Abstract
Consciousness, defined here as the quality of awareness of self and the corresponding sensory environment, is considered to be one of most enigmatic and contentious areas of scholarly dissection and investigation. The subjective experience of pain is constructed and modulated by a myriad of sensory, cognitive and affective dimensions. Thus, the study of pain can provide many inroads to a concept like consciousness that the traditional sense modalities do not. Mindfulness defined here as non-reactive awareness of the present moment, can uniquely control and/or modulate particular substrates of conscious experience. Thus, in combination with brain imaging methodologies, we propose that the interactions between pain and mindfulness could serve as a more comprehensive platform to disentangle the biological and psychological substrates of conscious experience. The present review provides a brief synopsis on how combining the study of pain and mindfulness can inform the study of consciousness, delineates the multiple, unique brain mechanisms supporting mindfulness-based pain relief, and describes how mindfulness uniquely improves the affective dimension of pain, an important consideration for the treatment of chronic pain.
Pain is the breaking of the shell that encloses the understanding ~ Khalil Gibran
How, or why, does conscious experience arise? What brain processes or regions determine whether we will have a particular experience? Identifying the biological substrates that lead to and/or modify our conscious experience is regarded as one of the most enigmatic and contentious areas of scholarly dissection and investigation. To be clear, there are multiple issues to be resolved, some more tractable than others. In this paper, we barely scratch the surface on the topic(s) of the ‘not-so-easy’ easy problems of consciousness [9] that may be advanced through the study of pain and mindfulness [17].
For the sake of simplicity, we define consciousness as the quality of awareness of the experience corresponding to self and the respective sensory environoment [4; 52; 74; 86]. If we make the reasonable assumptions that consciousness does indeed exist and that it is, ‘roughly synonymous with having qualia’ there are still myriad puzzles to be solved [8]. These generally relate to the neurobiological correlates of consciousness and the neural processes which allow and influence our experiences, that is—to explain how the brain works. Neuroimaging and other objective methodologies have enabled neuroscientists to identify the behavioral and neural processes that support a wide array of sensory and cognitive constructs which at the very least, should relate to consciousness. For example, an ‘easy problem’ of consciousness would be explaining why it is that in one condition a peri-threshold stimulus is detected while in another condition it fails to reach conscious awareness [16]. The classical sensory and motor domains have all been used to explore consciousness and have taken us forward in our understanding of which brain areas underlie which aspects of experience. Viewing images inside of a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanner will produce significant activation in a number of visual cortical areas (i.e., occipital lobe)[67]. Auditory experiments have compared detected and non-detected sound stimuli revealing brain areas which are more active when a stimulus is consciously perceived [3]. A ‘finger tapping’ task will lead to significant activation of the primary motor cortex (i.e., parietal lobe)[5]. Yet, despite these once unfathomable feats, there is still much to learn about the brain regions and processes that sculpt our ever changing stream of consciousness.
Pain and consciousness
Unlike the classical sensory domains, the experience of pain is far from unimodal [12; 18]. Nociception is the physiological processing that facilitates noxious information, which at some point in the process becomes the conscious experience of pain (see [56; 65] for counter perspectives). Nociceptive processing is largely non-conscious and interacts with a cascade of psychosocial, cultural, and demographic factors to produce pain. The International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) defines pain as “an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage’ [15]. It is well established that the experience involves sensory, cognitive, and affective dimensions which, remarkably parallel the processes that comprise our conscious experience. Furthermore, pain is influenced by a host of factors ranging from cognitive processes such as attention to psychological constructs such as expectations [2; 40; 46], with the addition of clear genetic, social, and cultural influences [23; 34; 51; 68]. Thus, with such a broad range of influences, the study of pain provides many inroads to a concept like consciousness that the traditional sense modalities do not.
Mindfulness and Consciousness
Of course, for pain to inform us about consciousness we must be able to manipulate it and specifically in ways that are relevant to understanding consciousness. Converging lines of evidence demonstrate that a spectrum of self-regulatory techniques, including mindfulness-based meditation, can uniquely control and/or modulate particular substrates of conscious experience. To this extent, the combination of psychophysics, mindfulness and neuroimaging methodologies has provided unique insights into the capacity of an individual’s ability to self-regulate the salient and intrusive experience of pain and other sensory experiences. For these reasons we suggest that studying the interactions between pain and mindfulness could serve as a more comprehensive model or platform to disentangle the manifestations of conscious experience.
While much could be said concerning the definition of mindfulness, for present purposes we consider mindfulness as a form of non-reactive awareness of one’s present-moment experience [81]. Mindfulness can be considered a meta-cognitive state that exists to varying degrees in everyone. It can also be trained and developed by mindfulness-based practices (for reviews-see [85], [50], [48] [13; 71]). Traditional Buddhist texts have long described the benefits of mindfulness-based practices to influence pain. The Sallatha Sutta, a Theravadin Buddhist scripture from the Pali Canon (translated as The Arrow) is particularly interesting. The verse explicitly states that there is a key difference in how trained and untrained individuals experience pain [27]. The Sutta likens the reaction to pain to being struck by an arrow and, in the case of untrained individuals, being struck immediately after by a second arrow. The two arrows are described as representing physical and mental pain. Physical pain seems to refer to the noxious sensation one initially feels while mental pain is described as the worry, distress and suffering that follows. The claim of Buddhist contemplatives is that mindfully-trained individuals, because they do not cling to sensory pleasure, also do not experience this follow-up, seemingly self-produced experience of pain. This traditional account is remarkably similar to modern views of pain in several ways.
Mindfulness and Pain
Scientifically a pain response to a physical insult is known to involve two components. First pain is experienced very quickly and is typically sharp, propagating along myelinated A-delta fibres. Second pain arises somewhat slower, being carried by unmyelinated C-fibres [77; 78]. While first pain performs protective functions such as withdrawal, second pain lasts longer, is often more dull and likely plays many more diverse functions. It has been suggested, consistent with the traditional Buddhist sutta, that second pain is ‘affective’ in nature [59–61; 63; 64]. At an experiential level it is also well established that there are distinctions between pain intensity (sensory) and pain unpleasantness (affective)[45; 87]. Low level neural regions including the thalamus, primary and secondary somatosensory cortices are more involved with processing the sensory dimension of pain, whereas the evaluative aspects of pain and pain affect is processed in the anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulate, and prefrontal cortices [11]. The Sallatha Sutta was also surprisingly accurate in describing how mindfulness training uniquely attenuates affective pain (the second arrow) in contrast to sensory pain (the first arrow). In comparison to other pharmacological and behavioral pain therapies, mindfulness meditation has been repeatedly shown to be significantly more effective at attenuating the affective dimension of pain more than the intensity/sensory aspect of pain. Consistent with the notion that mindfulness is a quality or skill that can be trained, it appears that different mechanisms underlie the effects at different stages of proficiency. This is very important as it helps give texture to the largely intangible concept of consciousness.
The human experience is driven by reciprocal connectivity between so-called subject-object dichotomies [22; 58; 70]. Yet, one of the biggest hurdles in studying consciousness is pinning it down enough to make a meaningful comparison between two conditions. While there may be a constant underlying state that we experience as consciousness, which would only change when we’re something other than conscious, we can learn about that state by comparing how it’s content changes in people with varying skill sets—such as mindfulness. Thus, it is imperative that we specify the disparate mechanisms that appear to be involved across different meditative traditions (techniques) and proficiency. Brain imaging findings reveal that mindfulness meditation impacts the subjective experience of pain through multiple mechanisms including the modulation of cognitive-affective appraisals of noxious stimuli, as opposed to attenuating nociceptive information supra-spinally and/or at the level of the spinal cord [24; 28; 29; 49; 80; 82]. However, some of said mechanisms are dependent on the level of mindfulness-based mental training.
Pain relieving mechanisms supporting novice and adept mindfulness practitioners
We propose that, at the early stages of training (less than 8 weeks of training), mindfulness meditation alters the evaluation and meaning of pain by modulating self-appraisal processes. That is, novice meditators employ higher order brain regions [orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC), right anterior insula] to down regulate ascending nociceptive input at the level of the thalamus. We suggest that this occurs through shifts in executive attention and the newly trained ability to assuage affective reactions to noxious stimuli [80; 82]. Activation of the OFC may facilitate inhibitory connections of the GABAergically mediated thalamic reticular nuclei to further reduce the proliferation of nociceptive information throughout the cortex [evidenced by reductions in thalamic, periaqueductal gray matter (PAG), primary somatosensory activation (SI)]. Thus, in our view, brief mental training in mindfulness meditation engages cortico-thalamo-cortical interactions to reduce pain through mechanisms supporting the filtering of ascending nociceptive information [79; 81] via unique reappraisal processes to essentially “close the gate” on sensory (first) and affective (second) pain dimensions. We propose that novice mindfulness practitioners can effectively attend to qualitative aspects of noxious stimulus and attenuate accompanying affective appraisals by reorienting attention back to the meditative object (i.e., breath) in a non-reactive process.
In comparison, we propose that longer bouts of mindfulness meditation training (greater than 8 weeks) can produce more stabilized, trait-like improvements on behavioral and neural pain responses [10; 25; 37; 38; 54; 55; 75]. Adept mindfulness practitioners report feeling less pain in response to a noxious stimulus when compared to age-matched controls even when not practicing mindfulness[31], demonstrating that long-term, plastic changes in how one experiences pain can be produced with longer bouts of training. Though, it remains to be firmly established that these results are causal and not the product of pre-existing differences. Contrary to beginner meditators, the neural mechanisms supporting long-term mindfulness practice are associated with significantly greater activation in somatosensory, insular and cingulate regions coupled with greater deactivation of appraisal-related brain regions (vmPFC)[24; 29]. Thus, it seems that ascending nociceptive information is robustly perceived and processed, but importantly appraisals of said information are attenuated. The proposed decoupling between low level nociceptive processing and the corresponding meaning and/or contextualization of what the pain means to the self, in long-term meditation practitioners, provides evidence that the analgesic effects of meditation can be developed and enhanced through greater practice [30], a critical consideration for those seeking long-lasting narcotic-free pain relief [66]. In terms of consciousness, the mindfulness/pain literature teaches us more about how our ‘final’ experiences are shaped and importantly, suggests there may be an alternative to the suffering we seem to believe is inherent to physical pain. That is, advanced mindfulness training may result in feeling only the first (physical) arrow of pain.
Considerations for mindfulness, pain and consciousness
The potential implications of these findings for clinical pain are non-trivial. Evidence that the analgesic effects of meditation can be developed and enhanced through greater practice is critical for those seeking long-lasting, narcotic-free pain relief. Unlike the majority of pharmacological and behavioral approaches for pain, mindfulness hasn’t been shown to habituate or plateau in efficacy. Chronic pain conditions are accompanied with physiological dysfunction (i.e., nerve damage, herniated disc; inflammation) that renders the intensity of pain to be present, daily. This is important because it is now well established that a primary target for pain management therapies corresponds to the affective dimension of pain (i.e., pain interference; pain bothersome; pain unpleasantness)[1; 19–21; 35; 39; 42; 57]. This makes sense as, it is likely the more mental or affective aspects of pain that color the patient’s quality of life. Thus, mindfulness-based therapies may be particularly effective for pain because they enhance one’s ability to self-regulate reactions to sensory experiences through acceptance-based strategies without the emphasis of changing one’s attitude towards said experience(s).
In a seminal study conducted by Donald Price and colleagues [61], cancer patients rated their pain significantly higher on the affective dimension of pain when compared to the sensory aspect of pain. In contrast, labor pain was associated with significantly higher pain intensity when compared to pain unpleasantness ratings. It was postulated that the contextual evaluation of welcoming a new baby into the world when compared to facing one’s own death has profound implications for what might have otherwise been very similar experiences, in terms of pain. We have argued that mindfulness-based mental training resembles physical exercise [81; 85]. That is, when an individual exercises a muscle group, said muscle group progressively strengthens as a function of training frequency. Thus, it seems as if one’s ability to be aware of awareness can be strengthened like a muscle. Mindfulness training can strengthen one’s ability to sustain attention in the present moment, non-reactively in a fashion where the state of mindfulness transitions to a mindfulness-based trait. These effects demonstrate that mindfulness holds promise in improving components of the pain experience that significantly debilitate one’s quality of life and may lead to the comorbidities commonly exhibited in chronic pain patients. Mindfulness may uniquely impact how one relates to and/or may chance one’s relationship with their respective pain. This bridges us back to appreciating how the interaction between mindfulness and pain informs consciousness.
We postulate that mindfulness training reduces the significance and or motivational influences of pain on said organism. Some have proposed that mindfulness decouples pain-related sensory discrimination from self-related appraisals[81; 85]. Others have stated that mindfulness integrates intrinsic and extrinsic processes that reduce the dualistic nature of my (intrinsic) pain (extrinsic). Recent evidence suggests that the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) is a mechanistic target for mindfulness-based therapies [7; 14; 26; 73; 83]. The precuneus/PCC has been characterized as a central node in the default mode network, a neural network associated with facilitating self-referential and the integration of intrinsic, self-narrative and extrinsic-perceptual processes [62; 69; 76; 88]. The PCC is highly integrated across a host of cortical and subcortical regions that renders it well suited to varying levels of consciousness[44]. Higher PCC activation is believed to reflect higher levels of sensory and perceptual integration [43]. Mindfulness is postulated to dissolve subject-object dichotomies [32; 36; 53]. To this extent, adept meditators have exhibited significantly greater deactivation of the PCC when compared to age matched non-meditators [7; 33; 72]. We recently discovered that higher dispositional mindfulness, defined as the innate capacity to be aware of the present moment in a non-reactive manner, is associated with greater deactivation of the PCC and lower pain ratings[84]. Thus, the PCC may prove to be a significant mechanistic target for mindfulness-based pain relief as a function of training experience [6; 41; 47]. Through the scientific investigation of pain, we have learned that mindfulness impacts the moment-to-moment subjective experience by a) top-down attenuation of ascending low-level afferent processing at the level of the thalamus reducing the elaboration/significance of ascending sensory information, b) decoupling higher order appraisals from sensory discriminative processes, and c) reducing self-referential processes that enhance non-conceptual perceptual processes. These findings demonstrate that mindfulness modulates the subjective experience in distinct mechanistic fashion from other cognitive manipulations.
As the research in this field advances it may provide scientists with more testable hypotheses in terms of how the brain works. Said another way, it may help us tackle some of the easy problems of consciousness. For example, hypnosis is often used as an experimental tool as it allows the researchers to systematically vary aspects of experience that are not touchable by other means. Mindfulness could be used similarly. If advanced practitioners only experience first pain, or relatively little second pain, what influence does this have on x, y or z? It may provide a more comprehensive or ‘clean’ view on what first and second pain actually comprises, how separable they really are, and what brain areas contribute to each. We are a long way from understanding the intricacies of conscious experience but the coupling of mindfulness and pain promises to be an important tool along the way.
Highlights.
The study of pain can uniquely inform the enigmatic nature of consciousness.
Mindfulness uniquely impacts the subjective experience of pain.
The impact of mindfulness on consciousness processes can inform pain therapies.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work was supported by the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (R00-AT008238, R01AT009693).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
DISCLOSURES
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
REFERENCES
- [1].Abbasi J As Opioid Epidemic Rages, Complementary Health Approaches to Pain Gain Traction. Jama 2016;316(22):2343–2344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Atlas LY, Wager TD. How expectations shape pain. Neuroscience letters 2012;520(2):140–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Binder JR, Rao SM, Hammeke TA, Yetkin FZ, Jesmanowicz A, Bandettini PA, Wong EC, Estkowski LD, Goldstein MD, Haughton VM, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human auditory cortex. Annals of neurology 1994;35(6):662–672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Block N Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. The Behavioral and brain sciences 2007;30(5–6):481–499; discussion 499–548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Boecker H, Kleinschmidt A, Requardt M, Hanicke W, Merboldt KD, Frahm J. Functional cooperativity of human cortical motor areas during self-paced simple finger movements. A high-resolution MRI study. Brain : a journal of neurology 1994;117 ( Pt 6):1231–1239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Brewer JA, Garrison KA. The posterior cingulate cortex as a plausible mechanistic target of meditation: findings from neuroimaging. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Brewer JA, Worhunsky PD, Gray JR, Tang YY, Weber J, Kober H. Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2011;108(50):20254–20259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Chalmers DJ. The conscious mind : in search of a fundamental theory [Google Scholar]
- [9].Chalmers DJ. The puzzle of conscious experience. Scientific American 1995;273(6):80–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Cherkin DC, Sherman KJ, Turner JA. Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction vs Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Chronic Low Back Pain--Reply. Jama 2016;316(6):663–664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Coghill RC, McHaffie JG, Yen YF. Neural correlates of interindividual differences in the subjective experience of pain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2003;100(14):8538–8542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Coghill RC, Sang CN, Maisog JM, Iadarola MJ. Pain intensity processing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanism. Journal of neurophysiology 1999;82(4):1934–1943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Creswell JD. Mindfulness Interventions. Annual review of psychology 2017;68:491–516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Creswell JD, Taren AA, Lindsay EK, Greco CM, Gianaros PJ, Fairgrieve A, Marsland AL, Brown KW, Way BM, Rosen RK, Ferris JL. Alterations in Resting-State Functional Connectivity Link Mindfulness Meditation With Reduced Interleukin-6: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Biological psychiatry 2016;80(1):53–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Davis KD, Flor H, Greely HT, Iannetti GD, Mackey S, Ploner M, Pustilnik A, Tracey I, Treede RD, Wager TD. Brain imaging tests for chronic pain: medical, legal and ethical issues and recommendations. Nature reviews Neurology 2017;13(10):624–638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].de Lafuente V, Romo R. Neural correlate of subjective sensory experience gradually builds up across cortical areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2006;103(39):14266–14271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Dehaene S, Charles L, King JR, Marti S. Toward a computational theory of conscious processing. Current opinion in neurobiology 2014;25:76–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Derbyshire SW, Jones AK, Gyulai F, Clark S, Townsend D, Firestone LL. Pain processing during three levels of noxious stimulation produces differential patterns of central activity. Pain 1997;73(3):431–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Dowell D, Haegerich TM, Chou R. CDC Guideline for Prescribing Opioids for Chronic Pain--United States, 2016. Jama 2016;315(15):1624–1645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Dworkin RH, Turk DC, Farrar JT, Haythornthwaite JA, Jensen MP, Katz NP, Kerns RD, Stucki G, Allen RR, Bellamy N, Carr DB, Chandler J, Cowan P, Dionne R, Galer BS, Hertz S, Jadad AR, Kramer LD, Manning DC, Martin S, McCormick CG, McDermott MP, McGrath P, Quessy S, Rappaport BA, Robbins W, Robinson JP, Rothman M, Royal MA, Simon L, Stauffer JW, Stein W, Tollett J, Wernicke J, Witter J, Immpact. Core outcome measures for chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. Pain 2005;113(1–2):9–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Dworkin RH, Turk DC, Peirce-Sandner S, Burke LB, Farrar JT, Gilron I, Jensen MP, Katz NP, Raja SN, Rappaport BA, Rowbotham MC, Backonja MM, Baron R, Bellamy N, Bhagwagar Z, Costello A, Cowan P, Fang WC, Hertz S, Jay GW, Junor R, Kerns RD, Kerwin R, Kopecky EA, Lissin D, Malamut R, Markman JD, McDermott MP, Munera C, Porter L, Rauschkolb C, Rice AS, Sampaio C, Skljarevski V, Sommerville K, Stacey BR, Steigerwald I, Tobias J, Trentacosti AM, Wasan AD, Wells GA, Williams J, Witter J, Ziegler D. Considerations for improving assay sensitivity in chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. Pain 2012;153(6):1148–1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Eccleston C Chronic pain as embodied defence: implications for current and future psychological treatments. Pain 2018;159 Suppl 1: S17–S23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Fabian LA, McGuire L, Page GG, Goodin BR, Edwards RR, Haythornthwaite J. The association of the cortisol awakening response with experimental pain ratings. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009;34(8):1247–1251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Gard T, Holzel BK, Sack AT, Hempel H, Lazar SW, Vaitl D, Ott U. Pain attenuation through mindfulness is associated with decreased cognitive control and increased sensory processing in the brain. Cerebral cortex 2012;22(11):2692–2702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Garland EL, Howard MO. Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement reduces pain attentional bias in chronic pain patients. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics 2013;82(5):311–318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Garrison KA, Scheinost D, Worhunsky PD, Elwafi HM, Thornhill TAt, Thompson E, Saron C, Desbordes G, Kober H, Hampson M, Gray JR, Constable RT, Papademetris X, Brewer JA Real-time fMRI links subjective experience with brain activity during focused attention. NeuroImage 2013;81:110–118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].Grant JA. Meditative analgesia: the current state of the field. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 2014;1307:55–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Grant JA, Courtemanche J, Duerden EG, Duncan GH, Rainville P. Cortical thickness and pain sensitivity in zen meditators. Emotion 2010;10(1):43–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Grant JA, Courtemanche J, Rainville P. A non-elaborative mental stance and decoupling of executive and pain-related cortices predicts low pain sensitivity in Zen meditators. Pain 2011;152(1):150–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Grant JA, Courtemanche J, Rainville P. A non-elaborative mental stance and decoupling of executive and pain-related cortices predicts low pain sensitivity in Zen meditators. Pain 2011;152(1):150–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Grant JA, Rainville P. Pain sensitivity and analgesic effects of mindful states in Zen meditators: a cross-sectional study. Psychosomatic medicine 2009;71(1):106–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Hanley AW, Nakamura Y, Garland EL. The Nondual Awareness Dimensional Assessment (NADA): New tools to assess nondual traits and states of consciousness occurring within and beyond the context of meditation. Psychological assessment 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Hasenkamp W, Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Duncan E, Barsalou LW. Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: a fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. NeuroImage 2011;59(1):750–760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Hastie BA, Riley JL 3rd, Kaplan L, Herrera DG, Campbell CM, Virtusio K, Mogil JS, Wallace MR, Fillingim RB Ethnicity interacts with the OPRM1 gene in experimental pain sensitivity. Pain 2012;153(8):1610–1619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].Jacob JA. As Opioid Prescribing Guidelines Tighten, Mindfulness Meditation Holds Promise for Pain Relief. Jama 2016;315(22):2385–2387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Josipovic Z Love and compassion meditation: a nondual perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 2016;1373(1):65–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Kabat-Zinn J An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General hospital psychiatry 1982;4(1):33–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Kabat-Zinn J, Lipworth L, Burney R. The clinical use of mindfulness meditation for the self-regulation of chronic pain. Journal of behavioral medicine 1985;8(2):163–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].King WC, Chen JY, Belle SH, Courcoulas AP, Dakin GF, Elder KA, Flum DR, Hinojosa MW, Mitchell JE, Pories WJ, Wolfe BM, Yanovski SZ. Change in Pain and Physical Function Following Bariatric Surgery for Severe Obesity. Jama 2016;315(13):1362–1371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Koyama T, McHaffie JG, Laurienti PJ, Coghill RC. The subjective experience of pain: where expectations become reality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2005;102(36):12950–12955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [41].Lee J, Protsenko E, Lazaridou A, Franceschelli O, Ellingsen DM, Mawla I, Isenburg K, Berry MP, Galenkamp L, Loggia ML, Wasan AD, Edwards RR, Napadow V. Encoding of Self-Referential Pain Catastrophizing in the Posterior Cingulate Cortex in Fibromyalgia. Arthritis & rheumatology 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Lee TH. Zero Pain Is Not the Goal. Jama 2016;315(15):1575–1577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [43].Leech R, Braga R, Sharp DJ. Echoes of the brain within the posterior cingulate cortex. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 2012;32(1):215–222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [44].Leech R, Kamourieh S, Beckmann CF, Sharp DJ. Fractionating the default mode network: distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 2011;31(9):3217–3224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [45].Lenz FA, Gracely RH, Romanoski AJ, Hope EJ, Rowland LH, Dougherty PM. Stimulation in the human somatosensory thalamus can reproduce both the affective and sensory dimensions of previously experienced pain. Nature medicine 1995;1(9):910–913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [46].Lobanov OV, Zeidan F, McHaffie JG, Kraft RA, Coghill RC. From cue to meaning: Brain mechanisms supporting the construction of expectations of pain. Pain 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [47].Loggia ML, Kim J, Gollub RL, Vangel MG, Kirsch I, Kong J, Wasan AD, Napadow V. Default mode network connectivity encodes clinical pain: An arterial spin labeling study. Pain 2013;154(1):24–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [48].Lutz A, Jha AP, Dunne JD, Saron CD. Investigating the phenomenological matrix of mindfulness-related practices from a neurocognitive perspective. The American psychologist 2015;70(7):632–658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [49].Lutz A, McFarlin DR, Perlman DM, Salomons TV, Davidson RJ. Altered anterior insula activation during anticipation and experience of painful stimuli in expert meditators. NeuroImage 2013;64:538–546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [50].Lutz A, Slagter HA, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ. Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in cognitive sciences 2008;12(4):163–169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [51].Malfait AM, Seymour AB, Gao F, Tortorella MD, Le Graverand-Gastineau MP, Wood LS, Doherty M, Doherty S, Zhang W, Arden NK, Vaughn FL, Leaverton PE, Spector TD, Hart DJ, Maciewicz RA, Muir KR, Das R, Sorge RE, Sotocinal SG, Schorscher-Petcu A, Valdes AM, Mogil JS. A role for PACE4 in osteoarthritis pain: evidence from human genetic association and null mutant phenotype. Ann Rheum Dis 2012;71(6):1042–1048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [52].Miller G What is the biological basis of consciousness? Science 2005;309(5731):79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [53].Mills PJ, Peterson CT, Pung MA, Patel S, Weiss L, Wilson KL, Doraiswamy PM, Martin JA, Tanzi RE, Chopra D. Change in Sense of Nondual Awareness and Spiritual Awakening in Response to a Multidimensional Well-Being Program. Journal of alternative and complementary medicine 2018;24(4):343–351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [54].Morone NE, Greco CM, Moore CG, Rollman BL, Lane B, Morrow LA, Glynn NW, Weiner DK. A Mind-Body Program for Older Adults With Chronic Low Back Pain: A Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA internal medicine 2016;176(3):329–337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [55].Morone NE, Greco CM, Weiner DK. Mindfulness meditation for the treatment of chronic low back pain in older adults: a randomized controlled pilot study. Pain 2008;134(3):310–319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [56].Mouraux A, Diukova A, Lee MC, Wise RG, Iannetti GD. A multisensory investigation of the functional significance of the “pain matrix”. NeuroImage 2011;54(3):2237–2249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [57].Mulla SM, Maqbool A, Sivananthan L, Lopes LC, Schandelmaier S, Kamaleldin M, Hsu S, Riva JJ, Vandvik PO, Tsoi L, Lam T, Ebrahim S, Johnston BC, Olivieri L, Montoya L, Kunz R, Scheidecker A, Buckley DN, Sessler DI, Guyatt GH, Busse JW. Reporting of IMMPACT-recommended core outcome domains among trials assessing opioids for chronic non-cancer pain. Pain 2015;156(9):1615–1619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [58].Nicholas MK, Ashton-James C. Embodied pain: grasping a thorny problem? Pain 2017;158(6):993–994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [59].Price DD. Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science 2000;288(5472):1769–1772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [60].Price DD, Barrell JJ, Gracely RH. A psychophysical analysis of experimential factors that selectively influence the affective dimension of pain. Pain 1980;8(2):137–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [61].Price DD, Harkins SW, Baker C. Sensory-affective relationships among different types of clinical and experimental pain. Pain 1987;28(3):297–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [62].Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2001;98(2):676–682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [63].Rainville P, Carrier B, Hofbauer RK, Bushnell MC, Duncan GH. Dissociation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain using hypnotic modulation. Pain 1999;82(2):159–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [64].Rainville P, Feine JS, Bushnell MC, Duncan GH. A psychophysical comparison of sensory and affective responses to four modalities of experimental pain. Somatosensory & motor research 1992;9(4):265–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [65].Salomons TV, Iannetti GD, Liang M, Wood JN. The “Pain Matrix” in Pain-Free Individuals. JAMA neurology 2016;73(6):755–756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [66].Salomons TV, Kucyi A. Does Meditation Reduce Pain through a Unique Neural Mechanism? The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 2011;31(36):12705–12707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [67].Schneider W, Noll DC, Cohen JD. Functional topographic mapping of the cortical ribbon in human vision with conventional MRI scanners. Nature 1993;365(6442):150–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [68].Somogyi AA, Sia AT, Tan EC, Coller JK, Hutchinson MR, Barratt DT. Ethnicity-dependent influence of innate immune genetic markers on morphine PCA requirements and adverse effects in postoperative pain. Pain 2016;157(11):2458–2466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [69].Sormaz M, Murphy C, Wang HT, Hymers M, Karapanagiotidis T, Poerio G, Margulies DS, Jefferies E, Smallwood J. Default mode network can support the level of detail in experience during active task states. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2018;115(37):9318–9323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [70].Tabor A, Keogh E, Eccleston C. Embodied pain-negotiating the boundaries of possible action. Pain 2017;158(6):1007–1011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [71].Tang YY, Holzel BK, Posner MI. The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nature reviews Neuroscience 2015;16(4):213–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [72].Taylor VA, Daneault V, Grant J, Scavone G, Breton E, Roffe-Vidal S, Courtemanche J, Lavarenne AS, Marrelec G, Benali H, Beauregard M. Impact of meditation training on the default mode network during a restful state. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 2013;8(1):4–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [73].Taylor VA, Grant J, Daneault V, Scavone G, Breton E, Roffe-Vidal S, Courtemanche J, Lavarenne AS, Beauregard M. Impact of mindfulness on the neural responses to emotional pictures in experienced and beginner meditators. NeuroImage 2011;57(4):1524–1533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [74].Tsakiris M, Hesse MD, Boy C, Haggard P, Fink GR. Neural signatures of body ownership: a sensory network for bodily self-consciousness. Cerebral cortex 2007;17(10):2235–2244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [75].Turner JA, Anderson ML, Balderson BH, Cook AJ, Sherman KJ, Cherkin DC. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and cognitive behavioral therapy for chronic low back pain: similar effects on mindfulness, catastrophizing, self-efficacy, and acceptance in a randomized controlled trial. Pain 2016;157(11):2434–2444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [76].Vatansever D, Menon DK, Manktelow AE, Sahakian BJ, Stamatakis EA. Default Mode Dynamics for Global Functional Integration. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 2015;35(46):15254–15262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [77].Yarnitsky D, Ochoa JL. Studies of heat pain sensation in man: perception thresholds, rate of stimulus rise and reaction time. Pain 1990;40(1):85–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [78].Yarnitsky D, Simone DA, Dotson RM, Cline MA, Ochoa JL. Single C nociceptor responses and psychophysical parameters of evoked pain: effect of rate of rise of heat stimuli in humans. The Journal of physiology 1992;450:581–592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [79].Zeidan F, Adler-Neal AL, Wells RE, Stagnaro E, May LM, Eisenach JC, McHaffie JG, Coghill RC. Mindfulness-Meditation-Based Pain Relief Is Not Mediated by Endogenous Opioids. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 2016;36(11):3391–3397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [80].Zeidan F, Emerson NM, Farris SR, Ray JN, Jung Y, McHaffie JG, Coghill RC. Mindfulness Meditation-Based Pain Relief Employs Different Neural Mechanisms Than Placebo and Sham Mindfulness Meditation-Induced Analgesia. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 2015;35(46):15307–15325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [81].Zeidan F, Grant JA, Brown CA, McHaffie JG, Coghill RC. Mindfulness meditation-related pain relief: evidence for unique brain mechanisms in the regulation of pain. Neuroscience letters 2012;520(2):165–173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [82].Zeidan F, Martucci KT, Kraft RA, Gordon NS, McHaffie JG, Coghill RC. Brain mechanisms supporting the modulation of pain by mindfulness meditation. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 2011;31(14):5540–5548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [83].Zeidan F, Martucci KT, Kraft RA, McHaffie JG, Coghill RC. Neural correlates of mindfulness meditation-related anxiety relief. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 2014;9(6):751–759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [84].Zeidan F, Salomons T, Farris SR, Emerson NM, Adler-Neal A, Jung Y, Coghill RC. Neural Mechanisms Supporting the Relationship between Dispositional Mindfulness and Pain. Pain 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [85].Zeidan F, Vago DR. Mindfulness meditation-based pain relief: a mechanistic account. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 2016;1373(1):114–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [86].Zeman A Consciousness. Brain : a journal of neurology 2001;124(Pt 7):1263–1289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [87].Zubieta JK, Smith YR, Bueller JA, Xu Y, Kilbourn MR, Jewett DM, Meyer CR, Koeppe RA, Stohler CS. Regional mu opioid receptor regulation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain. Science 2001;293(5528):311–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [88].Zuo N, Song M, Fan L, Eickhoff SB, Jiang T. Different interaction modes for the default mode network revealed by resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging. The European journal of neuroscience 2016;43(1):78–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]