Abstract
Objective:
Little is known about the concordance of parent and child reports of children's media consumption, even though parents are often asked to report for their children in clinical care settings. We aimed to understand how parent and child reports of children’s media consumption differ in an era of changing screen media consumption via personal devices.
Methods:
As part of a larger study about the reception of health-related cues from children's media, children ages 9 to 11 (N=114) and their parents independently completed identical questionnaires about specific media use and health behaviors. To examine concordance between child and parent reports of children’s screen media use, we calculated mean number of minutes per day and proportions reported by the child and parent, and assessed concordance with t-tests and chi-square tests.
Results:
On a typical day, children reported nearly an hour each of video and app game use, computer use, and television exposure. Overall, child and parent reports were similar, usually within 10 minutes of each other. However, among 3 measures of TV use, parents consistently reported less TV exposure than children. There was significant discordance in the percentages of parents and children reporting the presence of a TV in the child’s room.
Conclusions:
Parent and child reports of children's media use were generally concordant. However, there were important disagreements, such as TV use in the child's room and during meals. We discuss possible causes of discrepancies and implications.
Keywords: media use, screen time, parent-child concordance
Introduction
Screen media use, such as watching television, playing video games, and engaging with social media, has been associated with attention and behavior problems,1,2 cognitive delay,3 sleep problems,4 and obesity.5-7 Suggested causes for the link between time using screen media and children’s BMI include the replacement of physical activity with screen use and the promotion of unhealthy foods in advertisements, which may negatively affect children’s food preferences and dietary intake.5,7-9 A recent systematic review and meta-analysis showed that both screen media access and use in the bedroom were associated with inadequate sleep quantity and poor sleep quality.4 Given the physical and psychological health consequences caused by obesity10 and poor sleep11,12 and the ubiquity of mobile devices, increasing screen media use is a concerning trend. However, screen media use can also have significant positive effects and can be appropriately incorporated to enhance child development, in addition to being a necessary mode of learning and development for school-age children.
Recent US reports estimate that, on an average day, excluding school work, those under age 8 spend nearly 2 hours with digital screen media, children ages 8 to 12 years spend more than 4.5 hours using screen media, and adolescents ages 13 to 18 spend more than 6.5 hours using screen media. One quarter of children ages 8-18 use more than 8 hours per day of screen media.13,14
Understanding media use in different age groups presents methodological challenges to media researchers. Very young children are mostly dependent on their parents for media exposure, and so relying on parents’ reports is typical and may be relatively accurate. For older children and adolescents, however, parents may not report use as accurately due to less parental supervision as well as perceived social desirability to underestimate time. Given the increased use of screen media on unmonitored personal devices such as smartphones, it may be impossible for parents to accurately report screen use.
While concordance between child and parent report has been evaluated in health-related quality of life,15 there has been little work done to assess differences in specific health-related behaviors.16,17 Others have reported a correlation between parent and child reports of the child’s television use time,18 but to our knowledge, no more recent published reports of concordance in screen media use have been published. Children and their parents may have different perceptions and/or reporting of media use in the new environment of mobile screen media use. We aimed to measure concordance between child and parent reports of various types of screen media use.
Methods
Our analysis is part of a larger study examining the influence of messaging in movies on children’s health beliefs and behaviors. Parent-child dyads (N=114) were recruited through flyers in university-affiliated clinics, and through university employee and school district parent email listservs to watch a movie and participate in a focus group. Dyads were eligible if the child was 9, 10, or 11 years old, had access to a laptop or desktop computer to complete study questionnaires, was capable of watching and discussing a movie in English and attending a focus group, and did not have food allergies or eating/feeding problems.
Prior to watching the movie and participating in the focus group, parents and children independently completed questionnaires, which collected demographic information in addition to a series of questions related to screen media use. Questions asked about usual screen media use, in addition to established rules around such use, and were modified from the Annenberg Media Environment Survey (AMES),19 the 2015 Youth Risk Behavior Survey (YRBS),20 and from instruments used by Common Sense Media,13,14 the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B),21 and Duke Children’s Hospital & Health Center.22 Parents were asked to enter the amount of time in hours and minutes that their child spent with various modes of screen use with the following questions:
-
Thinking about a typical school day, about how much time does your child spend….
…playing video games or app games.
…using apps other than games.
…using a computer.
…watching movies.
On a typical school day, about how much time is the TV on when your child is in the room (even if he/she is “not watching. ”)?
On a typical school day, about how much time does your child spend watching TV?
On a typical school day, for about how much of that time are you watching TV with your child?
An additional three questions measured television usage with close-ended response categories:
How many hours a day does a TV stay on in your household? [Rarely on; On 1-3 hours per day; On 4-6 hours per day; On 7-9 hours per day; Usually on when someone is home; Other]
Is the TV on during your child’s eating times? [On – sometime; On – usually; Off]
Is there a working TV or other screens on which they can watch TV shows or movies in the room where your child sleeps? [No; Yes]
Children answered identical questions, edited only to correct syntax. We calculated the number of minutes per day in each category of media use (video games, apps, etc.), comparing means and differences with t-tests. For categorical responses, we calculated differences with Pearson’s chi-square tests. In secondary analyses, we also tested differences in concordance in each category by race/ethnicity. All analyses were performed in Stata version 13. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.
Results
Children were 9-11 years old. More than two-thirds were white (69%) and children were from households with a large income range, although most (79%) had an annual household income of $75,000 or more. Parents were well educated (68% with graduate degrees) and mostly married (79%).
During a typical school day, children reported nearly an hour of video and app game use (50 min), computer use (50 min), and being present in a room where the television was on (57 min). Overall, child and parent reports of media use were generally similar and within 10 minutes difference in each category of use (Table 1).
Table 1.
Child, mean(SD) |
Parent, mean(SD) |
Difference | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Video/app games | 50 (55) | 40 (68) | −10 (76) | 0.17 |
Apps other than games | 23 (32) | 15 (24) | −7.7 (34) | 0.02 |
Computer | 50 (57) | 42 (43) | −8.2 (54) | 0.11 |
Movies | 31 (55) | 19 (43) | −13 (42) | 0.002 |
TV on, child in room | 57 (79) | 61 (78) | 4.1(61) | 0.48 |
Active TV watching | 48 (62) | 40 (38) | −7.1 (50) | 0.14 |
Active TV watching with parent | 25 (35) | 22 (27) | −2.6 (29) | 0.35 |
Of the seven separate measures comparing child- and parent-reported screen use per school day, reported time was statistically different in only two categories: use of apps and other games, and watching movies. These differences, although significant, were relatively small. Parents reported nearly 8 minutes less time their child used apps and other games (23 min child, 15 min parent, p=0.02) and 13 minutes less time their child watched movies (31 min child, 19 min parent, p=0.002).
In each of three categories of reported TV use, parents generally reported that the television was on less when compared to their child’s report. Most families reported that the television was on fewer than three hours per day, and children reported that the television was usually (12%) or sometimes (33%) on while eating (p<0.001). Interestingly, while 26% of children reported having a television in their room, only 18% of parents reported that their child had a television in their room (p<0.001, Table 2).
Table 2.
Child | Parent | p-value* | |
---|---|---|---|
Hours TV on (%) | <0.001 | ||
Rarely | 24 | 33 | |
1-3 hours | 49 | 45 | |
4-6 hours | 9 | 12 | |
7-9 hours | 2 | 0 | |
Usually | 9 | 6 | |
Other | 9 | 4 | |
TV while eating (%) | <0.001 | ||
On, usually | 12 | 11 | |
On, sometimes | 33 | 25 | |
Off | 55 | 64 | |
TV in child’s room (% Yes) | 26 | 18 | <0.001 |
Pearson’s chi-square
No significant differences in concordance between child and parents were found in each category of use when compared between race/ethnicity groups.
Discussion
In our sample, reported use of screen media was well below national averages, yet parents and children generally agreed on daily time spent using various devices. Parents reported fewer minutes per day in nearly all categories of screen use, and there was some discordance for for seemingly straightforward reports: television use while eating and presence of a television in the child’s room.
Despite acknowledgment that screen use has benefits for education and development, parents may understand recommendations of non-educational use and may have exhibited social desirability bias. It is also likely that parents of 9-11 year-old children are unable to quantify all screen media used by their children. Children use screen media without being supervised, for example, in their bedrooms at night. Since parents may not have a full knowledge of the kind and quantity of screen use, older children and adolescents may report their screen use and screen presence in their bedroom more accurately.
Our results suggest that seemingly straightforward recall of screen use and family media use structure, such as whether or not there is a television in the child’s room, vary by whether the reporter is the parent or the child. As media devices evolve, the conceptualization of “television” may differ between parents and children. Others have found correlation between parent and child estimates, although they used different questions for parents and children, with children completing a diary log of television watched and parents estimating how much time per week the child watched television on a six-point scale.18
A major strength of our study was the use of identical questions for both parents and children. However, we were limited by relying on perceptions rather than objective measures of media use and do not have a reference standard with which to compare parent and child reports. We were also limited in our ability to measure differences in content, particularly of games and apps, and whether the content is beneficial or detrimental to child health and development. Although most categories were functionally exclusive, there may have been overlap in media use between categories. Given the breadth of screen media available for mobile use, it is essential to advance measurement using electronic monitoring techniques23 to determine unsupervised mobile device and computer/internet usage. Our design did not allow us to deeply examine the content of child screen use, especially that which may not be directly supervised by parents. Research in the rapidly evolving media domain should attempt to precisely measure concurrent use of devices (e.g. multitasking using a tablet or phone while watching television) and its influence on health outcomes.
Assessments of concordance between parent and child responses to health-related questionnaires show that levels of agreement vary based on the type of question asked.24 There is substantial evidence that school-age children, including those as young as six years old, can reliably report about their own health.24 Furthermore, parents tend to inaccurately report their children’s behaviors associated with health risk, such as smoking, drinking, and sexual activity.24 Although reporting time-based behaviors such as screen use may be different than other health risk behaviors, there may be value in estimating screen media use directly from children.
Our study sample was majority non-Hispanic white, relatively high income, and educated. Literature suggests that there are differences in time spent using media depending on race/ethnicity, family income, and parent education.13,14 Our inclusion criteria for computer-access may have selected for families with higher incomes and less-than-average screen time use. Non-Hispanic black and Hispanic children, children of parents who have no more than a high school education, and children from low-income families spend more time using screen media than white children, children of parents who have a college education, and children from high-income families.13,14 Our sub-analysis by race/ethnicity did not reveal any differences in media use reporting, but our sample size was not large enough to perform reliable secondary analyses. This study should be replicated in more diverse populations that use more screen time, as we acknowledge our sample may not be representative of the US population, or among academic pediatric practices, regarding total amount of daily media consumption. Ongoing work in this area should attempt to understand how numeracy, cultural differences, and conceptualization of time affect reporting of screen media use.
Recent nationally-representative data show that nearly one-fifth of older adolescents report more than 5 hours per school day of media use on devices other than television (i.e., smartphones, tablets, videogames, and computers), a level of use associated with increased sugar-sweetened beverages consumption, inadequate physical activity, and obesity.25 Our results suggest that media researchers may be able to ask school-age, early adolescents directly about their screen media usage. As mobile screen use and multitasking become routine among children of all ages, an increased focus on the nuances of beneficial and detrimental types and quantities of screen use is necessary. We hope our research prompts deeper investigations into screen media use and its intricate relationship to child health.
What’s New.
We examine whether there are differences in parent reporting and child reporting of children’s screen media use. Our findings suggest there is general concordance, and with additional validation, either parent or child could be a source of reported screen time.
Acknowledgments
FUNDING/SUPPORT: This study was supported by NIH/NCI (R24CA186212). Dr. Callie Brown and Dr. Wood were supported by a grant from the Health Resources and Service Administration’s National Research Service Award (NRSA) (T32 HP014001).
Footnotes
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT: No competing financial interests exist.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ, DiGiuseppe DL, McCarty CA. Early television exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children. Pediatrics. 2004;113(4):708–713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Johnson JG, Cohen P, Kasen S, Brook JS. Extensive television viewing and the development of attention and learning difficulties during adolescence. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2007;161(5):480–486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chassiakos YLR, Radesky J, Christakis D, Moreno MA, Cross C. Children and Adolescents and Digital Media. Pediatrics. 2016:e20162593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Carter B, Rees P, Hale L, Bhattacharjee D, Paradkar MS. Association Between Portable Screen-Based Media Device Access or Use and Sleep Outcomes: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA pediatrics. 2016;170(12):1202–1208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Robinson TN. Reducing children's television viewing to prevent obesity: a randomized controlled trial. Jama. 1999;282(16):1561–1567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Viner RM, Cole TJ. Television viewing in early childhood predicts adult body mass index. The Journal of pediatrics. 2005;147(4):429–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gortmaker SL, Must A, Sobol AM, Peterson K, Colditz GA, Dietz WH. Television viewing as a cause of increasing obesity among children in the United States, 1986–1990. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 1996;150(4):356–362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Dixon HG, Scully ML, Wakefield MA, White VM, Crawford DA. The effects of television advertisements for junk food versus nutritious food on children's food attitudes and preferences. Social science & medicine (1982). 2007;65(7):1311–1323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Halford JC, Boyland EJ, Hughes GM, Stacey L, McKean S, Dovey TM. Beyond-brand effect of television food advertisements on food choice in children: the effects of weight status. Public health nutrition. 2008; 11(9):897–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dietz WH. Health consequences of obesity in youth: childhood predictors of adult disease. Pediatrics. 1998;101(Supplement 2): 518–525. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Chen MY, Wang EK, Jeng YJ. Adequate sleep among adolescents is positively associated with health status and health-related behaviors. BMC public health. 2006;6:59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Paruthi S, Brooks LJ, D'Ambrosio C, et al. Recommended Amount of Sleep for Pediatric Populations: A Consensus Statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Journal of clinical sleep medicine : JCSM: official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. 2016; 12(6):785–786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rideout V Zero to Eight: Children’s Media Use in America 2013. 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rideout V The common sense census: Media use by tweens and teens. Common Sense Media Research. 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pieper P, Garvan C. Concordance of Child and Parent Reports of Health-Related Quality of Life in Children With Mild Traumatic Brain or Non-Brain Injuries and in Uninjured Children: Longitudinal Evaluation. Journal of Pediatric Health Care. 2015;29(4):343–351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Berge JM, MacLehose RF, Meyer C, Didericksen K, Loth KA, Neumark-Sztainer D. He Said, She Said: Examining Parental Concordance on Home Environment Factors and Adolescent Health Behaviors and Weight Status. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2016; 116(1): 46–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mercado MC, Quan L, Bennett E, et al. Can you really swim? Validation of self and parental reports of swim skill with an inwater swim test among children attending community pools in Washington State. Injury prevention : journal of the International Society for Child and Adolescent Injury Prevention. 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Levine LE, Waite BM. Television Viewing and Attentional Abilities in Fourth and Fifth Grade Children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2000;21(6):667–679. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Bleakley A, Vaala S, Jordan AB, Romer D. The Annenberg Media Environment Survey. Media and the well-being of children and adolescents. 2014:1. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Youth Risk Behavior Survey. www.cdc.gov/yrbs. Published 2015. Accessed March 2016.
- 21.Najarian M, Snow K, Lennon J, Kinsey S, Mulligan G. Early childhood longitudinal study, birth cohort (ECLS-B). Preschool-Kindergarten 2007 psychometric report. 2010:2010–2009. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Healthy Lifestyles Program . Your Child's Healthy Habits. Duke Children’s Hospital & Health Center,. http://www.dukechildrens.org/repository/dukechildrens/2011/06/27/14/50/00/1325/healthy_habits_questionnaire.pdf Published 2011. Accessed March 2016.
- 23.Vandewater EA, Lee SJ. Measuring Children's Media Use in the Digital Age: Issues and Challenges. The American behavioral scientist. 2009;52(8):1152–1176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Riley AW. Evidence that school-age children can self-report on their health. Ambulatory pediatrics : the official journal of the Ambulatory Pediatric Association. 2004;4(4 Suppl):371–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kenney EL, Gortmaker SL. United States Adolescents' Television, Computer, Videogame, Smartphone, and Tablet Use: Associations with Sugary Drinks, Sleep, Physical Activity, and Obesity. The Journal of pediatrics. 2016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]