Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Clin Psychol Sci. 2019 Feb 8;7(3):488–501. doi: 10.1177/2167702618810227

Suicidal behavior and stress generation in adolescents

Richard T Liu 1, Anthony Spirito 1
PMCID: PMC6602543  NIHMSID: NIHMS1509446  PMID: 31263632

Abstract

Stress generation has potential to account for recurrent suicidal behavior. The current study represents a first step toward evaluating this possibility in a sample of adolescent psychiatric inpatients (n = 99; 79.80% female) followed over six months. At index admission, participants completed baseline measures of depressive symptom severity, suicidal ideation, lifetime history of suicide attempts, and negative life events using a contextual threat life stress interview. Negative life events since baseline were assessed at the follow-up assessment. Consistent with the stress generation hypothesis, lifetime number of suicide attempts prospectively predicted higher rates of dependent, but not independent, stress. Bayesian analyses also yielded substantial support for an association with overall dependent stress, but provided more modest support for specificity to this form of life stress relative to independent stress. Implications of these findings and directions for future research further clarifying the role of stress generation in suicidal behavior are discussed.

Keywords: Bayesian analysis, life events, suicide, suicide attempts, stress generation

Introduction

One of the most robust predictors of future suicide attempts in the empirical literature is a past history of this behavior. Indeed, in a recent meta-analysis (Ribeiro et al., 2016), a medium-to-large pooled effect was observed for this relationship across a median follow-up period of two years. Although the raw number of suicides is higher among older adults (Nock et al., 2008), it is a particular concern in adolescence, as it is the second leading cause of death at this age (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2017), and the onset of suicidal behavior increases markedly during this period of development (Nock et al., 2008). Adding to this concern, the national suicide rate has increased 24% over the last 15 years, with females aged 10 to 14 experiencing the greatest increase during this time period (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2016). Within this context, and given the current want of evidence-based psychosocial treatments for adolescent suicidal behavior (Ougrin, Tranah, Stahl, Moran, & Asarnow, 2015), elucidating the processes underlying the association between past and future suicidal behavior remains a high priority for the potential to inform future intervention strategies for addressing this major public health concern.

One potential mechanism underlying the recurrence of suicidal behavior is stress generation (Hammen, 1991, 2005), the tendency for certain individuals to experience higher rates of dependent stress (i.e., life stressors that are at least in part influenced by their own behaviors), such as the dissolution of a romantic relationship, but not to differ in terms of rates of independent stress (i.e., life stressors that occur outside the influence of the their behavior), such as the death of a close relative. This stress generation hypothesis was originally conceived as a theoretical framework to account for the tendency for a past history of depression to be associated with a heightened risk for its future occurrence (Hammen, 1991, 2005). Specifically, not only are life stressors an established risk factor for depression (Hammen, 2005), but depression itself may pose a risk for the future occurrence of dependent stress in what is essentially a reciprocal relationship. Furthermore, stress generation in depression has been found in several studies to be particular to dependent stress within interpersonal domains (i.e., interpersonal dependent stress; Hankin, Stone, & Wright, 2010; Hernandez, Trout, & Liu, 2016; Rudolph, 2008; Shih, 2006). This specificity of stress generation to dependent stress is of etiological importance, for this type of stress appears to be particularly relevant to risk for depression (Hammen, 2009a; Sheets & Craighead, 2014; Vrshek-Schallhorn et al., 2015). That is, depression may lead to the generation of the very stressors that confer greatest risk for depressive recurrence (Hammen, 1991, 2006). In the first study empirically to evaluate the stress generation hypothesis, depressed individuals were found prospectively to experience higher levels of dependent stress, particularly interpersonal ones, but not independent stress, over a one-year period (Hammen, 1991). In the decades since this original study, the role of stress generation in depression has received considerable empirical support (Liu & Alloy, 2010).

Although stress generation has yet to be empirically examined in relation to suicidal behavior, there are several notable phenomenological similarities between suicidal behavior and depression, as well as their respective relation with life stress. Both are often episodic and frequently follow a recurrent course: just as a past history of depression is a strong predictor of its future recurrence (Burcusa & Iacono, 2007), so too is a history of suicidal behavior associated with risk for its recurrence, with one study finding risk of suicide attempts increasing by approximately 32% with each attempt (Leon, Friedman, Sweeney, Brown, & Mann, 1990). The potential involvement of dependent stress in accounting for repeat suicidal behavior is an intriguing possibility warranting investigation, given findings supporting the relevance to suicide risk of life stressors, particularly within interpersonal domains, even after accounting for psychopathology (Gould, 2003; Liu & Miller, 2014). The generation of these stressors in at-risk individuals may in some measure account for the link between past suicide and future suicidal behavior. Indeed, several researchers have either posited a role for stress generation in the recurrence of suicide attempts (Liu & Miller, 2014), or noted that certain diatheses for suicidal behavior (e.g., impaired decision-making and interpersonal problem-solving) are associated with greater difficulties in interpersonal relationships (Jollant et al., 2007; Pollock & Williams, 2004), consistent with stress generation, and may thereby potentially worsen risk for future suicidal behavior (van Heeringen, 2012). Still others have commented on how several interpersonal tendencies that often characterize suicidal individuals (e.g., excessive reassurance-seeking) may lead to greater interpersonal difficulties in their lives (e.g., experiencing interpersonal rejection; Stellrecht, Joiner, & Rudd, 2006). Furthermore, these behavioral tendencies, as viewed within the interpersonal theory of suicide, may in part be motivated by feelings of thwarted belongingness (Stellrecht et al., 2006). Of particular relevance for the potential role of suicidal behavior in stress generation, some of these same risk factors have been previously implicated in stress generation (Eberhart & Hammen, 2009; Hernandez et al., 2016), including in children (Shih, Abela, & Starrs, 2009).

In addition to being of particular clinical importance to suicidal behavior, adolescence may be an age especially relevant to stress generation, for this period of transition is marked by increased autonomy and individuation (Grotevant & Cooper, 1985; Steinberg, 2002; Steinberg & Silverberg, 1986; Wray-Lake, Crouter, & McHale, 2010), decreased parental monitoring (Parke & Bhavnagri, 1989), and a greater focus on peer relations and social networks (Brown, 1990). Concomitant with this larger role in navigating and shaping the social environment is a greater opportunity for the occurrence of dependent stress (e.g., conflicts with parental authorities as a consequence of striving for greater autonomy). Indeed, adolescence has generally been viewed as a period accompanied by higher rates of life stress (Compas, Davis, & Forsythe, 1985). Moreover, in one study examining independent and dependent stress separately in preadolescent and adolescent children (Rudolph & Hammen, 1999), adolescents experienced greater dependent stress than did their preadolescent counterparts.

Also worth noting is that several methodological limitations characterize much of the suicide-life stress and stress generation literatures, respectively. First, in both cases, a minority of studies featured interview-based measures of life stress, the majority adopting self-report measures of life stress (Liu, 2013; Liu & Miller, 2014). Even rarer still are studies utilizing contextual threat approaches to measuring life stress, with only three (3.2%) identified in a recent systematic review of studies of life stress and suicidal ideation and behavior (Liu & Miller, 2014). Contextual threat life stress interviews involve drawing from the individual a narrative of the context within which each event occurred and its consequences (Brown & Harris, 1978). This technique produces detailed information surrounding each event, thus allowing for a much more sensitive evaluation of the event’s impact on the individual (e.g., the loss of a close friend of several years due to betrayal and with no other friends in the individual’s social network is likely to have a considerably higher impact than the loss of a friend of several weeks due to mutual drifting apart and with several close friends remaining in the individual’s social network). In the context of stress generation research, such considerations have added importance for accurately differentiating between dependent and independent stress. For example, determining whether a child changed schools due to expulsion from a previous school (i.e., dependent stress) or due to the child’s parents job change requiring a move to a different city (i.e., independent stress) would be impossible with other life stress methodologies.

Another major limitation that constrains interpretability of much of the research in these areas is the considerable temporal overlap in assessment of suicidal behavior and depression, respectively, in relation to life stress. In the case of the empirical literature on life stress and suicide, only 9.5% of studies employed a prospective design, allowing for clean temporal separation of life stress from suicidal behavior (Liu & Miller, 2014). Among studies of stress generation and depression, 55% examined depression in relation to temporally preceding life stress (Liu, 2013), which is problematic insofar as stress generation is conflated with stress exposure in these studies.

The third methodological concern is a statistical one. The stress generation hypothesis involves contrasting predictions for dependent and independent stress; dependent stress is predicted to occur at higher levels in at-risk individuals, and just as importantly, stress generation posits no differences to occur in rates of independent stress. This second component of stress generation is essentially a null hypothesis. All the studies to date to have included an evaluation of this second component of the stress generation hypothesis have done so, without exception, using null hypothesis significance testing (NHST). This is a concern because of the standard prohibition against accepting the null hypothesis with NHST (Gallistel, 2009; Wilkinson & the Task Force on Statistical Inference, 1999). That is, NHST becomes particularly problematic in situations where the predicted result is a null effect. Consequently, more advanced statistical techniques are required adequately to evaluate this aspect of the stress generation hypothesis. One statistical approach well suited for addressing this limitation of NHST in providing support for the null hypothesis is Bayesian analysis (Dienes, 2011). With NHST, a p value is used to quantify the probability that the observed data (or a more extreme set of results) would occur if the null hypothesis were true. In contrast, Bayesian statistics allows for a direct evaluation of the strength of support for a hypothesis (i.e., H1) relative to the null hypothesis (i.e., H0) based on observed data, with Bayes factors used to quantify the strength of empirical support for H1 relative to H0 (i.e., BF10), as well as H0 relative to H1 (i.e., BF01; for a guide to Bayesian statistics, see Jarosz & Wiley, 2014).

The objective of the current study was to provide an essential first step towards evaluating the applicability of stress generation to suicidal behavior. That is, it examined whether lifetime number of suicide attempts was prospectively associated with higher rates of dependent stress, but not independent stress, in a sample of adolescent psychiatric inpatients. In so doing, the current study also aimed to address methodological limitations of past research in this area, adopting a contextual threat life stress interview to assess for the prospective occurrence of life stress over a six-month interval. To address statistical limitations in prior stress generation studies, the current investigation supplemented conventional statistical analyses (NHST) of the study hypotheses with Bayesian analysis.

Method

Participants

Participants included 99 adolescents (79.8% female; 22.5% Hispanic) of ages 13 to 17 (mean age = 14.68, SD = 1.50). Approximately 78.4% of participants were white, 3.4% were African American, 4.6% were Asian American, and 13.6% were mixed/other. All participants were recruited from a university-affiliated adolescent psychiatric inpatient facility. Acute suicidality was high in this sample, being a primary reason for hospitalization. Patients were ineligible for recruitment if they were acutely psychotic, had intellectual or developmental disabilities, or were under the guardianship of child protective services. Parental consent and adolescent assent were obtained from all participating families.

Measures

Lifetime history of psychiatric disorders.

Adolescents were interviewed with the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children (KSADS-PL; Kaufman et al., 1997) to assess lifetime history of diagnoses according to the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edition; DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 2000). κ was determined based on interrater reliability between interviewers and an experienced, licensed clinical psychologist for interviews randomly selected from 20% of the sample. In the current sample, mean κ across diagnoses was .92.

Depressive symptom severity.

The Children’s Depression Rating Scale-Revised (CDRS-R; Poznanski & Mokros, 1995) was used to assess current depressive symptom severity. This measure is a 17-item semi-structured interview that encompasses all DSM-5 depressive symptoms and includes three items based on behavioral observation (i.e., depressed facial affect, listless speech, and hypoactivity). Items are rated on 5- or 7-point scales and individual items are summed and converted in to a T score. Greater T scores on this scale are indicative of greater depressive symptom severity, with scores ≥ 65 indicating clinical severity. This measure demonstrated good internal consistency in the current study (Cronbach’s α = .83).

Suicidal ideation.

The Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire Jr. (SIQ Jr.; Reynolds, 1988) was used to measure current suicidal ideation. This measure is a 15-item self-report scale of current severity of suicidal ideation, with each item rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 0 to 6. Clinical significance is indicated by scores ≥ 31. Internal consistency in the current sample was high (Cronbach’s α = .95).

Suicide attempt history.

Lifetime number of suicide attempts was assessed using the Columbia-Suicide Severity Rating Scale (C-SSRS; Posner et al., 2011), a semi-structured interview assessing suicidal thoughts and behaviors. Following the standard conventions of the field, suicide attempts were defined as any “self-destructive behavior with at least implied intent to die” (Posner, Oquendo, Gould, Stanley, & Davies, 2007). All interviewers received extensive training and supervision from the first author in the administration of this interview and rating of its data. A rigorous protocol developed by the first author was implemented, with an average training period of three to four months before interviewers administered the measure independently. Given the centrality of this measure to the study, and to prevent interviewer drift, each interview was reviewed by the first author soon after its administration, while blind to participants’ data for all other study variables, and individualized feedback was provided as needed. Interviewers conferred with the first author whenever coding questions arose. This measure has been found to be reliable for use with adolescent psychiatric samples, with κ = .88 for suicide attempts (Posner et al., 2011), as well as to possess predictive validity (Brent et al., 2009; Posner et al., 2011).

Life stress.

The adolescent version of the UCLA Life Stress Interview (LSI; Hammen & Brennan, 2001) was used to assess the occurrence of life stressors. The LSI is a semi-structured interview that uses a contextual threat approach (Brown & Harris, 1978) to allow for the assessment of life stressors across a wide range of content domains relevant to adolescents (e.g., friendships, romantic relationships, family, academics, and physical health). Structured initial probes, temporal anchors (e.g., generally salient dates such as holidays, and personally salient ones such as birthdays), and a calendar were used to increase accuracy in dating of events as well as to ensure that the events fell within the assessment period. Adolescents were also presented with a standard list of developmentally relevant stressors to facilitate recall. Contextual narratives of stressors reported during the interview were presented to a rating team, blind to each participant’s psychopathology (including suicide attempt history) and subjective response so as to prevent subjective rating biases (Brown, 1989; Hammen, 2006; Harkness & Monroe, 2016). The rating team scored each event on 5-point Likert scales for the objective severity of the event (from 1 = “no or minimal impact” to 5 = “severe impact”) as well as on dependence versus independence (from 1 = “entirely independent of the person” to 5 = “entirely dependent on the person”). For several reasons, we focused our analyses on non-mild life stress (i.e., moderate to severe stressors), operationalized as events with objective severity ratings ≥ 3 (Stroud, Davila, Hammen, & Vrshek-Schallhorn, 2011). First, previous stress generation studies have used this approach (e.g., Uliaszek et al., 2012). Second, there is evidence of specificity of stress generation to severe life stress (Uliaszek et al., 2012). Third, insofar as moderate to severe life stress, relative to mild stress, are more etiologically relevant to stress-related psychopathology (e.g., depression and suicidal behavior; Hammen, 2005; Paykel, Prusoff, & Myers, 1975), demonstrating the role of moderate to severe life stress to stress generation in suicidal behavior is important for establishing its clinical relevance. Also consistent with prior studies of life stress (Conway, Hammen, & Brennan, 2012; Hammen, 1991; Hammen, Henry, & Daley, 2000), events with independence versus dependence ratings ≥ 3 were categorized as dependent for the purposes of analysis. Finally, the rating team also categorized each event as interpersonal (i.e., events that primarily involve another person) or non-interpersonal (i.e., events that primarily relate to other domains, such as academics). The objective severity ratings of relevant events within each category of life stress were summed to create the dependent variables for analysis. The LSI has been used with suicidal adolescent inpatient samples (Pettit, Green, Grover, Schatte, & Morgan, 2011). Such contextual threat life stress interview are widely regarded as the gold standard in the field (Dohrenwend, 2006; Hammen, 2005; Kessler, 1997; Monroe, 2008), and are less susceptible than self-report checklists to idiosyncratic or subjective response biases (Brown & Harris, 1978; Harkness & Monroe, 2016; Johnson & Roberts, 1995). In the current sample, intraclass correlation coefficients were .87 and .93 for severity and dependence, respectively. Reliability was determined by having a second rating team separately rate LSI data for 20% of the sample, blind to the ratings of the initial rating team.

Procedures

Baseline assessments were conducted during participants’ index hospitalization. At the baseline assessment, participants completed measures of current depressive symptom severity (CDRS-R), lifetime history of major depression (K-SADS-PL), suicidal ideation (SIQ Jr.), and suicide attempt history (C-SSRS), as well as negative life events during the six months immediately preceding their index hospital admission (LSI). At a six-month follow-up (MDays = 199.43, SD = 20.27),1 participants completed the LSI for negative life events that had occurred since the baseline evaluation. The six-month follow-up interval was specifically chosen based on evidence that recollection of major life events tend to fade in adolescents after approximately seven months (Monck & Dobbs, 1985). At the same time, this time interval allowed for sufficient variability in the occurrence of stressful life events under consideration.

Data analysis

A series of χ2 and independent-samples t-tests were conducted to evaluate whether participants who attrited and those who completed the six-month follow-up assessment differed in demographic and baseline study characteristics (i.e., sex, age, depressive symptom severity, suicidal ideation, and lifetime number of suicide attempts). Spearman and Pearson correlations were conducted to examine bivariate relationships between study variables.

To evaluate the study hypotheses that lifetime number of suicide attempts prospectively predicted higher levels of dependent stress, this latter variable was entered into a multivariate linear regression model as the criterion variable, with baseline age, sex, depressive symptom severity, suicidal ideation, as well as dependent and independent life stress in the preceding six months included as covariates and lifetime number of suicide attempts as the predictor. To assess the hypothesis that lifetime number of suicide attempts did not prospectively predict independent stress, this analysis was repeated with independent stress as the criterion variable. Sensitivity analyses were conducted with these models reassessed with lifetime history of major depression substituting for current depressive symptom severity as a covariate. Finally, all analyses were repeated using Bayesian analysis (JASP Team, 2016), with Bayes factor used to quantify the strength of the evidence in favor of the study hypothesis relative to the null hypothesis (BF10) in the case of dependent stress as the criterion variable (i.e., BF10 = likelihood of the data given H1 ÷ likelihood of the data given H0). Bayes factor was also used to quantify the empirical support for the null hypothesis (BF01) in the case of independent stress (i.e., BF01 = likelihood of the data given H0 ÷ likelihood of the data given H1). When BF10 = 1, the data are neither supportive of H1 nor H0, and when BF10 < 1, the data are supportive of H0 over H1. A BF10 falling between 1 and 3 provide anecdotal support, between 3 and 10 indicates substantial evidence, between 10 and 30 reflect strong support, between 30 and 100 is indicative of very strong evidence, and above 100 offers extreme evidence in support of H1 (Jeffreys, 1961; Wagenmakers, Wetzels, Borsboom, & van der Maas, 2011). For prior probability, given that no prior studies of suicidal behavior and stress generation have been conducted, equal likelihood of the predictor variable – suicidal behavior – being included and not included in the final model was assigned (i.e., 0.5).

Results

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations

At baseline, 54.55% of adolescents had a lifetime history of attempted suicide,2 of which 44.45% were repeat attempters MNumber of attempts = 1.82, SD = 4.59, range = 0 to 35),3 and depressive symptom severity and suicidal ideation were both, on average, clinically elevated (MCDRS-R T-score = 73.44, SD = 8.87, range = 50 to 85; MSIQ Jr. = 42.87, SD = 24.15, range = 0 to 77). Frequency of current diagnoses at baseline was: 73.8% for unipolar depression, 42.6% for generalized anxiety disorder, 23.9% for social anxiety disorder, 19.1% for oppositional defiant disorder, 18.3% for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, 17.2% for post-traumatic stress disorder, 17.0% for panic disorder, 8.1% for specific phobia, 7.5% for agoraphobia, 3.3% for conduct disorder, 3.2% for obsessive-compulsive disorder, and 2.1% for bipolar disorder. Among the participants who completed the baseline assessment, 83.8% provided six-month follow-up data.

Participants who completed the six-month follow-up assessment did not differ at baseline from those who attrited in terms of sex (χ2 = .27, p = .60), age (t = .33, p = .74), depressive symptom severity (t = 1.00, p = .32), suicidal ideation (t = 1.02, p = .31), baseline dependent stress (t = −.25, p = .80), baseline independent stress (t = .26, p = .80), or lifetime number of suicide attempts (t = .59, p = .56).

Table 1 presents zero-order correlations between the study variables. Depressive symptoms severity was higher in females compared to males, and positively correlated with suicidal ideation and lifetime number of suicide attempts. Suicidal ideation was also correlated with lifetime frequency of suicide attempts. As for the different forms of life stress at the bivariate level, consistent with study hypotheses, prospective dependent stress, but not independent stress, was correlated with lifetime number of suicide attempts. Dependent and independent stress at the time of the index hospitalization (i.e. baseline), however, were not. Means, standard deviations, and percentage of participants prospectively experience each stressor type at follow-up were: MDependent Stress = 2.87, SD = 4.23; and MIndependent Stress = 1.30, SD = 2.23.

Table 1.

Bivariate correlations between study variables.

Variable  1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Age  —
2. Sex (female) −.03
3. Depressive symptoms  .18 .22*
4. Suicidal ideation −.08 .19 .49***
5. Lifetime number of suicide attempts −.05 .18 .25* .24*
6. Baseline dependent stress  .11 .13 .05 .06 .15
7. Baseline independent stress −.07 .05 −.02 <.01 −.09 .10
8. Follow-up dependent stress  <.01 .07 .05 .11 .28* .15 .09
9. Follow-up independent stress −.26* .15 .07 .12 .15 .12 .01 .06
*

Note: p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001

Spearman correlations are reported for analyses with sex. Pearson correlations are reported for all remaining analyses.

Multivariate analyses

In the multivariate linear regression model with dependent stress as the criterion variable, only lifetime number of suicide attempts significantly predicted prospectively occurring dependent stress with conventional NHST, such that a higher number of suicide attempts was associated with higher rates of dependent stress. A medium effect size was found for this association. In corresponding Bayesian analysis, we also quantified the strength of the evidence in favor of the study hypothesis that lifetime number of suicide attempts would be prospectively associated with greater dependent life stress (i.e., stress generation) relative to the null hypothesis that there was no association between lifetime number of suicide attempts and prospective dependent life stress. This analysis yielded a BF10 of 4.01, indicating that the stress generation hypothesis was 4.01 times more likely than the null hypothesis (i.e., substantial support in favor of the study hypothesis).

In the multivariate regression analysis with independent stress as the criterion variable, lifetime number of suicide attempts was not predictive of this outcome. Corresponding Bayesian analysis was conducted evaluating the strength of evidence in favor of the data supporting the null hypothesis (i.e., lifetime number of suicide attempts being not predictive of prospectively occurring independent life stress). In this analysis, BF01 = 1.50, indicating that it was 1.5 times more likely that there was no association between lifetime number of suicide attempts and prospective independent stress than that an association existed. Thus, although p = .41 in conventional NHST and Bayesian analysis was similarly supportive of the null hypothesis, the strength of this support in the Bayesian analysis was only anecdotal. Results of multivariate analyses are presented in Table 2.4

Table 2.

Conventional and Bayesian multivariate linear regression models for the prediction of dependent and independent stress.

Predictors
Predicting prospectively occurring dependent stress b SE p f2 BF10
Age .14 .36 .70 .54
Sex (female) .84 1.29 .52 .60
Baseline dependent stress .12 .15 .44 .64
Baseline independent stress .24 .28 .39 .67
Depressive symptoms −.03 .07 .68 .54
Suicidal ideation <.01 .02 .86 .51
Lifetime number of suicide attempts .28 .12 .02 .08 4.01
Predicting prospectively occurring independent stress b SE p f2 BF01

Age −.30 .18 .10 .69
Sex (female) .55 .66 .41 1.50
Baseline dependent stress .09 .08 .25 1.17
Baseline independent stress <.01 .14 .96 1.96
Depressive symptoms .01 .03 .84 1.93
Suicidal ideation <.01 .01 .95 1.96
Lifetime number of suicide attempts .05 .06 .41 .01 1.50

Discussion

The objective of the current study was to provide the first empirical evaluation of suicidal behavior within the context of stress generation. In this initial step toward determining the applicability of the stress generation hypothesis to recurrence of suicidal behavior, we assessed whether a greater lifetime history of suicidal behavior was prospectively associated with higher rates of dependent stress, but not independent stress in an adolescent psychiatric inpatient sample.

We found support for the study hypotheses, with the evidence strong for associations with dependent stress and relatively modest for the absence of associations with independent stress. Both conventional NHST and Bayesian analyses were consistent in yielding significant medium effects and substantial support, respectively, for lifetime number of suicide attempts being positively associated with prospective dependent stress. Although NHST yielded a non-significant p value and a small effect size, and Bayesian analyses similarly supported the absence of an association between lifetime number of suicide attempts and independent stress, the Bayes factor was modest despite the corresponding relatively large p value. Collectively, these findings are indicative of the limitations of NHST and the importance of more advanced statistical methodology in evaluating the stress generation hypothesis, especially the component involving independent stress. Given the modest support from Bayesian analyses for the hypotheses regarding independent stress, additional research replicating the current findings is required before greater confidence can be assigned to them.

It should be noted that the possibilities of suicidal behavior being associated with elevated dependent and independent stress, respectively, are not mutually exclusive. Elucidating the relation between suicidal behavior and independent stress arguably has equally important theoretical implications as does documenting its association with dependent stress. If suicidal behavior were predictive of prospectively occurring independent stress, it would not preclude the possible relevance of stress generation to this behavior, and it would be suggestive of the potential relevance of the harsh environment hypothesis, the notion that risk for recurrence of suicidal behavior may arise from being situated in an adverse environment wherein exposure to chronic external stressors is naturally more frequent (Grandin, Alloy, & Abramson, 2007).

This possibility of suicidal behavior prospectively predicting both dependent and independent stress may also be interpreted within the context of research on gene-environment correlation (rGE; Kendler, 2010; Rutter, Moffitt, & Caspi, 2006). rGE specifically refers to genetic influences on exposure to environmental risks through three processes. First, active rGE involves the individual actively selecting or shaping the environment in a manner consistent with their genetic predisposition. Second, evocative rGE refers to the tendency for the individual’s genetic predispositions to elicit certain environments. These concepts are congruent with the stress generation hypothesis inasmuch as the individual may self-select into environments that heighten the possibility of negative self-driven behavior (e.g., an adolescent being at greater risk of engaging in risky behavior in part because they self-selected into a deviant peer group) or the individual evokes negative interactions with others within their environment (e.g., an adolescent’s aggressive tendencies shaping their peer social environment). Third, passive rGE involves differential exposure to an environment for reasons other than the individual’s direct involvement (e.g., independent stress). Here, the individual’s environment is shaped by other influences driven by shared genetic variance (e.g., parents or siblings). Within this framework, it is conceivable that individuals with a history of suicidal behavior are prospectively at greater risk for exposure to independent life stress because they are affected by stress generation in their familial network (e.g., parental job loss and marital conflict), especially insofar as suicidal behavior aggregates within families (Brent, Bridge, Johnson, & Connolly, 1996; Kim et al., 2005). Adding to this possibility is the finding that familial aggregation of suicide is, in part, influenced by cluster B personality traits (McGirr et al., 2009), which in turn have been implicated in stress generation (Liu & Alloy, 2010). Finally, independent stress is of clinical importance given the negative mental health impact it may have (Harkness, Bruce, & Lumley, 2006). Thus, in addition to the aforementioned statistical grounds, there are clear theoretical and clinical reasons for further research to clarify the association between suicidal behavior and independent stress.

Although the current study is the first to provide empirical support for the role of suicidal behavior in the generation of dependent life stress, it provides only a partial evaluation of the etiological chain underlying the recurrence of suicidal behavior posited by the stress generation hypothesis. In this regard, it does not differ from the stress generation literature more generally, wherein there is a want of research fully evaluating the etiological chain linking past and future depressive episodes through the mediational effect of stress generation (Liu, 2013). That is, to our knowledge, only one study to date has examined whether stress generation mediates the association between past and future depression (Bos, Bouhuys, Geerts, van Os, & Ormel, 2007), doubtless a reflection of the significant challenge in capturing a sufficient number of prospectively occurring depressive episodes for statistically powered analyses. Similarly, the low-base rate for suicidal behavior, and attendant difficulties in achieving sufficient statistical power for meaningful analysis, have long been recognized as a significant challenge inherent in studying this phenomenon (Borges et al., 2006; Klonsky, May, & Saffer, 2016; Nock et al., 2008; Prinstein, 2008; Prinstein et al., 2008). Nonetheless, to demonstrate the clinical significance of stress generation in recurrent suicidal behavior, this challenge would need to be addressed.it is imperative for future investigations to ascertain whether higher rates of dependent stress mediate the association between past and future suicide attempts. Given accumulating evidence that dependent stressors, particularly within interpersonal domains, are most etiologically relevant to suicidal behavior (Liu & Miller, 2014), there is particularly a need for future studies to evaluate the mediational role of these dependent stressors in the recurrence of this behavior. Such research has the potential to inform our understanding of how to break the link between past and future suicide attempts. Several methodological strategies should therefore be considered adequately to address this challenge in future research, including the adoption of substantially large samples, high-risk samples, and longer follow-ups (Bredemeier & Miller, 2015; Franklin et al., 2017; Glenn & Nock, 2014; Wenzel et al., 2011).

In addition to determining the intrapersonal sequelæ of stress generation, the potential interpersonal consequences of the stress generation effect of suicidal behavior warrant consideration in future research. One phenomenologically interesting and clinically important possibility is that stress generation may be a mechanism underlying psychopathological contagion effects (Liu, 2013). It could be that the greater dependent stress, particularly within interpersonal domains, experienced by an individual with a history of suicidal behavior places a strain on the individual’s interpersonal relationships, which are likewise exposed to these same stressors. These stressors, in turn, may elevate risk of stress-related psychopathology (e.g., suicide and depression) within the individual’s social network. Congruent with this possibility, stress shared by college roommates has been found in one study to function as a potential mechanism underlying contagion of suicidal ideation (Joiner, 2003). The potential interpersonal pathogenic effects of stress generation may be all the more important during adolescence, given the heightened value placed on peer relationships at this developmental period (Choudhury, Blakemore, & Charman, 2006; Somerville, 2013).

Although not the principal focus of this study, depression was not associated with prospectively occurring dependent stress, an interesting finding when viewed within the context of the robustness of this association in the broader stress generation literature (Liu & Alloy, 2010). This was observed at the bivariate level, and in multivariate models with both conventional NHST and Bayesian analysis. A probable explanation for these findings is the high levels of depression in the current sample, with the mean CDRS-R score being almost a full standard deviation above the clinical cut-point and 86.1% of the sample having a history of major depression. Although the high degree of depressive symptom severity is not unexpected in a clinically acute inpatient sample, its associated restricted range likely limited the ability to evaluate depressive symptoms in relation to stress generation. Caution should therefore be taken against interpreting the current findings as evidence against stress generation in depression.

Similarly, the absence of a significant association between dependent stress leading up to the baseline hospitalization and lifetime history of suicide attempts should not be taken to be inconsistent with the stress generation hypothesis. Rather, it is important to consider this finding within context of the fact that patients were systematically recruited into the study at one of the most stressful times in their lives (i.e., psychiatric inpatient hospitalization), regardless of their suicide attempt history or psychiatric presentation. In fact, significant recent elevations in life stress are characteristic of and indeed determining factors in the decision to hospitalize the study participants.

The finding that suicidal behavior, rather than ideation, was associated with stress generation also warrants discussion. Several considerations may be noted. First, from a clinical perspective, stress generation was proposed as a mechanism accounting for the often recurrent nature of depression (Hammen, 2006), why it is that past depression is one of the most reliable predictors of its future occurrence. If stress generation were only a proximal or temporally delimited phenomenon, and insofar as depressive episodes often do not occur in close temporal proximity to each other, it would be limited in its ability to account for the etiological link between past and future depression. That is, stress generation would simply become a theory accounting for depressive relapse rather than recurrence. This seems unlikely to be the case, as stress generation also appears to occur during depressive remission (Hammen, 2006). With respect to suicidal ideation, this phenomenon is often very time-delimited, in contrast to lifetime suicide attempt history. Indeed, recent ecological momentary assessment studies have found considerable fluctuations in ideation over the course of even a day (Kleiman et al., 2017). It may therefore be an unstable candidate for stress generation over a six-month follow-up, and less able than suicide attempt history to account for processes linking past and future suicidal behavior. Second, stress generation is an example of an action theory (Hammen, 2006), in that the individual is conceptualized as an active agent in shaping his or her environment rather than being passively element in it. Thus, although cognitive factors have been implicated in stress generation, behavioral risk factors are likely more directly relevant to this phenomenon (Liu, 2013). Third, if suicidal ideation is involved in stress generation, we would be unlike to observe this effect in the current study, given that participants were drawn from a psychiatric inpatient unit and acutely suicidal, having been admitted for engaging in suicidal behavior or for expressing ideation to a degree that prompted concerns regarding imminent risk. Consequently, although our sample is well suited for evaluating suicide attempt history in relation to stress generation, the limited range in ideation limited the likelihood that a stress generation effect for suicidal ideation would be observed. That is, the number of suicide attempts was more likely to differentiate these participants than was an index of acute suicidality (i.e., current ideation) specifically because they were recruited at a time of particular acuity. This is not to say that suicidal ideation has no potential relevance to stress generation. Rather, it is unlikely to be the primary factor driving this phenomenon in individuals at risk for suicidal behavior, but instead may heighten it during times of clinical acuity (e.g., in within-person analyses of suicidal ideation and prospective life stress at multiple time-points). Beyond establishing prior suicidal behavior as a predictor of stress generation, it would be important to identify the specific stress generation mechanisms underlying this relationship. That is, what is it about suicidal behavior that makes it associated with stress generation? Just as the relation between depression and stress generation may be primarily accounted for by third variables conferring risk for both (e.g., personality pathology; Hammen, 2006), we propose that rather than inherently and directly driving stress generation, suicidal behavior may largely function as a proxy of risk for this phenomenon, with a greater number of suicide attempts being reflective of correspondingly greater risk. It then naturally follows to consider what these processes may be.

It is first important, however, to contextualize stress generation and the processes that precede and follow it along the pathway in its relation to suicidal behavior. Insofar as life stress may precipitate suicidal behavior (Bagge, Glenn, & Lee, 2013), stress generation may be conceptualized as a relatively proximal process in this pathway. If lifetime number of suicide attempts is a proxy for third-variable causal risk factors for suicidal behavior with stress generation serving as a mediating mechanism, these causal risk factors are necessarily distal and thus predominantly trait-like ones (Franklin et al., 2017; Glenn & Nock, 2014) in relation to suicidal behavior (although, as mentioned above, this does not preclude the possibility of state influences intermittently exacerbating their effect on stress generation, as may potentially be the case with suicidal ideation, for example).

As for what may be the risk factors driving stress generation in suicidal behavior, this is a potential avenue for future empirical consideration. Given the aforementioned relationships between suicidal behavior and deficits in decision-making and interpersonal problem-solving, as well as the associations between these suicide risk factors and difficulties in interpersonal relationships (Jollant et al., 2007; Pollock & Williams, 2004), these risk processes may be promising candidates. Similarly, given the association between personality pathology and suicide (McGirr et al., 2009) and stress generation (Liu & Alloy, 2010), respectively, dysfunctional interpersonal styles, more broadly, may also be important to evaluate in future research. Also a potential candidate for stress generation in suicidal behavior is impulsivity, among the most widely studied psychological constructs in relation to suicide (Wenzel & Beck, 2008). There is also preliminary evidence implicating impulsivity in stress generation (Liu & Kleiman, 2012). Furthermore, of relevance to the study of adolescent suicidal behavior, trait impulsivity is more strongly related to suicidality in this period of development than in early adulthood (Kasen, Cohen, & Chen, 2011; McGirr et al., 2008).

As for processes more proximally pertinent to the effect of stress generation on suicide risk (i.e., processes that account for why stress generation leads to suicidal behavior in certain individuals, deterioration of coping skills, particularly when confronted with negative affect produced by generated stress, may be a promising candidate for future investigation. Such decline in coping skills when exposed to negative affect has been documented in individuals with a history of suicidality (Williams, Barnhofer, Crane, & Beck, 2005).

It is worth mentioning that if suicidal behavior should indeed prove to be a proxy of risk for stress generation rather than directly lead to this outcome, its importance in this phenomenon should not be discounted. Rather, its role as a proxy of risk may be of particular value in clinical contexts, where determinations of clinical risk are reliant on brief assessments; assessments of suicide attempt history can generally be conducted more expediently than assessments of constructs potentially more directly driving stress generation (e.g., impulsivity and neuroticism).

Another intriguing avenue for future study may be to determine the types of the stressors most relevant to suicidal behavior and stress generation, as well as developmental influences on the types of stressors most pertinent to this association. Interpersonal stressors in particular have been implicated in risk for suicidal behavior (Gould, 2003; Liu & Miller, 2014). Furthermore, among younger adolescents with suicidal behavior, parent-child conflict appears to be most commonly observed, whereas among older adolescents counterparts, romantic stressors feature more prominently (Gould, 2003). Similarly fine-grained determinations of the stressors involved in stress generation in suicidal adolescents may advance strategies to detect particularly at-risk individuals as well as provide more precision to efforts to identify the areas of interpersonal functioning most in need of clinical intervention.

A limitation of the current study that would also be important to address in future investigations is whether a sex difference exists in the stress generation effect of suicidal behavior. The small proportion of males in the sample of the current study did not allow for an adequate assessment of this possibility. There is accumulating theoretical and empirical support, however, for sex differences in stress generation with depression, with the evidence to date indicating that this effect is stronger in females than in males (Hammen, 2009b; Hankin & Abramson, 2001; Liu & Alloy, 2010; Rudolph & Hammen, 1999; Shih, 2006). The extent to which female suicide attempters similarly experience greater stress generation than do their male counterparts is of clinical relevance. Sex differences in stress generation may, in some measure, account for the sex difference in rates of attempted suicide that emerges in early adolescence, with girls in engaging in non-fatal suicide attempts at a higher rate than boys (Nock et al., 2008).

Finally, further elucidating the involvement of stress generation and the potentially differential roles of dependent and independent stress in risk for recurrence of suicidal behavior has potential directly to inform treatment. Dependent stressors, unlike independent ones, are modifiable. Thus, support for stress generation in suicidal behavior suggests that intervention efforts with individuals with a history of this behavior would benefit from focusing on behavior modification strategies targeting stress generation mechanisms. Through such strategies, the severity and frequency of dependent stress may be reduced. If independent stress is relevant to recurrent suicidal behavior, intervention efforts with individuals with prior suicidal behavior may instead benefit from focusing on skills to cope with stressors when they do occur. Through such tailored strategies, vulnerability to these stressors may be reduced.

Supplementary Material

1

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention (Grant PDF-0–10-252). Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01MH101138. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies.

Footnotes

1

When length of follow-up was included as a covariate in analyses, the results remained essentially unchanged, and this variable was not a significant predictor in any of the analyses (see Supplemental Table 1).

2

Comparisons were conducted of adolescents with and without a history of suicide attempts on baseline study variables. These are presented in Supplemental Table 2.

3

Four participants reported a high lifetime history of suicide attempts (i.e., > 8). When outliers in number of lifetime attempts were accounted for by winsorizing the data, the results remained essentially unchanged (See Supplemental Table 3).

4

In sensitivity analyses, both multivariate models were reassessed with lifetime history of major depression substituting for current depressive symptom severity as a covariate. The results remained largely unchanged (see Supplemental Table 4). Additionally, for the variables included in the multivariate models presented here, tolerance > .67 and VIF < 1.49.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bagge CL, Glenn CR, & Lee H-J (2013). Quantifying the impact of recent negative life events on suicide attempts. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 359–368. doi: 10.1037/a0030371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Borges G, Angst J, Nock MK, Ruscio AM, Walters EE, & Kessler RC (2006). A risk index for 12-month suicide attempts in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Psychological Medicine, 36, 1747–1757. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706008786 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bos EH, Bouhuys AL, Geerts E, van Os TWDP, & Ormel J (2007). Stressful life events as a link between problems in nonverbal communication and recurrence of depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 97, 161–169. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bredemeier K, & Miller IW (2015). Executive function and suicidality: A systematic qualitative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 170–183. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.06.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brent DA, Bridge J, Johnson BA, & Connolly J (1996). Suicidal behavior runs in families. A controlled family study of adolescent suicide victims. Archives of general psychiatry, 53, 1145–1152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830120085015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brent DA, Greenhill LL, Compton S, Emslie G, Wells K, Walkup JT, Vitiello B, et al. (2009). The treatment of Adolescent Suicide Attempters Study (TASA): Predictors of suicidal events in an open treatment trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 987–996. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b5dbe4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brown BB (1990). Peer groups and peer cultures. In Feldman SS & Elliott GR (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 171–196). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  9. Brown GW (1989). Life events and measurement. In Brown GW & Harris TO (Eds.), Life events and illness (pp. 3–45). London: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  10. Brown GW, & Harris TO (1978). Social Origins of Depression New York: The Free Press. [Google Scholar]
  11. Burcusa SL, & Iacono WG (2007). Risk for recurrence in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 959–985. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016). Increase in Suicide in the United States, 1999–2014 Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/databriefs/db241.htm
  13. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2017). Web-based Injury Statistics Query and Reporting System (WISQARS) Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/injury/wisqars/LeadingCauses.html [Google Scholar]
  14. Choudhury S, Blakemore S-J, & Charman T (2006). Social cognitive development during adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 165–174. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Compas BE, Davis GE, & Forsythe CJ (1985). Characteristics of life events during adolescence. American Journal of Community Psychology, 13, 677–691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Conway CC, Hammen C, & Brennan PA (2012). Expanding stress generation theory: Test of a transdiagnostic model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 754–766. doi: 10.1037/a0027457 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dienes Z (2011). Bayesian versus orthodox statistics: Which side are you on? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 274–290. doi: 10.1177/1745691611406920 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dohrenwend BP (2006). Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology: Toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 477–495. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.477 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Eberhart NK, & Hammen CL (2009). Interpersonal predictors of stress generation. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 544–556. doi: 10.1177/0146167208329857 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Franklin JC, Ribeiro JD, Fox KR, Bentley KH, Kleiman EM, Huang X, Musacchio KM, et al. (2017). Risk factors for suicidal thoughts and behaviors: A meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 187–232. doi: 10.1037/bul0000084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gallistel CR (2009). The importance of proving the null. Psychological Review, 116, 439–453. doi: 10.1037/a0015251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Glenn CR, & Nock MK (2014). Improving the short-term prediction of suicidal behavior. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 47, S176–180. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2014.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gould MS (2003). Youth suicide risk and preventive interventions: A review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 386–405. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046821.95464.CF [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Grandin LD, Alloy LB, & Abramson LY (2007). Childhood stressful life events and bipolar spectrum disorders. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26, 460–478. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2007.26.4.460 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Grotevant HD, & Cooper CR (1985). Patterns of interaction in family relationships and the development of identity exploration in adolescence. Child Development, 56, 415–428. doi: 10.2307/1129730 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hammen C (1991). Generation of stress in the course of unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 555–561. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hammen C (2005). Stress and depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 293–319. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143938 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hammen C (2006). Stress generation in depression: Reflections on origins, research, and future directions. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 1065–1082. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20293 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hammen C (2009a). Adolescent depression: Stressful interpersonal contexts and risk for recurrence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 200–204. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01636.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hammen C (2009b). Stress exposure and stress generation in adolescent depression. In Nolen-Hoeksema S & Hilt LM (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent depression (pp. 305–333). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  31. Hammen C, & Brennan PA (2001). Depressed adolescents of depressed and nondepressed mothers: Tests of an interpersonal impairment hypothesis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 284–294. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.69.2.284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hammen C, Henry R, & Daley SE (2000). Depression and sensitization to stressors among young women as a function of childhood adversity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 782–787. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.5.782 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hankin BL, & Abramson LY (2001). Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability–transactional stress theory. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 773–796. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hankin BL, Stone L, & Wright PA (2010). Corumination, interpersonal stress generation, and internalizing symptoms: Accumulating effects and transactional influences in a multiwave study of adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 217–235. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990368 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Harkness KL, Bruce AE, & Lumley MN (2006). The role of childhood abuse and neglect in the sensitization to stressful life events in adolescent depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 730–741. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.730 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Harkness KL, & Monroe SM (2016). The assessment and measurement of adult life stress: Basic premises, operational principles, and design requirements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 727–745. doi: 10.1037/abn0000178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hernandez EM, Trout ZM, & Liu RT (2016). Vulnerability-specific stress generation: Childhood emotional abuse and the mediating role of depressogenic interpersonal processes. Child Abuse & Neglect, 62, 132–141. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.10.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jarosz AF, & Wiley J (2014). What are the odds? A practical guide to computing and reporting Bayes factors. Journal of Problem Solving, 7, 2–9. [Google Scholar]
  39. JASP Team. (2016). JASP (Version 0.8.0.1). [Google Scholar]
  40. Jeffreys H (1961). Theory of Probability Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  41. Johnson SL, & Roberts JE (1995). Life events and bipolar disorder: Implications from biological theories. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 434–449. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.434 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Joiner TE (2003). Contagion of suicidal symptoms as a function of assortative relating and shared relationship stress in college roommates. Journal of Adolescence, 26, 495–504. doi: 10.1016/S0140-1971(02)00133-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Jollant F, Guillaume S, Jaussent I, Castelnau D, Malafosse A, & Courtet P (2007). Impaired decision-making in suicide attempters may increase the risk of problems in affective relationships. Journal of Affective Disorders, 99, 59–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.07.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kasen S, Cohen P, & Chen H (2011). Developmental course of impulsivity and capability from age 10 to age 25 as related to trajectory of suicide attempt in a community cohort. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 41, 180–192. doi: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.2011.00017.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, et al. (1997). Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kendler KS (2010). Genetic and environmental pathways to suicidal behavior: reflections of a genetic epidemiologist. European Psychiatry, 25, 300–303. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kessler RC (1997). The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 191–214. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.191 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kim CD, Seguin M, Therrien N, Riopel G, Chawky N, Lesage AD, & Turecki G (2005). Familial aggregation of suicidal behavior: A family study of male suicide completers from the general population. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1017–1019. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.1017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kleiman EM, Turner BJ, Fedor S, Beale EE, Huffman JC, & Nock MK (2017). Examination of real-time fluctuations in suicidal ideation and its risk factors: Results from two ecological momentary assessment studies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126, 726– 738. doi: 10.1037/abn0000273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Klonsky ED, May AM, & Saffer BY (2016). Suicide, suicide attempts, and suicidal ideation. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12, 307–330. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Leon AC, Friedman RA, Sweeney JA, Brown RP, & Mann JJ (1990). Statistical issues in the identification of risk factors for suicidal behavior: the application of survival analysis. Psychiatry Research, 31, 99–108. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(90)90112-I [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Liu RT (2013). Stress generation: Future directions and clinical implications. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 406–416. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Liu RT, & Alloy LB (2010). Stress generation in depression: A systematic review of the empirical literature and recommendations for future study. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 582–593. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Liu RT, & Kleiman EM (2012). Impulsivity and the generation of negative life events: The role of negative urgency. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 609–612. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Liu RT, & Miller I (2014). Life events and suicidal ideation and behavior: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 34, 181–192. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2014.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. McGirr A, Alda M, Séguin M, Cabot S, Lesage A, & Turecki G (2009). Familial aggregation of suicide explained by cluster B traits: A three-group family study of suicide controlling for major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 1124–1134. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08111744 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. McGirr A, Renaud J, Bureau A, Seguin M, Lesage A, & Turecki G (2008). Impulsive-aggressive behaviours and completed suicide across the life cycle: A predisposition for younger age of suicide. Psychological Medicine, 38, 407–417. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001419 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Monck E, & Dobbs R (1985). Measuring life events in an adolescent population: Methodological issues and related findings. Psychological Medicine, 15, 841–850. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700005079 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Monroe SM (2008). Modern approaches to conceptualizing and measuring human life stress. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 33–52. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Nock MK, Borges G, Bromet EJ, Cha CB, Kessler RC, & Lee S (2008). Suicide and suicidal behavior. Epidemiologic Reviews, 30, 133–154. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ougrin D, Tranah T, Stahl D, Moran P, & Asarnow JR (2015). Therapeutic interventions for suicide attempts and self-harm in adolescents: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 97–107.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Parke RD, & Bhavnagri N (1989). Parents as managers of children’s peer relationships. In Belle D (Ed.), Children’s social networks and social supports (pp. 241–259). New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  63. Paykel ES, Prusoff BA, & Myers JK (1975). Suicide attempts and recent life events: A controlled comparison. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 327–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pettit JW, Green KL, Grover KE, Schatte DJ, & Morgan ST (2011). Domains of chronic stress and suicidal behaviors among inpatient adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 494–499. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2011.563466 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pollock LR, & Williams JMG (2004). Problem-solving in suicide attempters. Psychological Medicine, 34, 163–167. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703008092 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Posner K, Brown GK, Stanley B, Brent DA, Yershova KV, Oquendo MA, Currier GW, et al. (2011). The Columbia–Suicide Severity Rating Scale: Initial validity and internal consistency findings from three multisite studies with adolescents and adults. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 1266–1277. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10111704 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Posner K, Oquendo MA, Gould M, Stanley B, & Davies M (2007). Columbia Classification Algorithm of Suicide Assessment (C-CASA): Classification of suicidal events in the FDA’s pediatric suicidal risk analysis of antidepressants. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1035–1043. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.7.1035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Poznanski E, & Mokros H (1995). Children’s depression rating scale, Revised (CDRS-R) Manual
  69. Prinstein MJ (2008). Introduction to the special section on suicide and nonsuicidal self-injury: A review of unique challenges and important directions for self-injury science. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 1–8. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Prinstein MJ, Nock MK, Simon V, Aikins JW, Cheah CSL, & Spirito A (2008). Longitudinal trajectories and predictors of adolescent suicidal ideation and attempts following inpatient hospitalization. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 92– 103. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.1.92 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Reynolds W (1988). Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire: Professional Manual Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
  72. Ribeiro JD, Franklin JC, Fox KR, Bentley KH, Kleiman EM, Chang BP, & Nock MK (2016). Self-injurious thoughts and behaviors as risk factors for future suicide ideation, attempts, and death: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Medicine, 46, 225–236. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715001804 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rudolph KD (2008). Developmental influences on interpersonal stress generation in depressed youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 673–679. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.3.673 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Rudolph KD, & Hammen C (1999). Age and gender as determinants of stress exposure, generation, and reactions in youngsters: A transactional perspective. Child Development, 70, 660–677. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rutter M, Moffitt TE, & Caspi A (2006). Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: Multiple varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 226–261. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01557.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Sheets ES, & Craighead WE (2014). Comparing chronic interpersonal and noninterpersonal stress domains as predictors of depression recurrence in emerging adults. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 63, 36–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Shih JH (2006). Sex differences in stress generation: an examination of sociotropy/autonomy, stress, and depressive symptoms. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 434–446. doi: 10.1177/0146167205282739 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Shih JH, Abela JRZ, & Starrs C (2009). Cognitive and interpersonal predictors of stress generation in children of affectively ill parents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 195–208. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9267-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Somerville LH (2013). The teenage brain: Sensitivity to social evaluation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 121–127. doi: 10.1177/0963721413476512 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Steinberg L (2002). Adolescence (6th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  81. Steinberg L, & Silverberg SB (1986). The vicissitudes of autonomy in early adolescence. Child Development, 57, 841–851. doi: 10.2307/1130361 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Stellrecht NE, Joiner TE, & Rudd MD (2006). Responding to and treating negative interpersonal processes in suicidal depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 1129– 1140. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20298 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Stroud CB, Davila J, Hammen C, & Vrshek-Schallhorn S (2011). Severe and nonsevere events in first onsets versus recurrences of depression: Evidence for stress sensitization. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 142–154. doi: 10.1037/a0021659 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Uliaszek AA, Zinbarg RE, Mineka S, Craske MG, Griffith JW, Sutton JM, Epstein A, et al. (2012). A longitudinal examination of stress generation in depressive and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 4–15. doi: 10.1037/a0025835 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. van Heeringen K (2012). Stress–diathesis model of suicidal behavior. In Dwivedi Y (Ed.), The Neurobiological Basis of Suicide, Frontiers in Neuroscience Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis. [Google Scholar]
  86. Vrshek-Schallhorn S, Stroud CB, Mineka S, Hammen C, Zinbarg RE, Wolitzky-Taylor K, & Craske MG (2015). Chronic and episodic interpersonal stress as statistically unique predictors of depression in two samples of emerging adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124, 918–932. doi: 10.1037/abn0000088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wagenmakers E-J, Wetzels R, Borsboom D, & van der Maas HLJ (2011). Why psychologists must change the way they analyze their data: The case of psi: Comment on Bem (2011). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 426–432. doi: 10.1037/a0022790 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wenzel A, & Beck AT (2008). A cognitive model of suicidal behavior: Theory and treatment. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12, 189–201. doi: 10.1016/j.appsy.2008.05.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wenzel A, Berchick ER, Tenhave T, Halberstadt S, Brown GK, & Beck AT (2011). Predictors of suicide relative to other deaths in patients with suicide attempts and suicide ideation: A 30-year prospective study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 132, 375–382. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wilkinson L, & the Task Force on Statistical Inference. (1999). Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist, 54, 594–604. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.8.594 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Williams JMG, Barnhofer T, Crane C, & Beck AT (2005). Problem solving deteriorates following mood challenge in formerly depressed patients with a history of suicidal ideation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 421–431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Wray-Lake L, Crouter AC, & McHale SM (2010). Developmental patterns in decision-making autonomy across middle childhood and adolescence: European American parents perspectives. Child Development, 81, 636–651. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01420.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES