Abstract
Recent work reveals that the extinction of conditioned fear depends upon the interval between conditioning and extinction. Extinction training that takes place within minutes to hours after fear conditioning fails to produce a long-term extinction memory, a phenomenon known as the immediate extinction deficit (IED). Neurobiological evidence suggests that the IED results from stress-induced dysregulation of prefrontal cortical circuits involved in extinction learning. However, a recent study in humans suggests that an “event boundary” between fear conditioning and extinction protects the conditioning memory from interference by the extinction memory, resulting in high levels of fear during a retrieval test. Here, we contrast these hypotheses in rats by arranging extinction trials to follow conditioning trials with or without an event boundary; in both cases, extinction trials are delivered in proximity to shock-elicited stress. After fear conditioning, rats either received extinction trials 60-sec after the last conditioning trial (continuous, no event boundary) or 15-minutes after conditioning (segmented, a standard “immediate” extinction procedure associated with an event boundary). Both groups of animals showed decreases in conditional freezing to the auditory conditioned stimulus (CS) during extinction and exhibited an equivalent IED relative to non-extinguished controls when tested 48 hours later. Thus, eliminating the event boundary between conditioning and extinction with the continuous extinction procedure did not prevent the IED. These data suggest that the IED is the result of shock-induced stress, rather than boundary-induced reductions in memory interference.
Subject terms: Extinction, Fear conditioning
Introduction
Traumatic events often result in long-lasting emotional memories that persist for years. Commonly used therapeutic strategies for pathological fear include exposure therapy, a procedure in which reminders of the trauma are presented in a safe context in order to reduce maladaptive fear. Extinction procedures, such as exposure therapy, are thought to work by forming a new inhibitory “extinction” memory that interferes with the expression of the original “fear” memory. However, there are numerous circumstances by which extinction learning fails to result in a long-term extinction memory. Recent data from both humans and rodents suggest that extinction training that takes place soon (~15 min–6 hours) after fear conditioning fails to reduce fear to the extinguished cue when tested days (48 hr) later; a phenomenon that has come to be known as the immediate extinction deficit (IED)1,2.
Although the underlying mechanism for the IED is not understood, we have proposed that the stress and emotional arousal associated with fear conditioning impedes extinction learning3 (Fig. 1A). Consistent with this, the IED is not observed after fear conditioning procedures that use relatively weak shock4 or a single conditioning trial1. Moreover, delivering arousing footshocks prior to a delayed extinction procedure impairs long-term extinction1. It is well known that stress dysregulates the medial prefrontal cortical circuitry involved in extinction learning5. We have recently shown that fear conditioning is associated with a reduction in the spontaneous firing of infralimbic (IL) cortical neurons involved in extinction learning; systemic propranolol administration eliminates this effect and rescues the IED6. Hence, the suppression of IL activity precipitated by the stress and noradrenergic hyperarousal associated with fear conditioning may be responsible for the IED.
A recent study in humans, however, offers a different interpretation of the IED. As shown in Fig. 1B, Dunsmoor and colleagues7 have proposed that a temporal break (an “event boundary”) between conditioning and extinction might protect the conditioning memory from interference by the extinction memory7. This process, known as “event segmentation”, is critical for organizing daily events into unique memories that are distinct from previous or subsequent events8–11. Indeed, the “chunking” of daily events is thought to facilitate memory storage, processing, and retrieval, and information that crosses event boundaries is often remembered less well8,12. To test this hypothesis, Dunsmoor and colleagues7 examined episodic recall of a fear conditioning episode in healthy humans that underwent an extinction procedure in which there was either no event boundary (a normal 10 second inter-trial interval occurred) or an explicit event boundary (subjects were verbally informed of a 10 second break) separating conditioning and extinction. Subjects that experienced the explicit event boundary exhibited greater recall of the conditioning episode (and less durable extinction) relative to subjects that did not experience the event boundary. In other words, subjects that experienced an event boundary between conditioning and extinction exhibited an IED, whereas those that received a continuous procedure did not. The authors suggested that event segmentation of conditioning and extinction promotes the consolidation of the fear memory at the expense of the extinction memory. By this logic, the IED previously reported in both rodents and humans after fear conditioning could be due to event segmentation, rather than a stress-induced extinction impairment.
To test this hypothesis, we compared our standard IED procedure (extinction trials administered 15-min after the last conditioning trial) with a continuous extinction procedure (extinction trials administered 60-sec after the last conditioning trial) after auditory fear conditioning in rats. If event segmentation is necessary for the IED, then we hypothesized that the IED would be eliminated with a continuous extinction procedure. However, in contrast with this hypothesis, the results revealed that eliminating the event boundary between conditioning and extinction did not rescue the IED. Both the continuous and segmented extinction procedures yielded poor long-term extinction, which was manifest as high levels of conditioned freezing to the auditory conditioned stimulus (CS) that did not differ from non-extinguished controls. The most parsimonious interpretation of these results is that the arousal associated with fear conditioning impedes extinction procedures that occur within hours after conditioning.
Results
To determine if the IED is a result of event segmentation we devised an experiment (Fig. 2A) in which extinction sessions were presented continuously with fear conditioning (continuous, CONT), after a 15 min break (segmented, SEG), or animals were not extinguished at all immediately following fear conditioning (NO-EXT). Freezing behavior are shown in Fig. 2. All animals exhibited reliable increases in freezing across the conditioning session [main effect of trials: F(5,18) = 49.983, p < 0.0001] and the levels of freezing did not differ between the groups [trials x group interaction: F(10,18) = 0.919, p = 0.5195]. During the extinction procedure, all groups exhibited reliable decreases in freezing behavior [main effect of block: F(8,18) = 5.154, p < 0.0001], as shown in Fig. 2B,D. Cumulative freezing (Fig. 2C) throughout conditioning and immediate extinction indicates, however, that the magnitude of freezing in each of the extinction groups was different over the course of the session [time x group interaction, F(2,128) = 91.447, p = < 0.0001]. Specifically, rats receiving extinction trials after a 15-min break exhibited higher levels of freezing across the session, largely because the suppression of freezing that accompanies extinction training was delayed by 15-min in this group.
To keep in-line with previous investigations of the IED1, all animals received a retention test consisting of another extinction session 48-hours after conditioning. As shown in Fig. 2D, rats in all of the groups exhibited robust fear to the CS, and animals that underwent extinction did not differ from the no-extinction controls [main effect of group: F(2,18) = 0.677, p = 0.5204; block x group interaction: F(16,18) = 0.827, p = 0.6533]. There were no sex differences in conditioned freezing during any of the behavioral sessions [conditioning: main effect of sex, F(1,18) = 1.728, p = 0.2052; immediate extinction: main effect of sex, F(1,18) = 2.043, p = 0.1701; retention test: main effect of sex, F(1,18) = 4.035 p = 0.0598], and sex did not interact with the timing of extinction relative to conditioning [conditioning: group x sex interaction, F(2,18) = 0.448, p = 0.6457; immediate extinction: group x sex interaction: F(2,18) = 0.798, p = 0.4655; retention test: group x sex interaction, F(2,18) = 0.411, p = 0.6688].
Although these results suggest that event segmentation does not account for the IED in rodents, confirming the lack of difference between the two extinction groups (i.e., the null hypothesis) is difficult to prove. To address this issue, we calculated the Bayes factor for the first 5-trial block of retrieval testing, as differences in behavioral freezing following immediate and delayed extinction are commonly observed in the first 5 trials of retrieval testing1. We found that the null hypothesis (i.e., the IED is not due to event segmentation) was 3.723 times more likely (BF01 = 3.723) than the alternative (i.e., the IED is due to event segmentation)13. Collectively, these data indicate that the IED occurs with both continuous and segmented extinction procedures, and we provide statistical evidence (BF01 > 3) in support of the null hypothesis that event segmentation does not underlie the IED.
Discussion
Here we explored whether the IED is due to the temporal segmentation produced by a 15-min delay between fear conditioning and extinction7. We found that rats that underwent a continuous immediate extinction procedure (with no delay between fear conditioning and extinction) displayed similar within-session extinction to rats that underwent a standard immediate extinction procedure (15-min delay between fear conditioning and extinction). When tested 48 hours later, both groups of animals displayed high levels of freezing that were equivalent to animals that were never extinguished. In other words, animals exhibited an IED independent of whether they experienced an event boundary between conditioning and extinction. Importantly, these findings cannot be contributed to the 48-hour retention interval as we have previously shown that delayed extinction training is retained over 48-hours compared to no-extinction controls1. These results contrast with the findings of Dunsmoor and colleagues7 insofar as the IED was not overcome with a continuous extinction procedure. We instead point to recent evidence that suggests the IED is a stress-induced impairment.
Considerable work has demonstrated that immediate extinction procedures yield poor long-term extinction memories1–3,14. Dunsmoor and colleagues7 recently proposed that the IED might be due to a temporal gap (i.e., an event boundary) between fear conditioning and extinction that serves to prioritize the consolidation of the conditioning memory by protecting it from interference from extinction. By this logic, a truly continuous extinction procedure might limit the IED by weakening the fear memory and facilitating the formation of a long-term extinction memory; thus, immediate extinction without an event boundary may be more effective in producing long-term extinction memories than delayed extinction procedures which are not thought to significantly weaken CS-US associations. This echoes an earlier report by Myers and colleagues4 who suggested that immediate extinction results in the “unlearning” of the original CS-US association. Consistent with this hypothesis, one study in humans has shown that a continuous extinction procedure blocked the reinstatement of fear-potentiated startle15 (i.e., fear did not return after an unsignaled US), suggesting that immediate extinction had weakened the original CS-US associated fear memory. However, other studies in humans using continuous extinction procedures have found strong reinstatement14,16 and spontaneous recovery14 (i.e., return of fear over time) after immediate extinction as measured by skin conductance responses. Together with the present data, these results suggest that continuous extinction procedures are at least no more, if not less, effective in producing long-term extinction memories relative to both standard immediate and delayed extinction procedures17–20.
It is well established that stress influences medial prefrontal cortical circuits known to be involved in extinction learning5,21, specifically the infralimbic cortex (IL)22. Previous work in rodents has shown that immediate extinction results in diminished c-Fos expression (a correlate of neuronal activity) within the IL, and that the IED can be rescued by IL electrical stimulation23. Work from our lab has demonstrated that the IED is associated with impaired IL single-unit activity22,24; this decrement, as well as the IED, can be rescued with the β-adrenergic antagonist, propranolol22,25. Moreover, the IED can be rescued by pharmacological blockade of either norepinephrine25 or corticotrophin releasing factor26 within the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA). By this logic, it seems that impairments in IL activity following fear conditioning underlies the IED, and these impairments may be driven by the BLA. In line with this, stress is known to cause BLA hyperexcitability27, and a recent study demonstrated that chronic stress causes increased glutamatergic release from BLA to prefrontal cortical synapses28. Thus, we propose that the IED is a result of BLA hyperexcitability which acts to inhibit IL function necessary for extinction learning.
However, it is important to note that the IED has also been observed in paradigms that lack aversive stressors including appetitive conditioning tasks in rats and pigeons29,30 and a non-emotional learning task in humans31. Nonetheless, the neural circuits mediating the extinction and relapse of appetitive conditioned responses largely overlap with that of aversive conditioning32,33. For example, IL lesions enhance the recovery and reinstatement34, as well as the renewal35, of appetitive conditioned responses. Moreover, the reinstatement of extinguished appetitive conditioned responses in humans is correlated with both amygdala and ventromedial PFC (the human analog of the IL) BOLD responses36. Although appetitive conditioning does not use aversive reinforcers, it may nonetheless be stressful. Appetitive conditioning in humans is associated with increased galvanic skin conductance responses and is arousing36, and the conditioning and extinction of appetitive responses in rats triggers norepinephrine efflux in the IL37. Additionally, appetitive conditioning procedures commonly motivate animals by food deprivation, a stressor. It has previously been shown that neuronal activity in the locus coeruleus (the primary source of norepinephrine38) is increased in response to a conditioned food reward in food-deprived rats39, but not cats fed ad libitum40. This suggests that appetitive conditioning and extinction may be stressful in food-deprived animals. Thus, it is possible that the IED observed in both aversive and appetitive conditioning procedures is due to their similar dependence on the IL33 and its susceptibility to hyperarousal5.
When considering this evidence, it is therefore not surprising that a continuous extinction procedure did not rescue the IED, because stress-evoked impairments in IL function are present immediately after fear conditioning. Moreover, our previous work shows that impairments in the spontaneous firing of IL neurons are maximal within the first 10 minutes after fear conditioning22, which is precisely when the initial extinction trials are presented in a continuous extinction procedure. What then explains the lack of an IED in humans undergoing a continuous extinction procedure? Given that the USs used in this study were “highly annoying, but painful” wrist shocks7, it is probable that they were not sufficiently stressful to induce an IED. Similarly, the extinction deficit observed in subjects experiencing an event boundary between conditioning and extinction is therefore not likely to be a stress-induced IED. Alternatively, as Dunmoor and colleagues have suggested, the verbal declaration of a transition from conditioning to extinction in their task was sufficient to limit retroactive interference of the conditioning memory by the extinction procedure. It is well established that event segmentation weakens the long-term memory of information which crosses event boundaries11,41,42, therefore it is possible that removing the event boundary in humans (continuous extinction) disrupts the fear memory established during conditioning. Moreover, an IED has been reported in a non-emotional predictive learning task in humans, as long as acquisition and extinction of the task occured in the same context31. However, it is unclear if an explicit break was inserted between acquisition and extinction. If so, it is likely that these results are indeed due to event segmentation. Interestingly, the rodent hippocampus encodes boundaries between trial and inter-trial intervals43, which suggests a neurobiological substrate for event segmentation similar to that in humans44. Further work is needed to elucidate whether event segmentation influences memory encoding and retrieval in rodents independently of the stress effects that account for the IED.
Another difference between the current work and that by Dunsmoor and colleagues7, is the nature of the conditioning procedure in the two studies. In the human study, subjects received a differential fear conditioning procedure in which visual images from one item category were paired with a wrist shock (CS+, animals or tools; 50% US probability), while images from a second category were never paired with an aversive outcome (CS-, tools or animals, respectively). In the present work, rats were conditioned and extinguished using a single auditory CS. Although a differential fear conditioning procedure45, might have more closely mirrored the human work, we chose to use a non-differential conditioning procedure to replicate our previous work on the IED1,2,22,24,25. Moreover, in our view, there is no theoretical basis to expect that a differential conditioning procedure would undermine the IED with a continuous extinction procedure in either rats or humans. In contrast, the partial reinforcement schedule in the human study may have made the conditioning and extinction contingencies less discriminable (particularly in the continuous extinction procedure), and this might have encouraged generalization of the conditioning memory and the CS. Indeed, partial reinforcement yields resistance to extinction (i.e., the partial reinforcement extinction effect or PREE46) and the verbal instruction in the human study may have provided information that overcame the PREE by reducing generalization from conditioning to extinction. Future work exploring whether partial reinforcement schedules influence the IEDs in rodents would be important to address this issue.
In conclusion, we show that event segmentation is not responsible for the IED previously described in rats1. This challenges recent work in humans that suggests that an event boundary between fear conditioning and extinction prioritizes the consolidation of the initial conditioning memory, while protecting it from interference by the subsequent extinction memory7. Behavioral and neurobiological evidence in rats instead suggests that the hyperarousal that follows conditioning impairs extinction learning3. This is evident by pharmacological blockade of norepinephrine which can rescue both the IED25 and IED-associated neural impairments22, as well by the fact that delayed extinction can be impaired by a prior stressor1. However, translation between human and rodent work remains difficult. Further work is needed to understand rodent’s capacity for event segmentation and how event boundaries impact their learning and memory, as well as the potential role of stress in the IED observed in appetitive conditioning procedures. Moreover, future studies using humans are needed to investigate IED-associated neural deficits, and their effect on memory, that have been observed in rodents. We stress that future investigations using immediate extinction procedures in humans should take into special consideration the potentially confounding effects of event boundaries when designing experiments.
Methods
Subjects
Twelve male and twelve female adult Long-Evans rats (Blue-Spruce; 50–57 days old; weighing 200–224 g) were obtained from a commercial supplier (Envigo, Indianapolis, IN). Animals were individually housed in cages upon arrival and kept on a 14:10 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 AM) with ad libitum access to food and water. Each animal was handled for ~30 sec each day for 5 days prior to experimentation to habituate them to the experimenter. All of the experiments and procedures were conducted during the daytime (light phase) at the same time each day in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Texas A&M University.
Behavioral apparatus and procedure
All behavioral procedures were conducted in a single room containing 8 standard rodent conditioning chambers (30 × 24 × 21 cm; Med Associates, St. Albans, VT) housed in sound-attenuating cabinets. Each chamber was identical and consisted of two aluminum sides, a Plexiglass ceiling and rear wall, and a hinged Plexiglass door. The grid floor contained 19 steel rods (4 mm diameter) spaced 1.5 cm apart that were connected to a shock source and solid-state grid scrambler (Med Associates) for the delivery of footshocks. A loudspeaker mounted on the outside of a grating in one aluminum wall was used to deliver auditory stimuli. Locomotor activity was transduced into an electrical signal by a load cell under the floor of the chamber. Load cell activity was digitized (Threshold Activity Software; Med Associates) and later used to measure freezing behavior. Absences of locomotor activity were only considered freezing if they persisted for at least 1 sec. All procedures were carried out in the same context. The previously described conditioning chambers were illuminated by individual house lights that were mounted within one of the aluminum sides, a pan containing 3% acetic acid was placed under the grid floor, and the entire room was illuminated by ambient red light.
Behavioral testing for this study consisted of fear conditioning immediately followed by an extinction session, and one extinction retrieval session 48 hr after conditioning. Each group contained 4 females and 4 males (n = 8) and no animals were excluded from the study. Fear conditioning consisted of a 3-min stimulus-free baseline period, followed by five tone (10 sec, 80 dB, 2 kHz)-shock (2 sec, 1 mA) pairings (60 sec ISI). Immediate extinction sessions (45 trials, 60 sec ISI) proceeded within the same context in a group-dependent manner either continuously (CONT) following conditioning, 15 min after conditioning (segmented, SEG), or animals were not immediately extinguished (NO-EXT). For the CONT animals this meant that the first tone-alone (10 sec, 80 dB, 2 kHz) trial was presented after a normal 60 sec ISI following the last shock offset. Time spent in the chambers were equated for each group (i.e. CONT animals remained in the chamber for 15 min after the last extinction trial). All animals were brought back to the same chambers 48 hrs after conditioning to test extinction memory. This session also consisted of a 3 min baseline followed by 45 tone-alone trials with 60 sec ISIs. All freezing data presented here were measured during CS presentation.
Data analysis
All data were analyzed with analysis of variance (ANOVA) with variables of sex, group, and trial for conditioning, extinction, and retrieval testing.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (RO1MH065961, RO1MH117852).
Author Contributions
M.T. and M.P. conducted the experimental procedures. M.T. analyzed the data. M.T. and S.M. contributed to the writing of the manuscript and reviewed the completed manuscript. S.M. designed the experiment.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Maren S, Chang C. Recent fear is resistant to extinction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:18020–18025. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608398103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Chang C, Maren S. Early extinction after fear conditioning yields a context-independent and short-term suppression of conditional freezing in rats. Learn. Mem. 2009;16:62–68. doi: 10.1101/lm.1085009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Maren S. Nature and causes of the immediate extinction deficit: a brief review. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2014;113:19–24. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Myers KM, Ressler KJ, Davis M. Different mechanisms of fear extinction dependent on length of time since fear acquisition. Learn. Mem. 2006;13:216–223. doi: 10.1101/lm.119806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Maren S, Holmes A. Stress and fear extinction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:58–79. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Giustino TF, Maren S. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the conditioning and extinction of fear. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2015;9:298. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Dunsmoor JE, et al. Event segmentation protects emotional memories from competing experiences encoded close in time. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2018;2:291–299. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0317-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Radvansky GA, Zacks JM. Event boundaries in memory and cognition. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2017;17:133–140. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Clewett D, Davachi L. The ebb and flow of experience determines the temporal structure of memory. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2017;17:186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.08.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kurby CA, Zacks JM. Segmentation in the perception and memory of events. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2008;12:72–79. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ezzyat Y, Davachi L. Similarity breeds proximity: pattern similarity within and across contexts is related to later mnemonic judgments of temporal proximity. Neuron. 2014;81:1179–1189. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Radvansky GA, Copeland DE. Walking through doorways causes forgetting: situation models and experienced space. Mem. Cognit. 2006;34:1150–1156. doi: 10.3758/BF03193261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jarosz, A. F. & Wiley, J. What are the odds? A practical guide to computing and reporting bayes factors. J. Probl. Solving7 (2014).
- 14.Schiller D, et al. Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans. Learn. Mem. 2008;15:394–402. doi: 10.1101/lm.909208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Golkar A, Öhman A. Fear extinction in humans: effects of acquisition-extinction delay and masked stimulus presentations. Biol. Psychol. 2012;91:292–301. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.LaBar KS, Phelps EA. Reinstatement of conditioned fear in humans is context dependent and impaired in amnesia. Behav. Neurosci. 2005;119:677–686. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Huff NC, Hernandez JA, Blanding NQ, LaBar KS. Delayed extinction attenuates conditioned fear renewal and spontaneous recovery in humans. Behav. Neurosci. 2009;123:834–843. doi: 10.1037/a0016511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Alvarez RP, Johnson L, Grillon C. Contextual-specificity of short-delay extinction in humans: renewal of fear-potentiated startle in a virtual environment. Learn. Mem. 2007;14:247–253. doi: 10.1101/lm.493707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dirikx T, Hermans D, Vansteenwegen D, Baeyens F, Eelen P. Reinstatement of extinguished conditioned responses and negative stimulus valence as a pathway to return of fear in humans. Learn. Mem. 2004;11:549–554. doi: 10.1101/lm.78004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hermans D, et al. Reinstatement of fear responses in human aversive conditioning. Behav. Res. Ther. 2005;43:533–551. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Holmes A, Wellman CL. Stress-induced prefrontal reorganization and executive dysfunction in rodents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2009;33:773–783. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fitzgerald PJ, Giustino TF, Seemann JR, Maren S. Noradrenergic blockade stabilizes prefrontal activity and enables fear extinction under stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:E3729–37. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1500682112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kim SC, Jo YS, Kim IH, Kim H, Choi J-S. Lack of medial prefrontal cortex activation underlies the immediate extinction deficit. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:832–837. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4145-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Chang C, Berke JD, Maren S. Single-unit activity in the medial prefrontal cortex during immediate and delayed extinction of fear in rats. PLoS One. 2010;5:e11971. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Giustino TF, et al. β-Adrenoceptor Blockade in the Basolateral Amygdala, But Not the Medial Prefrontal Cortex, Rescues the Immediate Extinction Deficit. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:2537–2544. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hollis, F., Sevelinges, Y., Grosse, J., Zanoletti, O. & Sandi, C. Involvement of CRFR1 in the basolateral amygdala in the immediate fear extinction deficit. Eneuro3 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 27.Sharp BM. Basolateral amygdala and stress-induced hyperexcitability affect motivated behaviors and addiction. Transl. Psychiatry. 2017;7:e1194. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lowery-Gionta EG, et al. Chronic stress dysregulates amygdalar output to the prefrontal cortex. Neuropharmacology. 2018;139:68–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.06.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Woods AM, Bouton ME. Immediate extinction causes a less durable loss of performance than delayed extinction following either fear or appetitive conditioning. Learn. Mem. 2008;15:909–920. doi: 10.1101/lm.1078508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rescorla RA. Spontaneous recovery varies inversely with the training-extinction interval. Learn Behav. 2004;32:401–408. doi: 10.3758/BF03196037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Merz CJ, Wolf OT. The immediate extinction deficit occurs in a nonemotional learning paradigm. Learn. Mem. 2019;26:39–45. doi: 10.1101/lm.048223.118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Goode TD, Maren S. Common neurocircuitry mediating drug and fear relapse in preclinical models. Psychopharmacology. 2019;236:415–437. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-5024-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Quirk GJ, Mueller D. Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:56–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rhodes SEV, Killcross S. Lesions of rat infralimbic cortex enhance recovery and reinstatement of an appetitive Pavlovian response. Learn. Mem. 2004;11:611–616. doi: 10.1101/lm.79704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rhodes SEV, Killcross AS. Lesions of rat infralimbic cortex enhance renewal of extinguished appetitive Pavlovian responding. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2007;25:2498–2503. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05486.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ebrahimi C, et al. Opposing roles for amygdala and vmPFC in the return of appetitive conditioned responses in humans. Transl. Psychiatry. 2019;9:148. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0482-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Mingote S, de Bruin JPC, Feenstra MGP. Noradrenaline and dopamine efflux in the prefrontal cortex in relation to appetitive classical conditioning. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:2475–2480. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4547-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Giustino TF, Maren S. Noradrenergic modulation of fear conditioning and extinction. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018;12:43. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sara, S. J. & Segal, M. In Neurobiology of the locus coeruleus88, 571–585 (Elsevier, 1991).
- 40.Rasmussen K, Morilak DA, Jacobs BL. Single unit activity of locus coeruleus neurons in the freely moving cat. I. During naturalistic behaviors and in response to simple and complex stimuli. Brain Res. 1986;371:324–334. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)90370-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ezzyat Y, Davachi L. What constitutes an episode in episodic memory? Psychol. Sci. 2011;22:243–252. doi: 10.1177/0956797610393742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.DuBrow S, Davachi L. Temporal binding within and across events. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2016;134(Pt A):107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bulkin, D. A., Sinclair, D. G., Law, L. M. & Smith, D. M. Hippocampal State Transitions at the Boundaries between Trial Epochs. BioRxiv, 10.1101/443077 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 44.Baldassano C, et al. Discovering event structure in continuous narrative perception and memory. Neuron. 2017;95:709–721.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Letzkus JJ, et al. A disinhibitory microcircuit for associative fear learning in the auditory cortex. Nature. 2011;480:331–335. doi: 10.1038/nature10674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Bouton ME. Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn. Mem. 2004;11:485–494. doi: 10.1101/lm.78804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]