. Particularly, a thorough brain-wide examination of mesoscopic grid-like representations would be of interest, complementary to the brain-wide examination of grid cell density. Grid-like representations outside the EC may perform complementary computations for spatial navigation or they may support other cognitive functions that exploit similar computational principles as spatial navigation [66,77]. Furthermore, it remains elusive how the different scales of spatial representations are interconnected. This question is closely related to the fundamental issue of basic relationships between spiking activity, oscillatory power, and the BOLD signal.
Common grounds for multi-level spatial representations?
Basic relationships between spiking activity, LFPs, and BOLD
Although it is widely agreed that links between spiking activity, LFP power, and the BOLD signal exist (Fig. 2), the details of these interrelations remain elusive. Here, we summarize what is currently known and propose that deepening this knowledge will boost our understanding of how spatial codes can coexist at multiple levels of brain organization.
Various studies on the relationship between LFP power and spiking activity revealed that spiking activity correlates positively with broadband power and gamma power, in both human [78] and monkey neocortex [79,80], with high gamma (>80Hz) power being particularly tightly correlated with spiking activity in some studies [80,81]. Of note, neural synchrony may drive increases in gamma power to a greater extent than firing rate [80]. By contrast, low-frequency oscillations often have a negative relationship to spiking activity, as demonstrated for delta, theta, and alpha power in human and monkey neocortex [78,79]. In certain brain regions including the hippocampal area, the relationship between LFP power and spiking activity is even more complex. For example, in some studies spiking activity did not correlate with either broadband, theta-band, or gamma-band LFP power in human hippocampus and EC [82,83]. Such uncoupling between spiking activity and LFPs may be explained by multiple factors including reduced spike synchronization [82,84], pronounced recurrent processing, and high interneuron density [85,86]. Cell morphology and orientation, topological organization, and stimulus characteristics constitute additional determinants of the relationship between spiking activity and LFP power [87–89]. Notably, the spatial extent of gamma power is sometimes presumably too local [89] to establish indirect relationships between spiking activity and BOLD (given that gamma power is also associated with BOLD, see below).
Spiking activity is not only associated with oscillatory power, but is also modulated by the phase of neural oscillations, as demonstrated via the phenomena of phase locking [90] and phase precession [91]. Phase locking reflects the higher excitability of neurons during phase ranges that correspond to more depolarized membrane states. Since both theta [92] and alpha [44] oscillations putatively reflect rhythmically changing levels of inhibition, phase-locked neurons tend to fire during oscillatory troughs, which correspond to lower levels of inhibition. During rodent navigation, phase locking occurs in place cells [93], speed cells [33], grid cells [94], and less strongly in border cells [95]. In human virtual navigation, phase locking to oscillations in the delta, theta, and gamma frequency bands has been observed [96].
Phase precession describes the phenomenon in which a neuron fires at successively earlier phases of an ongoing oscillation, possibly resulting from an interaction of (theta) oscillatory inhibition and an (asymmetric) rate code, which can be induced, for example, when moving through the firing field of a place cell [92]. In rodents, both place [91] and grid cells [94] exhibit phase precession, while evidence for phase precession in humans is still missing. Various functions have been suggested for phase precession, including information processing via a phase code in which information is represented by the oscillatory phase at which a cell or cellular assembly activates [29,92].
Relating electrophysiology and BOLD via simultaneous iEEG/fMRI recordings is challenging for practical and safety issues [97]. Regarding the relationship between spiking activity and BOLD, simultaneous recordings in sensory neocortex showed that spiking activity and BOLD correlate less tightly than mean LFPs (in particular, gamma-band power) and BOLD [98]. Consecutive and parallel studies in both neocortex and hippocampus revealed variable results ranging from no correlation [83,99] and low degrees of concordance [100] to clear positive relationships between spiking activity and BOLD [101]. Again, divergent findings may be explained by region-specific recurrent processing and interneuron activity that cause dissociations between spiking activity and BOLD. In addition, region-specific neurovascular coupling [85,86] may account for inverse relationships between spiking activity and BOLD in situations when the ratio between blood flow and oxygen metabolism is atypically low [85].
Hence, given these caveats, the BOLD signal is generally thought to correlate more strongly with LFPs than with spiking activity [85,102]: Examining the relationship between high-frequency oscillations and BOLD, simultaneous recordings in monkey visual cortex showed pronounced positive correlations between BOLD and gamma power ([98]; for similar results in various regions and species, see e.g., [103,104]). These results were corroborated by consecutive recordings in human neocortex [105], as well as by parallel experiments in healthy participants and epilepsy patients in the medial temporal lobe [106]. Regarding low-frequency oscillations, simultaneous LFP/BOLD recordings showed negative relationships between BOLD and delta power in rat striatum [107], and between BOLD and alpha/beta power in human motor cortex [104]. Consecutive iEEG/fMRI studies showed similar negative associations between BOLD and beta power, however with pronounced differences between brain regions [108], again in line with the notion that the local circuitry and region-specific neurovascular coupling are modulating factors [85]. Particularly the investigation of theta power and BOLD in the hippocampal formation may lead to variable results, perhaps because this relationship is mediated by the presence or absence of theta/gamma-coupling [104], which depends on ongoing cognitive demands [109]. For example, theta (4–8Hz) power was found to correlate positively with BOLD in the parahippocampal region (including EC) but not in hippocampus [83].
Emergence of meso- and macroscopic spatial representations
Due to the complex associations between spiking activity, LFPs, and BOLD, it is under intense debate how meso- and macroscopic representations of space relate to the activity of spatially selective single neurons. Potential answers to this question have been suggested mainly for grid cells in the context of spatial representations. Apart from their main feature of firing fields being arranged with six-fold rotational symmetry, these cells exhibit unique functional and anatomical properties (Fig. 3A) that may facilitate the emergence of meso-/macroscopic grid-like representations. Specifically, grid cells are organized in a small number of modules [110] with grid cells of the same module exhibiting similar orientation, spacing, and theta modulation [6,110]. In the following paragraphs, we describe four potential explanations for how the features of grid cell spiking activity relate to hexadirectionally modulated meso-/macroscopic neural signals. All hypotheses assume that, across the grid cell population, the same number of firing fields (centers and peripheries) are traversed along paths of similar distance at any direction.
The “conjunctive grid by head-direction cell hypothesis” (Fig. 3B) builds upon prior research which suggested that the firing of conjunctive grid by head-direction cells located in deeper layers of the EC and in pre- and parasubiculum [71] is aligned with the grid axes ([14]; but see [111]). Assuming that the directional tuning width of these conjunctive grid by head-direction cells is not too broad, movements aligned with the grid axes (as compared to misaligned movements) will result in increased spiking activity of the conjunctive grid by head-direction cell population – thus causing the appearance of meso-/macroscopic grid-like representations [14]. Of note, (i) the hexadirectional modulation of oscillatory power or BOLD reflects the activity of the population of conjunctive grid by head-direction cells (rather than the grid cells themselves) in this scenario, (ii) such hexadirectional modulation may also occur when the conjunctive grid by head-direction cells are aligned with the grid axes plus a consistent angular offset, and (iii) pure head direction cells whose preferred firing directions are 60° offset from each other may also result in hexadirectional modulation of oscillatory power or BOLD.
The “repetition hypothesis” (Fig. 3C) is based on the assumption that the phenomenon of repetition suppression, i.e., neural activity being reduced for repeated stimuli [112], holds true in the context of grid cells [14,72]. Relatively fewer different grid cells are activated more often during aligned movements and relatively more different grid cells are activated less often during misaligned movements. Thus, during aligned movements there would be a higher degree of repetition suppression at the level of spiking activity (which to the best of our knowledge has yet to be confirmed) and/or LFP power and/or BOLD signal (i.e., fMRI adaptation) as compared to misaligned movements – resulting in grid-like representations. Repetition suppression at the level of spiking activity may lead to hexadirectional modulation of LFP power in the theta band, since grid cells exhibit phase locking and phase precession with respect to theta oscillations (Supplemental Information, Text S2 and Fig. S1).
Regarding the “structure-function mapping hypothesis” (Fig. 3D), recent evidence demonstrates that the firing fields of anatomically adjacent grid cells not only have similar spacing and orientation [110], but also a similar phase offset to a reference location [113]. Therefore, recordings from a small area of the EC may sample grid cells with similar firing fields. Such a grid cell population shows higher average firing rates during aligned movements (since more firing fields are traversed) versus misaligned movements, possibly resulting in meso-/macroscopic grid-like representations. Of note, the anatomical area over which adjacent grid cells share a common phase offset may be markedly smaller than the area that is sampled by iEEG or standard fMRI – suggesting that it could be useful to examine fMRI grid-like representations at the high spatial resolution achievable with high-field (7 Tesla) fMRI [114]. Recording from a restricted area of the EC may nevertheless sample grid cells that are biased towards similar phase offsets, enabling the detection of grid-like representations at lower resolution.
The previous three hypotheses assume consistent (positive or negative) relationships between spiking activity, LFP theta power, and BOLD in EC – an assumption for which there is currently only limited evidence (Fig. 2E; [83]) and that should be further clarified in future studies (particularly via simultaneous iEEG/fMRI recordings). By contrast, the “independence hypothesis” states that six-fold rotational symmetry occurs independently at different levels of brain organization. This lack of a common neural basis does not exclude a biological and behavioral relevance of meso-/macroscopic grid-like representations, and removes the need for explaining how a limited number of grid cells in medial EC (about 10–20% [33,65], see also [115]) can result in a meso-/macroscopically visible neural signal. Of note, all hypotheses imply that meso- and macroscopic grid-like representations may also be observable in animal models. Furthermore, combinations of two or more of the hypotheses presented above may hold true – either amplifying or attenuating each other.
Redundant or complementary information systems?
Do mesoscopic representations simply mirror the spatial information of single-neuron representations or do they provide independent information? Findings in rodents and humans favor the latter option, but it currently remains elusive whether mesoscopic spatial representations are indeed part of the neural code for spatial navigation or whether they are epiphenomena.
For example, the accuracy of estimating a rat’s position can be improved when information about the LFP phases at which place cells fire is added to the information obtained from place cell firing alone [51]. Similarly, the LFP phases at which a place cell fires and its firing rate can independently represent the animal’s location within the place field and its movement speed through the field, respectively [52]. Additionally, spike sequences of place cells within theta cycles can be used to predict movement direction [116]. Regarding speed, single-neuron representations and mesoscopic representations may contribute independent information, as they are dissociable from each other analytically [32] and physiologically, via inactivation of the medial septum [35]. Furthermore, phase-specific neuronal firing encodes information about a patient’s prospective navigational goal in the absence of firing rate changes [117], and human single-neuron representations of specific passengers or landmarks in a virtual navigation task were shown to be uncorrelated with theta- and gamma-band LFP representations of these stimuli [82]. These studies suggest that micro- and mesoscopic brain signals may carry (partially) independent information and propose that different levels of brain organization make independent contributions to the neural code of spatial navigation. Specifically, as shaped out in this review, mesoscopic theta oscillations may constitute an important partner of microscopic spatial representations. Since several different features relevant to spatial navigation are all represented by theta power, they may jointly shape the functional role of theta for representing and integrating multiple pieces of information.
Behavioral and clinical relevance of multi-level spatial representations
Having described how features of navigation and space relate to micro-, meso-, and macroscopic brain signals, examining their relevance for behavioral performance is important. At the microscopic level, experimentally induced shifts of place fields were associated with impaired spatial behavior [118], increased place field stability correlated with better task performance [119], and disrupting grid cell activity reduced the accuracy of path integration [67]. At the mesoscopic level, abolition of the hippocampal theta rhythm impaired spatial memory [120] and stronger speed modulation of hippocampal theta frequency correlated with better performance [121]. The strength of mesoscopic grid-like representations correlated positively with spatial memory [69] and increased after learning [68]. Similarly, macroscopic grid-like representations in EC and medial prefrontal cortex showed positive associations with memory performance [14–16,70]. Moreover, stronger goal direction signals in human EC correlated with better task accuracy [11]. At the purely behavioral level, grid cell properties predicted biases in human path integration [122]. However, no study demonstrated six-fold rotational symmetry (in contrast to four-fold symmetry [123]) at the behavioral level to date. Future studies are needed to corroborate these findings on how multi-level spatial codes support specific aspects of spatial memory performance – e.g., via optogenetics or deep brain stimulation.
Behavioral relationships between neural representations of space and spatial memory performance suggest that our ability to understand and treat neurological and psychiatric diseases whose symptoms include spatial disorientation will benefit if we have a detailed fundamental understanding of the neural representations underlying spatial navigation and how they are disrupted from neuropathologies. Particularly, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) may lead to subtle changes in navigational strategies and altered neural representations of space already in very early disease stages [124]. Specific types of spatial representations may be impaired at consecutive AD stages, following either the spreading of neurofibrillary tangles or the development of amyloid β deposition – possibly enabling a “neurocognitive Braak staging” [125]. AD mouse models already revealed the adverse impact of tau pathology on grid cell functioning [126] and the degradation of place cell firing due to amyloid plaque burden [127], both pathological changes being accompanied by spatial memory deficits. Besides, AD mouse models identified disrupted phase locking of place cells to hippocampal theta and gamma oscillations [128] as well as reduced cross-frequency coupling of gamma to theta oscillations in medial EC [129]. The investigation of the associations between AD neuropathology and multi-level spatial codes will both enable mechanistic explanations of symptoms and allow the establishment of early biomarkers for AD [124,130,131] such as reduced grid-like representations [15]. In this respect, establishing meso- and macroscopic representations of space has also practical relevance, because single-neuron spatial representations can be measured from the human brain only under rare circumstances.
Concluding remarks
Mesoscopic representations of space based on electrophysiological recordings in humans point towards a multi-level neural code for spatial navigation. These mesoscopic representations exhibit complex connections to spatial representations based on single neurons or on macroscopic fMRI patterns, and may yet mediate the relationship between these two other levels of brain organization. Follow-up studies should thus further clarify the emergence of meso-/macroscopic spatial representations and show how their information content relates to single-neuron activity. Future research may go beyond the two-dimensional spatial domain and examine to which extent mesoscopic brain signals represent three-dimensional spatial information [132–134], support navigation through other n-dimensional “spaces” [66,70,77,135–137], and provide biomarkers for neurological and psychiatric diseases (see Outstanding Questions).
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Neural representations of spatial navigation have mainly been studied at the microscopic level of single neurons or at the macroscopic level of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).
Recent intracranial EEG recordings in epilepsy patients revealed neural representations of spatial features including travelled distance, goal-proximity to boundaries, and grid-like hexadirectional orientation. These representations occur at the mesoscopic level of brain oscillations, particularly in the theta frequency band.
Mesoscopic representations of space bridge the gap between their micro- and macroscopic counterparts. Experimentally testable scenarios may explain how the mesoscopic spatial representations relate to single-neuron firing, other neural oscillations, and fMRI signals.
Neural spatial representations may offer novel tools for biomarkers of neurological and psychiatric diseases.
Acknowledgements
L.K. was supported by the BMBF (01GQ1705A), NSF grant BCS-1724243, and NIH grant 563386. S.M. and J.J. were supported by NIH Grants MH061975 and MH104606, and the National Science Foundation (BCS-1724243). L.W. was supported by the Strategic Priority Research Program of Chinese Academy of Science (XDB32010300), the Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Commission (Z171100000117014), CAS Interdisciplinary Innovation Team (JCTD-2018-07), and the Natural Science Foundation of China (81422024, 31771255). N.A. received funding by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) - Projektnummer 316803389 - SFB 1280 as well as via Projektnummer 122679504 - SFB 874.
Glossary
- Allocentric reference frame
representation of the spatial environment with regard to features of the external world independent of the moving subject; typically contrasted with an egocentric reference frame that is centered on the moving subject.
- Blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signal
signal recorded using fMRI that – by measuring blood oxygenation in brain voxels – allows indirect conclusions about the neural activity underlying changes in blood oxygenation.
- Frequency bands
canonical classes that summarize neural oscillations based on their frequency: delta (<4Hz), theta (4–8Hz), alpha (8–13Hz), beta (13–30Hz), and gamma (>30Hz).
- Grid cell
neuron whose firing field pattern consists of a regular grid that tessellates the entire environment into equilateral triangles. Mainly observed in medial entorhinal cortex and presumably a key component of path integration. The firing pattern exhibits six-fold rotational symmetry and may thus be the source of hexadirectional modulation of meso- and macroscopic brain signals.
- Hexadirectional modulation
characteristic of a neural signal (e.g., theta power or BOLD amplitude) to vary as a function of a circular variable (e.g., movement direction) after having transformed the circular variable from the classical 360° space into a 60° space.
- Intracranial electroencephalography (iEEG)
technique to measure electrical activity via surgically implanted electrodes from inside the skull. Electrodes comprise grids and strips attached to the surface of the brain (electrocorticography, ECoG) as well as depth electrodes inserted into the brain (stereotactic electroencephalography, SEEG).
- Local field potentials (LFPs)
electrophysiological signals that integrate neural activity within localized brain areas. Can be measured via iEEG that records the LFP from a few hundred micrometers up to a couple of millimeters depending on electrode size.
- Neural representations
patterns of neural activity related to one specific stimulus of the external world or one specific mental content. Here we define “macroscopic neural representations” as those measured via fMRI, scalp-EEG, or MEG, “mesoscopic neural representations” as those obtained via iEEG, and “microscopic neural representation” as those occurring at the level of single neurons.
- Oscillations
rhythmic fluctuations of neural activity, characterized by frequency (cycles per second), power (squared amplitude), and phase (momentary deflection angle). In general, neural oscillations bias input selection, temporally link neurons into assemblies, and facilitate synaptic plasticity, mechanisms that cooperatively support temporal representation and long-term consolidation of information.
- Phase coding
neural mechanism by which information is represented via the phase of a (slow) oscillation at which activity of single neurons or cellular assemblies occurs.
- Phase locking (here in the sense of spike-field coherence)
phenomenon that a neuron fires at identical phases of a concurrent oscillation across time.
- Phase precession
phenomenon that a neuron fires at successively earlier LFP phases across time.
- Virtual navigation task
in a weak sense, computerized task that lets the subject explore a virtual environment without the need of real-world movement. In a strong sense, task that allows subjects to navigate virtual environments by actual body movement.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Chersi F and Burgess N (2015) The Cognitive Architecture of Spatial Navigation: Hippocampal and Striatal Contributions. Neuron 88, 64–77 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.O’Keefe J and Dostrovsky J (1971) The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 34, 171–175 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ekstrom AD et al. (2003) Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature 425, 184–188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Taube JS et al. (1990) Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis. J. Neurosci 10, 420–35 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Solstad T et al. (2008) Representation of geometric borders in the entorhinal cortex. Science 322, 1865–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hafting T et al. (2005) Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature 436, 801–806 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Jacobs J et al. (2013) Direct recordings of grid-like neuronal activity in human spatial navigation. Nat. Neurosci 16, 1188–1190 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Brown TI et al. (2016) Prospective representation of navigational goals in the human hippocampus. Science 352, 1323–6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kyle CT et al. (2015) Successful retrieval of competing spatial environments in humans involves hippocampal pattern separation mechanisms. Elife 4, e10499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Marchette SA et al. (2014) Anchoring the neural compass: coding of local spatial reference frames in human medial parietal lobe. Nat. Neurosci 17, 1598–1606 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Chadwick MJ et al. (2015) A Goal Direction Signal in the Human Entorhinal/Subicular Region. Curr. Biol 25, 87–92 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Shine JP et al. (2016) The Human Retrosplenial Cortex and Thalamus Code Head Direction in a Global Reference Frame. J. Neurosci 36, 6371–81 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ferrara K and Park S (2016) Neural representation of scene boundaries. Neuropsychologia 89, 180–190 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Doeller CF et al. (2010) Evidence for grid cells in a human memory network. Nature 463, 657–661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kunz L et al. (2015) Reduced grid-cell-like representations in adults at genetic risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Science 350, 430–3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Stangl M et al. (2018) Compromised Grid-Cell-like Representations in Old Age as a Key Mechanism to Explain Age-Related Navigational Deficits. Curr. Biol 28, 1108–1115.e6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Buzsáki G (2002) Theta Oscillations in the Hippocampus. Neuron 33, 325–340 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ekstrom AD et al. (2005) Human hippocampal theta activity during virtual navigation. Hippocampus 15, 881–889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Watrous AJ et al. (2013) A comparative study of human and rat hippocampal low-frequency oscillations during spatial navigation. Hippocampus 23, 656–661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Jacobs J (2014) Hippocampal theta oscillations are slower in humans than in rodents: implications for models of spatial navigation and memory. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci 369, 20130304–20130304 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Buzsáki G and Draguhn A (2004) Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bohbot VD et al. (2017) Low-frequency theta oscillations in the human hippocampus during real-world and virtual navigation. Nat. Commun 8, 14415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.M Aghajan Z et al. (2017) Theta Oscillations in the Human Medial Temporal Lobe during Real-World Ambulatory Movement. Curr. Biol 27, 3743–3751.e3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Goyal A et al. (2018) Functionally distinct high and low theta oscillations in the human hippocampus. bioRxiv DOI: 10.1101/498055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Buzsáki G et al. (2003) Hippocampal network patterns of activity in the mouse. Neuroscience 116, 201–211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Jacobs J et al. (2010) Right-lateralized Brain Oscillations in Human Spatial Navigation. J. Cogn. Neurosci 22, 824–836 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Colgin LL et al. (2009) Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature 462, 353–357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Canolty RT and Knight RT (2010) The functional role of cross-frequency coupling. Trends Cogn. Sci 14, 506–15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Watrous AJ et al. (2015) More than spikes: common oscillatory mechanisms for content specific neural representations during perception and memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 31, 33–39 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.McFarland WL et al. (1975) Relationship between hippocampal theta activity and running speed in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol 88, 324–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Sławińska U and Kasicki S (1998) The frequency of rat’s hippocampal theta rhythm is related to the speed of locomotion. Brain Res. 796, 327–331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Góis ZHTD and Tort ABL (2018) Characterizing Speed Cells in the Rat Hippocampus. Cell Rep. 25, 1872–1884.e4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kropff E et al. (2015) Speed cells in the medial entorhinal cortex. Nature 523, 419–424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Fuhrmann F et al. (2015) Locomotion, Theta Oscillations, and the Speed-Correlated Firing of Hippocampal Neurons Are Controlled by a Medial Septal Glutamatergic Circuit. Neuron 86, 1253–1264 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hinman JR et al. (2016) Multiple Running Speed Signals in Medial Entorhinal Cortex. Neuron 91, 666–679 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kraus BJ et al. (2013) Hippocampal “Time Cells”: Time versus Path Integration. Neuron 78, 1090–1101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.MacDonald CJ et al. (2011) Hippocampal “Time Cells” Bridge the Gap in Memory for Discontiguous Events. Neuron 71, 737–749 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Tsao A et al. (2018) Integrating time from experience in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Nature 561, 57–62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Thavabalasingam S et al. (2018) Multivoxel pattern similarity suggests the integration of temporal duration in hippocampal event sequence representations. Neuroimage 178, 136–146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Montchal ME et al. (2019) Precise temporal memories are supported by the lateral entorhinal cortex in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 284–288 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Nakazono T et al. (2015) Theta oscillation and neuronal activity in rat hippocampus are involved in temporal discrimination of time in seconds. Front. Syst. Neurosci 9, 95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Vass LK et al. (2016) Oscillations Go the Distance: Low-Frequency Human Hippocampal Oscillations Code Spatial Distance in the Absence of Sensory Cues during Teleportation. Neuron 89, 1180–1186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bush D et al. (2017) Human hippocampal theta power indicates movement onset and distance travelled. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 114, 12297–12302 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Jensen O et al. (2014) Temporal coding organized by coupled alpha and gamma oscillations prioritize visual processing. Trends Neurosci. 37, 357–369 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Morgan LK et al. (2011) Distances between Real-World Locations Are Represented in the Human Hippocampus. J. Neurosci 31, 1238–1245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Howard LR et al. (2014) The Hippocampus and Entorhinal Cortex Encode the Path and Euclidean Distances to Goals during Navigation. Curr. Biol 24, 1331–1340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ekstrom AD et al. (2017) Interacting networks of brain regions underlie human spatial navigation: a review and novel synthesis of the literature. J. Neurophysiol 118, 3328–3344 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Diba K and Buzsáki G (2007) Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripples. Nat. Neurosci 10, 1241–1242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Pfeiffer BE and Foster DJ (2013) Hippocampal place-cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals. Nature 497, 74–79 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wikenheiser AM and Redish AD (2015) Hippocampal theta sequences reflect current goals. Nat. Neurosci 18, 289–294 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Jensen O and Lisman JE (2000) Position Reconstruction From an Ensemble of Hippocampal Place Cells: Contribution of Theta Phase Coding. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 2602–2609 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Huxter J et al. (2003) Independent rate and temporal coding in hippocampal pyramidal cells. Nature 425, 828–832 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Agarwal G et al. (2014) Spatially distributed local fields in the hippocampus encode rat position. Science 344, 626–30 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hassabis D et al. (2009) Decoding Neuronal Ensembles in the Human Hippocampus. Curr. Biol 19, 546–554 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Nolan CR et al. (2018) Evidence against the Detectability of a Hippocampal Place Code Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. eneuro 5, ENEURO.0177-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Redish AD et al. (2001) Independence of firing correlates of anatomically proximate hippocampal pyramidal cells. J. Neurosci 21, RC134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Dombeck DA et al. (2010) Functional imaging of hippocampal place cells at cellular resolution during virtual navigation. Nat. Neurosci 13, 1433–1440 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Redish AD and Ekstrom A (2013) Hippocampus and related areas: What the place cell literature tells us about cognitive maps in rats and humans In Handbook of spatial cognition. pp. 15–34, American Psychological Association [Google Scholar]
- 59.Lever C et al. (2009) Boundary vector cells in the subiculum of the hippocampal formation. J. Neurosci 29, 9771–7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.O’Keefe J and Burgess N (1996) Geometric determinants of the place fields of hippocampal neurons. Nature 381, 425–428 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Hardcastle K et al. (2015) Environmental boundaries as an error correction mechanism for grid cells. Neuron 86, 827–39 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Julian JB et al. (2016) The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation. Curr. Biol 26, 1104–9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lee SA et al. (2018) Electrophysiological Signatures of Spatial Boundaries in the Human Subiculum. J. Neurosci 38, 3265–3272 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Omer DB et al. (2018) Social place-cells in the bat hippocampus. Science 359, 218–224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Diehl GW et al. (2017) Grid and Nongrid Cells in Medial Entorhinal Cortex Represent Spatial Location and Environmental Features with Complementary Coding Schemes. Neuron 94, 83–92.e6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Behrens TEJ et al. (2018) What Is a Cognitive Map? Organizing Knowledge for Flexible Behavior. Neuron 100, 490–509 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Gil M et al. (2018) Impaired path integration in mice with disrupted grid cell firing. Nat. Neurosci 21, 81–91 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Chen D et al. (2018) Hexadirectional Modulation of Theta Power in Human Entorhinal Cortex during Spatial Navigation. Curr. Biol 28, 3310–3315.e4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Maidenbaum S et al. (2018) Grid-like hexadirectional modulation of human entorhinal theta oscillations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 115, 10798–10803 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Constantinescu AO et al. (2016) Organizing conceptual knowledge in humans with a gridlike code. Science 352, 1464–1468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Sargolini F et al. (2006) Conjunctive representation of position, direction, and velocity in entorhinal cortex. Science 312, 758–62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Killian NJ et al. (2012) A map of visual space in the primate entorhinal cortex. Nature 491, 761–764 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Julian JB et al. (2018) Human entorhinal cortex represents visual space using a boundary-anchored grid. Nat. Neurosci 21, 191–194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Nau M et al. (2018) Hexadirectional coding of visual space in human entorhinal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 188–190 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Staudigl T et al. (2018) Hexadirectional Modulation of High-Frequency Electrophysiological Activity in the Human Anterior Medial Temporal Lobe Maps Visual Space. Curr. Biol 28, 3325–3329.e4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Kahana MJ et al. (1999) Human theta oscillations exhibit task dependence during virtual maze navigation. Nature 399, 781–784 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Bellmund JLS et al. (2018) Navigating cognition: Spatial codes for human thinking. Science 362, eaat6766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Manning JR et al. (2009) Broadband shifts in local field potential power spectra are correlated with single-neuron spiking in humans. J. Neurosci 29, 13613–20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Rasch MJ et al. (2008) Inferring Spike Trains From Local Field Potentials. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 1461–1476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Ray S et al. (2008) Neural correlates of high-gamma oscillations (60–200 Hz) in macaque local field potentials and their potential implications in electrocorticography. J. Neurosci 28, 11526–36 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Ray S and Maunsell JHR (2011) Different Origins of Gamma Rhythm and High-Gamma Activity in Macaque Visual Cortex. PLoS Biol. 9, e1000610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Ekstrom A et al. (2007) Contrasting roles of neural firing rate and local field potentials in human memory. Hippocampus 17, 606–617 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Ekstrom A et al. (2009) Correlation Between BOLD fMRI and Theta-Band Local Field Potentials in the Human Hippocampal Area. J. Neurophysiol 101, 2668–2678 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Nir Y et al. (2007) Coupling between neuronal firing rate, gamma LFP, and BOLD fMRI is related to interneuronal correlations. Curr. Biol 17, 1275–85 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Ekstrom A (2010) How and when the fMRI BOLD signal relates to underlying neural activity: The danger in dissociation. Brain Res. Rev 62, 233–244 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Logothetis NK (2008) What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature 453, 869–878 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Buzsáki G et al. (2012) The origin of extracellular fields and currents — EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 13, 407–420 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Pesaran B et al. (2018) Investigating large-scale brain dynamics using field potential recordings: analysis and interpretation. Nat. Neurosci 21, 903–919 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Jia X et al. (2011) Stimulus selectivity and spatial coherence of gamma components of the local field potential. J. Neurosci 31, 9390–403 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Buzsaki G and Eidelberg E (1983) Phase relations of hippocampal projection cells and interneurons to theta activity in the anesthetized rat. Brain Res. 266, 334–339 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.O’Keefe J and Recce ML (1993) Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus 3, 317–330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Mehta MR et al. (2002) Role of experience and oscillations in transforming a rate code into a temporal code. Nature 417, 741–746 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Souza BC and Tort ABL (2017) Asymmetry of the temporal code for space by hippocampal place cells. Sci. Rep 7, 8507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Hafting T et al. (2008) Hippocampus-independent phase precession in entorhinal grid cells. Nature 453, 1248–1252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Tang Q et al. (2014) Pyramidal and stellate cell specificity of grid and border representations in layer 2 of medial entorhinal cortex. Neuron 84, 1191–7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Jacobs J et al. (2007) Brain oscillations control timing of single-neuron activity in humans. J. Neurosci 27, 3839–44 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Murta T et al. (2015) Electrophysiological correlates of the BOLD signal for EEGinformed fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp 36, 391–414 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Logothetis NK et al. (2001) Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature 412, 150–157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Ojemann GA et al. (2013) Relation between functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and single neuron, local field potential (LFP) and electrocorticography (ECoG) activity in human cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci 7, 34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Kayser C et al. (2004) A Comparison of Hemodynamic and Neural Responses in Cat Visual Cortex Using Complex Stimuli. Cereb. Cortex 14, 881–891 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Mukamel R et al. (2005) Coupling between neuronal firing, field potentials, and FMRI in human auditory cortex. Science 309, 951–4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Rauch A et al. (2008) The effect of a serotonin-induced dissociation between spiking and perisynaptic activity on BOLD functional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 105, 6759–64 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Niessing J et al. (2005) Hemodynamic signals correlate tightly with synchronized gamma oscillations. Science 309, 948–51 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Murta T et al. (2017) Phase–amplitude coupling and the BOLD signal: A simultaneous intracranial EEG (icEEG) - fMRI study in humans performing a finger-tapping task. Neuroimage 146, 438–451 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Khursheed F et al. (2011) Frequency-specific electrocorticographic correlates of working memory delay period fMRI activity. Neuroimage 56, 1773–1782 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Axmacher N et al. (2007) Sustained neural activity patterns during working memory in the human medial temporal lobe. J. Neurosci 27, 7807–16 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Jaime S et al. (2019) Delta Rhythm Orchestrates the Neural Activity Underlying the Resting State BOLD Signal via Phase–amplitude Coupling. Cereb. Cortex 29, 119–133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Conner CR et al. (2011) Variability of the relationship between electrophysiology and BOLD-fMRI across cortical regions in humans. J. Neurosci 31, 12855–65 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Axmacher N et al. (2010) Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 107, 3228–33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Stensola H et al. (2012) The entorhinal grid map is discretized. Nature 492, 72–78 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Keinath AT (2016) The Preferred Directions of Conjunctive Grid X Head Direction Cells in the Medial Entorhinal Cortex Are Periodically Organized. PLoS One 11, e0152041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Grill-Spector K et al. (2006) Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn. Sci 10, 14–23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Gu Y et al. (2018) A Map-like Micro-Organization of Grid Cells in the Medial Entorhinal Cortex. Cell 175, 736–750.e30 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Schroeder TN et al. (2018) Entorhinal cortex minimises uncertainty for optimal behaviour. bioRxiv DOI: 10.1101/166306 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Boccara CN et al. (2010) Grid cells in pre- and parasubiculum. Nat. Neurosci 13, 987–994 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Huxter JR et al. (2008) Theta phase–specific codes for two-dimensional position, trajectory and heading in the hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci 11, 587–594 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Watrous AJ et al. (2018) Phase-tuned neuronal firing encodes human contextual representations for navigational goals. Elife 7, e32554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Lenck-Santini P-P et al. (2001) Evidence for a relationship between place-cell spatial firing and spatial memory performance. Hippocampus 11, 377–390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Kentros CG et al. (2004) Increased Attention to Spatial Context Increases Both Place Field Stability and Spatial Memory. Neuron 42, 283–295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Winson J (1978) Loss of hippocampal theta rhythm results in spatial memory deficit in the rat. Science 201, 160–3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Richard GR et al. (2013) Speed modulation of hippocampal theta frequency correlates with spatial memory performance. Hippocampus 23, 1269–1279 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Chen X et al. (2015) Bias in Human Path Integration Is Predicted by Properties of Grid Cells. Curr. Biol 25, 1771–1776 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.McNamara TP et al. (2003) Egocentric and geocentric frames of reference in memory of large-scale space. Psychon. Bull. Rev 10, 589–595 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Coughlan G et al. (2018) Spatial navigation deficits — overlooked cognitive marker for preclinical Alzheimer disease? Nat. Rev. Neurol 14, 496–506 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Braak H and Braak E (1995) Staging of alzheimer’s disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Neurobiol. Aging 16, 271–278 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Fu H et al. (2017) Tau Pathology Induces Excitatory Neuron Loss, Grid Cell Dysfunction, and Spatial Memory Deficits Reminiscent of Early Alzheimer’s Disease. Neuron 93, 533–541.e5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Cacucci F et al. (2008) Place cell firing correlates with memory deficits and amyloid plaque burden in Tg2576 Alzheimer mouse model. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 105, 7863–8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Mably AJ et al. (2017) Impairments in spatial representations and rhythmic coordination of place cells in the 3xTg mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Hippocampus 27, 378–392 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Nakazono T et al. (2017) Impaired In Vivo Gamma Oscillations in the Medial Entorhinal Cortex of Knock-in Alzheimer Model. Front. Syst. Neurosci 11, 48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Vlcek K and Laczo J (2014) Neural Correlates of Spatial Navigation Changes in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Behav. Neurosci 8, 89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Lester AW et al. (2017) The Aging Navigational System. Neuron 95, 1019–1035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Hayman R et al. (2011) Anisotropic encoding of three-dimensional space by place cells and grid cells. Nat. Neurosci 14, 1182–1188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Yartsev MM and Ulanovsky N (2013) Representation of three-dimensional space in the hippocampus of flying bats. Science 340, 367–72 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Kim M et al. (2017) Multivoxel Pattern Analysis Reveals 3D Place Information in the Human Hippocampus. J. Neurosci 37, 4270–4279 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Theves S et al. (2019) The Hippocampus Encodes Distances in Multidimensional Feature Space. Curr. Biol. 29, 1226–1231.e3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Aronov D et al. (2017) Mapping of a non-spatial dimension by the hippocampal–entorhinal circuit. Nature 543, 719–722 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Bicanski A and Burgess N (2019) A Computational Model of Visual Recognition Memory via Grid Cells. Curr. Biol 29, 979–990.e4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Lachaux JP et al. (2003) Intracranial EEG and human brain mapping. J. Physiol. 97, 613–628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Jacobs J and Kahana MJ (2010) Direct brain recordings fuel advances in cognitive electrophysiology. Trends Cogn. Sci 14, 162–71 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Norman KA et al. (2006) Beyond mind-reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trends Cogn. Sci 10, 424–30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Kriegeskorte N et al. (2008) Representational similarity analysis – connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Front. Syst. Neurosci 2, 4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Miller J et al. (2018) Lateralized hippocampal oscillations underlie distinct aspects of human spatial memory and navigation. Nat. Commun 9, 2423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Colom LV et al. (2005) Characterization of medial septal glutamatergic neurons and their projection to the hippocampus. Synapse 58, 151–164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Frotscher M and Léránth C (1985) Cholinergic innervation of the rat hippocampus as revealed by choline acetyltransferase immunocytochemistry: A combined light and electron microscopic study. J. Comp. Neurol 239, 237–246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Freund TF and Buzsáki G (1998) Interneurons of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 6, 347–470 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Jensen O (2005) Reading the hippocampal code by theta phase-locking. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 551–3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Lisman JE and Idiart MA (1995) Storage of 7 +/− 2 short-term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science 267, 1512–5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Buzsáki G (1989) Two-stage model of memory trace formation: A role for “noisy” brain states. Neuroscience 31, 551–570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Huerta PT and Lisman JE (1993) Heightened synaptic plasticity of hippocampal CA1 neurons during a Cholinergically induced rhythmic state. Nature 364, 723–725 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Koenig J et al. (2011) The spatial periodicity of grid cells is not sustained during reduced theta oscillations. Science 332, 592–5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.