Abstract
When you ruminate about an unfortunate encounter with a loved one, savor a long sought accomplishment, hold in mind feelings from a marvelous or regretfully tragic moment, what mental processes orchestrate these psychological phenomena? Such experiences typify how affect interacts with working memory, which we posit can occur in three primary ways: (1) emotional experiences can modulate working memory, (2) working memory can modulate emotional experiences, and (3) feelings can be the mental representations maintained by working memory. We propose that this third mode constitutes distinct neuropsychological processes that support the integration of particular cognitive and affective processes: affective working memory. Accumulating behavioral and neural evidence suggests that affective working memory processes maintain feelings and are partially separable from their cognitive working memory counterparts. Affective working memory may be important for elucidating the contribution of affect to decision making, preserved emotional processes in later life, and mechanisms of psychological dysfunction in clinical disorders. We review basic behavioral, neuroscience, and clinical research that provides evidence for affective working memory, consider its theoretical implications, and evaluate its functional role within the psychological architecture. In sum, the perspective we advocate is that affective working memory is a fundamental mechanism of mind.
Keywords: working memory, emotion, affect, emotion regulation, emotional intelligence
In daily life feelings come and go, influencing our thoughts, decisions, and actions in ways we may or may not recognize. Dynamic interactions between emotion and cognition manifest in numerous ways. For example, consider being highly anxious about an upcoming speech, and consequently find yourself unable to maintain mental focus on the content of your presentation. In this instance, your current emotional state impairs your ability to hold in mind and work with important information; that is, emotion can impair your working memory abilities. In another example, consider working intently on an important project, carefully crafting how to pitch your new idea when you notice a spider scurrying away across the wall. In this instance, your typical fear from seeing a spider is diminished and dissipates quickly. Here, your mental focus and engaged working memory weakens your emotional reaction.
These examples capture the bidirectional ways that emotion and working memory can interact. However, there is a third possibility as well: You could focus on your emotional reactions, actively hold those feelings in mind, and work with them in various ways. Upon seeing the spider, you could focus directly on the fear it evokes, note the diminished intensity, and in so doing, actively maintain this feeling in mind. This third possibility represents what we and others refer to as affective working memory, a specific type of working memory that maintains and works with feeling states (see e.g., Barrett, Mesquita, Ochsner, & Gross, 2007; Davidson & Irwin, 1999; Mikels, Reuter-Lorenz, Beyer, & Fredrickson, 2008; Mikels & Reuter-Lorenz, 2013; Smith & Lane, 2015; cf. Baddeley, 2013). In other words, just as working memory serves to maintain and manipulate visual or verbal representations, it can similarly operate on emotional representations (i.e., feelings).
Our goal here is to clarify and develop the construct of affective working memory in the context of emotion-working memory interactions more generally by characterizing the three modes by which working memory and emotion interact. These are depicted in Figure 1: (Mode 1) Affect can influence the efficiency of on-going cognitive working memory processes; (Mode 2) Cognitive working memory can influence on-going emotional experiences; and (Mode 3) Emotional feelings themselves can be the mental representations maintained in working memory requiring specialized mechanisms that are distinct from verbal, visual, and other cognitive working memory processes. These 3 modes capture the broad and surging interest in how emotion and working memory interact. A PsychInfo search of “working memory” and “emotion” currently results in 1,456 publications. Since the first publication on this topic circa 1986, over one-third (approximately 637) of the articles have been published in the past few years (since 2014). This large literature ranges from clinical science and social psychology to neuroscience.
Figure 1.
The three modes of emotion and working memory interactions: Mode 1 represents the influence of affect on cognitive working memory, Mode 2 represents the influence of cognitive working memory on affect, and Mode 3 represents how emotional feelings can be mental representations in working memory (affective working memory).
Why Affective Working Memory?
Most of this literature to date treats working memory and affect as distinct and separate psychological systems that interact (in Figure 1, Modes 1 & 2). The distinguishing characteristic of Mode 3 is that emotional feelings are the mental representations held in working memory. That is, just as perceptual representations of stimuli from the environment (exteroceptive input) can be maintained in working memory, information about internal feeling states (interoceptive input) also can be actively maintained in the absence of the eliciting stimuli. The ability to maintain feeling states – apart from any verbal, semantic, or conceptual codes associated with emotions – is needed because the feelings themselves consist of valence, intensity, and hedonic qualities that are critical for guiding goal-directed thoughts and actions, as originally argued by Davidson and Irwin (1999). As illustrated in Figure 1, affective working memory differs from Mode 1, which addresses how affect influences “cognitive” working memory processes and from Mode 2, which entails “cognitive” working memory influences on emotion. Like verbal and visual-spatial working memory, affective working memory is a domain specific working memory subsystem, with specialized processes for maintaining feelings, and executive control processes that likely overlap, in whole or in part, with other working memory subsystems. In the following sections, we review the 3 modes of emotion-working memory interaction. We then focus on empirical evidence from behavioral studies with younger and older adults as well as several brain imaging studies that support the affective working memory construct. We go on to explain why affective working memory is relevant in translational domains including decision making and psychiatric disorders. Finally, we consider the theoretical relevance and the potential role of affective working memory in emotion regulation, emotional intelligence, and wisdom, and point to future directions in the study of affective working memory.
Conceptual and Definitional Clarity
We first ground our perspective by explaining its key constructs: emotion and working memory. Most theorists regard emotions to be particular types of affective processes (Ekman, 1994; Ellsworth & Smith, 1988; Fredrickson, 1998; Frijda, 1986; Gross, 1998; Lazarus, 1991; Levenson, 1994; Rosenberg, 1998; Scherer, 2000; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). Emotions are generally considered to be adaptive responses to personally significant events. They differ from moods, which are relatively diffuse and often disconnected from a specific event or object, whereas emotions are acute reactions to specific events and objects. Emotions are differentiated from relatively enduring affective predispositions or traits (e.g., anxiety or depression). Affective traits can predispose a person to certain characteristic emotional responses (e.g. anxiety-prone individuals are more likely to experience fear than those that are not anxiety-prone), but are nonetheless fundamentally distinguishable from emotions per se.
Most contemporary emotion theorists agree with a general process model of emotion (see e.g., Gross, 1998). This model posits that typically, when an emotion arises, appraisal of the relationship between the person and the environment triggers behavioral, physiological, and motivational changes (see Ellsworth, 2013; Moors et al., 2013). Appraisal is the process of evaluating the circumstances for goal relevance, self versus other responsibility, certainty, etc. Emotional experiences differ widely due to the type and number of appraisals; a minimal number of appraisals result in coarse emotional experiences (e.g., fear or joy), whereas more complex and differentiated appraisals will produce nuanced emotional experiences (e.g., guilt or pride). Although the exact nature and composition of an emotional experience are widely debated, as depicted in the “Emotion” box of Figure 1, most theorists agree that (1) upon appraisal, the unfolding emotional reaction at least consists of: (2) physiological reactivity, (3) the subjective feeling of the affective state (most critical to the affective working memory construct), (4) a facial expression, and (5) an urge to act, i.e., an action tendency (Ekman, 1994; Fredrickson, 1998; Levenson, 1994; Rosenberg, 1998; Smith & Kirby, 2000). Although this process model provides a detailed account of how an emotional experience unfolds, it is relatively silent about the myriad ways that emotions interface with cognition beyond appraisal processes. In particular, the emotion process model does not clearly specify how emotions interface with working memory.
Working memory is a form of short-term memory that is essential to goal-directed behavior. Working memory actively maintains and manipulates information held in mind for brief periods of time in the service of on-going cognitive abilities like reasoning, language comprehension, and problem solving (Baddeley, 2012). In contrast to long-term memory, working memory has capacity limits of about 3–4 items (see e.g., Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Cowan, 2001; Miller, 1956; Oberauer, Farrell, Jarrold & Lewandowsky, 2016). The precise nature and measurement of working memory are areas of on-going research and active debate (Cowan, 2017; Oberauer et al., 2018). The view we adopt here has been referred to as the “multi-component view” (Cowan, 2017). This view follows largely from the work of Baddeley and Hitch (1974; Baddeley, 2000), which includes modality-specific storage and executive processes. Early models proposed that information was held in separate stores (or buffers) upon which a central executive operated (Baddeley, 1986; e.g., via rehearsal in an articulatory loop and via attention for the visuospatial sketchpad). However rather than buffers, more recent varieties of the multi-component view refer to the maintenance of “distributed” domain-specific representations that rely on the same brain regions that encode the perceptual stimulation but maintain the representations even though the stimulus is no longer present (Christophel, Klink, Spitzer, Roelfsema, & Haynes, 2017; Postle, 2015, 2016; Lewis-Peacock et al., 2012). Figure 1 provides a general illustration of these in the box labeled “Working Memory.”
Despite on-going debates about the domain generality versus specificity of working memory processes, and their relationship with long-term memory (e.g., Cowan, 2008; Engle, Kane, & Tuholski, 1999; Miyake & Shah, 1999; Oberauer, Farrell, Jarrold & Lewandowsky, 2016), most behavioral and neuroscience evidence favors domain-specific representations as depicted in Figure 1 (see e.g., Baddeley, 2012; Jonides et al., 2008; Fougnie, et al., 2015). However, attentional processes (i.e., selection and inhibition) are widely considered to be core executive functions that facilitate maintenance and are likely domain general (see e.g., Cowan et al., 2011; Engle, 2018; Li, Christ, & Cowan, 2014); but this too is debated. Additional executive functions that operate on working memory representations include monitoring, updating, and task/set switching (for reviews see McCabe et al. 2010; Smith & Jonides, 1999; see Cowan, 2008 for alternative views).
Although considerable empirical progress has been made in affective science and cognitive science by treating these key constructs separately, in daily life they are closely intertwined. In the following sections we endeavor to characterize three major types of emotion-working memory interactions that have been the focus of empirical laboratory research, while recognizing that the real-time dynamics in daily life are far more nuanced such that multiple forms of interaction likely happen concurrently.
Three Modes of Emotion-Working Memory Interactions
We propose the three-mode framework to bring theoretical organization to the vast body of research on emotion-working memory interactions. Early in our thinking about these modes, we wrote a one-page entry in the Encyclopedia of the Mind, in which we briefly sketched out this framework (Mikels & Reuter-Lorenz, 2013). The current article provides a broader and more developed treatment of our framework.
Mode 1: Affect can modulate working memory.
This first mode of interaction seems intuitive because most of us encounter it every day; the positive and negative emotions we experience may influence how effectively we engage in ongoing cognitive tasks, especially those involving what we think about and hold in mind. Not surprisingly then, the lion’s share of research on working memory and affect falls under this mode of interaction. From our reading of the literature we have discerned two important principles governing how affect can modulate working memory. First, the properties of affect (e.g., negative versus positive affect; high versus low arousal; trait versus state affect) determine how it influences working memory performance.1 Second, these properties have different influences on verbal versus non-verbal working memory and on working memory sub-processes (e.g., encoding, maintaining, retrieving, or managing information). The corpus of research addressing this mode suggests affect that is not relevant to the task being performed (i.e., task-irrelevant affect) can influence working memory performance. However, depending on the type of material being retained, the working memory sub-processes targeted, and the specific ways that affect is manifest (e.g., dispositional trait affect, induced affective states), the consequences will differ.
One primary approach to understanding how affect influences working memory focuses on individual differences, with the main finding being that trait-level negative affect impairs cognitive working memory. For instance, clinically depressed individuals demonstrate decreased visual and verbal working memory performance (Christopher & MacDonald, 2005), with extensive research implicating reductions in cognitive control as the basis for working memory impairments (for a review see Gotlib & Joormann, 2010). Anxiety effects have also been found. For example, individuals with high levels of math anxiety can exhibit impaired verbal working memory (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001; Elliman, Green, Rogers, & Finch, 1997). Likewise, in a study that included individuals diagnosed with social anxiety and other anxiety disorders, along with healthy controls, higher levels of anxiety symptomatology were associated with poorer verbal working memory performance (Waechter et al, 2018). Indeed, a recent meta-analysis revealed a robust association between generally higher anxiety and lower working memory capacity, with larger effects for clinical than subclinical populations (Moran, 2016). Further, the strongest effects are evident in working memory tasks requiring attentional processes that exert cognitive control on different representations (e.g., verbal and visual). These results align with attentional control theory, according to which anxiety disrupts the central executive components of working memory needed to prevent distraction from irrelevant information and control attention shifting between different tasks (Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, 2007; Eysenck & Derakshan, 2011). Thus, anxiety can impair verbal and visuo-spatial working memory by interfering with cognitive attentional control processes. Although research examining trait positive affect is much more limited, interestingly, recent findings indicate that higher trait positive affect is related to better visual working memory performance (Figueira et al., 2018). Overall, it appears, then, that the more intense and pervasive the negative affective disposition, the greater the detrimental impact on working memory, especially on tasks that tap its executive components.
Beyond clinical and dispositional influences, another approach common to Mode 1 is to induce variations in positive and negative affective states, and examine the effects on verbal and nonverbal working memory performance. For example, Shackman et al. (2006) found that induced anxiety disrupts spatial working memory (see also Figueria et al., 2017), whereas Gray (2001) found that induced anxiety improved spatial working memory but impaired verbal working memory (see also Darke, 1988; Ikeda, Iwanaga, & Seiwa, 1996). Conversely, induced positive affect has also been found to impair spatial working memory while improving verbal working memory (Gray, 2001; see also Carpenter et al., 2013; Yang et al., 2013). The finding that positive and negative affective states exert opposite effects on verbal versus nonverbal working memory performance was subsequently replicated (Gray, Braver, & Raichle, 2002) and found to correspond closely with varying levels of brain activity in lateral prefrontal brain regions known to be involved in cognitive working memory, suggesting a neural signal of emotion-cognition interactions. Nevertheless, the evidence that positive and negative affect have differential and opposing effects on verbal and spatial or non-verbal working memory is mixed. For example, Storbeck and Maswood (2016) found that positive affect improved both verbal and spatial working memory, whereas other studies report that positive affect impairs verbal working memory (Allen et al., 2014; Martin & Kerns, 2011). In sum, the effects of induced affective states on working memory performance are clearly mixed, with results that vary due to the valence of the emotion, the type of material being held in memory, and likely the processing demands of the task. Inconsistencies among the outcomes may be due in part to the affect induction methods employed and the extent to which the working memory measures engaged executive processes. Future research aimed at replicating valence and material-dependent effects that generalize across induction procedures would be especially valuable to clarify this mode of emotion-working memory interactions.
Affective influences can also arise from the emotional properties of task-relevant verbal and non-verbal stimuli (e.g., emotional words, pictures, or faces presented as to-be-remembered items), which could then impact working memory for those materials due to the emotions they arouse. Using affectively charged stimuli to investigate emotional influences on working memory can be problematic however, because typically the emotional state of the rememberer is not assessed or analyzed, which may contribute to differing outcomes associated with this approach. For instance, one study found that high arousal negative images impaired spatial working memory for their locations relative to lower arousal images (Mather et al., 2006). Likewise, two related studies examined emotional distraction in working memory, and found that highly arousing negative emotional distracters disrupted visual working memory disproportionately relative to neutral distracters (Dolcos, Kragel, Wang, & McCarthy, 2006; Dolcos & McCarthy, 2006; see also Hur et al., 2017) and more so than positive emotional distracters (Iordan & Dolcos, 2017; Garcia-Pacios et al., 2015). An extensive review of emotional distraction in working memory from behavioral and neural studies draws similar conclusions (see Iordan, Dolcos, & Dolcos, 2013). More recently, Hur, Iordan, Dolcos, and Berenbaum (2017) examined how performance on a working memory task might differ when a two-back task requires matching the hues of emotional pictures (i.e., blue or yellow) versus matching their valence (i.e., negative or neutral). Most relevant to the present discussion, they observed that the color N-back task was performed more slowly when the images were negative than when they were neutral suggesting a disruptive effect of negative affect. Thus, this set of studies suggests that negative emotional material may impair working memory – consistent with the effects of dispositional affect.
In contrast, but consistent with the findings of Gray (2001), Jackson et al. (2009) found that negative stimuli (specifically angry faces) enhanced visual working memory performance relative to happy and neutral faces (see Gotoh et al., 2010 for similar effects with words). Moreover, this enhancement was found for encoding and maintenance processes in working memory but not for retrieval (Jackson et al., 2014). Likewise, evidence indicates that positive and negative emotional words and images facilitate certain working memory executive processes (namely, the resolution of interference from previous material; Levens & Phelps, 2008). Nevertheless, modulatory effects of emotional stimuli on working memory performance are not consistently observed (e.g., Fairfield et al., 2015; Kensinger & Corkin, 2003; Truong & Yang, 2014). Moreover, Gooding and Tallent (2003) found that working memory performance for emotional expressions and facial identities is highly correlated, suggesting that maintenance of emotional and non-emotional information relies on the same cognitive working memory system.
Despite these inconsistencies in the corpus of research categorized as Mode 1, the following conclusions can be offered about how affect modulates working memory performance. First, potential impact on working memory depends crucially on whether the affective influence arises from dispositional or induced affective states versus from emotional stimuli. Although the underlying mechanism may be the same, namely the subjective feeling state of the rememberer, dispositional negative affect appears to have more reliable and consistent effects on working memory performance than induced affective states or emotional stimuli (such as images of facial expressions and emotional words). A second related generalization is that the greater the intensity of the affective disposition, the greater the impact on working memory performance. Third, there may be valence specificity: Negative affect appears to impair verbal working memory, especially for dispositional affect – whereas the effects of positive affect are less studied and equivocal. Finally, encoding, maintenance, and especially executive control of working memory may be more susceptible to affective influences than retrieval processes, though more work is needed to identify process-specific effects. It is also important to note for studies examining the effects of affectively laden materials, the methods differ, the emotional nature of the stimuli vary, and consequently the emotional state of the rememberer is largely unknown. Most crucial, though, verbal or visuospatial working memory for emotional stimuli is distinct from working memory for emotional feelings, which constitutes the third mode discussed later.
Mode 2: Cognitive working memory can modulate emotional experiences.
This second mode concerns how cognitive working memory and individual differences in working memory capacity may modify emotional experiences. As we explain in detail below, Mode 2 is closely related to emotion regulation, in that, the evidence suggests that cognitive working memory can influence (i.e., up-regulate or down-regulate) emotions. This mode has been studied less than Mode 1, however we can offer two guiding principles based on our review: (1) cognitive working memory itself can provide one means of emotion regulation; and (2) cognitive working memory can influence other emotion regulatory strategies such as suppression and reappraisal.
Evidence that cognitive working memory can influence emotional experience (i.e., regulate emotion) comes from manipulations of working memory demand (also referred to as, cognitive load in some reports). For example, Van Dillen and colleagues demonstrated that as the complexity of the cognitive working memory tasks increased, the intensity of self-reported negative emotional reactions decreased (Van Dillen, Heslenfeld, & Koole, 2009; Van Dillen & Koole, 2007). Neuroimaging studies of such effects yield complementary results: Cognitive working memory load is associated with decreased activity in emotion processing regions, such as the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex (e.g., Kellermann et al., 2012; see also Van Dillen et al., 2009; and Kron, Schul, Cohen, & Hassin, 2010). Although explanations of these findings vary, the evidence suggests that working memory load can serve to regulate emotion. That is, when cognitive working memory load is high, fewer resources are available for other on-going tasks, including the appraisal and rating of an emotional stimulus. Alternatively, with higher working memory load, affective processing may be inhibited to divert resources towards the cognitive task, which could also explain diminished effects on emotional experience (Clarke & Johnstone, 2013). These ideas converge with load theory, which posits that with higher cognitive load fewer resources are available for other forms of processing (Lavie, Hirst, De Fockert, & Viding, 2004), potentially including the processing of emotional stimuli. Regardless of the underlying mechanisms, from an emotion regulation vantage point then, cognitive working memory load generally serves to down-regulate (i.e., diminish) the impact of emotional stimuli.
The second empirical approach for Mode 2 examines how cognitive working memory impacts concurrent yet unrelated emotion regulatory processes. For example, when people engage in cognitive working memory tasks, their ability to actively suppress emotional facial expressions in response to emotional stimuli is diminished (Schmeichel, 2007). An individual difference approach produced corresponding results; compared to people with lower working memory capacity, high capacity individuals were better able to suppress negative and positive emotional expressions, and they demonstrated superior reappraisal abilities (Schmeichel et al., 2008; Schmeichel & Demaree, 2010). In related work, poorer working memory updating and greater susceptibility to interference in working memory were associated with more negative thoughts and lower reappraisal abilities (Pe, Raes, & Kuppens, 2013; Pe, Raes, Koval, Brans, Verduyn, & Kuppens, 2013). Moreover, better updating for positive versus negative material in cognitive working memory has been related to greater well-being and life satisfaction (Pe, Koval, & Kuppens, 2013). These studies suggest that cognitive control processes contribute to certain forms of emotion regulation.
In sum, research addressing how cognitive working memory influences emotional experience and regulation yields more consistent results than the first mode: Cognitive working memory can influence emotion regulatory functions (via load effects) and it seems to undergird other unrelated emotion regulation processes. In particular, evidence indicates that cognitive working memory resources (i.e., executive processes) are needed for emotion regulation (e.g., when suppressing emotional facial expressions; Schmeichel, 2007). Also, cognitive working memory capacity is related to emotion regulation abilities; generally, individuals with higher cognitive working memory capacity have greater emotion regulation abilities relative to those with lower working memory capabilities.
Mode 3: Emotional feelings can be mental representations maintained in working memory.
For Mode 3, emotional feelings themselves are the contents of working memory. As reviewed above, according to the emotion process model, after appraisal, an emotional experience ensues consisting of such components as physiological reactivity, an action tendency, a facial expression, and the subjective feeling. Although there are multiple components of the emotion process, it is specifically the subjective feeling of the affective state that constitutes its core mental representation. Feelings have positive or negative valence, are low or high in arousal, and vary in intensity level (Barrett & Russell, 1999; Reisenzein, 1994). Working memory maintains and manipulates mental representations, and when those representations are strictly affective in nature – as is the case with the subjective feeling state – affective working memory is implicated. The crucial distinction is that for Mode 3, emotional subjective feeling states are the representations processed and maintained by working memory. This focus on the actual feeling differs from Mode 1, in which affect-laden stimuli are the representations processed and maintained by cognitive working memory. Although such stimuli likely evoke affect, we contend that Mode 3 functions specifically to maintain and work with emotional feeling states rather than maintaining representations of the stimuli that elicit the emotion.
Affective Working Memory
By analogy with cognitive working memory (see e.g., Jonides, 1995), affective working memory has several defining characteristics. First, over the short-term, it maintains a mental representation – the subjective emotional feeling – in the absence of the eliciting stimulus (i.e., emotion maintenance). Second, the maintained representation is instrumental for subsequent goals. That is, affective working memory involves the set of mental processes that maintain an emotional feeling that is integral to goal-directed behavior. Third, affective working memory entails actively holding in mind a feeling – in contrast to passively experiencing an emotion. In other words, working memory processes exert deliberative control over the emotional feeling. Within the context of the working memory models reviewed above, we consider affective working memory to fit within the multi-component view. Also, consistent with recent accounts of working memory that focus on the maintenance of distributed domain-specific representations (e.g., Christophel et al., 2017; Postle, 2015, 2016), we consider affective working memory similarly to involve the maintenance of distributed neural representations (including interoceptive signals) of feelings. As with other non-verbal forms of working memory, we posit that attentional control is required for active maintenance, or some form of refreshing. Emerging evidence, reviewed below, supports this characterization and suggests that affective working memory constitutes a neuropsychological subsystem composed of mental operations that are at least partially separable from cognitive working memory.
Desiderata for investigating affective working memory.
Guided by this theoretical conceptualization of affective working memory, we developed and tested a novel paradigm in which emotional feelings are themselves the mental representations. Moreover, to ascertain whether affective working memory is distinct from cognitive working memory we used the dissociation logic often employed to test the separability of psychological processes (see e.g., Baddeley, 1986; Jonides, 1995). This necessitated a parallel task requiring maintenance of a non-emotional subjective state to contrast with emotion maintenance. Comparing performance on these two delayed-response tasks allowed us to quantify the “accuracy” of maintaining a subjective emotional feeling relative to maintaining a subjective brightness representation. The emotion maintenance task required participants to maintain over a brief delay period the feeling elicited from viewing a static emotional image, and then compare the intensity of that feeling to the intensity of another one evoked by a second image of the same emotional valence (i.e., participants pressed a button to indicate whether the feeling evoked by the second image was of higher or lower intensity than the feeling evoked by the first image). In the non-emotional analog task the subjective brightness representation derived from a static neutral image was held in memory and subsequently compared to the brightness of a second image (see Figure 2).2
Figure 2.
Generalized schematic for two model working memory tasks: For both tasks a target image is presented typically for 5 seconds, followed by a retention interval of 3–10 seconds, and then a probe image is presented for 5 seconds. After the probe image in the emotion maintenance task, a response is made to indicate whether the emotional reaction to the probe image is higher or lower in intensity relative to the feeling evoked by the target image. In the analogous brightness maintenance task, a response is made regarding the relative perceptions of brightness intensity. In one experiment reported by Mikels et al. (2008), additional secondary tasks were performed during the retention interval to demonstrate the separability of maintenance processes underlying these two tasks (see also Figure 3). These representative images for the emotion maintenance task were selected to evoke varying intensities of awe for a positive emotion trial. Images evoking negative emotions were paired together and also used in our studies. Mikels et al., Emotion and working memory: Evidence for domain-specific processes for affective maintenance, Emotion, 8(2), 256–266, 2008, APA, adapted with permission.
Behavioral and neural evidence for affective working memory.
To test whether the processes supporting affective working memory are psychologically distinct, we examined the potentially disruptive effects of performing secondary tasks on performance of the emotion and brightness maintenance tasks, as is traditionally done (e.g., Barnes, Nelson, & Reuter-Lorenz, 2001; Logie, Gilhooly, & Wynn, 1994; Welford, 1952). Conceivably, both tasks could rely on the same maintenance processes in that visual working memory could be used to maintain a perceptual representation of the first picture. To assess this possibility, we used a secondary visual search task during the maintenance interval that placed maximal demand on visual processing to disrupt visual working memory and, potentially, both tasks. Another possibility was that both maintenance tasks also engaged verbal encoding or a combination of visual and verbal encoding. For instance, participants may generate a numeric code or verbal label to characterize the first image and maintain this representation over the delay. Thus, we also included a secondary verbal task during the retention interval intended to thwart verbal recoding strategies (Murray, 1967, 1968; Richardson & Baddeley, 1975). If affective working memory relies on separable mechanisms that are neither verbal nor visual in nature, we reasoned that these secondary tasks should interfere minimally in comparison to the brightness task, which is clearly visual in nature.
Likewise, to interfere selectively with emotion maintenance, we used an emotion regulation task (modeled after Gross, 1998; Ochsner et al., 2002). For this secondary task, participants viewed an additional emotional image during the retention interval, were instructed to think about it in a way that would make them feel less negative, and then rated the intensity of their down-regulated feeling during retention. We reasoned that if the emotion maintenance task requires that people actively hold a feeling in mind, then the requirement to process another feeling should disrupt emotion maintenance during the delay.
Using these tasks, we found that performing the secondary visual and verbal tasks selectively interfered with brightness maintenance yet facilitated performance on the emotion maintenance task (Mikels, Reuter-Lorenz, Beyer, & Fredrickson, 2008). This facilitation resembles the beneficial effects of articulatory suppression on visual imagery (Brandimonte et al., 1992) and suggests that blocking verbal recoding enables higher fidelity maintenance of affective feelings (see Mikels et al., 2008, for further discussion). Moreover, performing a secondary emotion regulation task selectively interfered with emotion maintenance leaving brightness maintenance unimpaired (see Figure 3). These selective interference effects suggest separable affective working memory processes support performance in the emotion maintenance task. Importantly, when participants are tested on the emotion maintenance task across multiple testing sessions, performance is significantly correlated, indicating good reliability (Broome, Gard, & Mikels, 2012).
Figure 3.
An adapted depiction of interference effects on emotion and brightness maintenance performance from Mikels et al. (2008): The affective secondary task interfered only with emotion maintenance, whereas the cognitive secondary tasks interfered with brightness maintenance but facilitated emotion maintenance. As interference scores were calculated by subtracting maintenance performance with the secondary tasks from maintenance performance without the secondary tasks, positive scores indicate interference (relative performance decrement) and negative scores indicate facilitation (relative performance benefit). Mikels et al., Emotion and working memory: Evidence for domain-specific processes for affective maintenance, Emotion, 8(2), 256–266, 2008, APA, adapted with permission.
In related adult life-span work, we examined the effects of normal aging on these same maintenance tasks, and found further evidence that affective and cognitive working memory processes are separable. Decades of research have documented age-related declines in many cognitive domains (Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009). Decline in working memory is particularly pervasive (e.g., Park et al., 2002; for a meta-analysis see Verhaeghen, Marcoen, & Goossens, 1993), with age differences noted for all types of information, including verbal information, visual images, objects, spatial locations, and even faces (see Swanson, 2017 for a life-span perspective on domain specificity in cognitive working memory). Given such widespread declines, one might expect deficits in working memory for emotional feelings as well. The socioemotional literature on aging suggests otherwise, though, based on evidence that emotional processes remain fairly stable across the life span (Carstensen, Mikels, & Mather, 2006). Accordingly, we hypothesized that affective and cognitive working memory would dissociate with age. We expected to observe age-related decline on our brightness maintenance task, but not on our emotion maintenance task, and this is precisely what we found. Whereas older adults showed typical decline in maintaining visual information, the ability to maintain emotional feelings was unaffected by age (Mikels, Larkin, Reuter-Lorenz, & Carstensen, 2005). Additionally, although younger adults showed superior memory for negative relative to positive feelings, older adults showed the opposite pattern. This finding supports the developmental pattern referred to as the “positivity effect,” whereby the disproportionate preference for negativity in youth shifts toward positivity in later life (Carstensen & Mikels, 2005; Mikels et al., 2014). Notably, performance on the brightness maintenance task was correlated with a standard working memory measure (digit span), whereas performance of the emotion maintenance task was not. Thus, our research with older adults dissociates emotion and brightness maintenance, supporting the proposal that separable psychological processes mediate emotion maintenance.
An additional piece of behavioral evidence indicating that the ability to deliberately maintain a feeling is at least partially separable from cognitive processing comes from a study by DeFraine (2016). Using a version of the Mikels et al. (2008) task, this study found that people could actively hold a feeling in mind while also doing a mental arithmetic task, and that the intensity of the feeling was relatively unchanged (DeFraine, 2016). In contrast, mental arithmetic did reduce the intensity of feelings in response to passively viewed images. These results suggest that the cognitive processes needed for mental arithmetic are not necessary for active emotion maintenance, consistent with the idea that affective working memory depends on domain-specific processes.
Although the neuroscience evidence pertaining directly to affective working memory is scant, the results from a handful of studies also suggest there may be distinct neural underpinnings of active emotion maintenance. First, early neuroimaging evidence indicated that when people were instructed to actively maintain subjective affective experiences induced by viewing negative emotional images, there was greater amygdala activation during the subsequent delay period compared to passive viewing of the same images (Schaefer et al., 2002). These results suggest that the deliberate, active maintenance of negative emotion operates in part by modulating the level of amygdala activity. The region of interest approach used by Schaefer and colleagues (2002) focused on the amygdala and did not include cortical regions that may have also been involved. However, more recent work from Waugh, Lemus, & Gotlib (2014) indicates that emotion maintenance has a unique cortical signature compared to a non-maintenance (passive) condition.3 They found that the active maintenance of the feelings evoked by an emotional image was associated with greater activation in dorsal medial frontal cortex (dMPFC) and in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dLPFC). The authors speculate that dMPFC involvement is associated with the maintenance and elaboration of the emotional state over the delay, whereas the dLPFC was involved in the information manipulation associated with comparing the two feeling states. Although this interpretation is speculative, it aligns well with related research indicating that regions of MPFC play an important role in attending to one’s internal affective state (Smith et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2016).
Finally, a recent study by Smith et al. (2018) presented emotional images followed by a maintenance period that required people to actively maintain the emotional feeling evoked by the emotional image, or a visual memory of the image itself. While both maintenance tasks activated canonical working memory regions typically found for cognitive tasks, the emotion maintenance condition was uniquely associated with greater activation in MPFC regions (including the anterior cingulate). Importantly, these prefrontal regions have extensive connectivity to insula and subcortical emotion regions and are consistently implicated in the top-down control of emotion (Etkin, Egner & Kalish, 2011; see Smith & Lane, 2015 for a neural model of conscious emotional states). Given the nature of the task, it is likely that the MPFC regions were involved primarily in affective maintenance processes specifically. Future research will be needed to determine precisely which prefrontal regions are involved in maintenance versus manipulation (potentially MPFC versus dLPFC, respectively). Taken together with the behavioral and lifespan findings, these converging lines of neural evidence support the proposal that active emotion maintenance is distinct from passive emotional experience, and at least partially separable from cognitive working memory.
Translational relevance of affective working memory.
Evidence for distinct affective working memory processes has translational implications for a variety of populations. Preserved affective working memory abilities in older age may underlie the sparing of other aspects of emotional functioning. In later life, affective working memory processes could potentially circumvent declines in the deliberative cognitive processes that are critical to optimal functioning and decision making. To test this idea, Mikels et al. (2010) examined the efficacy of different decision strategies in younger and older adults. Strategies that involve holding the details of decisions in working memory improved the decision quality of younger adults but impaired that of older adults. In contrast, when participants were encouraged to hold in mind their emotional reactions to decision options and base their decisions on their feelings, the age difference disappeared; older and younger adults made decisions of equally high quality. Thus, emotion-focused strategies may be beneficial in the decision making of older individuals.
The translational importance of the affective working memory construct is further documented in clinical studies that used the above-mentioned emotion maintenance paradigms. One application is the study of emotional processes in schizophrenia. The emotions experienced in response to positive and negative emotional stimuli by people with and without schizophrenia have been shown to be highly similar (see Cohen & Minor, 2010). However, people with schizophrenia have motivational deficits and problems with goal-directed behavior (see e.g., Gard et al., 2009), which could be due to how emotional feelings are processed. This possibility was supported in a study by Gard and colleagues that found a decreased ability to maintain negative and positive emotional feelings in affective working memory in individuals with schizophrenia, an impairment that was related to motivational deficits (Gard, Cooper, Fisher, Genevsky, Mikels, & Vinogradov, 2011). Regarding affective disorders, individuals with bipolar disorder demonstrate a heightened reactivity to positive but not negative stimuli (see e.g., Gruber et al., 2011). Interestingly, people with bipolar disorder have a deficit in maintaining negative but not positive emotional feelings (Gruber, Purcell, Perna, & Mikels, 2013). Difficulties in maintaining and processing negative feelings could play a mechanistic role in enhanced processing of positive feelings, associated with the manic state. Thus, specific deficits in basic affective working memory processes could conceivably contribute to higher-level emotional symptomatology that characterizes these clinical disorders, and provide potential targets for interventions.
In sum, these converging lines of behavioral, neuroscience, and clinical evidence provide foundational support for affective working memory, an additional domain-specific subsystem that can be readily incorporated into most current models of working memory, which are characterized by domain-specificity. For goal-directed tasks, processes involved in actively maintaining feelings are separable from those involved in maintaining non-emotional representations. Additionally, active emotion maintenance requires control processes that are at least partially separable from cognitive control processes. Certainly, each facet of affective working memory warrants further investigation. The next section considers several especially fertile directions for future research.
Future Directions, and Implications of Affective Working Memory
First and foremost, as this review makes clear, more work is needed to delineate the potential overlap and separability of affective working memory and other working memory processes to characterize the architecture and interrelations of these systems. New applications of the tasks reviewed in this paper, and the development of new paradigms to investigate affective working memory and emotion-working memory interactions will help to achieve these goals, behaviorally and at the neural level. Indeed, neuroimaging evidence reveals activity in medial prefrontal loci that is unique for active emotion maintenance, along with other activity that overlaps with cognitive control areas (Waugh et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2018). Future neuroimaging research that investigates the interactions between executive and affective brain networks (Iordan, Dolcos, & Dolcos, 2018) will also be helpful for delineating the neural bases of emotion-cognition dynamics. Understanding the extent of separability versus overlap in executive control processes may also have implications for other mental abilities. Certain shared executive control functions could, for example, integrate active affective and cognitive representations in the service of reasoning, problem solving, and decision making.
Other characteristics of affective working memory that are currently unknown and warrant further research include its capacity limits and time course. We suspect that it may be difficult to hold more than two feelings active at the same time – note that the task we developed (Mikels et al., 2008) explicitly requires comparison of two activated feeling states. However, future research is needed to examine how many feelings can be actively maintained and if performance suffers as a result in increased “working memory load.” Additionally, load could also be related to arousal, such that high arousal emotions may be more demanding to maintain than low arousal emotions; such arousal effects constitute another area for future research. Another common approach in working memory research is to examine forgetting functions, that is, how the amount of information maintained diminishes over time. In the verbal domain, the number of items retained over varying retention intervals is typically used as an index of working memory capacity and forgetting. For affective working memory, though, a more appropriate index maybe the fidelity with which an emotional state is maintained over an interval—similar to current work in the visual domain (e.g., Ma, Husain, Bays, 2014; Postle, 2015). Evidence that affective working memory is more limited in capacity than other forms of working memory would provide further support for its separable status as a working memory subsystem, and may be a critical individual difference dimension relevant to emotional intelligence and wisdom, discussed below.
Theoretically, affective working memory could be usefully applied to other perspectives and domains. Recent constructivist theoretical models of the emotion process (e.g., Barrett, Mesquita, Ochsner, & Gross, 2007) have proposed that all emotional experiences involve the application of conceptual knowledge to “core affect.” Core affect can be likened to what is traditionally considered the “subjective feeling.” Within the constructivist framework, affective working memory could be the mental workspace that maintains core affect, whereas interactions with verbal and visual working memory could apply conceptual knowledge. Thus, the framework we proposed here is potentially compatible with other theoretical models of emotion.
Affective working memory may also prove to be fundamental to the field of emotion regulation. Although some recent work has examined emotion regulation in the context of instrumental goals for maintaining or increasing negative emotions (see e.g., Tamir, 2009), the core processes involved in emotion maintenance versus emotion down-regulation have received limited attention. As described above, an emotion regulation task interfered with emotion maintenance, suggesting overlap of some processes. In many respects, self-focused rumination may be the one form of emotional regulation most likely to involve both cognitive and affective working memory processes. This form of rumination involves focused attention on a current feeling, its causes, or its consequences, resulting in the active maintenance or an increase in that feeling (Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1993; Nolen-Hoeksema, Morrow, & Fredrickson, 1993). Given the central role of attention on a current feeling in the conceptualization of rumination, affective working memory would constitute the underlying mechanism. The link between affective working memory and rumination underscores the role of emotion maintenance processes in emotion dysregulation. Specifically, rumination is associated with multiple psychopathologies from depression and anxiety to binge behaviors (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008). Given the potential role of affective working memory in rumination coupled with the evidence for affective working memory differences in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia reviewed above, considering the role of affective working memory in emotion dysregulation and psychopathology may prove fruitful for future research.
Likewise the efficiency of affective working memory may be critical to emotional intelligence (EI). EI broadly involves emotion perception accuracy, adaptive use of emotions in thought and behavior, emotional understanding and appraisal, as well as the management of emotions (Mayer, Barsade, & Roberts, 2008; Salovey & Mayer, 1990). Here again, cognitive and affective working memory may enable one to work effectively with one’s feelings: Above all, the feeling state must be held in mind (beyond visual and verbal representations), which implicates affective working memory. Within this workspace for feelings, executive processes and other cognitive operations could “work with” the affective representations. Relatedly, individual differences in affective working memory could relate to differences in EI; individuals with greater ability to maintain emotions may have higher EI abilities. Accordingly, affective working memory could constitute a core mechanism underlying EI.
Finally, affective working memory may be fundamental to the wisdom construct by providing the mental workspace for affective feelings. A core facet of wisdom is the integration of emotion and social/personal knowledge to achieve optimal decisions (Baltes & Staudinger, 2000; Staudinger & Glück, 2011). Affective working memory likely plays a central role in the use of affective representations for decision making (see e.g., Mikels et al., 2011). Wisdom involves not only effective emotion regulation, but also the simultaneous consideration and integration of negative and positive feelings. An intriguing direction for future research would be to investigate the potential contributions of affective working memory to mixed emotions—that is affective working memory might provide the essential workspace for maintaining and integrating feelings of both positive and negative valence.
Integration Across the Modes of Emotion-Working Memory Interactions
Our conceptual, three-mode framework is intended to organize the prevalent lab-based approaches used to investigate emotion-working memory interactions. However, in the “real world” these three modes may themselves interact dynamically and concurrently. For example, affective working memory may interfere with cognitive working memory, or affective states may themselves interfere with affective working memory, as well as cognitive working memory.
One recent brain imaging study by Iordan, Dolcos, and Dolcos (2018) captures some of this complexity by investigating the effects of emotionally distracting autobiographical memories from one’s past on cognitive working memory performance. Importantly, Iordan and colleagues (2018) manipulated whether participants focused on the emotional aspects (i.e., the feelings) of the memories or the memory’s spatial/temporal context (i.e., where, when, and with whom the event occurred). Behaviorally, the emotion-focus condition resulted in more interference of the ongoing visual working memory task (e.g., more errors) than the context-focus condition, which demonstrates that active maintenance of an emotional feeling state can interact with visual working memory. Additionally, the context-focus condition also decreased the emotionally disruptive consequences of remembering an unpleasant personal memory, which demonstrates that attention can help to reduce (i.e., regulate) the detrimental effects of affect on visual working memory performance. As the authors point out, these findings have implications for affective disorders, such as post-traumatic stress disorder, anxiety disorders, and depression. Insofar as rumination involves the excessive active maintenance of negative affect and thoughts from past memories, working memory processes may become dysfunctional. More generally, though, approaches that cut across the modes and examine interactions between cognitive and affective systems could provide insight into the mechanisms of clinical disorders. As this study suggests, a focus on contextual versus affective features of the memories may be a useful clinical intervention. Ultimately, studies like this one highlight the dynamic and complex interactions between emotion and working memory, and reminds us that even a three-mode framework is only an initial step towards a full understand of this important domain.
Conclusions
We propose three modes by which emotion and working memory interact. The first two treat emotion and working memory as relatively separate constructs with mutual influence. The third mode focuses on feelings that can be actively maintained and worked with: affective working memory. We reviewed evidence to support this construct, described its applied utility, clinical relevance, and its potential role in other higher-order constructs including emotion regulation, emotional intelligence, and wisdom. Affective working memory and the emotional feeling states it maintains and “works with” thus constitute a core mental capacity that may be essential for myriad higher-level processes. The study of affective working memory and its fundamental role in thought and behavior holds great promise for making new in-roads along the complex frontier between emotion and cognition.
Footnotes
Working memory performance is typically measured by assessing memory accuracy for small sets of items after a brief delay. Simple span tasks are thought to rely on rote memory of the items, whereas complex span measures may require that the items be manipulated, or they may include an intervening task, and thus place more demands on executive control operations. Some reports use working memory capacity scores as the dependent measure. Calculations of capacity vary but are derived from measures of performance accuracy (e.g., Wilhelm et al, 2013; Shipstead et al., 2014). We refer to either working memory capacity or working memory performance here to align with the terminology used in the reports we summarize. Working memory capacity can also serve as an individual difference variable, an approach used to assess the effects of working memory on emotion as described in the section on Mode 2.
“Accuracy” on the emotion maintenance task has been determined in two different ways across our studies. First, we have examined concordance with normative ratings, a measure of agreement between each participant’s relative intensity assignments when comparing two successive images in the working memory task and those derived from the normative ratings (see e.g., Mikels et al., 2008). Second, we have assessed subjective accuracy based on individualized intensity ratings obtained in a separate phase of the study, where each participant provides intensity judgments for each image used in the working memory task. These individualized (rather than normative) intensity assignments are then used to assess performance accuracy or agreement with responses in the working memory phase of the study (see e.g., Broome, Gard, & Mikels, 2012; Mikels et al., 2005). Both approaches yield comparable results and the same procedures were also used to assess performance on the brightness maintenance task.
The maintenance condition in this study required viewing two sequential images separated by a delay with instructions to maintain the feeling from the first image so as to compare it with the feeling from the second image. The comparison condition also involved viewing two sequential images separated by a delay but then a rating was to be provided only for the second image. As such, the comparison condition did not require maintenance of the feelings from the first image.
References
- Allen RJ, Schaefer A, & Falcon T (2014). Recollecting positive and negative autobiographical memories disrupts working memory. Acta Psychologica, 151, 237–243. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashcraft MH, & Kirk EP (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(2), 224–237. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley AD (1986). Working memory. Oxford, England UK: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A (2013). Working memory and emotion: Ruminations on a theory of depression. Review of General Psychology, 17(1), 20–27. doi: 10.1037/a0030029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley AD, & Hitch G (1974). Working memory In Bower G (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baltes PB, & Staudinger UM (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist, 55(1), 122–136. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.1.122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes LL, Nelson JK, & Reuter-Lorenz PA (2001). Object-based attention and object working memory: Overlapping processes revealed by selective interference effects in humans. Progress in Brain Research, 134, 471–481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett LF, Mesquita B, Ochsner KN, & Gross JJ (2007). The experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 373–403. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett LF, & Russell JA (1999). The structure of current affect: Controversies and emerging consensus. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(1), 10–14. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brandimonte MA, Hitch GJ, & Bishop DV (1992). Verbal recoding of visual stimuli impairs mental image transformations. Memory & Cognition, 20(4), 449–455. doi: 10.3758/BF03210929 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broome R, Gard DE, & Mikels JA (2012). Test–retest reliability of an emotion maintenance task. Cognition & Emotion, 26(4), 737–747. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.613916 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter SM, Peters E, Västfjäll D, & Isen AM (2013). Positive feelings facilitate working memory and complex decision making among older adults. Cognition & Emotion, 27(1), 184–192. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.698251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carstensen LL, & Mikels JA (2005). At the intersection of emotion and cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 117–121. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00348.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Carstensen LL, Mikels JA, & Mather M (2006). Aging and the intersection of cognition, motivation, and emotion. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, 343–362. doi: 10.1016/b978-012101264-9/50018-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Christopher G, & MacDonald J (2005). The impact of clinical depression on working memory. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10(5), 379–399. doi: 10.1080/13546800444000128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christophel TB, Klink PC, Spitzer B, Roelfsema PR, & Haynes J-D (2017). The distributed nature of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(2), 111–124. https://doi-org.ezproxy.depaul.edu/10.1016/j.tics.2016.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke R, & Johnstone T (2013). Prefrontal inhibition of threat processing reduces working memory interference. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen AS, Minor KS (2010). Emotional experience in patients with schizophrenia revisited: meta-analysis of laboratory studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36, 143–150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87–185. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01003922 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N (2008). What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? Progress in Brain Research, 169, 323–338. 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00020-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N (2017). Working memory, the information you are now thinking of In Wixted J (ed.), Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference, 2nd edition Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N, Li D, Moffitt A, Becker TM, Martin EA, Saults JS, & Christ SE (2011). A neural region of abstract working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2852–2863. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2011.21625 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darke S (1988). Anxiety and working memory capacity. Cognition & Emotion, 2(2), 145–154. doi: 10.1080/02699938808408071 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson RJ, & Irwin WM (1999). The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(1), 11–21. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01265-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFraine WC (2016). Differential effects of cognitive load on emotion: Emotion maintenance versus passive experience. Emotion, 16(4), 459–467. doi: 10.1037/emo0000140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Esposito M, Aguirre GK, Zarahn E, Ballard D, Shin RK, & Lease J (1998). Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. Cognitive Brain Research, 7(1), 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dolcos F, Kragel P, Wang L, & Mccarthy G (2006). Role of the inferior frontal cortex in coping with distracting emotions. NeuroReport, 17(15), 1591–1594. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000236860.24081.be [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dolcos F, & McCarthy G (2006). Brain systems mediating cognitive interference by emotional distraction. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(7), 2072–2079. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5042-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P (1994). Moods, emotions and traits In Ekman P & Davidson RJ (Eds.), The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions (pp. 56–58). Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Elliman NA, Green MW, Rogers PJ, & Finch GM (1997). Processing-efficiency theory and the working-memory system: Impairments associated with sub-clinical anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 31–35. 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00016-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ellsworth PC (2013). Appraisal theory: Old and new questions. Emotion Review, 5(2), 125–131. doi: 10.1177/1754073912463617 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ellsworth PC, & Smith CA (1988). Shades of joy: Patterns of appraisal differentiating pleasant emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 2(4), 301–331. [Google Scholar]
- Engle R, Kane MJ, & Tuholski SW (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex In Miyake A & Shah P (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 102–134). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Etkin A, Egner T, & Kalisch R (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(2), 85–93. https://doiorg.ezproxy.depaul.edu/10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck MW, & Derakshan N (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(7), 955–960. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, & Calvo MG (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–353. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fairfield B, Mammarella N, Di Domenico A, & Palumbo R (2015). Running with emotion: When affective content hampers working memory performance. International Journal of Psychology, 50(2), 161–164. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Figueira JSB, Oliveira L, Pereira MG, Pacheco LB, Lobo I, Motta-Ribeiro GC, & David IA (2017). An unpleasant emotional state reduces working memory capacity: Electrophysiological evidence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(6), 984–992. https://doi-org.ezproxy.depaul.edu/10.1093/scan/nsx030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Figueira JSB, Pacheco LB, Lobo I, Volchan E, Pereira MG, de Oliveira L, & David IA (2018) “Keep that in mind!” The role of positive affect in working memory for maintaining goal-relevant information. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1228. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01228 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fougnie D, Zughni S, Godwin D, & Marois R (2015). Working memory storage is intrinsically domain specific. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(1), 30–47. https://doi-org.ezproxy.depaul.edu/10.1037/a0038211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300–319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frijda NH (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Frijda NH (1993). Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions In Lewis M, Haviland JM, Lewis M, Haviland JM (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (pp. 381–403). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- García-Pacios J, Del Río D, Villalobos D, Ruiz-Vargas JM, & Maestú F (2015). Emotional interference-based forgetting in short-term memory: Cognitive inhibition of pleasant but not unpleasant biologically relevant distractors. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gard DE, Cooper S, Fisher M, Genevsky A, Mikels JA, & Vinogradov S (2011). Evidence for an emotion maintenance deficit in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 187, 24–29. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.12.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gard DE, Fisher M, Garrett C, Genevsky A, Vinogradov S, 2009. Motivation and its relationship to neurocognition, social cognition, and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 115, 74–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gooding DC, & Tallent KA (2003). Spatial, object, and affective working memory in social anhedonia: An exploratory study. Schizophrenia Research, 63(3), 247–260. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00326-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gotlib IH, & Joormann J (2010). Cognition and depression: Current status and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 285–312. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gotoh F, Kikuchi T, & Olofsson U (2010). A facilitative effect of negative affective valence on working memory. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51(3), 185–191. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00766.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray JR (2001). Emotional modulation of cognitive control: Approach-withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 436–452. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.436 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray JR, Braver TS, & Raichle ME (2002). Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(6), 4115–4120. https://doi-org.ezproxy.depaul.edu/10.1073/pnas.062381899 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271–299. [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 1–26. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gruber J (2011a). When feeling good can be bad: Positive emotion persistence (PEP) in bipolar disorder. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 217–221. doi: 10.1177/0963721411414632 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gruber J, Purcell AL, Perna MJ, & Mikels JA (2013). Letting go of the bad: Deficit in maintaining negative, but not positive, emotion in bipolar disorder. Emotion, 13(1), 168–175. doi: 10.1037/a0029381 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hur J, Iordan AD, Dolcos F, & Berenbaum W (2017). Emotional influences on perception and working memory. Cognition & Emotion, 31(6), 1294–1302. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1213703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikeda M, Iwanaga M, & Seiwa H (1996). Test anxiety and working memory system. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82(3c), 1223–1231. doi: 10.2466/pms.1996.82.3c.1223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iordan AD, Dolcos S, & Dolcos F (2013). Neural signatures of the response to emotional distraction: A review of evidence from brain imaging investigations. Frontiers In Human Neuroscience, 7. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iordan AD, Dolcos S, & Dolcos F (2018, in press). Brain activity and network interactions in the impact of internal emotional distraction. Cerebral Cortex. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izard CE (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 260–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iordan AD, & Dolcos F (2017). Brain activity and network interactions linked to valence-related differences in the impact of emotional distraction. Cerebral Cortex, 27(1), 731–749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson MC, Linden DE, & Raymond JE (2014). Angry expressions strengthen the encoding and maintenance of face identity representations in visual working memory. Cognition and Emotion, 28(2), 278–297. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.816655 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson MC, Chia-Yun W, Linden DJ, and Raymond JE (2009). Enhanced visual short- term memory for angry faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 363–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jonides J (1995). Working memory and thinking In Smith EE, Osherson DN, Smith EE, Osherson DN (Eds.), Thinking: An invitation to cognitive science, Vol. 3, 2nd ed (pp. 215–265). Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jonides J, Lewis RL, Nee DE, Lustig CA, Berman MG, & Moore KS (2008). The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 59,193–224. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093615 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kellermann TS, Sternkopf MA, Schneider F, Habel U, Turetsky BI, Zilles K, & Eickhoff SB (2012). Modulating the processing of emotional stimuli by cognitive demand. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 263–273. 10.1093/scan/nsq104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kensinger EA, & Corkin S (2003). Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and long-term memory. Emotion, 3(4), 378–393. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.4.378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kron A, Schul Y, Cohen A, & Hassin RR (2010). Feelings don’t come easy: Studies on the effortful nature of feelings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 520–534. doi: 10.1037/a0020008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavie N, Hirst A, Fockert JW, & Viding E (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 339–354. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lazarus R (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Levens SM, & Phelps EA (2008). Emotion processing effects on interference resolution in working memory. Emotion, 8, 267–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levenson RW (1994). Human emotion: A functional view In Ekman P & Davidson RJ (Eds.), The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions (pp. 123–126). Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lewis-Peacock JA, Drysdale AT, Oberauer K, & Postle BR (2012). Neural evidence for a distinction between short-term memory and the focus of attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(1), 61–79. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li D, Christ SE, & Cowan N (2014). Domain-general and domain-specific functional networks in working memory. Neuroimage, 102(Part 2), 646–656. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logie RH, Gilhooly KJ, & Wynn V (1994). Counting on working memory in arithmetic problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 22(4), 395–410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma WJ, Husain M, & Bays PM (2014). Changing concepts of working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 347–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Majerus S, Cowan N, Péters F, Van Calster L, Phillips C, & Schrouff J (2016). Cross-modal decoding of neural patterns associated with working memory: Evidence for attention-based accounts of working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 26(1), 166–179. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin EA, & Kerns JG (2011). The influence of positive mood on different aspects of cognitive control. Cognition & Emotion, 25(2), 265–279. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.491652 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mather M, Mitchell KJ, Raye CL, Novak DL, Greene EJ, & Johnson MK (2006). Emotional arousal can impair feature binding in working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(4), 614–625. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.614 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer JD, Barsade SG, & Roberts RD (2008). Human abilities: Emotional intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 507–536. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093646 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikels JA, Larkin GR, Reuter-Lorenz PA, & Carstensen LL (2005). Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: Changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. Psychology and Aging, 20(4), 542–553. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikels JA, Löckenhoff CE, Maglio SJ, Carstensen LL, Goldstein MK, & Garber A (2010). Following your heart or your head: Focusing on emotions versus information differentially influences the decisions of younger and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16(1), 87–95. doi: 10.1037/a0018500 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikels JA, Maglio SJ, Reed AE, & Kaplowitz LJ (2011). Should I go with my gut? Investigating the benefits of emotion-focused decision making. Emotion, 11(4), 743–753. doi: 10.1037/a0023986 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikels JA, Reed AE, Hardy LN, & Löckenhoff CE (2014). Positive emotions across the adult life span In Tugade MM, Shiota MN, Kirby LD, Tugade MM, Shiota MN, Kirby LD (Eds.), Handbook of positive emotions (pp. 256–271). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Mikels JA, & Reuter-Lorenz PA (2013). Emotion and working memory In Pashler H (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the Mind (pp. 308–310). Los Angeles: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Mikels JA, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Beyer JA, & Fredrickson BL (2008). Emotion and working memory: Evidence for domain-specific processes for affective maintenance. Emotion, 8(2), 256–266. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.256 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller GA (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97. doi: 10.1037/h0043158 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake A, & Shah P (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control, (pp. 1–27). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Miyake A, Friedman NP, Rettinger DA, Shah P, & Hegarty M (2001). How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 621–640. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.621 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moors A, Ellsworth PC, Scherer KR, & Frijda NH (2013). Appraisal theories of emotion: State of the art and future development. Emotion Review, 5(2), 119–124. doi: 10.1177/1754073912468165 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moran TP (2016). Anxiety and working memory capacity: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 142(8), 831–864. doi: 10.1037/bul0000051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray DJ (1967). The role of speech responses in short-term memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 21(3), 263–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray DJ (1968). Articulation and acoustic confusability in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78(4), 679–684. [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, & Lyubomirsky S (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S, & Morrow J (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition & Emotion, 7(6), 561–570. [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J, & Fredrickson BL (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(1), 20–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer K, Farrell S, Jarrold C, & Lewandowsky S (2016). What limits working memory capacity? Psychological Bulletin, 142(7), 758–799. doi: 10.1037/bul0000046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer K, Lewandowsky S, Awh E, Brown GDA, Conway A, Cowan N, Donkin C, Farrell S,. Hitch GJ, Hurlstone M, Ma WJ, Morey, Nee DE, Schweppe J, Vergauwe E, & Ward G (2018). Benchmarks for models of working memory. Psychological Bulletin, 144(9), 885–958. doi: 10.1037/bul0000153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ochsner KN, Bunge SA, Gross JJ, & Gabrieli JD (2002). Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1215–1229. doi: 10.1162/089892902760807212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park DC, Lautenschlager G, Hedden T, Davidson NS, Smith AD, & Smith PK (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299–320. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park DC, & Reuter-Lorenz P (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pe ML, Koval P, & Kuppens P (2013). Executive well-being: Updating of positive stimuli in working memory is associated with subjective well-being. Cognition, 126(2), 335–340. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pe ML, Raes F, Koval P, Brans K, Verduyn P, & Kuppens P (2013). Interference resolution moderates the impact of rumination and reappraisal on affective experiences in daily life. Cognition and Emotion, 27(3), 492–501. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.719489 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pe ML, Raes F, & Kuppens P (2013). The cognitive building blocks of emotion regulation: Ability to update working memory moderates the efficacy of rumination and reappraisal on emotion. PLoS ONE, 8(7). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Postle BR (2015). Activation and information in working memory research In Addis DR, Barense M, Duarte A, Addis DR, Barense M, Duarte A (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the cognitive neuroscience of memory (pp. 21–43). Wiley-Blackwell. doi: 10.1002/9781118332634.ch2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Postle BR, (2015). The cognitive neuroscience of visual short-term memory. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 40–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Postle BR (2016). How does the brain keep information ‘in mind’?. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(3), 151–156. doi: 10.1177/0963721416643063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reisenzein R (1994). Pleasure-arousal theory and the intensity of emotions. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 67(3), 525–539. [Google Scholar]
- Richardson JT, & Baddeley AD (1975). The effect of articulatory suppression in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 14(6), 623–629. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg EL (1998). Levels of analysis and the organization of affect. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 247–270. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.247 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Salovey P & Mayer JD (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185–211. [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer SM, Jackson DC, Davidson RJ, Aguirre GK, Kimberg D, & Thompson-Schill SL (2002). Modulation of amygdalar activity by the conscious regulation of negative emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(6), 913–921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scherer KR (2000). Psychological models of emotion In Borod JC (Ed.), The Neuropsychology of Emotion (pp. 137–162). Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Schmader T, Johns M, & Forbes C (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115, 336–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmeichel BJ, & Demaree WA (2010). Working memory capacity and spontaneous emotion regulation: High capacity predicts self-enhancement in response to negative feedback. Emotion, 10(5), 739–744. doi: 10.1037/a0019355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmeichel BJ, & Zell A (2007). Trait self-control predicts performance on behavioral tests of self-control. Journal of Personality, 75(4), 743–756. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00455.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmeichel BJ, Volokhov RN, & Demaree WA (2008). Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 1526–1540. doi: 10.1037/a0013345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shackman AJ, Sarinopoulos I, Maxwell JS, Pizzagalli DA, Lavric A, & Davidson RJ (2006). Anxiety selectively disrupts visuospatial working memory. Emotion, 6(1), 40–61. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shipstead Z, Lindsey DRB, Marshall RL, & Engle RW (2014). The mechanisms of working memory capacity: Primary memory, secondary memory, and attention control. Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 116–141. https://doi-org.ezproxy.depaul.edu/10.1016/j.jml.2014.01.004 [Google Scholar]
- Smith CA, & Ellsworth PC (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 48(4), 813–838. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith CA & Kirby LD (2000). Consequences require antecedents: Toward a process model of emotion elicitation In Forgas J (Ed.). Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 83–106). New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Smith EE, & Jonides J (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33(1), 5–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith EE, & Jonides J (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 1657–1661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith R, Baxter L, Thayer J & Lane R (2016). Disentangling introspective and exteroceptive attentional control from emotional appraisal in depression using fMRI: A preliminary study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 248, 39–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith R, Fass H & Lane R (2014). Role of medial prefrontal cortex in representing one’s own subjective emotional responses: A preliminary study. Conscious. Cogn 29, 117–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith R & Lane RD (2015). The neural basis of one’s own conscious and unconscious emotional states. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 57, 1–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith R, Lane RD, Alkozei A, Bao J, Smith C, Sanova A, Nettles M, & Killgore W (2018). The role of medial prefrontal cortex in the working memory maintenance of one’s own emotional responses. Scientific Reports, 8 10.1038/s41598-018-21896-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staudinger UM, & Glück J (2011). Psychological wisdom research: Commonalities and differences in a growing field. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 215–241. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131659 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storbeck J, & Maswood R (2016). Happiness increases verbal and spatial working memory capacity where sadness does not: Emotion, working memory and executive control. Cognition & Emotion, 30(5), 925–938. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1034091 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson HL (2017). Verbal and visual-spatial working memory: What develops over a life span? Developmental Psychology, 53(5), 971–995. https://doiorg.ezproxy.depaul.edu/10.1037/dev0000291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamir M (2009). What do people want to feel and why?: Pleasure and utility in emotion regulation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 101–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01617.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Truong L, & Yang L (2014). Friend of foe? Decoding the facilitative and disruptive effects of emotion on working memory in younger and older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Dillen LF, Heslenfeld DJ, & Koole SL (2009). Tuning down the emotional brain: An fMRI study of the effects of cognitive load on the processing of affective images. NeuroImage, 45(4), 1212–1219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Dillen LF, & Koole SL (2007). Clearing the mind: A working memory model of distraction from negative mood. Emotion, 7, 715–723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verhaeghen P, Marcoen A, & Goossens L (1993). Facts and fiction about memory aging: A quantitative integration of research findings. Journal of Gerontology, 48(4). doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.4.p157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waechter S, Moscovitch DA, Vidovic V, Bielak T, Rowa K, & McCabe RE (2018). Working memory capacity in social anxiety disorder: Revisiting prior conclusions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(3), 276–281. https://doiorg.ezproxy.depaul.edu/10.1037/abn0000341.supp [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waugh CE, Lemus MG, & Gotlib I (2014). The role of the medial frontal cortex in the maintenance of emotional states. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(12), 2001–2009. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welford AT (1952). The ‘psychological refractory period’ and the timing of high-speed performance-A review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology. General Section, 43(1), 2–19. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1952.tb00322.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wilhelm O, Hildebrandt A and Oberauer K (2013) What is working memory capacity, and how can we measure it? Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 433. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00433 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang WM, Yang S, & Isen AM (2013). Positive affect improves working memory: Implications for controlled cognitive processing. Cognition & Emotion, 27(3), 474–482. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.713325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]