Abstract
Fungiform papillae house taste buds on the anterior dorsal tongue. Literature is inconclusive as to whether taste perception correlates with fungiform papillae density (FPD). Gustatory reflexes modulate the amount and composition of saliva subsequently produced, and thus may be a more physiologically objective measure of tastant-receptor interactions. Taste perception fluctuates with time but the stability of individual fungiform papillae is unclear. This study followed ten healthy volunteers longitudinally at baseline, one and six months. FPD, diameter and position were measured and participants rated intensity perception of sucrose, caffeine, menthol and capsaicin solutions. Salivary flow rate, protein concentration and relative changes in protein composition were measured following each tastant. FPD, diameter and position were unchanged at six months. FPD did not correlate with intensity rating for any taste. FPD did correlate with changes in salivary protein output following sucrose (ρ = 0.72, p = 0.02) and changes in levels of proline-rich protein and mucin 7 following capsaicin (ρ = 0.71, p = 0.02, ρ = 0.68, p = 0.04, respectively). These results suggest that over six months fungiform papillae are anatomically stable, playing a greater role in mediating the physiological salivary response to stimuli rather than determining the perceived intensity of taste.
Subject terms: Saliva, Proteins, Neurophysiology
Introduction
Fungiform (mushroom shaped) papillae are small projections located on the anterior two thirds of the dorsum of the tongue, housing collections of chemosensory cells (taste buds) and nerves1. Fungiform papillae density (FPD) is invariably reported to vary widely between individuals, from 0 cm−2 to >200 cm−2, although mean density approximates 100 cm−2 2,3. Importantly, it has been shown that FPD is not uniform across the dorsum of the tongue, with a higher density occurring anteriorly and medially4,5. As with papillae density, the density of taste pores (the pore connecting taste buds with the oral cavity, used as an indicator of the presence of a taste bud)6, varies considerably. One papilla may house up to 22 taste pores with taste pore density ranging from 36 to 511 pores cm−2 7.
FPD changes throughout life, reducing relatively rapidly during early development between ages 7 and 10, coinciding with tongue development4. Morphological differences are also recognised with children having rounder papillae than adults5. During maturity there is evidence of a more gradual decline of 2.8 papillae cm−2 with every five years of age3. This data suggests that following early development, papillae density is relatively stable. However, these measurements are based on means from samples of individuals classified by age. It would therefore be conceivable that at an individual level there may be fluctuations in FPD that are masked at a population level. Individual taste perceptions have been shown to fluctuate with time, so should papillae density also fluctuate this may help explain these observations8.
A study of taste bud volume within fungiform papillae indicated that papillae diameter fluctuates week to week9, however, no literature following participants longitudinally to investigate stability of their fungiform papillae exists. While it is known that the turnover of taste bud cells is approximately 10 days10, it is unknown whether fungiform papillae themselves undergo turnover or migration at a macroscopic level.
Multiple studies have looked at measuring FPD as a marker of taste sensitivity, investigating the hypothesis that individuals with a higher FPD may therefore have a greater perception of intensity. Results have been conflicting, likely due to variability in protocol and difficulty in rating individual taste perception. It was originally observed that intensity perception of 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP), which is perceived as bitter at considerably lower concentrations depending on polymorphisms in the bitter receptor gene TAS2R3811, correlated with FPD12,13. Higher FPD has also been related to increased perception of sucrose, sodium chloride and citric acid in multiple studies2,14–17. More recently, epidemiological data (n = 2371) found no relationship between FPD and intensity rating for sucrose, quinine, sodium chloride citric acid or PROP3. There is further strong evidence that there is no relationship between PROP bitterness and FPD18.
Fungiform papillae density in relationship to other oral perceptions is less studied than for the basic tastes. There is evidence that fungiform papillae play a role in oral proprioception and perception of transient receptor potential (TRP) agonists, such as the perceived intensity of the oral sensation of ethanol19. The detection of creaminess has related to FPD, suggesting a role of papillae in the oral perception of fat20. While tactile sensitivity of the anterior tongue correlates with FPD and papillae diameter in an objective task21, such a relationship does not necessarily translate to perception of food roughness22.
There are numerous protocol considerations that may account for the variability in literature regarding oral perception and FPD. These include lack of a standardised region of the tongue being measured and difficulty identifying fungiform papillae. Measuring FPD requires training and where multiple operators are measuring FPD inter-operator consistency should be performed3,23. Other variables include concentration and route of administration of tastant. Tastant administration methods include application of tastant impregnated filter discs24, applying tastant via cotton buds14, having participants hold tastant solution in the mouth2, and use of customised application devices16,17. Concentrations of tastants vary considerably, ranging from 0.21 M to 1.8 M sucrose and 5.5 mM to 1 M PROP3,15,24. Excessively low or high levels of tastant appear to contribute to aberrant results as evidenced by PROP intensity failing to correlate with papillae density when applied by overly small or large filter discs, but correlating with intermediately sized discs24. Finally, assessment of taste perception is varied. While some studies measure taste acuity by determining detection thresholds2,16, more commonly perception intensity is rated on a scale. The labelled magnitude scale is commonly used3,21, however others such as the nine-point scale have been used14. The former scale has been identified as being superior to the latter8, whereas others caution its use in sensory research25.
An alternative method of assessing taste, which to our knowledge has not been investigated with respect to FPD, is the assessment of salivary response. Taste perception has long been recognised as an important physiological stimulus of reflex salivary production26. Following tastant binding at receptors, sensory information is relayed to the salivary glands via nuclei in the brainstem in an autonomic process27. This is true not only for the volume of saliva produced, which increases more markedly for sour tastes, followed by salt and then sweet and bitter28, but the salivary composition is also affected29. Dawes et al. found salt had a significant effect on salivary protein content and calcium concentration, whereas the effects of other basic tastants were generally comparable30. More recently, mass spectroscopy has been utilised to show salivary protein changes were most pronounced following sour stimulation, followed by bitter, umami, then sweet31. Due to the autonomic physiological relationship between taste perception and saliva production, analysis of salivary response following taste removes both the subjectivity of asking participants to rate their perception and the methodological complexity and inherent inaccuracy of determining taste thresholds. This study aimed to evaluate the anatomical stability of fungiform papillae by measuring papillae density, diameter and relative position of participants followed longitudinally at one and 6 months from baseline. The second aim was to compare the relationship between FPD and salivary response to taste with the relationship between FPD and oral perception measured by self-reported intensity ratings.
Results
Fungiform papillae density, diameter and position is stable over six months observation
Fungiform papillae density was measured in thirteen individuals (M = 6, F = 7) with a mean of 88 and range of 44–149 papillae/cm2. Inspection for normality suggested a right-skewed distribution. Eight participants returned at one month (28–35 days) and nine returned at 6 months (180–188 days). Papillae density comparisons are shown in Fig. 1a,b. Wilcoxon signed rank test revealed no significant differences in papillae density at one or six months (p = 0.54, 0.20) respectively. Although some fluctuation in FPD can be observed at different time points this variability was no greater than intra-operator measurement variability. Average papillae diameter ranged from 0.40 to 0.85 mm, across participants the mean was 0.56 mm. There was no significant change in fungiform papillae diameter for any participant between baseline and six months, Fig. 1c. Given no measurable differences were detected over after six months, measurements at one month were not performed. There were no significant differences found in the average distance between paired papillae between six months and baseline for any participant, Fig. 1d. The anatomical stability of fungiform papillae is shown in Fig. 2, revealing clusters of distinct papillae at baseline and six months in several participants.
Capsaicin, menthol and sucrose increase salivary flow rate without changing total protein concentration
At the initial visit participants scored tastant intensity in the correct order of concentration in every case but one, where one participant confused the higher two sucrose concentrations. Choice of tastants for subsequent anchored assessment was based on the spread of initial participant ratings, shown in Supplementary Fig. 1. A tendency for clustering of ratings for excessively low or high concentrations can be seen, e.g. 1 M sucrose and 0.1 ppm capsaicin. Tastant concentrations selected for definitive rating were 0.25 M sucrose, 8 mM caffeine, 1 ppm capsaicin based on the symmetric distribution of responses, mean responses approximating the midpoint of the scale and the highest variance suggesting optimal discriminatory ability of participant taste sensitivity. For menthol, 250 ppm was selected. The mean rating was slightly higher than the scale midpoint compared ratings for 100 ppm menthol however the ratings for 100 ppm menthol appeared negatively skewed, due to an outlying high rating (Supplementary Fig. 1).
Flow rate was not significantly increased above unstimulated levels by vehicle control rinses. Significant increases relative to vehicle control were detected following sucrose, caffeine and capsaicin solutions (p = 0.036, 0.012, 0.046 respectively), Fig. 3a. Protein concentration was not significantly changed relative to vehicle controls for any tastant solution, Fig. 3b.
Capsaicin stimulation increases the relative abundance of several major salivary proteins
Analysis of changes in relative intensity of individual protein bands indicated that capsaicin-stimulated saliva showed statistically higher band intensity for cystatin, statherin, MUC7 and proline-rich protein bands when compared to the ethanol vehicle control stimulated saliva, Fig. 4. No significant differences were found between tastants and vehicle controls for sucrose, caffeine or menthol. No significant difference between any tastant and vehicle control were found for amylase or MUC5B.
Papillae density correlates with tastant stimulated changes in saliva composition
No correlation was found between intensity ratings and FPD for any tastant solution. Significant correlations were found between FPD and salivary changes for MUC7 and Proline-rich protein band intensity following 1 ppm capsaicin (ρ = 0.68, p = 0.04, ρ = 0.71 p < 0.02, respectively), Fig. 5. FPD was also found to correlate with changes in salivary protein output following sucrose 0.25 M (ρ = 0.72, p = 0.02), Fig. 5.
Discussion
This study intended to assess the anatomical stability of fungiform papillae over six months. Our results indicate fungiform papillae are essentially stable surface features on the tongue, with no change in papillae density, diameter, or position at least over a six-month time course in healthy individuals. This knowledge could be useful, for example, investigation into the pathogenesis of conditions involving oral dysesthesia such as burning mouth syndrome, in which FPD has been implicated32. Longitudinal studies of taste perception, where wide daily fluctuation in sensory response to the same stimuli can occur8, may also benefit from removing FPD as a variable contributing in the day-to-day variation in taste assessment. Although papillae themselves appear stable, taste buds within papillae may change in volume as taste receptor cells undergo turnover9. Whether this could transiently influence perception is unclear as, in health, taste bud turnover is thought to be a homeostatic process rather than displaying temporal net increase or reduction in taste receptor cells10.
A further aim was to investigate the relationship between FPD and both taste perception and taste stimulated salivary reflexes. Despite epidemiological evidence suggesting FPD does not relate to taste perception3, FPD remains a commonly used phenotype in taste research, although results continue to be inconclusive33,34. Sucrose and caffeine are common tastants representing sweet and bitter basic taste modalities, used in clinical taste assessment35,36. Capsaicin and menthol are agonists of transient receptor potential channels, conferring their respective warming and cooling sensations via channels TRPv1 (capsaicin) and TRPm8 (menthol). Sucrose and caffeine perception have previously been studied relating to FPD2,33, and while capsaicin has not been directly studied with respect to FPD, perception of ethanol (another TRPv1 agonist), has been37. No significant association between FPD and taste perception was detected in this study.
Salivary responses to TRP agonists are less studied than those of basic tastes. Our results showed that 0.25 M sucrose, 8 mM caffeine, and 1 ppm capsaicin significantly increased salivary flow rate above that stimulated by vehicle controls. Sucrose has been shown to stimulate salivary flow of whole mouth and parotid saliva, although effects are short lived in the order of minutes38,39. Capsaicin has also been shown to increase salivary flow, with greater effects being seen on the submandibular and sublingual glands, sustained for around five minutes, albeit at concentrations of 9 ppm40. Nasrawi et al. divided participants into “high” (flow rate >100 mg min−1) and “low” flow individuals based on their unstimulated flow rate and found capsaicin only significantly stimulated salivary flow in high flow rate individuals. Whether this approach is founded in a genuine physiological difference between individuals is unclear.
Regarding salivary protein changes, no significant changes were found for sucrose, caffeine or menthol. While mass-spectroscopy based studies have found selected salivary protein changes following sweet stimuli these effects appear to be minimal and short-lived and would not likely be picked up gel electrophoresis31,41. Our results found that 1 ppm capsaicin was the only tastant to significantly increase protein band intensity and did so for several proteins. Such an effect has not been reported in literature, however there is evidence in rats that capsaicin will induce cystatin-s like protein production in the submandibular glands42. The salivary proteins observed to be increased by capsaicin stimulation were cystatin, statherin, proline-rich protein and MUC7. Cystatin and MUC7 are produced by the submandibular and sublingual glands. Cystatin is a cysteine protease inhibitor whereas MUC7 is a glycoprotein contributing to the viscoelasticity of saliva29,43. Proline-rich proteins are largely produced by the parotid gland and along with statherin are involved in pellicle formation. Statherin is a calcium binding protein and its concentration reduces upon entering the mouth due to proteolysis and tissue binding29,44, thus can indicate a rapidly produced, stimulated saliva. These observations taken together suggest capsaicin caused a rapid increased protein output from all major salivary glands.
In contrast to sensory responses, several of the observed salivary changes correlated positively with FPD. These were for proline-rich protein and MUC7 increases following 1 ppm capsaicin and protein output following 0.25 M sucrose. It therefore appears that these stimulatory effects of capsaicin are enhanced in individuals with higher FPD. These associations may reflect the objective nature of measuring saliva compared to complexities of measuring participant intensity perception which can be influenced by more complex neural inputs such as by hedonic response, memory and expectation45,46. An explanation for why capsaicin stimulated changes were found to have the strongest association with FPD may lie in the fact TRPv1 nerve fibres have been shown to enter human fungiform papillae although are absent from surrounding epithelium47. Furthermore, fMRI imaging has shown capsaicin activates autonomic brain regions (anterior short gyrus of the insula) to a significantly greater extent than gustatory centres (middle and posterior short gyri of the insula)48,49. Therefore, TRPv1 expressing neurones within fungiform papillae could be relaying autonomic rather than sensory responses to the brain, initiating subsequent salivary reflexes.
This study aimed to investigate the stability of fungiform papillae in the same individuals over six months. Correlation between FPD and taste perception measured by both participant ratings of intensity and physiological changes in salivary flow and composition were also investigated. Fungiform papillae did not change in density, size or position over a six-month period. FPD did not correlate with intensity ratings, however significant correlations between salivary changes were observed for protein output following sucrose and MUC7 and proline-rich protein following capsaicin. To our knowledge FPD and taste-stimulated salivary changes have not been studied previously. While this work does not prove definitively that FPD is proportionate to the magnitude of salivary reflexes, it does suggest fungiform papillae mediate the physiological salivary response to capsaicin more so than perceived intensity of the stimulus. This concept could be explored further in the fields of sensory science and oral pathology.
Methods
Participant recruitment and study design
All research was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, following ethical approval from King’s College London ethics committee (HR-15/16–2508). Participants were invited to volunteer and then screened for compliance against the following exclusion criteria: no diagnosed disorders of taste, no acute oral conditions and no objection to the tastants. Written informed consent was obtained. Thirteen healthy volunteers (six male and seven female) participated, aged 23 to 42 at the time of commencing the study.
Volunteers were assessed on three occasions. At the initial visit participants were familiarised with the tastants, rated an initial panel of tastants to determine appropriate concentrations (i.e. above recognition threshold but avoiding ceiling effects) and their tongues were photographed. One month subsequently, participants were asked to rate their perception of tastants. At this second visit, saliva was collected for biochemical analysis at baseline and after each tastant. Flow rates were recorded, and the tongue was photographed. At the third and final visit, timed six months from the initial visit, the tongue was photographed a third time. Data collected at each visit was treated as distinct and not averaged between visits.
Tastant preparation, administration and participant intensity rating
Sucrose, caffeine, menthol and capsaicin were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich at either food-grade or pharmaceutical-grade rating. Food-grade ethanol was purchased to assist in solubilising the latter two tastants. Bottled water (Buxton) was used as a standardised solvent source. Tastants evaluated at visit one for purposes of concentration selection are summarised in Table 1.
Table 1.
Tastant | Taste/oral sensation | Concentrations investigated | Solvent |
---|---|---|---|
Sucrose | Sweet | 0.25 M, 0.5 M, 1 M | Water |
Caffeine | Bitter | 4 mM, 8 mM, 20 mM | Water |
L-Menthol, (1 R,2 S,5 R stereoisomer) | Cooling sensation | 50 ppm, 100 ppm, 250 ppm | Water and ethanol (9.5 × 10−3%, 0.019% and 0.475% respectively) |
Capsaicin | Warming sensation | 0.1 ppm, 1 ppm, 2.5 ppm | Water and ethanol (9.5 × 10−3%, 0.095% and 0.24% respectively) |
All taste assessment was performed at room temperature of 19 °C. Participants were instructed to avoid eating, drinking, smoking and chewing gum for one hour before tasting. Tastants were presented at different concentrations in 10 ml volumes. Participants were seated and asked to hold the solution into the mouth, not rinsing or swallowing, pooling the liquid at the floor of the mouth. Tastants were expectorated after 30 seconds and ratings were recorded. Participants were asked to inform the researcher when the previous taste was no longer present in the mouth before moving to the next tastant, usually after several minutes. The order of tastant administration was standardised across participants (sucrose, caffeine, menthol then capsaicin) since capsaicin had a long lasting oral effect. Where multiple concentrations of the same tastant were investigated, participants were blinded to the concentration and order of the different concentrations. Ratings were recorded on a 0–10 graphic rating scale labelled with both verbal descriptors and numerical graduations, Supplementary Fig. 2. Participants were blinded to the tastant concentrations.
Tastant concentrations for intensity rating at the second visit were selected based on the results of the first visit. Concentrations were 0.25 M sucrose, 8 mM caffeine, 250 ppm menthol and 1 ppm capsaicin. Tasting was performed as described for the initial visit, however for purposes of standardisation both ends of the scale were anchored. The low end anchor was essentially the solvent in the absence of tastant, the high end anchor represented a higher concentration of the same tastant solution. The tastant and anchor solution concentrations are summarised in Supplementary Table 1.
Saliva collection and analysis
Following expectoration of tastant, saliva was collected into pre-weighed universal tubes over a period of two minutes. An initial unstimulated sample was obtained, followed by samples subsequent to water and ethanol vehicle controls (the zero anchors for the rating scale) and the four tastants, giving a total of seven samples per participant. Tubes were re-weighed and flow rate was calculated in g/min.
Samples were kept on ice during collection and centrifuged at 4600 g for ten minutes and supernatant was rapidly frozen and stored at −80 °C. Supernatant was assayed for total protein content (BCA assay, Thermo Scientific, Rockford, IL, USA). Protein output was calculated as the product of flow rate and total protein concentration.
Individual proteins were analysed by 1-dimensional sodium dodecyl sulphate polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis (1D-SDSPAGE). 15 µl supernatant was mixed with 5 µl sample buffer (NuPAGE LDS sample buffer, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Carlsbad, CA, USA) and 2 µl 0.5 M dithiothreitol (DTT, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA). Samples were vortexed, heated at 100 °C for 3 mins then centrifuged at 4600 g for 3 mins and 10 µl of buffered sample was loaded into pre-cast polyacrylamide gels (NuPAGE 4–12% Bis-Tris Gel, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Carlsbad, CA, USA) and electrophoresed at 200 V, 250 mA, for 32 mins in a 5% running buffer solution (NuPAGE MES SDS running buffer, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Carlsbad, CA, USA).
To compare protein staining between gels every gel contained one lane with a sample of pre-aliquoted reference saliva prepared in the same way. Following electrophoresis, gels were stained with Coomassie brilliant blue (Sigma- Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) diluted 2:3 with acetic acid for 30 minutes. Gels were destained with 10% acetic acid and imaged using a ChemiDocTM MP imaging system and analysed in ImageLab 5.2.1 (Bio-Rad Laboratories, Hercules, CA, USA). Lanes and bands were automatically detected, corrected manually and band intensity peaks were referenced to the cystatin band on the reference saliva lane, which was run on every gel. A uniform background filter (equivalent to the signal of an empty lane) was applied to every lane. Protein band assignment was conducted as described previously29, using the molecular weight marker as a guide. Where proline-rich protein is mentioned this describes the prominent band around 30 kDa.
Gels were then stained for glycoproteins with periodic acid Schiff’s stain (PAS). Gels were stained in 2% periodic acid (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) for 20 minutes, washed three times in Milipore filtered water for 5 minutes then stained with Schiff’s reagent (VWR) for thirty minutes in a light-proof tray. Gels were destained in UHQ water, imaged and analysed as for coomassie stained gels using the MUC5B band of the reference saliva lane to reference peak intensity, as previously described43. One participant was excluded from analysis of MUC7 as the band was not detected in any of their salivary samples. Supplementary Fig. 3 illustrates a typical stained gel showing the protein bands analysed.
Fungiform papillae measurements
The anterior dorsal surface of the tongue was stained with one drop of Brilliant Blue FCF food dye (PME, Enfield, UK). Due to the potential for the dye to affect taste and salivary chemistry this step was conducted after tasting and saliva collection. A sterilised 6 mm diameter filter disc was placed on the tongue for calibration purposes and a digital photograph was taken. Images were analysed in ImageJ (NIH, USA). To ensure a standardised tongue region was measured a circular area approximately 6 mm in diameter was measured centred around a point 3 mm lateral to the medial lingual sulcus and 3 mm posterior to the non-keratinised mucosal border of the tongue (Fig. 6a). The diameter of the circular area was measured, the papillae within the circle were counted and a density of papillae/cm2 was calculated (Fig. 6b). For every image this process was conducted by a single operator and repeated on three non-consecutive days without reference to previous counts to establish a mean papillae density.
To investigate changes in papillae size and position diameter was recorded for the ten papillae closest to the central point at baseline and the same papillae were identified 6 months later and re-measured. As many papillae are not perfectly circular, the greatest dimension was measured as opposed to a true diameter. Additionally, pairs of papillae identified at baseline and six months were identified and their relative separation measures, Supplementary Fig. 4. The same papillae were identified by their morphology at both time points. Measurements between the same five papillae pairs in the antero-posterior direction and five pairs medial-laterally were performed at both times points and differences were assessed by two-tailed paired t-test.
Statistical analysis
All data were analysed in SPSS 24 (IBM). Data were inspected and tested for normality by Q-Q plots and Shapiro-Wilk test and appropriate statistical tests selected. P-values below 0.05 were considered to be significant. Changes in FPD were assessed by Wilcoxon-signed rank test. Papillae diameter and papillae position were measured by two-tailed paired t-test. All salivary data (flow rate, total protein and protein band intensity) were analysed by repeated-measures ANOVA with Greenhouse–Geisser correction of sphericity and Bonferroni post-hoc tests to account for multiple comparisons. Having satisfied the ANOVA, comparisons between tastant and vehicle control bands and analysed by two-tailed paired t-test. For correlation analyses Spearman’s rank correlation was used given FPD was non-parametric.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by BBSRC under grant BB/M015211/1 and Diageo plc. The authors wish to thank Professor Peter Lillford, Dr. Martin Walker and Steve Wilson for their advice and input.
Author Contributions
A.G. collected and processed all data. G.H.C. and A.G. contributed to the study design, data analysis and manuscript writing.
Data Availability
Gel scans and images are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-46093-z.
References
- 1.Breslin, P. & Huang, L. Human Taste: Peripheral Anatomy, Taste Transduction, and Coding. in Taste and smell: An Update. Vol. 63, 152–190 (Karger Publishers) (2006). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 2.Zhang G-H, et al. The relationship between fungiform papillae density and detection threshold for sucrose in the young males. Chemical senses. 2009;34:93–99. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjn059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fischer ME, et al. Factors related to fungiform papillae density: the beaver dam offspring study. Chemical senses. 2013;38:669–677. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjt033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Correa M, Hutchinson I, Laing DG, Jinks AL. Changes in fungiform papillae density during development in humans. Chemical senses. 2013;38:519–527. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjt022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Segovia C, Hutchinson I, Laing DG, Jinks AL. A quantitative study of fungiform papillae and taste pore density in adults and children. Developmental Brain Research. 2002;138:135–146. doi: 10.1016/S0165-3806(02)00463-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Arvidson K. Scanning Electron Microscopy of Fungiform Papillae on the Tongue of Man and Monkey. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 1976;81:496–502. doi: 10.3109/00016487609119990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Miller IJ, Reedy FE. Quantification of fungiform papillae and taste pores in living human subjects. Chemical Senses. 1990;15:281–294. doi: 10.1093/chemse/15.3.281. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Prutkin J, Duffy V.B, Etter L, Fast K, Gardner E, Lucchina L.A, Snyder D.J, Tie K, Weiffenbach J, Bartoshuk L.M. Genetic variation and inferences about perceived taste intensity in mice and men. Physiology & Behavior. 2000;69(1-2):161–173. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00199-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Srur E, et al. Change of the human taste bud volume over time. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2010;37:449–455. doi: 10.1016/j.anl.2009.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Feng P, Huang L, Wang H. Taste Bud Homeostasis in Health, Disease, and Aging. Chemical Senses. 2014;39:3–16. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjt059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bufe B, et al. The Molecular Basis of Individual Differences in Phenylthiocarbamide and Propylthiouracil Bitterness Perception. Current Biology. 2005;15:322–327. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bartoshuk LM. The biological basis of food perception and acceptance. Food Quality and Preference. 1993;4:21–32. doi: 10.1016/0950-3293(93)90310-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bartoshuk LM, Duffy VB, Miller IJ. PTC/PROP tasting: Anatomy, psychophysics, and sex effects. Physiology & Behavior. 1994;56:1165–1171. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90361-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Miller IJ, Jr., Reedy FE., Jr. Variations in human taste bud density and taste intensity perception. Physiology & Behavior. 1990;47:1213–1219. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90374-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Stein N, Laing DG, Hutchinson I. Topographical differences in sweetness sensitivity in the peripheral gustatory system of adults and children. Developmental Brain Research. 1994;82:286–292. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(94)90170-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Doty RL, Bagla R, Morgenson M, Mirza N. NaCl thresholds: relationship to anterior tongue locus, area of stimulation, and number of fungiform papillae. Physiology & Behavior. 2001;72:373–378. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00416-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Zuniga JR, et al. Taste performance on the anterior human tongue varles with fungiform taste bud density. Chemical Senses. 1993;18:449–460. doi: 10.1093/chemse/18.5.449. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Garneau, N. L. et al. Crowdsourcing taste research: genetic and phenotypic predictors of bitter taste perception as a model. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience8, 10.3389/fnint.2014.00033 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 19.Duffy VB, Peterson JM, Bartoshuk LM. Associations between taste genetics, oral sensation and alcohol intake. Physiology & Behavior. 2004;82:435–445. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hayes JE, Duffy VB. Revisiting Sugar–Fat Mixtures: Sweetness and Creaminess Vary with Phenotypic Markers of Oral Sensation. Chemical Senses. 2007;32:225–236. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjl050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Essick GK, Chopra A, Guest S, McGlone F. Lingual tactile acuity, taste perception, and the density and diameter of fungiform papillae in female subjects. Physiology & Behavior. 2003;80:289–302. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2003.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bakke A, Vickers Z. Relationships Between Fungiform Papillae Density, Prop Sensitivity and Bread Roughness Perception. Journal of Texture Studies. 2008;39:569–581. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-4603.2008.00158.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Shahbake M, Hutchinson I, Laing DG, Jinks AL. Rapid quantitative assessment of fungiform papillae density in the human tongue. Brain research. 2005;1052:196–201. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.06.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Delwiche JF, Buletic Z, Breslin PA. Relationship of papillae number to bitter intensity of quinine and PROP within and between individuals. Physiology & Behavior. 2001;74:329–337. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00568-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Schifferstein HNJ. Labeled Magnitude Scales: A critical review. Food Quality and Preference. 2012;26:151–158. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.04.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hector, M. P. In Neural mechanisms of salivary gland secretion Vol. 11, 196–217 (Karger Publishers, 1999).
- 27.Proctor GB, Carpenter GH. Regulation of salivary gland function by autonomic nerves. Autonomic Neuroscience. 2007;133:3–18. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2006.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Pedersen AM, Bardow A, Jensen SB, Nauntofte B. Saliva and gastrointestinal functions of taste, mastication, swallowing and digestion. Oral diseases. 2002;8:117–129. doi: 10.1034/j.1601-0825.2002.02851.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Carpenter GH. The secretion, components, and properties of saliva. Annual review of food science and technology. 2013;4:267–276. doi: 10.1146/annurev-food-030212-182700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dawes C. Stimulus effects on protein and electrolyte concentrations in parotid saliva. The Journal of physiology. 1984;346:579–588. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Neyraud E, Sayd T, Morzel M, Dransfield E. Proteomic Analysis of Human Whole and Parotid Salivas Following Stimulation by Different Tastes. Journal of Proteome Research. 2006;5:2474–2480. doi: 10.1021/pr060189z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Nasri-Heir C, et al. The role of sensory input of the chorda tympani nerve and the number of fungiform papillae in burning mouth syndrome. Oral Surgery, Oral Medicine, Oral Pathology, Oral Radiology, and Endodontology. 2011;112:65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.tripleo.2011.02.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cattaneo C, et al. New insights into the relationship between taste perception and oral microbiota composition. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:3549. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-40374-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Dinnella C, et al. Individual Variation in PROP Status, Fungiform Papillae Density, and Responsiveness to Taste Stimuli in a Large Population Sample. Chem. Senses. 2018;43:697–710. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjy058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mueller C, et al. Quantitative assessment of gustatory function in a clinical context using impregnated” taste strips”. Rhinology. 2003;41:2–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ahne G, Erras A, Hummel T, Kobal G. Assessment of Gustatory Function by Means of Tasting Tablets. The Laryngoscope. 2000;110:1396–1401. doi: 10.1097/00005537-200008000-00033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Duffy VB, et al. Bitter Receptor Gene (TAS2R38), 6-n-Propylthiouracil (PROP) Bitterness and Alcohol Intake. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2004;28:1629–1637. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000145789.55183.D4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lagerlöf F, Dawes C. Effect of sucrose as a gustatory stimulus on the flow rates of parotid and whole saliva. Caries research. 1985;19:206–211. doi: 10.1159/000260845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Neyraud E, Heinzerling CI, Bult JHF, Mesmin C, Dransfield E. Effects of Different Tastants on Parotid Saliva Flow and Composition. Chemosensory Perception. 2009;2:108–116. doi: 10.1007/s12078-009-9041-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Dunér-Engström M, Fredholm BB, Larsson O, Lundberg JM, Saria A. Autonomic mechanisms underlying capsaicin induced oral sensations and salivation in man. The Journal of Physiology. 1986;373:87–96. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Silletti E, Bult JHF, Stieger M. Effect of NaCl and sucrose tastants on protein composition of oral fluid analysed by SELDI-TOF-MS. Archives of Oral Biology. 2012;57:1200–1210. doi: 10.1016/j.archoralbio.2012.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Katsukawa H, Ninomiya Y. Capsaicin Induces Cystatin S-like Substances in Submandibular Saliva of the Rat. Journal of dental research. 1999;78:1609–1616. doi: 10.1177/00220345990780100501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Chaudhury N, Shirlaw P, Pramanik R, Carpenter G, Proctor G. Changes in saliva rheological properties and mucin glycosylation in dry mouth. J. Dent. Res. 2015;94:1660–1667. doi: 10.1177/0022034515609070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Helmerhorst EJ, Traboulsi G, Salih E, Oppenheim FG. Mass spectrometric identification of key proteolytic cleavage sites in statherin affecting mineral homeostasis and bacterial binding domains. J. Proteome Res. 2010;9:5413–5421. doi: 10.1021/pr100653r. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Faurion A, Cerf B, Le Bihan D, Pillias A. fMRI study of taste cortical areas in humans. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1998;855:535–545. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb10623.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Woods AT, et al. Expected taste intensity affects response to sweet drinks in primary taste cortex. NeuroReport. 2011;22:365–369. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283469581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Yilmaz Z, et al. Burning mouth syndrome as a trigeminal small fibre neuropathy: Increased heat and capsaicin receptor TRPV1 in nerve fibres correlates with pain score. Journal of clinical neuroscience: official journal of the Neurosurgical Society of Australasia. 2007;14:864–871. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2006.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Rudenga K, Green B, Nachtigal D, Small DM. Evidence for an Integrated Oral Sensory Module in the Human Anterior Ventral Insula. Chemical Senses. 2010;35:693–703. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kawakami S, et al. The Brain Mechanisms Underlying the Perception of Pungent Taste of Capsaicin and the Subsequent Autonomic Responses. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2015;9:720. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Gel scans and images are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.