Abstract
Aim
The current study had two aims: (1) to investigate whether single-item measures of subjective evaluation of neighborhood (i.e., perceived neighborhood safety and quality) predict long-term risk of mortality and (2) to test whether these associations depend on race and gender.
Methods
The data came from the Americans’ Changing Lives Study (ACL), 1986–2011, a nationally representative longitudinal cohort of 3361 Black and White adults in the USA. The main predictors of interest were perceived neighborhood safety and perceived neighborhood quality, as measured in 1986 using single items and treated as dichotomous variables. Mortality due to all internal and external causes was the main outcome. Confounders included baseline age, socioeconomic status (education, income), health behaviors (smoking, drinking, and exercise), and health (chronic medical conditions, self-rated health, and depressive symptoms). Race and gender were focal effect modifiers. Cox proportional hazard models were ran in the pooled sample and stratified by race and gender.
Results
In the pooled sample, low perceived neighborhood safety and quality predicted increased risk of mortality due to all causes as well as internal causes, net of all covariates. Significant interaction was found between race and perceived neighborhood safety on all-cause mortality, indicating a stronger association for Whites compared to Blacks. Race did not interact with perceived neighborhood quality on mortality. Gender also did not interact with perceived neighborhood safety or quality on mortality. Perceived neighborhood safety and quality were not associated with mortality due to external causes.
Conclusion
Findings suggest that single items are appropriate for the measurement of perceived neighborhood safety and quality. Our results also suggest that perceived neighborhood safety better predicts increased risk of mortality over the course of 25 years among Whites than Blacks.
Keywords: neighborhood, safety, mortality, life course, race, ethnic groups
Introduction
According to the lifecourse epidemiology approach, health outcomes are under the influence of exposures that occur several decades earlier in life [1]. According to this view, late health outcomes such as mortality are shaped by processes experienced decades earlier in life [2]. To test such hypotheses, long-term epidemiological studies are needed; their results may suggest previously unrecognized opportunities for increasing life expectancy through interventions earlier in life [3].
Using a lifecourse epidemiology perspective [4, 5, 6] and building upon a literature which suggests subjective evaluation of neighborhood quality and safety have long-term health consequences several decades later in life [7, 8], the current study investigates whether or not perceived neighborhood safety and quality during mid adulthood (average age = 42) predict subsequent risk of mortality. To generate results that are representative to the US population, we used data from the Americans’ Changing Lives (ACL) study, a 25-year longitudinal cohort with a nationally representative sample of US adults [9, 10]. Data from the ACL study has previously shown that baseline psychosocial risk factors better predict risk of mortality for Whites than Blacks [11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16], a finding which has been replicated across populations and outcomes [17, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22]. It is, however, unknown whether or not race also alters the effects of neighborhood quality and safety on mortality or not.
Previous research has shown that subjective evaluation of the social environment (e.g., perceived neighborhood safety and quality) has major implications for a wide range of physical and mental health outcomes, net of demographics, socioeconomics, lifestyle factors, and baseline health [23]. Individuals who state that the social aspects of their neighborhood need improvement are also more likely to report poor health [23]. Not only objective measures of neighborhood quality [24, 25], but also subjective perceptions about neighborhood safety and quality have major implications for health outcomes [26, 27, 28].
Most research on the link between place and health has focused on physical rather than social aspects of the environment. In addition, most of this research has used a cross-sectional design, and when it comes to physical health outcomes, very few studies have focused on the risk of mortality over a long period of time. Thus, there is a need to study the role of perceptions about neighborhood safety and quality on risk of mortality in nationally representative cohorts over extended time periods.
The effect of a deprived neighborhood on health may not, however, be equal for all social groups [29]. It has been shown that race [30] and gender [7, 8, 31] alter salience of neighborhood on health outcomes. For instance, Stafford, Marmot, and others found that trust, integration into wider society, and physical quality of the residential environment have systematically larger effects on women than men, suggesting that women may be more vulnerable to the influence of the environment on health outcomes [32].
The current longitudinal study investigates (1) whether single items that measure perceiving one’s neighborhood as unsafe and low quality predict subsequent risk of all-cause mortality over a 25-year period in a nationally representative sample, and (2) whether race and gender alter these associations.
Methods
Design and Setting
Data came from the Americans’ Changing Lives (ACL) Study, conducted from 1986 to 2011. The ACL study is a nationally representative study of US adults 25 years and older. More information on the sampling and data collection has been published elsewhere [9, 10].
Ethics
The University of Michigan Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the study protocol. Informed consent was received from all participants. All procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975.
Sampling and Participants
The ACL study used a stratified multistage probability sample of non-institutionalized US adults and oversampled older adults (age > 60) and Blacks. The study enrolled 3617 adults who were 25 years or older and were living in the continental USA in 1986 (representing 70 % of sampled households and 68 % of sample individuals at baseline).
Analytical Sample
In the current study, analysis is limited to non-Hispanic Whites and non-Hispanic Blacks. The analytic N is 3361, which is composed of 2205 Whites and 1156 Blacks.
Process
Data at baseline were collected via face to face interviews in 1986. Mortality data were collected from death certificates, informants, and the National Death Index (NDI), which were used to assess date of death (discussed below).
Measures
Perceived Neighborhood Safety and Quality
Baseline perceived neighborhood safety and quality were measured in 1986 as single items. In line with the literature [33, 34], we treated these variables as dichotomous variables. The item for perceived neighborhood safety was “How true is the following statement about your neighborhood: This is a neighborhood where I feel safe from personal attacks. Is this very true, mostly true, somewhat true, or not true at all?” Items included (1) very true, (2) mostly true, (3) somewhat true, or (4) not true at all. We created a dichotomous variable, unsafe (not true at all) versus all other responses (very true, mostly true, or somewhat true). The item for perceived neighborhood quality reads: “How satisfied are you with your neighborhood? Are you completely satisfied, very satisfied, somewhat satisfied, not very satisfied, or not at all satisfied?” Item responses included (1) completely satisfied, (2) very satisfied, (3) somewhat satisfied, (4) not very satisfied, or (5) not at all satisfied. We created a dichotomous variable, not satisfied (not very satisfied, not at all satisfied) versus satisfied (completely satisfied, very satisfied, or somewhat satisfied). Single-item measures have been commonly used for the measurement of subjective evaluation of neighborhood characteristics [8, 35, 36]. Thus, hazard ratios >1 associated with our independent variables were indicative of associations between poor subjective evaluation of neighborhood quality and safety and increased risk of mortality.
Mortality
Mortality data for all deaths from mid-1986 through 2011 were obtained through the National Death Index (NDI), death certificates, as well as informants. In most cases, time and cause of death were verified with death certificates. Mortality data was evaluated for all participants regardless of their follow-up status. This means that mortality was monitored for all respondents who were in the baseline sample, even if they failed to respond to a follow-up interview. With a handful of exceptions, the death status of all the participants was determined. These handfuls of cases where death could not be verified with death certificates were reviewed carefully, and actual death was certain in all cases. Only in these few cases was the date of death ascertained from the informants or the NDI report, rather than the death certificate [12, 13].
Internal and External Causes of Death
Based on specific causes of death, we also determined internal and external causes of death. Internal causes of death were defined as death due to natural causes, as primarily attributed to an illness or an internal malfunction of a body organ not directly influenced by external forces. Internal causes of death included death due to cardiovascular conditions, blood, metabolic disorders such as diabetes, kidney disease, liver disease, respiratory conditions, gastroenterological disease, infectious causes, and cancer [37]. External causes of death (unnatural death) relate to cases where the underlying cause of death is determined to be one of a group of causes external to the body (for example, suicide, transport accidents, falls, poisoning, etc.). External causes of death included intentional and unintentional injuries. Main examples of external causes of death included death secondary to suicides, homicides, accidents, and other external causes [38].
Demographic Factors
Demographic characteristics in this study included age (a continuous variable) and gender (male versus female).
Socioeconomic Factors
Socioeconomic factors in this study included educational attainment (years of schooling) and income of the respondent (and spouse if present), both operationalized as continuous variables, and both collected in 1986. Education was measured in 11 categories.
Exercise
A physical activity index was derived from answers to survey questions regarding engagement in exercise, active sports, gardening/yard work, household chores, and walking. Higher scores on this index were indicative of more exercise frequency [39, 40].
Smoking
Information was collected on self-reported history of smoking. We used a dichotomous variable (current smoker = 1, never or ex-smoker = 0).
Drinking
A measure was used concerning alcohol use, that is, whether or not the respondent currently drinks (1 = current drinker and 0 = non-drinker) [41].
Obesity
Obesity was defined based on the body mass index (BMI) of larger than 30 kg/m2. The BMI level was calculated based on self-reported weights and heights. Weight and height were originally collected in pounds (1 lb = 0.453 kg) and feet (1 ft = 0.3048 m)/inches (1 in. = 0.0254 m), respectively. BMI calculated based on self-reported weight and height is known to be closely correlated with BMI based on direct measures of height and weight [42].
Number of Chronic Medical Conditions
Baseline Chronic Medical Conditions (CMC) were measured using self-reports on whether a health care provider has ever told the respondents that they had each of seven focal chronic medical conditions (hypertension, diabetes, chronic lung disease, heart disease, stroke, cancer, and arthritis). Responses were summed, resulting in a score ranging from 0 to 7 [10].
Self-Rated Health (SRH)
Respondents were asked to classify their self-rated health as excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor. The literature has treated SRH as dichotomous variable, a continuous measure, or an ordinal variable [43, 44, 45, 46]. We collapsed to two categories (fair/poor vs. excellent/very good/good), a cut-off point that is common in the literature [18].
Depressive Symptoms
The severity of depressive symptoms was measured with an 11-item version of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale (CES-D) [47]. CES-D items measure the extent to which respondents felt depressed, happy, lonely, sad, that everything was an effort, that their sleep was restless, that people were unfriendly, that they did not feel like eating, that people dislike them, that they could not get going, and that they enjoyed life. Positively worded items were reverse-coded. This abbreviated CES-D scale has shown acceptable reliability and a similar factor structure compared to the original version [48, 49, 50]. Possible item responses ranged from never or hardly ever (score 1) to most of the time (score 3), resulting in a continuous measure of depressive symptoms with a potential range from 11 to 33. Higher scores indicated a greater severity of depressive symptoms.
Moderators
In this study, race and gender were the focal moderators. Gender was a dichotomous variable (with male respondents as the referent category). Self-identified race was defined as Black versus White (with White respondents as the referent category). The ACL study collected data on participant’s race at baseline in 1986 with multiple survey items. Participants were asked about Hispanic origin and gave an open-ended response to the question, “In addition to being American, what do you think of as your ethnic background or origins?” Participants were then asked: “Are you White, Black, American Indian, Asian, or another race?” and were allowed to answer with multiple categories. Individuals who responded with more than one non-white group were asked to identify which “best described” their race. The ACL study also assessed the state or foreign country of birth. Together, these items were used to construct race categories of “Non-Hispanic White,” “Non-Hispanic Black,” “Non-Hispanic Native American,” “Non-Hispanic Asian,” and “Hispanic.” As our second aim was to compare Black and Whites for the effect of our predictor of interest on mortality, we decided not to include Hispanics in this study. Thus, our sample is composed of Non-Hispanic Whites and Non-Hispanic Blacks [11, 12, 13, 17, 18].
Statistical Analysis
As the ACL study utilized a complex sample design, Stata version 13.0 (Stata Corp., College Station, TX, USA) was used for data analysis. Using Taylor series linearization, standard errors were re-estimated based on the survey design, using sampling and non-response weights (wave 1 weights). Several proportional hazard models were estimated in the pooled sample, specific to race groups. First, models were ran in the pooled sample of Blacks and White to evaluate the effects of race and perceived neighborhood safety and quality on mortality outcomes, net of all covariates. Then, interaction terms were entered between race and gender with perceived neighborhood safety and quality. Finally, the model ran was stratified by race and gender. Models were replicated for mortality due to all causes. Hazard ratios, standard errors, 95 % confidence intervals (CI), and p values were reported. We considered p values less than 0.05 as significant.
Proportional hazard models require two outcomes: (1) a binary outcome (event) and (2) the time that the outcome occurred (time to event). Mortality was coded as 1 if death happened due to any cause between 1986 and 2011, and 0 otherwise. Time to death was calculated in months from baseline to month of death or the end of the year 2011. For Schoenfeld residual analysis, -estat phtest- in Stata was used to evaluate the proportional hazard assumptions for the proportional hazard models.
The main predictors of interest were baseline perceived neighborhood safety and quality measured in 1986, while the main outcome was mortality (due to all causes, internal or external) over a 25-year follow-up. Covariates included baseline demographic factors (age), socioeconomic characteristics (education and income), health behaviors (smoking, drinking, and exercise), and health status (depressive symptoms, SRH, and CMC), all measured in 1986. Race and gender were the main moderators.
Results
The study followed 3361 Blacks and Whites, ages 25 and older, for 25 years. Table 1 presents detailed descriptive statistics on study variables in the pooled sample, based on race. While Whites and Blacks did not differ in age and gender, they differed in education, income, BMI, SRH, depressive symptoms, and mortality. Compared to Whites, Blacks had significantly lower socioeconomic status (education and income), higher depressive symptoms, and poorer health (CMC, BMI, and SRH) at baseline. Blacks had worse perceived neighborhood quality and safety as well.
Table 1.
Baseline characteristics for the analytic sample, stratified by race and overall
Whites | Blacks | All | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SE) | 95%CI | Mean (SE) | 95%CI | Mean (SE) | 95%CI | |
Age | 47.96 (0.60) | 46.75–49.17 | 46.33 (0.72) | 44.89–47.78 | 47.77 (0.53) | 46.69–48.84 |
Education* | 12.69 (0.11) | 12.48–12.90 | 11.37 (0.23) | 10.90–11.84 | 12.53 (0.10) | 12.34–12.73 |
Income* | 5.57 (0.10) | 5.36–5.77 | 4.25 (0.18) | 3.88–4.62 | 5.41 (0.09) | 5.22–5.60 |
Chronic medical conditions* | 0.78 (0.03) | 0.71–0.84 | 0.91 (0.05) | 0.81–1.02 | 0.79 (0.03) | 0.74–0.85 |
Body mass index* | 25.34 (0.12) | 25.11–25.58 | 26.94 (0.20) | 26.53–27.34 | 25.54 (0.11) | 25.32–25.75 |
Depressive symptoms | ||||||
% (SE) | 95%CI | % (SE) | 95%CI | % (SE) | 95%CI | |
Gender | ||||||
Male | 47.82 (0.01) | 45.12–50.52 | 43.18 (0.02) | 38.79–47.69 | 47.26 (0.01) | 44.86–49.68 |
Female | 52.18 (0.01) | 49.48–54.88 | 56.82 (0.02) | 52.31–61.21 | 52.74 (0.01) | 50.32–55.14 |
Self-rate health* | ||||||
Good-excellent | 85.97 (0.01) | 84.15–87.60 | 78.38 (0.02) | 74.68–81.68 | 85.06 (0.01) | 83.33–86.64 |
Poor-fair | 14.03 (0.01) | 12.40–15.85 | 21.62 (0.02) | 18.32–25.32 | 14.94 (0.01) | 13.36–16.67 |
Neighborhood Safety* | ||||||
High | 97.34 (0.01) | 96.35–98.06 | 88.81 (0.01) | 85.52–91.43 | 96.32 (0.01) | 95.31–97.12 |
Low | 2.66 (0.01) | 1.94–3.65 | 11.19 (0.01) | 8.57–14.48 | 3.68 (0.01) | 2.88–4.69 |
Neighborhood Quality* | ||||||
High | 90.53 (0.01) | 89.01–91.86 | 84.04 (0.01) | 80.30–87.18 | 89.75 (0.01) | 88.43–90.94 |
Low | 9.47 (0.01) | 8.14–10.99 | 15.96 (0.01) | 12.82–19.70 | 10.25 (0.01) | 9.06–11.57 |
CES-D Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale
p < 0.05 for all comparisons between Blacks and Whites
Overall, 1737 deceased Black or White participants were detected, while 1624 Black or White individuals survived. Table 2 presents the results of three proportional hazard models in the pooled sample, with perceived neighborhood safety at baseline as the main predictor. Model 1 did not include any interaction term and tested the effect of perceived neighborhood safety at baseline on mortality, net of all covariates. Model 2 tested the interaction term between baseline perceived neighborhood safety and race. Model 3 included gender by baseline perceived neighborhood safety interaction in the model. In Model 1, poor perceived neighborhood safety at baseline was associated with an increased risk of all-cause mortality (HR = 1.47, 95%CI = 1.05–2.04) and also internal causes of mortality (HR = 1.49, 95%CI = 1.06–2.11), net of all covariates. Model 2 showed an interaction between race and perceived neighborhood safety on all-cause (HR = 0.55, 95%CI = 0.31–0.98) as well as internal causes of mortality (HR = 0.53, 95%CI = 0.29–0.98), suggesting stronger effects of baseline perceived neighborhood safety on long-term risk of mortality due to all causes as well as internal causes among Whites compared to Blacks. In Model 3, gender did not interact with perceived neighborhood safety at baseline on mortality due to all causes or internal causes (p > 0.05).
Table 2.
Association between neighborhood safety and all-cause mortality in the pooled sample
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All causes | Internal causes | All causes | Internal causes | All causes | Internal causes | |||||||
HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | |
Blacks | 1.03 | 0.89–1.19 | 0.96 | 0.82–1.12 | 1.09 | 0.92–1.28 | 1.02 | 0.86–1.21 | 1.03 | 0.89–1.19 | 0.95 | 0.82–1.11 |
Women | 0.52*** | 0.45–0.61 | 0.51*** | 0.43–0.59 | 0.52*** | 0.45–0.61 | 0.50*** | 0.43–0.59 | 0.53*** | 0.45–0.61 | 0.51*** | 0.43–0.60 |
Age | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.09 | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.09 | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.09 | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 |
Education (years) | 0.99 | 0.97–1.01 | 0.99 | 0.96–1.02 | 0.99 | 0.97–1.01 | 0.99 | 0.97–1.02 | 0.99 | 0.97–1.01 | 0.99 | 0.96–1.02 |
Income | 0.93*** | 0.90–0.97 | 0.93*** | 0.90–0.97 | 0.93*** | 0.90–0.97 | 0.93*** | 0.90–0.97 | 0.93*** | 0.90–0.96 | 0.93*** | 0.90–0.97 |
Smoking (current smoking) | 1.76*** | 1.51–2.07 | 1.86*** | 1.58–2.19 | 1.76*** | 1.50–2.07 | 1.86*** | 1.58–2.19 | 1.76*** | 1.50–2.07 | 1.86*** | 1.58–2.19 |
Drinking (drinks per month) | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 |
Exercise | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.94 | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.95 | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.94 | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.95 | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.94 | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.95 |
Obese | 1.05 | 0.90–1.23 | 1.08 | 0.90–1.28 | 1.06 | 0.90–1.24 | 1.08 | 0.91–1.29 | 1.05 | 0.90–1.23 | 1.08 | 0.90–1.28 |
CMC | 1.16*** | 1.11–1.22 | 1.16*** | 1.10–1.23 | 1.16*** | 1.10–1.22 | 1.16*** | 1.10–1.23 | 1.16*** | 1.11–1.22 | 1.16*** | 1.10–1.23 |
SRH | 1.42*** | 1.21–1.66 | 1.46*** | 1.23–1.74 | 1.42*** | 1.21–1.66 | 1.47*** | 1.23–1.75 | 1.42*** | 1.21–1.66 | 1.46*** | 1.23–1.74 |
Depressive symptoms | 1.01 | 0.93–1.08 | 1.02 | 0.94–1.10 | 1.01 | 0.93–1.08 | 1.02 | 0.95–1.10 | 1.01 | 0.93–1.08 | 1.02 | 0.94–1.10 |
Neighborhood safety (poor) | 1.47* | 1.05–2.04 | 1.49* | 1.06–2.11 | 1.72** | 1.15–2.56 | 1.75** | 1.18–2.61 | 1.59# | 0.93–2.70 | 1.70# | 1.00–2.89 |
Neighborhood safety × race | - | - | - | - | 0.55* | 0.31–0.98 | 0.53* | 0.29–0.98 | - | - | - | - |
Neighborhood safety × gender | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | 0.88 | 0.54–1.44 | 0.82 | 0.50–1.34 |
CMC chronic medical conditions, SRH self-rated health
p < 0.1;
p < 0.05;
p < 0.01;
p < 0.001
Table 3 presents the results of three proportional hazard models in the pooled sample with perceived neighborhood quality at baseline as the main predictor. Model 1 did not include any interaction term; Model 2 tested the interaction term between perceived neighborhood quality at baseline and race. Model 3 included gender by perceived neighborhood quality at baseline interaction in the model. In Model 1, baseline perceived neighborhood quality was associated with risk of all-cause mortality (HR = 1.33, 95%CI = 1.02–1.71), net of all covariates. Model 2 did not show an interaction between race and perceived neighborhood quality (p > 0.05). In Model 3, gender did not interact with neighborhood quality at baseline on all-cause mortality (p > 0.05). Perceived neighborhood quality at baseline was not associated with increased risk of mortality due to internal causes (p > 0.05). Race and gender also did not interact with perceived neighborhood quality at baseline on risk of mortality due to internal causes (p > 0.05).
Table 3.
Association between neighborhood quality and all-cause mortality in the pooled sample
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All causes | Internal causes | All causes | Internal causes | All causes | Internal causes | |||||||
HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | |
Blacks | 1.04 | 0.89–1.20 | 0.97 | 0.83–1.13 | 1.07 | 0.90–1.27 | 1.01 | 0.85–1.21 | 1.04 | 0.89–1.20 | 0.97 | 0.83–1.13 |
Women | 0.53*** | 0.45–0.62 | 0.51*** | 0.43–0.60 | 0.53*** | 0.46–0.62 | 0.51*** | 0.43–0.60 | 0.52*** | 0.44–0.61 | 0.50*** | 0.42–0.59 |
Age | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 |
Education (years) | 0.99 | 0.97–1.01 | 0.99 | 0.96–1.02 | 0.99 | 0.97–1.01 | 0.99 | 0.96–1.02 | 0.99 | 0.97–1.01 | 0.99 | 0.961.02 |
Income | 0.94*** | 0.91–0.97 | 0.94*** | 0.90–0.97 | 0.94*** | 0.91–0.97 | 0.94*** | 0.90–0.97 | 0.94*** | 0.91–0.97 | 0.94*** | 0.90–0.97 |
Smoking (current smoking) | 1.78*** | 1.52–2.09 | 1.88*** | 1.60–2.20 | 1.78*** | 1.52–2.09 | 1.88*** | 1.602.20- | 1.78*** | 1.52–2.08 | 1.87*** | 1.60–2.19 |
Drinking (drinks per month) | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 |
Exercise | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.94 | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.95 | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.94 | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.95 | 0.88*** | 0.83–0.94 | 0.89*** | 0.84–0.95 |
Obese | 1.06 | 0.91–1.24 | 1.08 | 0.91–1.29 | 1.06 | 0.91–1.24 | 1.08 | 0.91–1.29 | 1.06 | 0.91–1.24 | 1.08 | 0.91–1.29 |
CMC | 1.16*** | 1.11–1.22 | 1.16*** | 1.10–1.22 | 1.16*** | 1.11–1.22 | 1.16*** | 1.10–1.22 | 1.16*** | 1.11–1.22 | 1.16*** | 1.10–1.23 |
SRH | 1.42*** | 1.21–1.67 | 1.47*** | 1.23–1.75 | 1.42*** | 1.21–1.67 | 1.47*** | 1.23–1.75 | 1.42*** | 1.21–1.66 | 1.47*** | 1.23–1.75 |
Depressive symptoms | 1.00 | 0.93–1.08 | 1.02 | 0.94–1.10 | 1.00 | 0.93–1.08 | 1.02 | 0.94–1.10 | 1.00 | 0.93–1.08 | 1.02 | 0.94–1.10 |
Neighborhood quality (poor) | 1.33* | 1.02–1.71 | 1.26 | 0.93–1.72 | 1.38* | 1.01–1.88 | 1.34 | 0.93–1.92 | 1.20 | 0.72–2.01 | 1.11 | 0.59–2.08 |
Neighborhood quality × race | - | - | - | - | 0.79 | 0.46–1.37 | 0.68 | 0.39–1.19 | - | - | - | - |
Neighborhood quality × gender | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | 1.20 | 0.70–2.07 | 1.27 | 0.65–2.44 |
CMC chronic medical conditions, SRH self-rated health
p < 0.1;
p < 0.05;
p < 0.01;
p < 0.001
Table 4 presents the results of proportional hazard models specific to race groups, with perceived neighborhood safety at baseline as the main predictor of interest. Among Whites (Model 1), baseline perceived neighborhood safety was a predictor of all cause (HR = 1.72, 95%CI = 1.14–2.59) as well as internal causes (HR = 1.75, 95%CI = 1.16–2.63) of mortality. In Blacks (Model 2), baseline perceived neighborhood safety was not a predictor of all-cause or internal causes of mortality (p > 0.05).
Table 4.
Association between neighborhood safety and all-cause mortality based on race
Whites | Blacks | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All causes | Internal causes | All causes | Internal causes | |||||
HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | |
Women | 0.50*** | 0.42–0.59 | 0.48*** | 0.40–0.57 | 0.68*** | 0.54–0.85 | 0.67** | 0.52–0.86 |
Age | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 | 1.09*** | 1.09–1.10 | 1.07*** | 1.06–1.08 | 1.07*** | 1.06–1.09 |
Education (years) | 0.98 | 0.96–1.01 | 0.98 | 0.95–1.01 | 1.00 | 0.97–1.03 | 1.01 | 0.97–1.04 |
Income | 0.94** | 0.91–0.97 | 0.94*** | 0.91–0.97 | 0.90** | 0.85–0.97 | 0.92* | 0.85–1.00 |
Smoking (current smoking) | 1.81*** | 1.49–2.20 | 1.92*** | 1.57–2.35 | 1.50*** | 1.22–1.84 | 1.53*** | 1.27–1.84 |
Drinking (drinks per month) | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.01 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 |
Exercise | 0.88** | 0.83–0.95 | 0.89*** | 0.83–0.95 | 0.90# | 0.80–1.00 | 0.92# | 0.83–1.01 |
Obese | 1.04 | 0.86–1.25 | 1.08 | 0.88–1.33 | 1.12 | 0.89–1.42 | 1.02 | 0.79–1.31 |
CMC | 1.17*** | 1.11–1.23 | 1.17*** | 1.10–1.24 | 1.12# | 1.00–1.25 | 1.14* | 1.00–1.30 |
SRH | 1.49*** | 1.25–1.78 | 1.51*** | 1.24–1.84 | 1.08 | 0.90–1.29 | 1.22# | 0.99–1.51 |
Depressive symptoms | 1.02 | 0.93–1.11 | 1.03 | 0.94–1.13 | 0.93 | 0.85–1.03 | 0.92 | 0.82–1.03 |
Neighborhood safety (poor) | 1.72** | 1.14–2.59 | 1.75** | 1.16–2.63 | 0.94 | 0.64–1.39 | 0.95 | 0.60–1.51 |
CMC chronic medical conditions, SRH self-rated health
p < 0.1;
p < 0.05;
p < 0.01;
p < 0.001
Table 5 presents the results of proportional hazard models specific to race groups, with perceived neighborhood quality at baseline as the main predictor. Among Whites (Model 1), baseline perceived neighborhood quality was a predictor of all-cause mortality (HR = 1.40, 95%CI = 1.02–1.92). Among Whites (Model 1), baseline perceived neighborhood quality was not a predictor of mortality due to internal causes (p > 0.05). In Blacks (Model 2), baseline perceived neighborhood quality was not a predictor of mortality due to all causes or internal causes (p > 0.05).
Table 5.
Association between neighborhood quality and all-cause mortality based on race
Whites | Blacks | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All causes | Internal causes | All causes | Internal causes | |||||
HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | HR | 95%CI | |
Women | 0.50*** | 0.43–0.60 | 0.49*** | 0.41–0.58 | 0.69*** | 0.55–0.87 | 0.68** | 0.53–0.88 |
Age | 1.09*** | 1.08–1.10 | 1.09*** | 1.09–1.10 | 1.07*** | 1.06–1.08 | 1.07*** | 1.06–1.09 |
Education (years) | 0.98 | 0.96–1.01 | 0.98 | 0.95–1.01 | 1.00 | 0.97–1.03 | 1.01 | 0.97–1.04 |
Income | 0.94*** | 0.91–0.98 | 0.94*** | 0.91–0.97 | 0.91** | 0.85–0.96 | 0.92* | 0.85–0.99 |
Smoking (current smoking) | 1.83*** | 1.51–2.22 | 1.93*** | 1.59–2.35 | 1.51*** | 1.24–1.86 | 1.54*** | 1.28–1.86 |
Drinking (drinks per month) | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.01 | 1.00 | 1.00–1.00 |
Exercise | 0.88** | 0.82–0.94 | 0.89*** | 0.83–0.95 | 0.90# | 0.80–1.01 | 0.92# | 0.83–1.01 |
Obese | 1.04 | 0.87–1.25 | 1.09 | 0.89–1.33 | 1.12 | 0.89–1.41 | 1.03 | 0.80–1.32 |
CMC | 1.17*** | 1.11–1.23 | 1.17*** | 1.10–1.23 | 1.11# | 0.99–1.24 | 1.13# | 1.00–1.29 |
SRH | 1.50*** | 1.26–1.79 | 1.52*** | 1.25–1.86 | 1.06 | 0.88–1.28 | 1.19 | 0.96–1.48 |
Depressive symptoms | 1.01 | 0.92–1.11 | 1.03 | 0.93–1.13 | 0.94 | 0.85–1.03 | 0.93 | 0.83–1.04 |
Neighborhood quality (poor) | 1.40* | 1.02–1.92 | 1.35 | 0.94–1.95 | 0.96 | 0.63–1.45 | 0.81 | 0.54–1.21 |
CMC chronic medical conditions, SRH self-rated health
p < 0.1;
p < 0.05;
p < 0.01;
p < 0.001
Perceived neighborhood safety and quality at baseline were not associated with increased risk of mortality due to external causes (results not shown due to non-significance of all paths).
Discussion
Building on a lifecourse epidemiological approach and using nationally representative data, the current study showed three major findings: first, single-item perceived neighborhood safety and quality at baseline predicted 25-year risk of mortality due to all causes as well as internal causes, but not external causes. Second, the predictive role of perceived neighborhood safety on all-cause and internal cause mortality was larger for Whites than Blacks. Third, gender did not alter the predictive role of subjective neighborhood evaluation on mortality risk.
Our first finding lends support to the lifecourse epidemiological literature that late health outcomes have social antecedents several decades earlier [51, 52, 53, 54]. Previous research has documented a link between perceived neighborhood characteristics and health, above and beyond socioeconomic and lifestyle factors [23]. Current findings also suggest that single-item measures are appropriate for the measurement of subjective evaluation of neighborhood characteristics, as they predict subsequent risk of mortality [6].
Our second finding is in line with larger effects of a wide range of psychosocial factors such as education [55, 56], self-rated health [18], depressive symptoms [12, 13], perceived control over life [14], and self-efficacy [15] on chronic disease and mortality for Whites than Blacks. One explanation for these systematic Black-White differences is that, in general, Blacks have lived their lives under adversities which has possibly prepared them for and enhanced their ability to cope with adversities, while Whites may have less experience dealing with adversities and have not mastered their coping skills. This view is in contrast to the multiple adversity hypothesis [57] suggesting that due to exposure to multiple risk factors, Blacks are more vulnerable than Whites to the effect of each risk factor.
In our study, subjective evaluation of neighborhood predicted mortality due to chronic disease. Perceived low neighborhood quality is shown to increase the risk of a wide range of chronic medical conditions including obesity [7], hypertension [58], diabetes [59], cardiovascular conditions [58], stroke [60], asthma [61], and cancer [62]. Low perceived neighborhood social cohesion increases risk for stroke mortality, an effect which remains significant after adjusting for a comprehensive list of potential risk factors [63]. Perception of unsafe neighborhoods also predicts subsequent deterioration of SRH among youth [8]. Perceived unsafe environment also increases the risk of mobility disability among elders at retirement age with income below the poverty line [64].
We do not know whether subjective evaluation of one’s neighborhood as unsafe directly deteriorates health or if it is a proxy of a disadvantaged environment and undesired life condition that cumulatively cause poor health. Several aspects of the social environment (i.e., social capital and cohesion) are associated with health outcomes [65, 66]. Promotion of connectedness with family, friends, colleagues, and other members of a social network may protect the health of community residents [67]. Some of this effect may be through social support which flows between individuals, buffering the effect of stress and adversities and reducing feelings of vulnerability and loneliness, while enhancing sense of control over life. Supportive social relations also increase availability of material and emotional resources that are needed at the time of dealing with stress [68]. Social support minimizes adverse effects of stressors [69]. When the neighborhood is perceived as safe, social network members spend more time in the community which increases flow of social support in the community. In high quality neighborhoods, vibrant formal and informal community organizations connect individuals that result in new relationships, trust, and reciprocity. Social organizations in safe neighborhoods provide opportunities for volunteer work and altruism [70, 71, 72], as well as social capital and cohesion [73, 74], all of which protect health. A shortage of social organizations, resources, trust, and a sense of safety may deteriorate a wide range of health outcomes among community residents [75].
Obesity and metabolic disorders may partially explain the effects of the social environment on mortality [76, 77]. Poor neighborhood quality increases the risk of obesity [77]. Perceived neighborhood disorder is associated with increased energy and sodium intake and decreased potassium levels [76]. Perceived neighborhood disorder is also associated with poor physical activity [77]. In a study by Assari et al., fear of neighborhood violence predicted development of obesity a decade later among female but not male Black youth [7]. In another study, perceived neighborhood disorder was associated with an increased risk of obesity, an association entirely mediated by psychological distress [78].
Psychological distress is another potential mechanism through which poor neighborhood quality can impact physical health. Perceiving one’s neighborhood as unsafe increases the risk of depression and psychological distress beyond socioeconomic status [79]. Wilson et al. showed that perceptions of physical and social characteristics of one’s neighborhood were linked to self-rated health and emotional distress [23].
Perceived neighborhood safety may be one of the mechanisms by which living in areas with high rates of crime, homelessness, drug trafficking, and prostitution increase health problems of its residents [23]. Policies and programs that enhance perceived safety through reducing social disorder and prevention of violent crime may slow the deterioration of the health of residents, particularly women who live in unsafe neighborhoods. To promote well-being of individuals, more investments should be made to enhance subjective aspects of the neighborhood that they live in [79].
This study did not find gender differences in the role of perceived neighborhood safety on mortality risk. The literature, however, suggests that women may be more prone to the health effects of the physical and social aspects of their environments [7, 8, 80, 81, 82]. In 2015, Assari, Caldwell, and Zimmerman did find gender differences in longitudinal associations between an increase in perceived neighborhood fear and depressive symptoms among Black youth over a short period of time [31]. The Moving to Opportunity (MTO) Study has shown that benefits associated with change in neighborhood may be stronger for females than males, as moving to low-poverty neighborhoods has lowered the risk behavior of females but not males [83]. Osypuk and colleagues have also shown gender differences in reduction of psychological distress and risky behaviors following moving to lower-poverty neighborhoods [55].
Our study had a few limitations. We used single items to measure perceived neighborhood quality. Thus, we cannot rule out the possibility of measurement bias. The current study exclusively focused on the social environment; however, physical aspects of neighborhoods also influence health outcomes [84]. Although subjective evaluation of one’s neighborhood is dynamic and subject to change over time, we only measured them at baseline. In addition to the social environment [85, 86, 87, 88, 89, 90, 91, 92], neighborhood socioeconomic status [4, 53, 54] also affects health. We did not include neighborhood-level factors such as racial composition, density of resources, or high level socioeconomic status [65, 66]. We also did not measure access to care, which affects health. Our study was still a unique contribution to the literature by showing that subjective evaluation of one’s neighborhood better predicts mortality for Whites than Blacks. Despite these limitations, using a nationally representative sample with 25 years of follow-up was a major strength. While several other outcomes have been investigated [85, 87, 91, 93, 94, 95], this study linked the social environment to mortality.
Future research may operationalize perception about one’s neighborhood as time varying covariates. Future research should also use multi-item standard measures that are already available for measurement of neighborhood quality [96]. Research should also investigate the mechanisms by which subjective and objective aspects of neighborhood quality influence health outcomes [65, 66, 77, 97, 98, 99]. Additional research may also examine why race alters the effects of neighborhood characteristics on health outcomes. Finally, research should examine whether or not enhancing subjective neighborhood quality through higher level contextual interventions that promote sense of neighborhood safety would result in health promotion or not [55, 83, 100, 101, 102, 103, 104, 105].
According to our study, single items are appropriate tools to measure subjective neighborhood safety and quality, as they predict long-term risk of mortality due to all causes as well as internal causes. Neighborhood safety, however, better predict mortality risk for Whites than Blacks.
Acknowledgments
Shervin Assari is supported by the Heinz C. Prechter Bipolar Research Fund and the Richard Tam Foundation at the University of Michigan Depression Center.
Funding
The Americans’ Changing Lives (ACL) study was supported by Grant No. AG018418 from the National Institute on Aging (DHHS/NIH). The NIH Public Access Policy requires that peer-reviewed research publications generated with NIH support are made available to the public through PubMed Central. NIH is not responsible for the data collection or analyses represented in this article. The ACL study was conducted by the Institute of Social Research at the University of Michigan.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest
The author declares that he has no conflicts of interest.
Animal Studies
No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.
Ethics
All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study. University of Michigan Institutional review board (IRB) approved the study protocol.
Reference
- 1.Kuh D, Ben-Shlomo Y, editors. A lifecourse approach to chronic disease epidemiology: tracing the origins of ill-health from early to adult life. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gjelsvik A, Dumont DM, Nunn A. Incarceration of a household member and Hispanic health disparities: childhood exposure and adult chronic disease risk behaviors. Prev Chronic Dis. 2013;10:E69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The adverse childhood experiences (ACE) study. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14(4):245–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Liu S, Jones RN, Glymour MM. Implications of lifecourse epidemiology for research on determinants of adult disease. Public Health Rev. 2010;32(2):489–511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ben-Shlomo Y, Kuh D. A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. Int J Epidemiol. 2002;31(2):285–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kuh D, Ben-Shlomo Y, Lynch J, Hallqvist J, Power C. Life course epidemiology. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57:778–83. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.10.778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Assari S, Lankarani MM, Caldwell CH, Zimmerman MA. Fear of neighborhood violence during adolescence predicts development of obesity a decade later: gender differences among African Americans. Archives of Trauma Research. 2016;5(2):e31475. doi: 10.5812/atr.31475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Assari S, Caldwell CH, Zimmerman MA. Perceived neighborhood safety during adolescence predicts subsequent deterioration of subjective health two decades later; gender differences in a racially-diverse sample. Int J Prev Med. 2015;6:117. doi: 10.4103/2008-7802.170431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.House James S., Lepkowski James M., Kinney Anne M., Mero Richard P., Kessler Ronald C., and Herzog A. Regula. The social stratification of aging and health. J Health Social Behavior 1994; 35(3):213–234. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.House JS, Kessler RC, Herzog AR. Age, socioeconomic status, and health. Milbank Q. 1990;68(3):383–411. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Assari S, Lankarani MM. Race and urbanity alter the protective effect of education but not income on mortality. Front Public Health. 2016;4:100. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Assari S, Burgard SA. Black-White differences in the effect of baseline depressive symptoms on deaths due to renal diseases: 25 year follow up of a nationally representative community sample. J Renal Inj Prev. 2015;4(4):127–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Assari S, Moazen-Zadeh E, Lankarani MM, Micol-Foster V. Race, depressive symptoms, and all-cause mortality in the United States. Front Public Health. 2016; doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Assari S. Race, sense of control over life, and short-term risk of mortality among older adults in the United States. Arch Med Sci. 2016; doi: 10.5114/aoms.2016.59740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Assari S General self-efficacy and mortality in the USA; racial differences. J Racial Ethnic Health Dispar. 2016. doi: 10.1007/s40615-016-0278-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Assari S. Hostility, anger, and cardiovascular mortality among blacks and Whites. Res Cardiovasc Med. 2016. e34029. doi: 10.5812/cardiovascmed.34029. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Assari S Combined racial and gender differences in the long-term predictive role of education on depressive symptoms and chronic medical conditions. J Racial Ethnic Health Dispar. 2016; doi: 10.1007/s40615-016-0239-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Assari S, Lankarani MM, Burgard SA. Black white difference in long term predictive power of self-rated health on all-cause mortality in United States. Annals Epidemiol. 2016;26(2):106–14. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2015.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Assari S, Lankarani MM. Chronic medical conditions and negative affect; racial variation in reciprocal associations over time. Front Psychiatr. 2016;24;7:140. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Assari S, Sonnega A, Leggett A, Pepin RL. Residual effects of restless sleep over depressive symptoms on chronic medical conditions: race by gender differences. J Racial Ethnic Health Dispar. 2016. doi: 10.1007/s40615-015-0202-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Assari S, Lankarani MM. Association between stressful life events and depression; intersection of race and gender. J Racial Ethnic Health Dispar. 2016;3(2):349–56. doi: 10.1007/s40615-015-0160-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Assari S Association between obesity and depression among American Blacks: role of ethnicity and gender. J Racial Ethnic Health Dispar. 2014;1:36–44. doi: 10.1007/s40615-014-0007-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wilson K, Elliott S, Law M, Eyles J, Jerrett M, Keller-Olaman S. Linking perceptions of neighbourhood to health in Hamilton. Canada J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004;58(3):192–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lovasi GS, Neckerman KM, Quinn JW, Weiss CC, Rundle A. Personal and neighborhood socioeconomic status and indices of neighborhood walk-ability predict body mass index in New York City. Soc Sci Med. 2008;67(12):1951–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rundle A, Field S, Park Y, Freeman L, Weiss CC, Neckerman K. Effect of individual or neighborhood disadvantage on the association between neighborhood walkability and body mass index. Am J Public Health. 2009;99(2):279–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wilson-Genderson M, Pruchno R. Effects of neighborhood violence and perceptions of neighborhood safety on depressive symptoms of older adults. Soc Sci Med. 2013;85:43–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Clark CR, Kawachi I, Ryan L, Ertel K, Fay ME, Berkman LF. Perceived neighborhood safety and incident mobility disability among elders: the hazards of poverty. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Balfour JL, Kaplan GA. Neighborhood environment and loss of physical function in older adults: evidence from the Alameda County study. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;155:507–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Stafford M, Marmot M. Neighbourhood deprivation and health: does it affect us all equally? Int J Epidemiol. 2003;32(3):357–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Everson-Rose SA, Skarupski KA, Barnes LL, Beck T, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF. Neighborhood socioeconomic conditions are associated with psychosocial functioning in older black and white adults. Health Place. 2011;17(3):793–800. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2011.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Assari S, Smith JR, Caldwell CH, Zimmerman MA. Gender differences in longitudinal links between neighborhood fear, parental support, and depression among African American emerging adults. Societies. 2015;5(1):151–70. doi: 10.3390/soc5010151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Stafford M, Cummins S, Macintyre S, Ellaway A, Marmot M. Gender differences in the associations between health and neighbourhood environment. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60(8):1681–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Weden MM, Carpiano RM, Robert SA. Subjective and objective neighborhood characteristics and adult health. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(6):1256–70. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hale L, Hill TD, Friedman E, Nieto FJ, Galvao LW, Engelman CD, Malecki KM, Peppard PE. Perceived neighborhood quality, sleep quality, and health status: evidence from the survey of the health of Wisconsin. Soc Sci Med. 2013;79:16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.07.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kawachi I, Berkman L. Social cohesion, social capital, and health. Social epidemiology. 2000:174–90. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Clark CR, Ommerborn MJ, Hickson DA, Grooms KN, Sims M, Taylor HA, Albert MA. Neighborhood disadvantage, neighborhood safety and cardiometabolic risk factors in African Americans: biosocial associations in the Jackson Heart study. PLoS One. 2013;8(5):e63254. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bryant Clifton D . Handbook of death & dying. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; p. 968 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sonderman JS, Munro HM, Blot WJ, Tarone RE, McLaughlin JK. Suicides, homicides, accidents, and other external causes of death among blacks and whites in the southern community cohort study. PLoS One. 2014;9(12):e114852. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.House JS, Lepkowski JM, Kinney AM, Mero RP, Kessler RC, Herzog AR. The social stratification of aging and health. J Health Soc Behav. 1994;35(3):213–34. doi: 10.2307/2137277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lantz PM, Golberstein E, House JS, Morenoff J. Socioeconomic and behavioral risk factors for mortality in a national 19-year prospective study of U.S. adults. Soc Sci Med. 2010;70(10):1558–66. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Harvey IS, Alexander K. Perceived social support and preventive health behavioral outcomes among older women. J Cross Cult Gerontol. 2012;27(3):275–90. doi: 10.1007/s10823-012-9172-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Gavin AR, Rue T, Takeuchi D. Racial/ethnic differences in the association between obesity and major depressive disorder: findings from the comprehensive psychiatric epidemiology surveys. Public Health Rep. 2010;125(5):698–708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Singh-Manoux A, Martikainen P, Ferrie J, Zins M, Marmot M, Goldberg M. What does self rated health measure? Results from the British Whitehall II and French Gazel cohort studies. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60(4):364–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Perlman F, Bobak M. Determinants of self rated health and mortality in Russia—are they the same? Int J Equity Health. 2008;7:19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Manor O, Matthews S, Power C. Dichotomous or categorical response? Analysing self-rated health and lifetime social class. Int J Epidemiol. 2000;29(1):149–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Schnittker J, Bacak V. The increasing predictive validity of self-rated health. PLoS One. 2014;9(1):e84933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Amtmann D, Kim J, Chung H, Bamer AM, Askew RL, Wu S, et al. Comparing CESD-10, PHQ-9, and PROMIS depression instruments in individuals with multiple sclerosis. Rehabil Psychol. 2014;59(2):220–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Zhang W, O’Brien N, Forrest JI, Salters KA, Patterson TL, Montaner JS, et al. Validating a shortened depression scale (10 item CES-D) among HIV-positive people in British Columbia. Canada PLoS One. 2012;7(7):e40793. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Andresen EM, Malmgren JA, Carter WB, Patrick DL. Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale). Am J Prev Med. 1994;10(2):77–84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Evrard SG. Prenatal alcohol exposure as an etiological factor in neuropsychiatric diseases of childhood, adolescence and adulthood. Vertex. 2010;21(92):260–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Power C, Hertzman C. Social and biological pathways linking early life and adult disease. Br Med Bull. 1997;53:210–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Galobardes B, Smith GD, Lynch JW. Systematic review of the influence of childhood socioeconomic circumstances on risk for cardiovascular disease in adulthood. Ann Epidemiol. 2006;16:91–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Galobardes B, Lynch JW, Smith GD. Childhood socioeconomic circumstances and cause-specific mortality in adulthood: systematic review and interpretation. Epidemiol Rev. 2004;26:7–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Osypuk TL, Schmidt NM, Bates LM, Tchetgen-Tchetgen EJ, Earls FJ, Glymour MM. Gender and crime victimization modify neighborhood effects on adolescent mental health. Pediatrics. 2012;130(3):472–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Spielman SE, Yoo EH. The spatial dimensions of neighbourhood effects. Social Science Medicine. 2009;68:1098–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Parker KF, McCall PL. Structural conditions and racial homicide patterns: a look at the multiple disadvantages in urban areas. Criminology. 1999;37(3):447–78. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Tang X, Laskowitz DT, He L, Østbye T, Bettger JP, Cao Y, Li N, Li J, Zhang Z, Liu J, Yu L, Xu H, Hu Y, Goldstein LB. Neighborhood socioeconomic status and the prevalence of stroke and coronary heart disease in rural China: a population-based study. Int J Stroke. 2015;10(3):388–95. doi: 10.1111/ijs.12343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Grigsby-Toussaint DS, Lipton R, Chavez N, Handler A, Johnson TP, Kubo J. Neighborhood socioeconomic change and diabetes risk: findings from the Chicago childhood diabetes registry. Diabetes Care. 2010;33(5):1065–8. doi: 10.2337/dc09-1894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Honjo K, Iso H, Nakaya T, Hanibuchi T, Ikeda A, Inoue M, Sawada N, Tsugane S. Japan public health center-based prospective study group. Impact of neighborhood socioeconomic conditions on the risk of stroke in Japan. J Epidemiol. 2015;25(3):254–60. doi: 10.2188/jea.JE20140117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Apter AJ, Garcia LA, Boyd RC, Wang X, Bogen DK, Ten Have T. Exposure to community violence is associated with asthma hospitalizations and emergency department visits. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2010;126(3):552–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jaci.2010.07.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Miki Y, Inoue M, Ikeda A, Sawada N, Nakaya T, Shimazu T, Iwasaki M, Yamaji T, Sasazuki S, Shibuya K, Tsugane S, JPHC Study Group. Neighborhood deprivation and risk of cancer incidence, mortality and survival: results from a population-based cohort study in Japan. PLoS One. 2014;9(9):e106729. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Clark CJ, Guo H, Lunos S, Aggarwal NT, Beck T, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon C, Everson-Rose SA. Neighborhood cohesion is associated with reduced risk of stroke mortality. Stroke. 2011;42(5):1212–7. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.609164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.McEniry M, Palloni A. Early life exposures and the occurrence and timing of heart disease among the older adult Puerto Rican population. Demography. 2010;47(1):23–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Diez Roux AV. Investigating neighborhood and area effects on health. Am J Public Health. 2001;91:1783–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Diez Roux AV. Places, people, and health. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;155:516–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Aye M, Champagne F, Contandriopoulos AP. Economic role of solidarity and social capital in accessing modern health care services in the Ivory Coast. Social Science Medicine. 2002;55:1929–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Habtom GK, Ruys P. Traditional risk-sharing arrangements and informal social insurance in Eritrea. Health Policy. 2007;80:218–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Lightman E, Mitchell A, Wilson B. Poverty is making us sick: a comprehensive survey of income and health in Canada. Wellesley Institute.2008. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Post SG. Altruism and health: Perspectives from empirical research. Oxford University Press;2007. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Oman D, Thoresen CE, McMahon K. Volunteerism and mortality among the community-dwelling elderly. J Health Psychol. 1999;4(3):301–16. doi: 10.1177/135910539900400301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Harris AH, Thoresen CE. Volunteering is associated with delayed mortality in older people: analysis of the longitudinal study of aging. J Health Psychol. 2005;10(6):739–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Molinari C The relationship of community quality to the health of woman and men. Social Science Medicine. 1998;47(8):1113–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Fone DL, Dunstan FD, Lloyd K, Williams G, Watkins J. Palmer SR. Does social cohesion modify the association between area income deprivation and mental health? A multilevel analysis. Int J Epidemiol, 2007, 36, 338–345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.The Hong Kong Polytechnic University. A study on mapping the associational life in Tin Shui Wai. Central Policy Unit, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Keita AD, Casazza K, Thomas O, Fernandez JR. Neighborhood perceptions affect dietary behaviors and diet quality. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2011;43(4):244–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Dulin-Keita A, Kaur Thind H, Affuso O, Baskin ML. The associations of perceived neighborhood disorder and physical activity with obesity among African American adolescents. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:440. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Burdette AM, Hill TD. An examination of processes linking perceived neighborhood disorder and obesity. Soc Sci Med. 2008;67(1):38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.McEniry M Early-life conditions and older adult health in low- and middle-income countries: a review. J Developmental Origins Health Disease. 2012;4(1):10–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Ann Karb R. Neighborhood social and physical environments and health: examining sources of stress and support in neighborhoods and their relationship with self-rated health, cortisol and obesity in Chicago. 2010.
- 81.Auchincloss AH, Mujahid MS, Shen M, Michos ED, Whitt-Glover MC, Diez Roux AV. Neighborhood health-promoting resources and obesity risk (the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis). Obesity (Silver Spring). 2013;21(3):621–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Foster S, Giles-Corti B. The built environment, neighborhood crime and constrained physical activity: an exploration of inconsistent findings. Prev Med. 2008;47(3):241–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Clampet-Lundquist S, Kling JR, Edin K, Duncan GJ. Moving teenagers out of high-risk neighborhoods: how girls fare better than boys. AJS. 2011;116(4):1154–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Cohen DA et al. Neighbourhood physical conditions and health. Am J Public Health. 2003;93(3):467–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Pampalon R et al. Perception of place and health: difference between neighbourhoods in the Quebec city region. Social Science Med. 2007;65:95–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Collins PA, Michael VH, Lisa NO. Neighbourhood quality and self-rated health: a survey of eight suburban neighbourhoods in the Vancouver census metropolitan area. Health Place. 2009;15:156–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Wilson K et al. Health in Hamilton neighbourhoods: exploring the determinants of health at the local level. Health Place. 2009;15:374–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Altschuler A, Somkin CP, Adler NE. Local services and amenities, neighbourhood social capital and health. Social Science Medicine. 2004;9:1219–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Wilkinson RG. Income inequality, social cohesion, and health: clarifying the theory—a reply to Muntaner and Lynch. Int J Health Services. 1999;29:525–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Song L Social capital and psychological distress. J Health Social Behaviour. 2011;52(4):478–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Ellaway A, Macintyre S, Kearns A. Perceptions of place and health in socially contrasting neighbourhoods. Urban Stud. 2001;38(12):2299–316. [Google Scholar]
- 92.Diez-Roux AV. Invited commentary: places, people and health. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;155:516–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Cho Y, Park GS, Echevarria-Cruz S. Perceived neighbourhood characteristics and the health of adult Koreans. Social Science Medicine. 2004;60:1285–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Cho Y The impact of individual and contextual-level characteristics on the health of Metropolitan Seoul adult residents. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, the University of Texas, Austin: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 95.Wong T, Yan Y. Perception of neighborhood environments and self-rated health in Hong Kong. Int J Public Health. 2012. 2(1). [Google Scholar]
- 96.Kim SY, Nair R, Knight GP, Roosa MW, Updegraff KA. Measurement equivalence of neighborhood quality measures for European American and Mexican American families. J Community Psychol. 2008;37(1):1–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Bowling A, Barber J, Morris R, Ebrahim S. Do perceptions of neighbourhood environment influence health? Baseline findings from a British survey of aging. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60:476–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Poortinga W, Dunstan FD, Fone DL. Perceptions of the neighbourhood environment and self rated health: a multilevel analysis of the caerphilly health and social needs study. BMC Public Health. 2007;7:285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Ziersch AM, Baum FE, Macdougall C, Putland C. Neighbourhood life and social capital: the implications for health. Social Science Medicine. 2005;60(1):71–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Fauth RC, Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. Short-term effects of moving from public housing in poor to middle-class neighborhoods on low-income, minority adults’ outcomes. Soc Sci Med. 2004;59(11):2271–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Fauth RC, Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. Seven years later: effects of a neighborhood mobility program on poor black and Latino adults’ well-being. J Health Soc Behav. 2008;49(2):119–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Kessler RC, Duncan GJ, Gennetian LA, Katz LF, Kling JR, Sampson NA, Sanbonmatsu L, Zaslavsky AM, Ludwig J. Associations of housing mobility interventions for children in high-poverty neighborhoods with subsequent mental disorders during adolescence. JAMA. 2014;311(9):937–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
- 103.Osypuk TL, Tchetgen EJ, Acevedo-Garcia D, Earls FJ, Lincoln A, Schmidt NM, Glymour MM. Differential mental health effects of neighborhood relocation among youth in vulnerable families: results from a randomized trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(12):1284–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Leventhal T, Dupéré V. Moving to opportunity: does long-term exposure to ‘low-poverty’ neighborhoods make a difference for adolescents? Soc Sci Med. 2011;73(5):737–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Fergus S, Zimmerman MA. Adolescent resilience: a framework for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Annual Review Public Health. 2005;26:399–419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]