Abstract
Synaptic plasticity, a key process for memory formation, manifests itself across different time scales ranging from a few seconds for plasticity induction up to hours or even years for consolidation and memory retention. We developed a three-layered model of synaptic consolidation that accounts for data across a large range of experimental conditions. Consolidation occurs in the model through the interaction of the synaptic efficacy with a scaffolding variable by a read-write process mediated by a tagging-related variable. Plasticity-inducing stimuli modify the efficacy, but the state of tag and scaffold can only change if a write protection mechanism is overcome. Our model makes a link from depotentiation protocols in vitro to behavioral results regarding the influence of novelty on inhibitory avoidance memory in rats.
Keywords: consolidation, modeling, synaptic tagging
Introduction
A continuous stream of sensory events bombards us, but we only retain a small subset in memory. This selectivity avoids memory overload, preventing overwriting previously formed memories (Nadal et al., 1986; Carpenter and Grossberg, 1987; Amit and Fusi, 1994).
Several brain areas contribute to memory storage (Dudai, 1989), including the hippocampus (Tulving and Markowitsch, 1998; Quiroga et al., 2005). Memory formation in the hippocampus, as well as in other areas, rests upon changes in synaptic connections (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Rogan et al., 1997; Martin et al., 2000; Whitlock et al., 2006). From this perspective, sensory events imprint traces at the level of synapses, reflecting potential memories. These synaptic memory traces occur through changes in the synaptic weight, or as modulations of synapse shape and chemical composition (Redondo and Morris, 2011). Selectivity of memory implies that some synaptic traces decay whereas others persist.
Experimentally, various stimulation protocols induce synaptic memory traces, including high-frequency stimulation of presynaptic pathways (Bliss and Lomo, 1973), stimulation of presynaptic pathways in combination with postsynaptic depolarization (Artola et al., 1990; Ngezahayo et al., 2000), eliciting tightly timed presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes (Markram et al., 1997; Bi and Poo, 2001), or combinations thereof (Sjöström et al., 2001). All of the above protocols involve “Hebbian” coactivation of the presynaptic and postsynaptic neuron (Hebb, 1949) triggering long-term potentiation (LTP) or long-term depression (LTD) of synapses as a “visible” manifestation of synaptic traces.
Synaptic strength changes induced by Hebbian protocols deteriorate over a few hours (early LTP or e-LTP) unless they are consolidated (Dudai and Morris, 2000; Nader et al., 2000). The concept of synaptic tagging and capture (STC) (Frey and Morris, 1997) suggests that the initial trace of synaptic plasticity sets a tag at the synapse, serving as a marker for potential consolidation of the changes in synaptic efficacy. Consolidation then requires the capture of plasticity-related products (PRPs; Redondo and Morris, 2011), which include proteins (Frey and Morris, 1997; Govindarajan et al., 2006) and mRNA synthesis (Nguyen et al., 1994).
Importantly, only tagged synapses can be consolidated (Frey and Morris, 1997). Conceptually, the synaptic tag represents both the eligibility of specific synapses for consolidation and the functional unit that captures PRPs to transform e-LTP into late LTP (l-LTP; Krug et al., 1984; Reymann and Frey, 2007). Subsequent experiments have suggested that tags do not correspond to single molecules but rather to a complex state of the synapse (Lisman et al., 2002; Lee et al., 2009; Okamoto et al., 2009; Ramachandran and Frey, 2009; Redondo et al., 2010; Redondo and Morris, 2011).
Here, we not only translate the above conceptual theory into a compact computational model, but extend it by a write-protection mechanism critical to describing a wealth of in vitro data including experiments on cross-tagging (Frey and Morris, 1997; Sajikumar and Frey, 2004b), tag-resetting and depotentiation (Bashir and Collingridge, 1994; Sajikumar and Frey, 2004a), metaplasticity (Abraham, 2008), and the influence of novelty on behavior (Moncada and Viola, 2007).
Materials and Methods
Similar to previous plasticity models (Rochester et al., 1956; Bienenstock et al., 1982; Gerstner et al., 1996; Song et al., 2000; Fusi, 2002; Pfister and Gerstner, 2006; Morrison et al., 2008), coactivation of presynaptic and postsynaptic neurons induces synaptic memory traces. A central ingredient of our computational model is that synaptic traces are described with three different variables that evolve on different effective timescales. Consolidation requires the transmission of information from a “fast” synaptic trace to a “slow” one (Fusi et al., 2005) through a “write” process. However, in contrast to earlier models of consolidation (Fusi et al., 2005; Clopath et al., 2008; Barrett et al., 2009), the state of the slow synaptic trace is normally protected against overwriting and cannot change unless a “write-now” signal is given (Crow, 1968; Carpenter and Grossberg, 1987; Izhikevich, 2007; Frémaux et al., 2010; Frégnac et al., 2010; Pawlak et al., 2010). The write-now signal in our new model has two stages. The first stage is linked to the formation of tags and is required for the transition from initiation of LTP to e-LTP; the second stage is linked to the production of PRPs for the transition from e-LTP to l-LTP. In this section, we describe the synapse model in detail and finish with a few remarks on network simulation, neuron model, and numerical procedures.
Synaptic variables and write protection.
A model synapse connects a presynaptic neuron (index j) to a postsynaptic neuron (index i). Synapse-specific variables have a double index ij, whereas variables that are shared among synapses onto the same postsynaptic neuron only an index i. In the case of spatial effects in dendrites of compartmental neuron models, the index i would denote one local compartment with shared PRPs. Spatial compartmentalization is beyond the scope of the present study (O'Donnell and Sejnowski, 2014).
The state of a synapse is characterized by three synapse-specific variables: first, the weight variable wij, which is linked to the experimentally measurable synaptic conductance Δgij by a linear transform Δgij = w− + (wij + 1)(w+ − w−)/2 with parameters w− and w+; second, the tagging-related variable Tij, which cannot be measured directly in experiments; and finally the scaffold variable zij. All three variables follow the same bistable dynamics: τxẋ = f (x) = with U(x) = corresponding to the double well potential of Figure 1A. These dynamics have two stable fixed points, x = +1 and x = −1, corresponding to the high and low states, respectively. If the weight variable takes the value wij = −1 or +1, then the measured weight is Δgij = w− or w+. We set the value of the high weight to be w+ = kww− (Table 1).
Table 1.
Neuron model | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Membrane | Threshold | Connections | |||||
V exc | 0 mV | τthr | 5 ms | τampa | 5 ms | τadapt | 250 ms |
Vrest | −70 mV | ϑrest | −50 mV | τgaba | 10 ms | 100 msb | |
V inh | −80 mV | ϑspike | 100 mV | τnmda | 100 ms | gspike | 10 |
τm | 20 ms (10 msa) | β | 0.5 | 1b |
Synaptic model | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Synaptic state | Plasticity induction | PRPs | |||||
τw | 200 s (20 sa) | awT/Tz | 3.5 | A− | 2 × 10−4 | kup | 1 s−1 |
τT | 200 s | aTw | 1.3 | A+ | 5 × 10−4 | kdown | 1/7200 −1 |
τz | 200 s | azT | 0.95 | τx | 16.8 ms | ||
kw | 3 (5a) | τγ | 600 s | τy | 33.7 ms | ||
σ | 10−4 | ϑγ | 0.37 | τtriplet | 40 ms |
Behavioral simulations contexts | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Input → spatial | Spatial → fear | ||||||
Afamiliar− | 1.5 · 10−4 | Afamiliar+ | 10−4 | Afamiliar− | 10−6 | Afamiliar+ | 10−6 |
Anew− | 5 · 10−3 | Anew+ | 10−4 | Anew− | 10−6 | Anew+ | 10−6 |
AIA− | 1.5 · 10−4 | AIA+ | 3 · 10−2 | AIA− | 10−6 | AIA+ | 10−4 |
Behavioral simulations neural network | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group size | Connection weight | Input firing rate | |||||
Ninput | 3 × (500 ± 5) | w−input→spatial | 0.2 | Δgexc→inh | 0.1 | νinputon | 10 Hz |
Nspatialexc | 1000 | w−spatial→fear | 0.1 | Δginh→exc | 0.4 | 8 Hza | |
Nspatialinh | 250 | Δgfear→action | 1 | νinputoff | 0.1 Hz | ||
Nfear | 100 | Δginput→action | 0.5 | 1 Hza | |||
Naction | 100 | Δgext→* | 0.5 | νextspatial | 100 Hz | ||
νextfear | 20 Hz | ||||||
150 Hzb |
Neuron Model: afor inhibitory neurons (Figure 5). bused in behavioral simulations. Synaptic model: aused in behavioral simulations (kw = 5 only for the spatial → fear connection). Behavioral simulations neural network: afiring rates during OF exploration. brate during IA training.
Importantly, the above three variables are coupled via time-dependent gating variables which implement the write protection mechanism. The gate between the weight and the tagging-related variable is given by the synapse-specific variable Gij and the gate from tag to scaffold is given by the neuron-specific variable pi. The full system reads as follows:
where the terms ξij(t) are independent Gaussian white noise processes with the properties 〈ξij(t)〉=0 and 〈ξij(t)ξij(t′)〉 = δ(t − t′).
Note that write protection of the tagging-related variable corresponds to Gij = 0; that is, the weight variable is unable to influence the tagging-related variable (whereas the tagging-related variable can influence the weight). The write protection is completely removed if Gij = 1. Similarly, the scaffold is “write protected” if pi = 0 and write protection is removed for pi = 1.
Plasticity-inducing protocols (to be discussed in the next subsection) act on the dynamics in two different ways. First, plasticity can directly drive the weight evolution as an input Iijw to Equation 1. Second, plasticity also drives the gating dynamics via a plasticity-inducing stimulus Iijγ. The subtle difference between Iijw and Iijγ is discussed further below. To evaluate the momentary state of the gating variable Gij(t), we calculate a low-pass filter of the plasticity-inducing stimulus Iijγ as follows:
and switch the value of the gating variable Gij if γij passes a threshold ϑγ:
where H(x) is the Heaviside step function with H(x) = 1 if x > 0 and 0 else. In other words, a strong plasticity inducing protocol or a weak one that is maintained on a timescale τγ can drive the gating variable above threshold. As a result, the write protection is removed and the momentary weight value wij can influence the tagging-related variable Tij.
The coupling variable pi between the tagging-related variable and the scaffold represents the availability or concentration of PRP in the postsynaptic neuron i. To increase the PRP concentration and remove the second write protection, an external reward or novelty signal, likely to be related to dopamine (DA), is required. In our model, the action of dopamine is described by the following equation:
The dopamine variable (DA) has no index because we assume that the same dopamine signal is received by many neurons in parallel. The presence of dopamine corresponds to a value (DA) = 1. Dopamine is necessary to trigger synthesis of PRPs. In our model, we take the rate kup as a constant, but in a more detailed model, kup could also depend on the availability of additional molecules necessary for PRP synthesis. We emphasize that the concentration of PRP is, in our model, uniform across the whole neuron so that pi has a single subscript, that of the postsynaptic neuron, but extensions to spatial compartments are possible. For all values of the different parameters, see Table 1.
Plasticity induction.
In this section, we describe the input terms IijW and Iijγ acting on the weight and on the gating variable responsible for the tagging process. They are both based on the same standard Hebbian learning rule but differ in the details. We use the triplet spike-timing-dependent plasticity (STDP) rule (Pfister and Gerstner, 2006) with the original set of parameters extracted from hippocampal slices. The spike train of a neuron j is defined as Sj(t) = , with tjk being its kth spike time and δ the Dirac δ-function.
In the triplet rule, LTP induction is as follows:
is driven at the moment of postsynaptic spikes Si(t) and proportional to the two ‘traces’ xj+(t) and y1triplet (t) left by earlier presynaptic or postsynaptic spikes. Therefore, an optimal drive occurs for a spike triplet in a rapid sequence of post-pre-post (hence the name triplet rule). ε is a small positive number. LTD induction is as follows:
LTD induction is independent from LTP induction and occurs in the model at the moment of presynaptic spikes Sj(t) and proportional to a trace yi−(t) left by earlier postsynaptic spikes. Therefore, LTD is optimally stimulated by a pair “post-pre,” just like in standard STDP. The traces are given by the following:
where ξα ∈ {x+, ytriplet, y−} (see Table 1 for the time constants).
In the original paper, parameters of the triplet rule have been optimized for weight induction (Pfister and Gerstner, 2006). Because weight induction and tagging are two independent processes (as corroborated by the tag-resetting protocols; Sajikumar and Frey, 2004a) and are represented by two different variables, wij and Gij, in our model, there is no fundamental reason to assume that the triplet rule would act completely symmetrically on both variables. Moreover, the original paper did not consider the scaffold state. We assume that LTD induction is enhanced, if the momentary weight variable wij is larger than the scaffold variable zij and LTP induction is enhanced if the momentary weight variable wij is smaller than the scaffold variable zij. Therefore, we write for the drive of weight induction as follows:
where [x]+ = xH(x) denotes linear rectification. Note that we multiplied the potentiation and depression terms by factors (1± wij) to ensure that STDP does not push the weight beyond the boundaries of the bistable dynamics. We emphasize that if the initial state coincides with one of the stable states of the synapse (wij = Tij = zij), as is probably the case in standard LTP and STDP experiments, then our model of induction of LTP and LTD is identical to that of Pfister and Gerstner (2006). Differences only arise in sequences of potentiation and depotentiation protocols spread out over several minutes.
In the model, the write protection of the tagging-related variable for LTP induced tags can only be removed if the momentary weight wij is larger than the scaffold value zij, whereas that for LTD can only be removed if the momentary weight wij is smaller than the scaffold value zij. Therefore we set:
Where, again, the term 1 − γij is to ensure that the variable stays between 0 and 1.
Simulation of slice experiments.
Inputs consisted of two groups of 2000 Poisson neurons, each projecting onto 10 leaky integrate-and-fire (LIF) neurons with a connection probability of 10% (Fig. 2A). To simulate a stimulation pulse occurring at time t0, we drew for each presynaptic neuron a spike time tj from a Gaussian distribution with mean t0 and a SD of 3 ms (Fig. 2D). Because triplets of spikes are needed for potentiation, only tetanic stimuli lead to potentiation (Fig. 2B,C), whereas low-frequency protocols lead to depression (Fig. 2D–F) or depotentiation (Fig. 2F).
For slow onset LTP, the stimulations followed the timing of the protocol of Navakkode et al. (2007) and were given 1, 3, 5, 11, 15, 21, 25, and 30 min after start of DA delivery and then every 15 min up to 3 h after the start of DA delivery. In the simulations of the model, each stimulation was mimicked by randomly selecting 5% of synapses and setting their tagging-related variable to “up” Tij = 1. Dopamine was given (i.e., its value was set to DA = 1) during 60 s before the first stimulation (cf. Eq. 6).
Simulation of behavioral data.
For information on the different parameters used in our neural network implementing behavioral experiments, see Table 1. The basic structure of the network consisted of a population of input neurons connected to a spatial node and to action neurons, whereas fear is represented by a group of amygdala-like neurons in the pathway from the spatial node to action neurons.
Population of input neurons.
The population of input neurons consisted of three groups of 500 ± 5 spiking neurons, representing patterns of activity in the home cage, the experimental setup (training cage) during inhibitory avoidance (IA) training, and the open field environment, respectively. On average, 50 neurons were shared by any two patterns. Only one pattern was active at a given time. An active neuron is represented by a Poisson spike train of firing rate vinputon = 10 Hz (8 Hz in the case of the open field exploration), and an inactive neuron by a rate vinputoff = 0.1 Hz (1 Hz for the open field). The values for open fields are different because we assume that it is a novel stimulus for which the network does not yet have strongly tuned neurons.
Population for spatial representation (spatial node).
We simulated a population of 1000 excitatory adaptive LIF neurons and 250 inhibitory LIF neurons. Each excitatory neuron received 25 inputs from presynaptic neurons in the inhibitory population, as well as 135 ± 10 inputs from the input population. Connections from the input to the excitatory population included our new synapse model; others were nonplastic (see Table 1 for the connection weights). Each inhibitory neuron received exactly 100 inputs from presynaptic neurons in the excitatory population. Every neuron (excitatory or inhibitory) received also independent noise in the form of a Poisson spike train with rate vextspatial = 100 Hz.
Population for fear encoding.
The population consisted of 100 inhibitory LIF neurons. Each neuron received 100 inputs from the excitatory population of the spatial node. These connections were plastic and described by our new synapse model. Each neuron received also independent noise in the form of a Poisson spike train with rate vextfear = 20 Hz. This external input became stronger in the case of a fearful event, with a firing rate of 150 Hz.
Population of action neurons.
We simulated 10 groups of 100 adaptive LIF neurons each. Every neuron received 10 inputs from the fear population as well as 50 ± 1 inputs from the pattern coding for the experimental setup (training cage). All connections were nonplastic.
For each group m, we calculated the time-averaged population activity νm on-line via the following:
where Nm is the size of group m and tmk is the time of the kth spike among all neurons in group m. We chose a timescale of τν = 100 ms.
The jump time in the model was defined by the time when, for the majority of the action groups, the rate νm had reached at least once a threshold ϑjump. We set an upper bound of 100 s on the jump time.
Neuron model.
In our simulations, we used LIF neurons (parameters in Table 1) with conductance-based synapses (Gerstner et al., 2014). The evolution of the membrane potential of neuron i is given by the following:
where giexc and Vexc are the excitatory conductance and reversal potential (and analogously for inhibition). A spike is emitted when the potential reaches the threshold ϑi. After a spike, Vi is reset to Vrest and ϑi is set to ϑspike to implement refractoriness. The threshold then relaxes back to its rest value according to the following:
The excitatory conductance giexc has an AMPA and an NMDA component and is defined as giexc = βgiampa + (1 − β)ginmda where β is the relative contribution of AMPA. The time course of conductances is given by a first-order low-pass filter as follows:
for the contribution of the AMPA receptor channel giampa = giα, where Δgij is the plastic synaptic weight of the connection from neuron j to i; and by a second-order low-pass filter as follows:
for the contribution of the NMDA channel. The voltage dependence of NMDA is neglected for the sake of computational efficiency.
For nonadaptive leaky integrate-and-fire neurons, the inhibitory conductance is giinh=gigaba = 0 in the slice experiments; in the behavioral experiments, it is described by giinh=gigaba where gigaba follows the dynamics of Equation 15 with a fixed weight Δgij = Δg and Sj the spike trains arriving at the inhibitory synapse. For adaptive integrate-and-fire neurons, we add a spike-triggered self-inhibition giinh = gigaba + giadapt where the adaptation conductance is increased by an amount gspike at each spike of neuron i, and else relaxes exponentially to zero as follows:
For parameter values, see Table 1.
Numerical simulations.
Numerical simulations were performed with a time step of 0.1 ms. For differential equations that could be solved analytically, we used the exact solution. For all the others, a forward Euler method with the same time step was used. An exception to this were the three internal synaptic variables wij, Tij, and zij for which we used a time step of Δt = 100 ms. This updating time was chosen so that for all x ∈ {wij, Tij, zij}.
The code was written in C++ using Open MPI and the Boost libraries and compiled with the GNU C compiler. Simulations were run on a Linux workstation equipped with Intel Core i7 CPUs. The bottleneck for simulation time was the total number of plastic synapses, first because their number grows as ∼N2 (with N the number of neurons) and second because of their large amount of internal variables. For simulating our behavioral network, the speed-up factor was ∼2, meaning that it took approximately half a day to simulate 24 h of biological time.
In all behavioral paradigms where the interval between IA training and the final testing was 1 d, we stopped the simulations after 8 h biological time. We justify this choice by the fact that no significant weight change compared with the level of noise could be measured after this time in a preliminary longer test run.
Choice of model and parameter tuning.
Our model of synaptic plasticity has a total of five variables: three variables (related to synaptic weight, tag, and scaffold) to describe the state of the synapses and two variables that control the read-write process between these. The choice of the variables was based on the following considerations.
In the classic model of synaptic tagging and capture (Frey and Morris, 1997), there was no fundamental distinction between e-LTP and tagging. More recent experiments, however, showed that tagging and e-LTP are dissociable (Ramachandran and Frey, 2009; Redondo et al., 2010; Redondo and Morris, 2011). Similarly, an early model of synaptic tagging and capture (Clopath et al., 2008) identified tagging with e-LTP and therefore had only two state variables (see also Fusi, 2002; Brader et al., 2007). The increase in the number of state variables to three is necessary to account for the separation of tagging and e-LTP and to explain the tag-resetting experiments involving a sequence of potentiation and depotentiation events (see Fig. 4D).
The dynamic write protection mechanism requires the two additional variables mentioned above. In the classic cascade model (Fusi et al., 2005), a dynamic write protection is absent because transitions between discrete states occur at constant rates. In the state-based model (Barrett et al., 2009), write protection is not a part of the dynamics, but indirectly implemented by explicit stimulus-dependent conditions.
Our final model of synaptic plasticity has 17 parameters (Table 1). The five parameters of the triplet model of plasticity induction in Equations 7–9 were adopted from (Pfister and Gerstner, 2006), except A− and A+, which were retuned (Table 1). The remaining 12 parameters of e-LTP, tagging, and scaffold were hand-tuned to the slice data, guided by theoretical consideration, and kept fixed for all figures. The round values given in Table 1 for these parameters indicate that no fine-tuning was necessary, suggesting that the model behavior is robust against changes in the parameters.
For the neuron model (Gerstner et al., 2014), a plausible set of standard parameters with “round” values were chosen before starting any plasticity modeling and kept fixed throughout the modeling process. The network connectivity for behavioral data was set up by hand using plausible numbers.
Relation to other models of synaptic consolidation.
In the cascade (Fusi et al., 2005) or the state-based (Barrett et al., 2009) models, a single discrete variable, which can be in one of N different states, characterizes each synapse. In our model, multiple variables describe the state of a synapse, which grants a variety of transitions and provides the flexibility necessary to implement plastic and metaplastic phenomena. Although only two states in our model (weak-down-small and strong-up-large) exhibit long-term stability (in contrast with, for example, a tristable system; Pi and Lisman, 2008), the model also has six metastable states (e.g., strong-up-small). Metastability arises because of the write protection mechanism that prevents changes to be transmitted into long-term memory.
In electrophysiological experiments in which only the weight variable is measured, there is typically no control of the initial state of the synapse. When a simplified learning rule is extracted from such plasticity experiments, metastable “hidden” states lead to an interpretation of “metaplasticity” (Abraham and Bear, 1996) or a “sliding threshold” (Bienenstock et al., 1982). Plasticity models depending on internal parameters and PRP availability have been called “neo-Hebbian” (Lisman et al., 2011).
Results
Synaptic plasticity is a widespread phenomenon in the brain, but details vary across brain areas, species, neuron and synapse type and age. Although basic conceptual aspects of plasticity are probably conserved across several variants and are therefore describable by the same modeling framework, quantitative fits of the model to experimental data need to be limited to one synapse type. For model fitting, we mainly focus on glutamatergic excitatory synapses onto hippocampal CA1 neurons.
The Results section is organized in three parts. First, we introduce the model of synaptic consolidation and highlight a few essential results based on simulations and analysis of the model. Second, we show that the consolidation model can account for previously unexplained experimental observations after a sequence of potentiation and depotentiation stimuli. Finally, we use the model in a simulation of “behavioral tagging” experiments so as to predict macroscopic effects from microscopic synaptic dynamics.
Basic model features
Plasticity manifests itself experimentally across many different timescales (Kelleher et al., 2004; Reymann and Frey, 2007) that correspond to three different variables in our model of synaptic consolidation. First, minimal LTP stimulation protocols indicate that changes in the amplitude of the postsynaptic current (PSC) or postsynaptic potential (PSP) are visible already a few seconds after the induction protocol (Petersen et al., 1998; O'Connor et al., 2005). In our model, observations on the rapid timescale of synaptic plasticity correspond to changes of the synaptic weight wij between the presynaptic neuron j and the postsynaptic neuron i. The synaptic weight, which is “visible” in electrophysiological experiments, can be related to the number and phosphorylation state of AMPA receptors (Lisman and Spruston, 2005), as well as the release probability of presynaptic vesicles (Tsodyks et al., 1998). After plasticity induction, the synapse is in the state of initial LTP (i-LTP), likely to be related to associative short-term plasticity (Erickson et al., 2010).
Second, tags are set on a timescale of a few minutes and spontaneously decay after 1 or 2 h (Frey and Morris, 1997; Reymann and Frey, 2007). Tags correspond to complex synaptic states (Redondo and Morris, 2011) which are not directly visible in electrophysiological experiments but must be inferred indirectly by pharmacological (Lisman et al., 2002; Sajikumar and Frey, 2004a; Ramachandran and Frey, 2009; Redondo et al., 2010) or spine imaging experiments (Lee et al., 2009; Okamoto et al., 2009). In our model, the internal state of the synapse from neuron j onto neuron i corresponds to the tagging-related variable Tij.
Third, the synapses can switch to a consolidated state of I-LTP on the timescale of <1 h and remain in this state for many hours (Frey and Morris, 1997). In our model, the consolidated state of a synapse from neuron j to neuron i is described by a variable zij, which may be interpreted as the state of the synapse scaffold (Redondo et al., 2010).
Long-term stability of the scaffold variable zij in the presence of molecular turnover requires multistability (Crick, 1984; Lisman, 1985). In the model, we assume generic bistable dynamics of zij, which can be visualized as a ball moving downward in a double-well potential (Fig. 1A). A classic example of bistability in biochemistry is an autocatalytic process such as autophosphorylation (Lisman, 1985), but many other processes with self-feedback also show the same generic form of bistability. Because we interpret the zij variable as the scaffold variable, our model effectively assumes bistability of the scaffold. Described by a standard bistable equation (see Materials and Methods), the continuous variable zij can switch between a stable low value zij = −1 (“small” scaffold) and an equally stable high value zij = +1 (“large” scaffold).
Because the notion of “tagging” is binary (i.e., a tag is either set or not), the tagging-related variable Tij is described with an equation analogous to that of zij with two stable states at Tij = +1 (up) and Tij = −1 (down). The tagging-related variable can be up either because of a recent LTP-inducing event or because the synapse has been in the stable, “large-scaffold” state for a long time. We say that a tag is set if the tagging-related variable is in a different state than the scaffold variable. For example, a configuration Tij ≈ + 1, whereas zij ≈ − 1 implies that a tag for LTP has been set; whereas Tij ≈ − 1 and zij ≈ + 1 implies tagging for LTD. A transition from Tij ≈ − 1 to Tij ≈ + 1 can occur either because a tag for LTP has been set or because a tag for LTD has decayed. In the following, we sometimes refer to the tagging-related variable Tij simply as the “tag” even though the tag is formally the difference between the tagging-related and the scaffold variable.
Finally, minimal stimulation protocols suggest that induction of plasticity leads to switch-like events (Petersen et al., 1998; O'Connor et al., 2005). We therefore assume that the maximal synaptic conductance Δgij that characterizes the amplitude of the excitatory PSC can switch from “weak” Δgij = w− to the “strong” Δ gij = w+. We refer to Δgij also as the “weight” of the synapse. In the model, Δgij is linearly related to the weight variable wij (see Materials and Methods). In summary, the three variables, wij, Tij, and zij, that together characterize the momentary state of a synapse, all have similar bistable dynamics except for a difference in response time: after induction of LTP by a strong protocol (see Materials and Methods), the weight switches after a few seconds, the tagging-related variable after a few minutes, and the scaffold after half an hour (Fig. 1D).
In the absence of external stimulation, the synapse will converge to one of two stable states. In the globally stable low state, the synaptic weight is weak, the tagging-related variable is down, and the scaffold is small (Fig. 1A, left), whereas in the globally stable high state, all three variables are at their other stable point, strong, up, and large, respectively (Fig. 1A, right).
Stimulation of ∼2000 synapses by a weak LTP induction protocol (see legend of Fig. 1C and Materials and Methods) transiently shifts the mean value of the weight to strong, followed by a transient increase in the mean value of the tagging-related variable (Fig. 1C). However, after the tagging-related variable has decayed back to its resting value, the weight will also, over a time of 2–3 h, return back to rest (Fig. 1C and see Fig. 4A).
These simulation results are a consequence of the coupling between the three variables. The tagging-related variable is bidirectionally coupled to the weight and the scaffold so that the total number of vertical coupling arrows between variables is four. However, in the absence of stimulation, the (relatively) slow variables are write protected so that they cannot be influenced by a faster one (write protection indicated by crossed downward arrows in Fig. 1A). Therefore, in the absence of stimulation, the present value of the slowest variable (i.e., the state of the scaffold) influences the dynamics of the tagging-related variable, which in turn influences the dynamics of the weight (upward arrows in Fig. 1A), leading to the asymmetric slightly tilted bistability curves in Figure 1A favoring the weak weight if the scaffold is small and the strong weight value if the scaffold is large.
The effect of an LTP induction protocol on the synapse model is twofold (Fig. 1B). First, a potentiation protocol tilts the bistable weight dynamics toward larger values, so that the strong weight becomes the only stable state of the system. The state of i-LTP corresponds to a strong weight, whereas the tagging-related variable and scaffold remain down and small, respectively. Second, if the potentiation protocol is strong or sustained (see Materials and Methods), the write protection from weight to tag is removed so that the tagging-related variable is now influenced by the weight. Therefore, a few minutes later, the tagging-related variable increases to the up value and the synapse exhibits e-LTP. However, because the value of the tagging-related variable is only metastable, it will eventually decay back to the down state, followed by a decay of the weight.
Tag setting thus leads to e-LTP, but requires that a first write protection is removed. Consolidation of a synapse to the high state of l-LTP is possible only if a second layer of write protection is overcome so as to allow the tagging-related variable to influence the scaffold variable. In our model, the second write protection can be removed only if PRPs are available (see Materials and Methods). PRPs in turn require the presence of a neuromodulator such as dopamine. Indeed, strong LTP induction protocols in slices costimulate dopaminergic fibers (Reymann and Frey, 2007; Lisman et al., 2011). Therefore, under the assumption that dopamine is present after a strong LTP induction protocol, weights are consolidated (Fig. 1D).
Simulated tagging and cross-tagging experiments
Simulations of the model can reproduce a wide variety of experimental results in vitro, accumulated by different laboratories (Figs. 3, 4). Because most classical LTP and consolidation protocols in slices use extracellular stimulation of fiber bundles, we mimic experiments by stimulating one or two groups of ∼2000 model synapses converging onto 10 different postsynaptic neurons (see Materials and Methods). In each group, two-thirds of the synapses are initially in the low state and one-third in the high state. A single extracellular pulse corresponds in the model to the near-synchronous arrival of spikes at all 2000 synapses with a temporal jitter of 3 ms. The postsynaptic neuron is an adaptive LIF model (Gerstner et al., 2014) and LTP induction is described by a standard model of STDP (Pfister and Gerstner, 2006). For details, see Materials and Methods.
First, we find that a weak tetanic stimulation (21 pulses at 100 Hz) leads to e-LTP but not to l-LTP, whereas a strong tetanic stimulation (3 × 100 pulses at 100 Hz) leads to consolidated synapses (L-LTP, Fig. 3A) consistent with experiments (Frey and Morris, 1997). Similarly, a weak low-frequency stimulus (900 pulses at 1 Hz) leads to e-LTD but not l-LTD, whereas a strong low-frequency stimulus (3 pulses at 20 Hz, 900 times, every second) causes l-LTD (Fig. 3A), consistent with experiments (Sajikumar and Frey, 2004b).
Note that, in our model, PRPs are shared among synapses onto the same neuron, whereas weight, tagging, and scaffold variables are synapse specific. Indeed, the model accounts for the fact that e-LTP induced by weak tetanic stimulation can be transformed into l-LTP (Fig. 3B) if a second group of synapses receives, within 1 h, a strong tetanic stimulation (Frey and Morris, 1997). Synapses along additional control pathways that are not stimulated are not affected by the strong tetanic stimulation, indicating synapse-specificity of l-LTP (data not shown).
Interestingly, a strong tetanic stimulation that leads to l-LTP in a first group of synapses can also transform e-LTD after a weak depression inducing stimulus into l-LTD (Fig. 3C), consistent with the experimental observation of cross-tagging between potentiation and depression (Sajikumar and Frey, 2004b). Other combinations of weak or strong depressing or potentiating stimuli are also reproduced by the model (Fig. 3D–I).
Simulated tag-resetting experiments
Although previous models of consolidation (Clopath et al., 2008; Barrett et al., 2009; Smolen et al., 2012) were able to account for the above results, experiments on tag resetting (Sajikumar and Frey, 2004a) remained unexplained. The so-called tag-resetting paradigm consists of a sequence of potentiation and depotentiation events. The most intriguing scenario is that of an LTP-inducing stimulus followed 10 min later by a depotentiating stimulus. In this case, the measured synaptic strength goes down after the depotentiation event, but comes back up again within the next half hour (Sajikumar and Frey, 2004a); our model is able to account for this behavior (Fig. 4A).
Analysis of the model dynamics shows that, during depotentiation, the synaptic weight is reset while the tagging-related variable persists in the up-state (when the resetting stimulus occurs after 10 min) or has not yet been upregulated (when the resetting stimulus occurs after 5 min; Fig. 1E). The reason is that, during the 10 min of waiting time between the potentiating and depotentiating stimulus, the tagging-related variable has had enough time to go from the down to the up state, so that the synapse is in the metastable state of e-LTP (Fig. 1E). The subsequent depotentiating event (Fig. 1A, top) is able to shift the fast weight variable to the weak state, whereas the tagging-related variable remains in the up state (Fig. 1E). Because the depotentiating stimulus is not strong enough to remove the write protection, the tagging-related variable remains write protected in the up state. Therefore, the new weight cannot influence the tagging-related variable, but the tagging-related variable in the up state can influence the weight and pull it back up to the strong value. It is only over the next hour that background noise will make the tagging-related variable and later the weight decay back to their down and weak values, respectively. Therefore, the data of Sajikumar and Frey (2004a) are explained in our theory by the dissociation of early weight changes and tagging into two separate variables, whereas an earlier model that identified e-LTP with tagging (Clopath et al., 2008) is unable to account for this set of experiments.
The back-and-forth between weak and strong weights does not occur if the depotentiation stimulus is given within 5 min and is less pronounced if given 15 min after potentiation (Fig. 4A), consistent with experiments (Sajikumar and Frey, 2004a). Moreover, if PRPs are available after a strong tetanic stimulation of a second group of neurons, then synapses in a first group that underwent a sequence of potentiation and depotentiation events separated by 10 min can be consolidated in the high state, both in the model (Fig. 4B) and in experiments (Sajikumar and Frey, 2004a).
A sequence of potentiation and depotentiation events in slice experiments can be interpreted as an approximation to ongoing stimulus-induced activity that a synapse is expected to experience in vivo. We checked the model (data not shown) under different potentiation-depotentiation protocols (Table 2), including 100 Hz high-frequency stimulation, theta burst stimulation consisting of a sequence of short bursts given at 5 Hz, and 250 Hz tetanic stimulation for potentiation, and, for depression, 2 Hz low-frequency stimulation and 5 Hz theta pulse stimulations (Bashir and Collingridge, 1994; Martin, 1998; Stäubli and Chun, 1996). Because the slice preparations are different across experiments coming from different laboratories, we modified three parameters of the model (the learning constants A+ and A− and the noise level) to describe these data; all other parameters were kept at their previous values.
Table 2.
Mechanism | Protocol | Study | Z | CZ | BB |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Early/late LTP & e-LTP → l-LTP | TET | Frey and Morris, 1997 | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ |
Early/late LTD & e-LTD → l-LTD | LFS | Sajikumar and Frey, 2003 | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ |
Cross tagging | TET, LFS | Sajikumar and Frey, 2004b | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ |
e-LTP/tag-setting dissociation | Pharmacology (actin) | Ramachandran and Frey, 2009 | ✓ | X | ✓ |
Pharmacology (CaMKII) | Redondo et al., 2010 | ||||
Depotentiation | TET, LFS | Sajikumar and Frey, 2004a | ✓ | X | X |
TET, LFS (2Hz) | Bashir and Collingridge, 1994 | ✓a | |||
TBS, TPS | Stäubli and Chun, 1996 | ✓a | |||
TET (250Hz), LFS (5Hz) | Martin, 1998 | ✓a | |||
Measurement frequency influence on e-LTP | TET | Fonseca et al., 2006 | ✓ | X | X |
Slow-onset LTP | Pharmacology | Navakkode et al., 2007 | ✓ | X | X |
Selected articles on important aspects of STC theory. A check sign means that a particular model can account for the data of the study of the same line, whereas an X means that it cannot. Z, Ziegler, the new model presented in this article; CZ, Clopath and Ziegler, the model from Clopath et al. (2008); BB, Barrett and Billings, the model from Barrett et al. (2009)). TBS, Theta burst stimulation; TPS, theta pulse stimulation.
aSmall modifications on A+, A−, gspike, and σ were sometimes necessary.
We wondered whether the resetting of the early phase of LTP is functionally useful. In a noisy environment, the presence of a reset mechanism implies that evidence needs to be accumulated before imprinting plasticity (Brader et al., 2007; Elliott and Lagogiannis, 2012). Ideally, a synapse should remain unchanged when driven by noise. Let us consider a discrete synapse experiencing a noisy but meaningless signal with rate λ+ = 0.1/s of potentiation and λ− = 0.9 s for depotentiation. The synapses might be upregulated by the first potentiating stimulus and immediately afterward reset by a depotentiating one. The net result is no change. The probability of staying in (or returning to) the initial low state approaches rapidly a stationary value of 9/10.
We contrast this result with a hypothetical model in which early LTP cannot be reset. In this case, the first potentiation event would trigger the tag immediately even if a depotentiation event follows thereafter. In this case, the probability of staying in the initial low state decays exponentially with λ+T. Therefore, the probability that a synapse stays unaffected by noise (i.e., not tagged) is bigger for the model with reset than for the one without.
Recording frequency and slow-onset LTP
In experiments, synaptic weights are usually recorded at most once per minute, corresponding to a frequency of 0.0167 Hz. However, going to higher recording frequencies accelerates the decay of LTP when no PRPs are available (Fonseca et al., 2006). In our model of consolidation, we found that the mean synaptic weight measured 30 min after induction was up to 40% lower when recorded at 0.2 Hz than when recorded at one pulse per minute or not at all (Fig. 4D,F), but was affected much less by the recording frequency in the presence of PRP (Fig. 4E,F). Therefore, paradoxically, the early phase of plasticity does, at high levels of synaptic activity, depend on the availability of PRP. The reason is that PRPs remove the write protection from the tagging-related variable to the scaffold so that the scaffold variable can switch to the new stable state before background stimulation leads to a depotentiation of the synapse.
Slow onset LTP is observed experimentally when dopamine receptors D1/D5 agonists are applied to a hippocampal slice (Navakkode et al., 2007). The slow rise in synaptic weight requires that a presynaptic stimulus, even a weak one, is applied regularly. Slow onset LTP has been shown to rely on the presence of PRPs and also to depend on NMDA receptors, which are known to mediate the tagging process. We modeled this phenomenon by turning the dopamine signal on for 60 s so as to trigger formation of PRP, followed by randomly setting 5% of the tags to their up-state Tij → +1 at each weak synaptic stimulation to mimic the contribution of NMDA receptors toward tag formation. During the 3 h of weak stimulation repeated every few minutes (see Materials and Methods, above), we observed a slow increase of the mean weight that finally settled at around 150% (Fig. 4C). We emphasize that the sequence of very weak stimuli would not be sufficient by itself to cause LTP. However, because of the presence of PRP after the initial dopamine stimulus, the write protection from the tagging-related variable to scaffold was removed. Therefore, in our model, the tagging-related variable could stay for a sufficiently long time in the up state (Tij = +1) to pull the weights upward, eventually leading the synapse to its globally stable high state. Our consolidation model with three coupled variables can therefore simulate classic experiments in which weight changes induced by an LTP protocol lead to tagging and consolidation, as well as those in which tagging precedes the visible weight changes.
Behavioral tagging
Experimental work (Frey et al., 1990; Sajikumar and Frey, 2004b; Lisman and Grace, 2005) has linked the production of PRP and the maintenance of memory to the presence of neuromodulators such as dopamine. A phasic increase in dopamine in turn can be caused by behavioral and environmental cues such as novelty, reward prediction error, aversive events,or attention (Ljunberg et al., 1992; Hollerman and Schultz, 1998; Redgrave and Gurney, 2006; Schultz, 2007; Bethus et al., 2010; Lisman et al., 2011). We therefore wondered whether our synaptic model of consolidation could potentially explain behavioral experiments that suggested an important role of novelty for memory retention (Moncada and Viola, 2007; Ballarini et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2010; de Carvalho Myskiw et al., 2013).
In the “behavioral tagging” experiment of Moncada and Viola (2007), a weak electric foot shock in an IA task did not lead to a significant change in the behavior of rats 24 h later, unless rats were exposed to a novel open field (OF) environment within 1 h before or after the electric foot shock. Because exploration of an OF for at least 5 min is known to trigger dopamine release in the rat hippocampus (Li et al., 2003), it has been hypothesized earlier that these behavioral results could potentially be related to the transformation of tagged synapses from early LTP to late LTP through protein synthesis triggering events (Moncada and Viola, 2007).
To test this hypothesis in the model, we designed a network consisting of several populations of model neurons (Fig. 5A). A central role is played by a population of excitatory and inhibitory neurons (hippocampal cells or spatial module) representing different spatial contexts (Treves and Rolls, 1992). This spatial context module is driven by a population of input neurons which signal different stimuli representing the known home cage, the training cage containing an elevated platform for IA training, or the novel environment for OF exploration. When an unknown pattern (e.g., training cage or open field) is presented for the first time, the excitatory neurons of the spatial population fire irregularly at ∼1 Hz and with a log-normal firing rate distribution (Fig. 5B; cf. Hromádka et al., 2008). Connections from the input neurons to excitatory neurons in the spatial context pool can undergo plasticity, described by our model of synaptic consolidation. After training, we find that each of the three input patterns is represented by a set of highly active hippocampal neurons that form a memory engram (Ramirez et al., 2013) of the spatial context, similar to a k-winner-take-all system in artificial neural networks (Kohonen, 1982; Maass, 2000) or to sets of place cells in biology (O'Keefe and Nadal, 1971).
The excitatory population of the spatial module projects to the fear population (which represents amygdala neurons; Davis, 1992), again, using plastic synapses described by our model. In nonfearful situations, the fear neurons have a firing rate that is too low to cause any change in the weights of their incoming synapses. In the case of a fearful event, a strong external input increases neuronal activity so that synapses from the currently active spatial neurons to the fear population undergo long-term potentiation, whereas synapses from less active presynaptic neurons to the fear neurons show a limited, transient increase in strength (Fig. 5C).
Finally, fear neurons are connected by inhibitory model synapses to action neurons representing the action “jump down.” Excitatory input from the input pattern coding for the training cage onto the action neurons normally produces the urge to jump down, unless strong inhibitory input from the fear population suppresses the action (“freezing”). In the model, the jump occurs when the population activity of the pool of action neurons reaches a preset threshold (Fig. 5D; see Materials and Methods for details).
Fear memory was formed in a single trial in the training cage in all four groups of 10 simulated animals each. During training, our simulated rats remained inactive for ∼10 s before the activity threshold for the action jump down was reached. Test sessions after different waiting times (Fig. 6A) indicated that, 15 min after fear encoding, the latency increased to ∼90 s (Group 1), whereas after 60 min (Group 2), latencies were ∼20 s and thus still significantly higher than before training. However, 24 h after the fearful event (Group 3), all memory was lost, indicating that model synapses did not consolidate. A stronger foot shock during training (Group 4), implemented in our model as a phasic dopamine signal, increases the waiting time, measured after 24 h, to ∼100 s (Fig. 6A). In this situation, PRPs triggered by dopamine, can remove the write protection from the tagging-related variable to the scaffold so that synapses from the input to the place representation as well as from the spatial module to the fear neurons are consolidated.
In a second set of simulations, we inserted 5 min of exposure to a novel OF environment either before or after a training session with a weak foot shock. At the end of the exploration period, a dopamine signal (Lisman et al., 2011) triggered by the novel experience was turned on in the model. Six groups of 10 simulated rats were tested 24 h after IA training. In trials in which the OF exploration preceded the IA training, the mean latency of the action jump down was ∼20 s (23 ± 14 s, mean and SD across 10 simulated animals) when the OF exposure was 2 h (Group 1) before, whereas it was 100 ± 2 s when the OF exposure was 1 h (Group 2) before IA training (Fig. 6B). In our model, this outcome was determined by the PRP concentration that remained at the time of exposure to the training cage. The closer the novelty exploration to IA training, the higher the protein concentration and thus the stronger the consolidation of any encoded change in synaptic efficacy (Fig. 7A,B). In experiments, the half-life of PRPs has been measured to be ∼1–2 h (Korz and Frey, 2004).
In trials in which the OF exploration followed the IA training, memory retention is maximal for a delay of +15 min (Group 4) with latencies of ∼60 s (57 ± 38 s mean and SD across 10 simulated animals; Fig. 6B), in agreement with experimental results. In the model, memory retention after 24 h is weak (not significantly different from the first trial) if the interval between OF and avoidance training is +60 min or more (Groups 5 and 6).
Interestingly, the increase in latency to jump is negligible (Fig. 6B) if the delay is +0 min (Group 3), indicating an interference of the two spatial environments, in agreement with experiments (Moncada and Viola, 2007).
In the model, a 5 minute exploration of the OF is described by an input population where many neurons fire at ∼1 Hz, whereas a few neurons fire at a higher rate (see Materials and Methods). During the creation of a neural representation of the OF in the spatial module, neurons in the module are active and receive at the same time low presynaptic activity from the input neurons. The coactivation of presynaptic and postsynaptic neurons (with low presynaptic activity) leads to depotentiation of synapses that were previously potentiated during the training cage exposure (Fig. 7C). Because depotentiation within <5 min implies that initial LTP cannot be transformed to e-LTP (Fig. 4A), the spatial context of the training cage is not remembered, observable in the behavioral experiment as “interference” between the two environments.
Our model of synaptic traces thus links behavioral results (Moncada and Viola, 2007) to the electrophysiology of synapses during depotentiation protocols (Sajikumar and Frey, 2004a).
Based on the above link between synapse electrophysiology and behavior, we hypothesized that delayed OF exposure would correspond in our model to a delayed depotentiation event on previously potentiated synapses. As discussed earlier in Figure 1E, a delayed depotentiation event gives the synaptic dynamics enough time to overcome write protection of the tagging-related variable so that e-LTP is formed. In analogy to Figure 1E, we therefore expect that, during an experiment in which IA is followed by OF 15 min later, some synapses switch from weak to strong (induced by potentiation during IA training), then to weak (induced by depotentiation during OF exposure), and finally back to strong values because the tagging-related variable in the up state pulls the weights back up (Fig. 7D). In the behavioral experiment, the spatial context of the training cage is encoded during IA training in the synapses from the input neurons to the spatial module neurons.
Indeed, we find that, during IA training, synapses that connect highly active input neurons to active spatial module neurons switch to the high state (see three sample traces in Fig. 7D). These synapses are subsequently tagged and tags have not yet decayed back when, after a delay of 15 min, the OF stimulus starts. While the OF stimulus rapidly resets the weights to low values (see the three sample traces in Fig. 7D at t = 15 min), the previously tagged synapses can take advantage of the PRP triggered by the dopamine wave caused by the OF environment. Therefore the tagging-related variable in the up state can be further stabilized by changes in the scaffold, leading to a final switch to the stable high state of ∼70% of the previously tagged synapses (Fig. 7D, black trace). Therefore, the spatial context of the IA training stored in the synapses onto neurons in the spatial module is consolidated and remembered over a long time.
We expect that more detailed behavioral memory models that include theta cycles and memory replay during sharp-wave ripples (Lubenov and Siapas, 2008) would change the quantitative latency results, but not the qualitative structure of the above argument.
Predictions of behavioral tagging model
Our simulations provide an explicit link between cellular and behavioral consolidation mechanisms (Bekinschtein et al., 2007; Moncada and Viola, 2007; Wang et al., 2010) and make two specific predictions.
Novel stimuli can rescue long-term memory when given either before or after fear memory encoding, but not if the novel stimulus directly follows the IA training (see the “hole” at + 0 min in Fig. 6B). First, our model predicts that this hole is caused by depotentiation of synapses. In particular, the model predicts that the hole is asymmetric in time: whereas a novel stimulus immediately after a “to-be-learned” event can reset the learning, a novel stimulus that preceded the learning event cannot. This prediction may look surprising in view of the existing literature (cf. Ballarini et al., 2009 vs Moncada and Viola, 2007).
Second, for a delay of the novel open field of 15 min, our model predicts that single synapses make multiple transitions (e.g., weak-strong-weak-strong; Fig. 7D). The sequence of transitions could be visible as a sequence of several abrupt changes in presynaptic release probability, postsynaptic density of AMPA receptors, intraspine actin networks, or spine volume. Similar changes of single synapses would also be expected in vitro under a suitable potentiation-depotentiation protocol. The sequence of back-forth transitions may be considered as a very characteristic, and strong, prediction of the model.
Discussion
Our synapse model accounts for slice and behavioral data using three variables: the synaptic weight, the tagging-related variable and the scaffold. The coupling between these quantities is controlled by a write-protection mechanism, which is described by two further variables. Here, we discuss the biological and functional interpretation and potential generalizations and predictions of the model.
Biological interpretation
A model with five variables likely represents the simplest formulation of a phenomenological model. A reduction in the number of variables would explain significantly fewer experiments (Materials and Methods). The variables represent “reporters” indicating important changes in the molecular configuration of the synapse and should not be viewed as single molecules.
Although the synaptic weight can be measured experimentally, the tagging-related and scaffold-related variables are hidden, requiring indirect deduction. The initial weight change in the model after LTP induction can connect to an increase in the amount, and phosphorylation state, of AMPA receptors (AMPARs) in the postsynaptic density of the postsynaptic membrane (Lisman et al., 2002; Redondo and Morris, 2011). In addition, presynaptic factors can also change synaptic efficacy (Tsodyks et al., 1998; Redondo and Morris, 2011).
Ca2+ influx in the postsynaptic density through NMDA receptors induces the early weight changes and tag setting in hippocampal synapses (Navakkode et al., 2007), but our model explicitly describes neither NMDA nor Ca2+ (Smolen et al., 2012). The tag corresponds to a complex synaptic state including several molecules and signaling cascades (Redondo and Morris, 2011). In hippocampal synapses, calcineurin participates in setting tags for LTD (Zhou et al., 2004). Fluorescence imaging of spines suggests a transient role for CaMKII during tag setting for LTP (Lee et al., 2009). Tag-related processes include transformations in intraspine actin networks (Lee et al., 2009; Okamoto et al., 2009; Redondo and Morris, 2011). Our phenomenological model summarizes the signal processing chain during tagging by one single tagging-related variable. Tag setting for LTP (LTD) corresponds to the state transition “tag down → tag up” (“tag up → tag down”) while the scaffold variable remains small (large). If the changes are consolidated, the tagging-related variable remains in its new state while the scaffold switches to large.
The scaffold variable also does not correspond to a single molecule, because at least two mechanisms contribute to sustained changes in synaptic efficacy (Cingolani and Goda, 2008): first, the creation of new slots for inserting AMPARs in the membrane and, second, a structural reconfiguration of the whole postsynaptic density. Furthermore, the list of proteins linked to maintenance of LTP remains incomplete, but likely includes GluR1, Homer1a, PKMζ, and ARC (Miyashita et al., 2008).
Write protection and neuromodulators
The three variables (weight, tag, and scaffold) are coupled to each other. In the absence of stimulation, the scaffold influences the tagging-related variable and the latter the weight, whereas influences in the opposite direction are impossible due to write protection. The write protection from weight to the tagging-related variable is removed only when the stimulus is sufficiently strong or sustained so that, after low-pass filtering, a threshold is attained. This integrate-and-threshold mechanism secures that ongoing small weight fluctuations cannot lead to long-term changes (Brader et al., 2007; Elliott and Lagogiannis, 2012).
Although the removal of this first write protection is synapse specific, the removal of the write protection from the tagging-related variable to the scaffold is not. However, because the tagging-related variable in our model is synapse specific, only those synapses that have previously been marked by a tag can be consolidated (Frey and Morris, 1997). The synthesis of PRPs, which is necessary for the removal of the second write protection, requires the presence of dopamine or other neuromodulators. Dopamine is broadly diffused across several brain areas (Foote and Morrison, 1987; Gasbarri et al., 1994) and would therefore be likely to be available in many cells at about the same time. However, whereas dopamine is necessary to trigger synthesis of PRPs in hippocampal neurons, it is not sufficient if other conditions are not met.
Complications of such a simple picture arise because of multiple neuromodulators and spatial compartmentalization. First, different neuromodulators may target different dendritic compartments of a given neuron (Reymann and Frey, 2007) and different neurons. Second, synthesized PRPs are not uniformly spread out across the dendrite, but remain localized in different dendritic compartments (Kelleher et al., 2004; Govindarajan et al., 2006; Redondo and Morris, 2011). Extensions of our synapse model toward multicompartment neurons with locally shared PRPs (Govindarajan et al., 2006; O'Donnell and Sejnowski, 2014) are possible and would allow for competition between synapses for proteins (Fonseca et al., 2006).
Because dopamine (or other neuromodulators) is necessary for consolidation of weight changes, models of synaptic tagging and capture formally resemble the reward signal of reinforcement learning and R-STDP (Sutton and Barto, 1998; Izhikevich, 2007). In these theories, learning rules make use of an eligibility trace that represents potential changes, only transformed into effective synaptic alterations upon arrival of an external reward signal (Crow, 1968; Izhikevich, 2007; Frémaux et al., 2010; Frégnac et al., 2010; Pawlak et al., 2010).
Three important differences are worthy of discussion. First, whereas eligibility traces in reinforcement learning are invisible, in our model, the synaptic changes after LTP induction are visible as real weight changes a few seconds or minutes after induction, even if they are not consolidated later on. Second, the relevant timescale of eligibility traces in reinforcement learning is one or a few seconds, whereas the lifetime of tags is on the order of an hour. Third, whereas in reinforcement learning, dopamine is related to the reward (Schultz et al., 1997; Sutton and Barto, 1998; Schultz, 2007), in our model, the neuromodulator could also represent novelty, surprise, or “interestingness” of a stimulus (Redgrave and Gurney, 2006; Bethus et al., 2010; Lisman et al., 2011), similar to classic (Carpenter and Grossberg, 1987) or modern (Schmidhuber, 2006; Brea et al., 2013; Rezende and Gerstner, 2014) learning theories.
Conceptual predictions
First, our model exhibits a two-step write protection mechanism that is very different from the classic STC model. Under a hypothetical drug that prevents tag-setting while allowing the induction of LTP in a behavioral experiment, our model would predict that, if injected before encoding, the drug would leave short-term memory unchanged, but would disrupt consolidation into long-term memory. The result would thus be similar to that with protein synthesis inhibitors (that disrupt the transition from tagged to consolidated state), but the signaling chain would be interrupted earlier (from LTP induction to tagging). Similarly, a weak LTP-inducing stimulus that is not strong enough to overcome the write protection to the tag could be sufficient to set tags in the presence of ryanodine, known to facilitate the tag transition (Sajikumar et al., 2009).
Second, imagine a hypothetical experiment in which a stable and large synapse is pharmacologically manipulated by a direct change of the scaffold to the small state while keeping the weight and tagging-related variables in the strong and up state, respectively. After the manipulation, the configuration of the synapse is then identical to an initially weak synapse that underwent a strong LTP protocol. Therefore, we predict that, in the absence of dopamine or PRPs, the synapse would decay on the timescale of 1 h, whereas in the presence of dopamine or PRPs, it would reconsolidate to the stable strong state—even if no potentiating or depotentiating stimulus is applied.
Third, in our model, the notion of tag has undergone a fundamental shift. In the classic conceptual model of STC (Frey and Morris, 1997), as well as in early mathematical models (Clopath et al., 2008), the tag is assumed to be some transiently activated chemical compound. In our model, however, the tag is the difference between two synaptic variables, one tagging related and the other scaffold related. Our model predicts that the tagging-related variable exhibits transient state changes only in synapses that are not consolidated after an LTP- or LTD-inducing protocol. This reinterpretation may change the spectrum of candidates for tagging-related compounds (Lisman et al., 2011; Redondo and Morris, 2011).
Finally, although parameters of our model have been optimized for hippocampus, we believe that the same model should also be applicable to other brain areas, albeit with different parameters. For example, we predict that, in brain areas where induction of weight changes and removal of the first write protection occur on the same timescale and with the same stimulation threshold, tag-resetting experiments with subsequent recovery of the weight will not be possible.
Extensions and conclusions
Our model of synaptic plasticity accounts for a large variety of STC experiments and makes a link from synapses to behavior. Functionally, the model combines several aspects contributing to memory lifetime, in particular write protection (Crow, 1968), multiple timescales (Fusi et al., 2005), and bistability of essential components (Crick, 1984; Lisman, 1985). Extensions of the model should aim at inclusion of spatial compartmentalization of PRP (Kelleher et al., 2004; Fonseca et al., 2006; Govindarajan et al., 2006; O'Donnell and Sejnowski, 2014), explicit modeling of neuromodulator signals in terms of novelty and surprise (Schmidhuber, 2006; Rezende and Gerstner, 2014), as well as tests of the model in a broader set of behavioral paradigms (Wang et al., 2010; Ramirez et al., 2013; Li et al., 2014). Our model focused on consolidation at the synaptic level, whereas other mechanisms at the system level, such as theta rhythms, replay, and slow-wave sleep, also affect memory maintenance (Lubenov and Siapas, 2008; Rossato et al., 2009). Nonetheless, we have been able to show that the STC data are sufficient to capture the essence of behavioral phenomena observed in an IA task (Moncada and Viola, 2007).
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant 200020 13287, Coding Characteristics), the European Research Council (Grant 268689, MultiRules and European FP7 Grant 2269921, BrainScaleS). D.B.K. was supported by a Fulbright award in cooperation with a Swiss Government Scholarship.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Abraham WC, Bear MF. Metaplasticity: the plasticity of synaptic plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 1996;19:126–130. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)80018-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abraham WC. Metaplasticity: tuning synapses and networks for plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:387. doi: 10.1038/nrn2356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amit D, Fusi S. Learning in neural networks with material synapses. Neural Computing. 1994;6:957–982. doi: 10.1162/neco.1994.6.5.957. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Artola A, Bröcher S, Singer W. Different voltage dependent thresholds for inducing long-term depression and long-term potentiation in slices of rat visual cortex. Nature. 1990;347:69–72. doi: 10.1038/347069a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballarini F, Moncada D, Martinez MC, Alen N, Viola H. Behavioral tagging is a general mechanism of long-term memory formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:14599–14604. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907078106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett AB, Billings GO, Morris RG, van Rossum MC. State based model of long-term potentiation and synaptic tagging and capture. PLoS Comput Biol. 2009;5:e1000259. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bashir ZI, Collingridge GL. An investigation of depotentiation of long-term potentiation in the ca1 region of the hippocampus. Exp Brain Res. 1994;100:437–443. doi: 10.1007/BF02738403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bekinschtein P, Cammarota M, Igaz LM, Bevilaqua LR, Izquierdo I, Medina JH. Persistence of long-term memory storage requires a late protein synthesis- and BDNF- dependent phase in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2007;53:261–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bethus I, Tse D, Morris RG. Dopamine and memory: Modulation of the persistence of memory for novel hippocampal nmda receptor-dependent paired associates. J Neurosci. 2010;30:1610–1618. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2721-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bi G, Poo M. Synaptic modification of correlated activity: Hebb's postulate revisited. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:139–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bienenstock EL, Cooper LN, Munro PW. Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1982;2:32–48. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-01-00032.1982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bliss TV, Collingridge GL. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 1993;361:31–39. doi: 10.1038/361031a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bliss TV, Lomo T. Long-lasting potentation of synaptic transmission in the dendate area of anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol. 1973;232:351–356. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brader JM, Senn W, Fusi S. Learning real-world stimuli in a neural network with spike-driven synaptic dynamics. Neural Comput. 2007;19:2881–2912. doi: 10.1162/neco.2007.19.11.2881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brea J, Senn W, Pfister JP. Matching recall and storage in sequence learning with spiking neural networks. J Neurosci. 2013;33:9565–9575. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4098-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter GA, Grossberg S. Art 2: self-organization of stable category recognition codes for analog input patterns. Applied Optics. 1987;26:4919–4930. doi: 10.1364/AO.26.004919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cingolani LA, Goda Y. Actin in action: the interplay between the actin cytoskeleton and synaptic efficacy. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:344–356. doi: 10.1038/nrn2373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clopath C, Büsing L, Vasilaki E, Gerstner W. Connectivity reflects coding: A model of voltage-based spike-timing-dependent-plasticity with homeostasis. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:344–352. doi: 10.1038/nn.2479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clopath C, Ziegler L, Vasilaki E, Büsing L, Gerstner W. Tag-trigger-consolidation: a model of early and late long-term-potentiation and depression. PLoS Comput Biol. 2008;4:e1000248. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crick F. Memory and molecular turnover. Nature. 1984;312:101. doi: 10.1038/312101a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crow TJ. Cortical synapses and reinforcement: a hypothesis. Nature. 1968;219:736–737. doi: 10.1038/219736a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M. The amygdala: neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory and mental dysfunction. New York: Wiley; 1992. The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear; pp. 255–306. (XXXX, ed.) [Google Scholar]
- de Carvalho Myskiw J, Benetti F, Izquierdo I. Behavioral tagging of extinction learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:1071–1076. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1220875110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudai Y, Morris RGM. To consolidate or not to consolidate: what are the questions? In: Bolhuis JJ, editor. Brain, perception, memory: advances in cognitive sciences. Oxford: Oxford UP; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Dudai Y. The neurobiology of memory: concepts, findings, trends. Oxford: OUP; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Elliott T, Lagogiannis K. The rise and fall of memory in a model of synaptic integration. Neural Comput. 2012;24:2604–2654. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erickson MA, Maramara LA, Lisman J. A single brief burst induces glur1-dependent associative short-term potentiation: a potential mechanism for short-term memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22:2530–2540. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fonseca R, Nägerl UV, Bonhoeffer T. Neuronal activity determines the protein synthesis dependence of long-term potentiation. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:478–480. doi: 10.1038/nn1667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foote SL, Morrison JH. Extrathalamic modulation of cortical function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1987;10:67–95. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.10.030187.000435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frégnac Y, Pananceau M, René A, Huguet N, Marre O, Levy M, Shulz DE. A reexamination of hebbian covariance rules and spike timing-dependent plasticity in cat visual cortex in vivo. Front Synaptic Neurosci. 2010;2:147. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frémaux N, Sprekeler H, Gerstner W. Functional rerquirements for reward-modulated spike timing-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci. 2010;30:13326–13337. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6249-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frey U, Morris RG. Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature. 1997;385:533–536. doi: 10.1038/385533a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frey U, Schroeder H, Matthies H. Dopaminergic antagonists prevent long-term maintenance of posttetanic ltp in the ca1 region of rat hippocampal slices. Brain Res. 1990;522:69–75. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91578-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fusi S, Drew PJ, Abbott LF. Cascade models of synaptically stored memories. Neuron. 2005;45:599–611. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fusi S. Hebbian spike-driven synaptic plasticity for learning patterns of mean firing rates. Biol Cybern. 2002;87:459–470. doi: 10.1007/s00422-002-0356-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gasbarri A, Verney C, Innocenzi R, Campana E, Pacitti C. Mesolimbic dopaminergic neurons innervating the hippocampal formation in the rat: a combined retrograde tracing and immunohistochemical study. Brain Res. 1994;668:71–79. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90512-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerstner W, Kempter R, van Hemmen JL, Wagner H. A neuronal learning rule for sub-millisecond temporal coding. Nature. 1996;383:76–81. doi: 10.1038/383076a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerstner W, Kistler WM, Naud R, Paninski L. Neuronal dynamics: from single neurons to networks and models of cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Govindarajan A, Kelleher RJ, Tonegawa S. A clustered plasticity model of long-term memory engrams. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:575–583. doi: 10.1038/nrn1937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebb DO. The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley; 1949. [Google Scholar]
- Hollerman JR, Schultz W. Dopamine neurons report an error in the temporal prediction of reward during learning. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:304–309. doi: 10.1038/1124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hromádka T, Deweese MR, Zador AM. Sparse representation of sounds in the unanesthetized auditory cortex. PLoS Biol. 2008;6:e16. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izhikevich EM. Solving the distal reward problem through linkage of STDP and dopamine signaling. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2443–2452. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jimenez Rezende D, Gerstner W. Stochastic variational learning in recurrent spiking networks. Front Comput Neurosci. 2014;8:38. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2014.00038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelleher RJ, 3rd, Govindarajan A, Tonegawa S. Translational regulatory mechanisms in persistent forms of synaptic plasticity. Neuron. 2004;44:59–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohonen T. Self-organized formation of topologically correct feature maps. Biological Cybernetics. 1982;43:59–69. doi: 10.1007/BF00337288. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Korz V, Frey JU. Emotional and cognitive reinforcement of rat hippocampal long-term potentiation by different learning paradigms. Neuron Glia Biol. 2004;1:253–261. doi: 10.1017/S1740925X05000153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krug M, Lössner B, Ott T. Anisomycin blocks the late phase of long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus of freely moving rats. Brain Res Bull. 1984;13:39–42. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(84)90005-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SJ, Escobedo-Lozoya Y, Szatmari EM, Yasuda R. Activation of CaMKII in single dendritic spines during long-term potentiation. Nature. 2009;458:299–304. doi: 10.1038/nature07842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Q, Rothkegel M, Xiao ZC, Abraham WC, Korte M, Sajikumar S. Making synapses strong: metaplasticity prolongs associativity of long-term memory by switching synaptic tag mechanisms. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:353–363. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li S, Cullen WK, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ. Dopamine-dependent facilitation of LTP induction in hippocampal CA1 by exposure to spatial novelty. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:526–531. doi: 10.1038/nn1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman J, Grace AA, Duzel E. A neohebbian framework for episodic memory; role of dopamine-dependent late ltp. Trends Neurosci. 2011;34:536–547. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman J, Schulman H, Cline H. The molecular basis of camkii function in synaptic and behavioural memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:175–190. doi: 10.1038/nrn753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman J, Spruston N. Postsynaptic depolarization requirements for LTP and LTD: a critique of spike timing-dependent plasticity. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:839–841. doi: 10.1038/nn0705-839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman JE, Grace AA. The hippocampal-vta loop: controlling the entry of information into long-term memory. Neuron. 2005;46:703–713. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman JE. A mechanism for memory storage insensitive to molecular turnover: a bistable autophosphorylating kinase. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1985;82:3055–3057. doi: 10.1073/pnas.82.9.3055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ljungberg T, Apicella P, Schultz W. Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral interactions. J Neurophysiol. 1992;67:145–163. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.1.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubenov EV, Siapas AG. Decoupling through synchrony in neuronal circuits with propagation delays. Neuron. 2008;58:118–131. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maass W. On the computational power of winner-take-all. Neural Comput. 2000;12:2519–2535. doi: 10.1162/089976600300014827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markram H, Lübke J, Frotscher M, Sakmann B. Regulation of synaptic efficacy by coincidence of postsynaptic AP and EPSP. Science. 1997;275:213–215. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5297.213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin SJ, Grimwood PD, Morris RG. Synaptic plasticity and memory: an evaluation of the hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:649–711. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin SJ. Time-dependent reversal of dentate ltp by 5 hz stimulation. Neuroreport. 1998;9:3775–3781. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199812010-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyashita T, Kubik S, Lewandowski G, Guzowski JF. Networks of neurons, networks of genes: an integrated view of memory consolidation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;89:269–284. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moncada D, Viola H. Induction of long-term memory by exposure to novelty requires protein synthesis: evidence for a behavioral tagging. J Neurosci. 2007;27:7476–7481. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1083-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morrison A, Diesmann M, Gerstner W. Phenomenological models of synaptic plasticity based on spike timing. Biol Cybern. 2008;98:459–478. doi: 10.1007/s00422-008-0233-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadal JP, Toulouse G, Changeux JP, Dehaene S. Networks of formal neurons and memory palimpsests. Europhysics Letters. 1986;1:349–381. [Google Scholar]
- Nader K, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Reply-reconsolidation: the labile nature of consolidation theory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2000;1:216–219. doi: 10.1038/35044580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navakkode S, Sajikumar S, Frey JU. Synergistic requirements for the induction of dopaminergic D1/D5-receptor-mediated ltp in hippocampal slices of rat ca1 in vitro. Neuropharmacology. 2007;52:1547–1554. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2007.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ngezahayo A, Schachner M, Artola A. Synaptic activation modulates the induction of bidirectional synaptic changes in adult mouse hippocamus. J Neurosci. 2000;20:2451–2458. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-07-02451.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nguyen PV, Abel T, Kandel ER. Requirement of a critical period of transcription for induction of a late phase of ltp. Science. 1994;265:1104–1107. doi: 10.1126/science.8066450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connor DH, Wittenberg GM, Wang SS. Graded bidirectional synaptic plasticity is composed of switch-like unitary events. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:9679–9684. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0502332102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Donnell C, Sejnowski TJ. Selective memory generalization by spatial patterning of protein synthesis. Neuron. 2014;82:398–412. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okamoto K, Bosch M, Hayashi Y. The roles of camkii and f-actin in the structural plasticity of dendritic spines: A potential molecular identity of a synaptic tag? Physiology. 2009;24:357–366. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00029.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map: preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 1971;34:171–175. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pawlak V, Wickens JR, Kirkwood A, Kerr JN. Timing is not everything: neuromodulation opens the stdp gate. Front Synaptic Neurosci. 2010;2:146. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen CC, Malenka RC, Nicoll RA, Hopfield JJ. All-or-none potentiation at CA3-CA1 synapses. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:4732–4737. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.8.4732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfister JP, Gerstner W. Triplets of spikes in a model of spike timing-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci. 2006;26:9673–9682. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1425-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pi HJ, Lisman JE. Coupled phosphatase and kinase switches produce the tristability required for long-term potentiation and long-term depression. J Neurosci. 2008;28:13132–13138. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2348-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quiroga RQ, Reddy L, Kreiman G, Koch C, Fried I. Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature. 2005;435:1102–1107. doi: 10.1038/nature03687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramachandran B, Frey JU. Interfering with the actin network and its effect on long-term potentiation and synaptic tagging in hippocampal CA1 neurons in slices in vitro. J Neurosci. 2009;29:12167–12173. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2045-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramirez S, Liu X, Lin PA, Suh J, Pignatelli M, Redondo RL, Ryan TJ, Tonegawa S. Creating a false memory in the hippocampus. Science. 2013;341:387–391. doi: 10.1126/science.1239073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redgrave P, Gurney K. The short-latency dopamine signal: a role in discovering novel actions? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:967–975. doi: 10.1038/nrn2022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redondo RL, Morris RG. Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:17–30. doi: 10.1038/nrn2963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redondo RL, Okuno H, Spooner PA, Frenguelli BG, Bito H, Morris RG. Synaptic tagging and capture: differential role of distinct calcium/calmodulin kinases in protein synthesis-dependent long-term potentiation. J Neurosci. 2010;30:4981–4989. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3140-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reymann KG, Frey JU. The late maintenance of hippocampal ltp: requirements, phases, ‘synaptic tagging’, ‘late-associativity’ and implications. Neuropharmacology. 2007;52:24–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.07.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rochester N, Holland J, Haibt L, Duda W. Tests on a cell assembly theory of the action of the brain, using a large digital computer. IRE Transactions on Information Theory. 1956;2:80–93. doi: 10.1109/TIT.1956.1056810. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rogan MT, Stäubli UV, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature. 1997;390:604–607. doi: 10.1038/37601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossato JI, Bevilaqua LR, Izquierdo I, Medina JH, Cammarota M. Dopamine controls persistence of long-term memory storage. Science. 2009;325:1017–1020. doi: 10.1126/science.1172545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sajikumar S, Frey JU. Late-associativity, synaptic tagging, and the role of dopamine during ltp and ltd. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004a;82:12–25. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sajikumar S, Frey JU. Resetting of ‘synaptic tags’ is time- and activity-dependent in rat hippocampal ca1 in vitro. Neuroscience. 2004b;129:503–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sajikumar S, Li Q, Abraham WC, Xiao ZC. Priming of short-term potentiation and synaptic tagging/capture mechanisms by ryanodine receptor activation in rat hippocampal CA1. Learn Mem. 2009;16:178–186. doi: 10.1101/lm.1255909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidhuber J. Developmental robotics, optimal artificial curiosity, creativity, music, and the fine arts. Connection Science. 2006;18:173–187. doi: 10.1080/09540090600768658. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275:1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:203–210. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sjöström PJ, Turrigiano GG, Nelson SB. Rate, timing, and cooperativity jointly determine cortical synaptic plasticity. Neuron. 2001;32:1149–1164. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00542-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smolen P, Baxter DA, Byrne JH. Molecular constraints on synaptic tagging and maintenance of long-term potentiation: a predictive model. PLoS Comput Biol. 2012;8:e1002620. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song S, Miller KD, Abbott LF. Competitive Hebbian learning through spike-time-dependent synaptic plasticity. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:919–926. doi: 10.1038/78829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stäubli U, Chun D. Factors regulating the reversibility of long-term potentiation. J Neurosci. 1996;16:853–860. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-02-00853.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton R, Barto A. Reinforcement learning: an introduction. Cambridge, MA: MIT; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Treves A, Rolls ET. Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input systems to the hippocampal CA3 network. Hippocampus. 1992;2:189–199. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450020209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsodyks M, Pawelzik K, Markram H. Neural networks with dynamic synapses. Neural Computation. 1998;10:821–835. doi: 10.1162/089976698300017502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E, Markowitsch HJ. Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 1998;8:198–204. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<198::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang SH, Redondo RL, Morris RG. Relevance of synaptic tagging and capture to the persistence of long-term potentiation and everyday spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:19537–19542. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008638107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitlock JR, Heynen AJ, Shuler MG, Bear MF. Learning induces long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Science. 2006;313:1093–1097. doi: 10.1126/science.1128134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Q, Homma KJ, Poo MM. Shrinkage of dendritic spines associated with long-term depression of hippocampal synapses. Neuron. 2004;44:749–757. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]