Skip to main content
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health logoLink to International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health
. 2019 Jun 12;16(12):2081. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16122081

A Scoping Review Mapping Research on Green Space and Associated Mental Health Benefits

Charlotte Wendelboe-Nelson 1,*, Sarah Kelly 2, Marion Kennedy 2, John W Cherrie 1,3
PMCID: PMC6616579  PMID: 31212860

Abstract

Background: There is a growing interest in research investigating the association between green space (GS) and mental health and wellbeing (HWB), in order to understand the underlying mechanisms. Accordingly, there is a need to map the literature and create an overview of the research. Methods: A scoping review approach was used to map literature on GS, including context and co-exposures (the GS exposome), and their associations with mental HWB. The review considers mental HWB definitions and measurements and how GS is characterized. Furthermore, the review aims to identify knowledge gaps and make recommendations for future research. Results: We identified a great diversity in study designs, definitions, outcome measures, consideration of the totality of the GS exposome, and reporting of results. Around 70% of the 263 reviewed studies reported a positive association between some aspect of GS and HWB. However, there is a limited amount of research using randomized controlled crossover trails (RCTs) and mixed methods and an abundance of qualitative subjective research. Conclusions: The discords between study designs, definitions, and the reporting of results makes it difficult to aggregate the evidence and identify any potential causal mechanisms. We propose key points to consider when defining and quantifying GS and make recommendations for reporting on research investigating GS and mental HWB. This review highlights a need for large well-designed RCTs that reliably measure the GS exposome in relation to mental HWB.

Keywords: green space, mental health and wellbeing, exposome

1. Introduction

Several reviews have highlighted the positive association between green space (GS) and mental health and wellbeing (HWB). These reviews have generally focused on GS in a narrow sense, such as forest therapy [1,2], community GS [3,4], or urban GS [5,6,7], and a number of reviews have looked at GS in relation to urbanicity and urban planning [8,9]. Other reviews have focused on specific GS activities, such as community gardening [10], horticultural therapy [11,12], therapeutic gardening for the elderly, [13], spending time in a forest [2,14], and GS in the living environment [15]. Reviews have also explored the connections between biodiversity, ecosystem services, and human health and wellbeing [16,17,18]. The reviews generally identify positive associations between the narrowly defined GS investigated and measures of mental HWB.

Design of and access to GS is particularly relevant in cities where GS, among other social and environmental factors, is under pressure due to urbanization [19]. It is estimated that by 2050, more than two-thirds of the world’s population will live in urban areas. This has led to a large number of research studies with a focus on mental HWB and access to urban GS. Urbanization is associated with increased levels of mental illness, including anxiety and depression [20,21,22]. Access to urban GS has been positively associated with mental HWB [23,24], but the underlying reasons for this are still not well-understood.

GS has also been shown to be associated with mental HWB in rural areas [25,26,27,28]. When Gilbert, Colley and Roberts [29] investigated subjective wellbeing in rural areas of Scotland, they found that residents living in remote rural areas reported higher levels of life satisfaction compared with non-rural areas. Other studies investigating associations between mental HWB and GS in rural areas have found a significant relationship with rurality [30,31].

Botanical gardens have been proposed as alternative ways to stay in touch with nature [32,33,34]. A number of studies have shown a positive relation between garden/horticultural therapy and a number of psychological issues, e.g., stress management [35,36,37,38], treatment of depression [39,40,41], rehabilitation of prison inmates [42], and wellbeing among elders [43,44,45].

There is increasing interest in understanding the factors that may make GS beneficial for HWB [46]. However, most reviews do not consider contextual factors, such as culture and accessibility, or co-exposures, such as sound and light. The developing concept of the exposome [47] encompasses the totality of exposures we face as humans, from conception onwards, and the combined effect of these exposures on HWB. An exposome approach to investigating GS could help us understand exactly what is beneficial for mental HWB.

We have carried out a scoping study to map the available literature on different types of GS, including the context and co-exposures, and their associations with mental HWB, considering how mental HWB is defined and measured and how GS is characterized. Furthermore, the review aims to identify any current knowledge gaps and make recommendations for future research on the subject.

2. Materials and Methods

A five-step scoping review methodology was used to collect, evaluate, and present the analysed literature [48]:

  • Identifying the research question(s);

  • Identifying relevant studies;

  • Study selection;

  • Charting the data;

  • Collating, summarizing, and reporting the results.

The following research questions (RQ) were used to underpin the search strategy:

  1. How do different types of GS (recreational, residential, urban, rural) affect HWB and how much green space is needed for health improvement?

  2. How can we best define, measure, and quantify GS and mental HWB?

  3. Do different co-exposures or contextual factors affect the mental HWB outcome?

  4. Do different age groups and population subgroups benefit differently from exposure to GS?

Theoretical, empirical, and experimental studies were included, with a focus on links between GS of any description and mental HWB of any definition. To our knowledge, no review has attempted to map the totality of literature on GS and the associated effects on mental HWB. In this scoping review, we adopt a wide definition of GS and GS activities, including small urban pockets of GS, remote rural areas, horticultural therapy, allotment gardening, and virtual green space. This was done to try to shed light on the effect of contextual factors and co-exposures potentially influencing the effects of GS on mental HWB.

Studies with a main emphasis on biological diversity or physical activity, not including a detailed investigation of associated mental HWB outcomes, were excluded. Studies focusing on children under the age of 18 were excluded, as the mechanisms and contextual factors related to mental HWB may be different in children than in adults. In situations where the age range of participants included people under the age of 18, a decision to include or exclude the paper was based on each individual study, considering the contribution the study findings and conclusions would make to this review. Studies with an emphasis on GS in war or disaster zones were excluded, as these are extreme circumstances and not applicable to the general population. Studies with a focus on urban design, not investigating any associated mental HWB outcomes, were also excluded. Only peer-reviewed literature was included, and grey literature and all conference proceedings, abstracts, or opinion pieces were excluded. Keywords for two main concepts were generated and used for the literature search (Table 1).

Table 1.

Concept 1: Green space. Concept 2: Mental health and wellbeing.

Concept 1
Alternative Terms/Synonyms (combined with OR) “green space*”, green*, “green environ*”, “green infrastruct*”, outdoor*, “outdoor experience*”, “nature experience*”, “natural space*”, “natural infrastruct*” “green health*”
Broader terms (combined with OR) “wilderness experience*”, “adventure therapy”, “outdoor therapy”, “nature therapy”, “nature connect*”, “near nature*”, ecotherap*, eco-therap*, “eco therap”, “green therap*”, “green-therap*”, “green exercis*”, “green-exercis*”
Narrower terms (combined with OR) ecopsychology, eco-psychology, “eco psychology”, “environmental psychology”, park, parks, forest*, horticultur*, “horticulture therap*”, garden*, allotment*, landscap*, highland*, wasteland*
Concept 2
Alternative Terms/Synonyms (combined with OR) “mental wellbeing”, “mental well-being”, “mental well being”, “mental health”, “emotional wellbeing”, “emotional well-being”, “emotional well being”, “emotional health”, “psychological wellbeing”, “psychological well-being”, “psychological well being”, “psychological health”
Broader terms (combined with OR) “self-concept”, “self concept”, “self-esteem”, “self esteem”, “self-image”, “self image”, “sense of coherence”, “sense of personal control”, “social wellbeing”, “social well-being”, “social well being”, “psychological issue*”, ruminat*, restorative
Narrower terms (combined with OR) “well-being”, wellbeing, “well being” “quality of life”, “life satisfaction”, emotion, depress*, anxi*, stress*, fear*, frustrate*, agress*, lonely, loneliness, isolation, happy, happiness, resilien*, optimis*, hope*, empower*

Relevant studies and literature reviews from peer-reviewed journals were identified using Web of Science, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, PubMed, Scopus, GreenFILE, and SPORTDiscus. Additionally, research evidence was sought from topic-related networks and relevant organizations, and reference lists of earlier key studies were used to detect relevant publications not identified in the original main search [48].

All papers were pooled and duplicates removed, resulting in a total of 7042 papers. The literature was initially screened by two members of the research team (CWN, JC), using a comparative and consensus orientated method (Figure 1). After exclusion based on the title and abstract, there were 417 papers for review. When applying the inclusion/exclusion criteria, another 173 papers were excluded. An additional 19 papers were included from the reference lists from key-papers, taking the total papers for review to 263.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Article screening and selection process.

The included literature was charted following the technique described by Arksey and O’Malley [47], to synthesize and interpret the studies by sorting them according to key issues and themes. Each study was analysed according to the type of GS investigated, health outcomes and measures, experimental design, and methods used. The quality of the included studies was not systematically assessed, so this review does not determine the robustness of findings from the included literature. The reviewed literature was then collated, summarized, and reported in four thematic groups (Table 2).

Table 2.

The literature was divided into thematic groups based on the type of GS investigated (literature reviews are not included).

Type of Green Space
Group 1 Horticulture, garden, allotment (n = 43)
Group 2 Urban and mixed green space (n = 140)
Group 3 Wild, natural or rural green space (n = 34)
Group 4 Virtual or indoor green space (n = 24)

The literature was further divided into ‘type of study’ (cross-sectional or longitudinal, controlled trial, randomized or non-randomized, with or without crossover); ‘methods’ (what methods have been used to measure mental HWB and GS. Quantitative or qualitative data collection methods); ‘health outcome’ (the type of mental HWB assessed); and whether the study has reached a positive or negative conclusion (were initial hypothesis proven right or wrong). Comprehensive lists were generated, comprising all the different mental HWB outcomes investigated and all the different tools used to assess the health outcomes. This was done to get an overview of the totality and complexity of studies, and to identify the most commonly used methods for assessing mental HWB.

3. Results

3.1. Numerical Analysis

This analysis is used to highlight the dominant areas of research with respect to the study design, type of participants, methods used, main conclusions, and country where the study has been conducted. The papers were divided into groups based on the study design (Table 3). The majority of studies were cross-sectional (86.3%), with only 13 studies being longitudinal (4.9%). There were nine studies with a Randomized Controlled Trial (RCT) study design with a crossover element and 21 studies using an RCT study design without a crossover element.

Table 3.

The included studies were divided into groups based on their study design (some papers are represented in more than one group, i.e., a cross-sectional study with an RCT design).

Type of Study # of Studies
Cross-sectional 227
Longitudinal 13
Review 22
Historic, secondary narrative analysis 1
Total 263
RCT with crossover 9
RCT, no crossover 21
Non-randomized CT, cross over 2
Non-randomized CT, no crossover 6

The majority of studies used only qualitative methods 212 (80.6%), with only 29 studies using a combination of qualitative and quantitative data collection methods (11%). Twenty-two of the publications were reviews (8.4%).

Different countries will face different co-exposures and contextual factors, which may potentially affect the HWB outcomes in different ways (RQ 3). To understand the representation from around the world, the literature was charted according to the continent where the study took place (Table 4). The majority of studies were conducted in Europe (46.8%), followed by North America (24.3%), Asia (11%), and Australia (6.8%). Most of the studies conducted in Europe were from western and northern parts; the UK (38%), followed by Sweden (15.4%) and the Netherlands (6.5%). Based on Table 4, it is evident that a majority of studies have been carried out in the developed part of the world. The identified benefits of GS in developed countries may not be applicable to less developed countries. The same is the case between temperate and tropical areas, with most studies being carried out in the former.

Table 4.

The studies were grouped, based on the continent where the study took place (not including reviews, n = 22).

Continent # of Studies
North America 64
South America 3
Asia 29
South Africa 1
New Zealand 3
Australia 18
Europe 123
 -east 5
 -west 56
 -north 38
 -south 6
 -central 14
 -across regions 4
Total 241

Different population subgroups might benefit differently from exposure to GS (RQ 4). To investigate what population subgroups have typically been used to assess the effects of GS on mental HWB, the literature was sorted according to participant type (Table 5). For ease of overview, the different participant types have been grouped together where reasonable overlap and similarity was identified. The most common type of study participant was the general public (30% of all included studies), followed by university and college students (14.1%) and individuals with mental health issues and disorders (12.2%). There is a long list of studies that have used more specific participant types, i.e., park users, allotment gardeners, adults with burnout syndrome, depression, mental health issues, female prisoners, woodland workers, people building their own houses etc. Therefore, despite the majority of papers focusing on the general public, there is a great variety of specific population subgroups being investigated in relation to the health benefits of various types of GS exposure (Table 5).

Table 5.

The literature was charted based on the type of participant included in the study.

Participant Type # of Studies
General public, parents, twins 79
University students, undergraduates, college students, students, graduate students, university students, healthy and physically inactive, male university students, pupils 37
Psychiatric patients, individuals with clinical depression, mental health patients, stress-related mental health patients, adults with depression, adults with increased psychological stress, adults with mental health issues, individuals with burnout, exhaustion disorder, individual with stress, patients with depression, individuals with burnout, diagnosed with depression/anxiety/stress, females diagnosed with exhaustion disorder, mental disorder clients, individuals with stress injuries, people with mental health problems, people with significant mental ill-health, people diagnosed with chronic mental illness 32
Older adults, over 65’s, elderly women 15
Office workers, science park employees, university office staff, employees, workers 11
Park users, allotment gardeners, recreational walkers, botanical garden visitors, forest users, GS users, greenway trail users, forest users/volunteers 18
Female healthcare workers, health care workers, caregivers, rehabilitation team members, practitioners/decision-makers, public sector employees 6
Athletes, physically active, active runners, experienced runners 6
Dementia sufferers, cancer patients, palliative care patients, individuals with hypertension, chronic stroke patients 7
Deprived communities, vulnerable, homeless women, female prisoners, deprived urban neighborhoods 9
Rural elders, rural population, local residents (predominantly farmers), local residents (farmers and visitors) 4
Adults with disabilities, individuals with disabilities, individuals with learning difficulties, people with disability 4
Postmenopausal women, pregnant women, women 4
Tourists, experienced physically fit backpackers 3
Forest workers, woodland workers 3
African Americans 1
People building houses 1
Alcoholics 1

Studies were charted as ‘positive’ if the hypotheses were confirmed, ‘negative’ if the main hypotheses were not confirmed, and ‘mixed’ if the hypotheses were only partly confirmed. Note that a study charted as ‘negative’ does not necessarily mean the study found a negative effect of GS exposure on mental HWB. Only 4.6% of studies were charted as negative (see e.g., [49,50]), 25.7% of studies were charted as ‘mixed’, and 70.1% of studies were charted as ‘positive’. It should be noted that a proportion of the studies report a positive finding in the abstract, but when investigating the results in more detail, we found that any mixed or negative findings were played down in the summary. The percentages presented here are based on the abstracts.

3.2. Thematic Analysis

The literature has been organized according to thematic groups to address research question 1. There were 22 literature reviews identified, which are not included in this thematic analysis. An in-depth evaluation of these is beyond the scope of this review.

Group 1 encompasses studies focusing on horticulture, gardens, and allotments (43 studies). Included in this group is garden or horticultural therapy, and access and use of private and public gardens. The literature in this group was further divided into seven categories: private gardens (4.6% of Group 1 studies), complex interventions (14%), allotments (18.6%), horticultural therapy (30.2%), occupation (4.7%), public gardens (18.6%), and community gardening (9.3%). Out of these 43 studies, only seven had a quantitative element [45,51,52,53,54,55,56]. A range of quantitative measurements were used, such as salivary alpha amylase (sAA) levels, an electrocardiogram (ECG), a surface electromyogram (sEMG), a respiration rate, body composition, physical functional ability, hand function ability, BMI, cortisol, sick leave status, and healthcare consumption.

Group 2 encompasses studies focusing on urban GS or mixed GS (140 studies). Included in this group was any GS located in an urban setting, and studies that used a mixture of GS types where it was not possible to assign the study to one of the other groups and where there was a main focus on urbanicity. This group is large and very diverse and for many of the studies, it was difficult to categorize and determine exactly what type of GS was being investigated, due to the lack of details used to describe the space. It was therefore not practical to further divide this group into subgroups in a meaningful way. Out of the 140 studies focusing on urban green space, 130 were cross-sectional and 10 were longitudinal, 125 studies used qualitative methods, and 15 used quantitative or mixed methods.

Group 3 encompasses wild, natural, and rural GS (34). This group includes GS types such as care farms; adventure therapy; rural neighborhoods; and wild nature like mountains, national parks, beaches, and large forests. Due to the diversity of the investigated GS, this group was further divided into eight subgroups: care farms (5.9%), forest GS (29.4%), natural green exercise (2.9%), nature connectedness and restorativeness (8.8%), nature interventions (17.6%), occupational (5.9%), rural communities (11.8%), and wild camping and adventures (17.6%). Out of these 34 studies, 32 were cross-sectional, one was longitudinal, and one study was a secondary narrative analysis. Qualitative data collection methods were used in 27 of the studies, with only seven of the studies using quantitative or mixed methods [1,57,58,59,60,61,62]. A range of objective quantitative data collection methods were used, such as cortisol measurements, cytokine serum levels, blood pressure, and heart rate variability.

The last group, Group 4 (24), includes virtual and indoor GS, e.g., photos, images, videos, and any type of GS enclosed under a roof. This group can be further subdivided into four groups: assessment by questionnaire only and no exposure to GS (50%), indoor GS exposure (8.3%), video of GS (4.2%), and images or photos of GS (37.5%). None of the studies in this group included a quantitative element; 22 studies relied on questionnaire data and two studies have used interviews. Out of the 24 studies, 23 were cross-sectional, with only one study being described as longitudinal [63]. Erikson, Westerberg and Jonsson [63] investigated a therapeutic gardening program taking place in a greenhouse; however, the longitudinal aspect of the study only stretched over three months.

The type of health outcome investigated varied greatly between the included studies. The total number of primary mental HWB outcomes observed and the number of times each outcome has been investigated were summarized (Table 6). Mental health (37), wellbeing (35), and stress (34) were the most used mental HWB outcomes. These were followed by restorativeness (22), depression (19), quality of life (13), psychological wellbeing (12), general health (11), and mental wellbeing (8). It is likely that some of these outcomes are intended to cover the same aspect of mental HWB. However, as a clear definition of the health outcome is rarely presented, it is not possible to confidently and accurately combine these outcomes and group them into fewer groups.

Table 6.

The studies were grouped according to the primary mental HWB outcome investigated in the study. Some studies investigate more than one primary outcome.

Primary Mental HWB Outcome # of Times Used
Mental health 37
Wellbeing 35
Stress 34
Restorativeness 20
Depression 19
Quality of life 13
Psychological wellbeing 12
General health 11
Mental wellbeing 8
Life satisfaction 6
Aggression 4
Affect 3
General wellbeing 3
Anxiety, cognition, emotion, happiness, mood, psychological distress, self-esteem, stress reduction 16 (2 papers for each of the health endpoints)
Chronic stress, clinical depression, emotional wellbeing, general preference for GS, health anxiety, job stress, mental stress, personal development, psychological health, psychological restoration, psychological stress, rumination, severe stress, social integration, stress-related mental illness, stress restoration, stressful life events 17 (1 paper for each of the health endpoints)

The number of tools used to measure mental HWB and the number of times each tool has been used are summarized in Table 7. Despite the availability of a vast range of validated tools developed to investigate mental HWB, the most common approach was to develop new questionnaires (DOQ; 15.8% of the studies). The most used validated questionnaire was PRS (7.9%), closely followed by PANAS (7.1%), PSS (6.6%), GHQ (6.2%), PS (5.8%), WEMWBS (5.4%), and HS SF-36 (4.1%), and the abbreviations are listed in Table 7.

Table 7.

An overview of the tools used to measure mental HWB and the number of times each tool has been used (where the available primary reference for each tool is added in brackets).

Abbreviation Health Outcome Measure # of Times Used
DOQ Developed own questions and questionnaires 38
PRS Perceived Restoration Scale [64,65] 19
PANAS Positive and Negative Affect Schedule [66] 17
PSS Perceived Stress Scale [67] 16
GHQ General Health Questionnaire [68] 15
PS Population survey with incorporated health and wellbeing assessments 14
WEMWBS Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale [69] 13
HS SF-36 Health Survey (SF-36) [70] 10
BDI Beck Depression Inventory [71,72,73]. 9
POMS Profile of Mood States [74,75,76] 8
CN Connected to nature [77] 8
STAI State-Trait Anxiety Inventory [78] 8
CES-D Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale for Research in the general population [79] 7
NCPC Necker Cube Pattern Control [80] 6
RSE Rosenberg self-esteem scale [81] 5
K10 Kessler Psychological Distress Scale [82] 5
SWLS Satisfaction with Life Scale [83] 4
WHOQOL WHO Quality of Life Questionnaire [84] 4
DASS Depression Anxiety Stress Scale [85] 4
PHQ Patient Health Questionnaire [86] 4
SMBQ Shirom-Melmed Burnout Questionnaire [87] 4
SVS Subjective Vitality Scale [88] 4
GDS Geriatric Depression Scale [89] 4
HS SF-12 Health Survey (SF-12) [90,91] 3
EQ-5D EuroQol-5Dimensions [92] 3
INS Inclusion of Nature in Self scale [93] 3
ICD The International Classification of Diseases (WHO) 3
PWB Psychological Wellbeing Scale [94] 2
QPS QPSNordic-ADW; Nordic Questionnaire for Monitoring the Age Diverse Workforce [95] 3
BAI Beck Anxiety Inventory [96] 2
MHI-5 Mental Health Inventory [70,97] 2
MMSE Mini-Mental state examination (Folstein test) [98] 2
REQ Recovery Experience Questionnaire [99] 2
RRQ Rumination-Reflection Questionnaire [100] 2
UWES Utrecht Work Engagement Scale [101] 2
MAAS Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale [102] 2
FS Feeling Scale, affective valence assessed by the FS [103] 2
HAM-17 Hamilton Depression Rating Scale [104] 2
IPA Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis [105] 2
SHCI Subjective Health Complaints Inventory [106] 2
LSIA Life satisfaction inventory A [107] 2
SPNE Scale of Positive and Negative Experience [108] 2
ZIPERS Inventory of Personal Reactions, measuring affect [109] 2
PAQ Place attachment questionnaire [110,111] 2
MSS Mood Survey Scale [112] 1
GSES General Self-efficacy Scale [113] 1
CRC-QOL Instrument developed by [114,115] to measure quality of life 1
TPI Trier Personality Inventory [116] 1
ABS Affect Balance Scale [117] 1
AFI Attentional function index [118] 1
BM Behaviour mapping [119] 1
BRFSS Behavioural Risk Factor Surveillance System. (A United States health survey that looks at behavioural risk factors. It is run by Centre for Disease Control and Prevention and conducted by the individual state health departments and is the world’s largest such survey). 1
BF Big Five [120] 1
BSI Brief Symptom Inventory (anxiety) [121] 1
BS Brooding Scale (Rumination) [122] 1
BPAQ Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire [123] 1
CMAI Cohen-Mansfield agitation inventory [124] 1
CSAI-2 Competitive state anxiety inventory-2 [125] 1
CD-RS Connor-Davidson resilience scale [126] 1
CS-DD Cornell scale for depression in Dementia [127] 1
DSI Daily Stress Inventory [128] 1
DEMQOL Dementia quality of life instrument [129] 1
SCL-90-R Symptom Check List [130] 1
ES-SF Ecology Scale, Short-Form [131] 1
EPDS Edinburgh postnatal Depression Scale [132] 1
EES Elevating Experience Scale [133] 1
EFI Exercise-Induced Feeling Inventory [134,135] 1
FAS Felt Arousal Scale [136] 1
ES Ecocentrism scale. Use of natural environments for psychological restoration [137] 1
GEBS General Ecological Behaviour Scale [138] 1
MUNSH Happiness Scale based on Memorial University of Newfoundland Scale of happiness [139] 1
Urban HEART-2 Health Equity Assessment and Response Tool-2 (Urban HEART-2) (http://sdh.umin.jp/heart/) 1
HAD Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale [140] 1
HPLP-II Health promoting Lifestyle Profile II [141] 1
ISS Importance for Survival Scale [142] 1
IWG-2006 International Wellbeing Group 2006. Used to evaluate self-reported, subjective well-being 1
ISEL Interpersonal Support Evaluation List [143] 1
JSS-N Job Stress Survey [144] 1
MDI Major Depression inventory [145,146] 1
MANSA Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of life [147] 1
MC-SDS Marlow-Crowne Social Desirability Scale [148] 1
MBI-GS Korean version of Maslach Burnout Inventory-General Survey [149] 1
MINI Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview [150,151] 1
MDBF Multidimensional Comfort Questionnaire [152] 1
MMS-SF Multiple Mood Scale-Short form [153] 1
NCQ Nature Contact Questionnaire [154] 1
NMS Negative Mood Scale [155] 1
OHI Oxford Happiness Inventory [156] 1
OHS Overall Happiness scale [157] 1
PGIS Personal Growth Initiative Scale [158] 1
PPWB Physical and Psychological Wellbeing questionnaire [159] 1
PGWB Psychological General Well-Being Index [160] 1
PRQOL Influence of parks and recreation on quality of life [161,162] 1
QLCELQ Quality of Life Concern in End of Life Questionnaire [163,164] 1
QOLI Quality of Life Inventory (Frisch, 2009). [165] 1
QLS Quality of Life Scale [166] 1
QLS-ACI Quality of Life Scale in adults with chronic illness [165] 1
QOLT Quality of Life tool [167] 1
QEWB Questionnaire for Eudaimonic Well-Being [168] 1
QEACL Questionnaires measuring Eudemonia, Apprehension, childhood location. Environments and experiential states. Eliciting participants feelings about place [65] 1
MOS SF-20 Rand medical Outcomes Study Health survey (MOS SF-20) [169,170] 1
RVP Reason for Visiting the Park, 23-item scale [171,172,173] 1
REP Recreation Experience Preference scales [174] 1
ROS Restorative Outcome Scale [175,176] 1
RQE Restorative quality in environments [65] 1
RCAS Role conflict and ambiguity scales [177] 1
SMS Sense of Meaning Scale [178] 1
SCI-93 Stress and crisis inventory [179,180] 1
SEES Subjective Exercise Experiences Scale [181] 1
BSCS Self-Control Scale [182] 1
SRRS Self-rating restoration scale [183] 1
SRSA Self-reported stress and arousal [184] 1
SOC Sense of coherence [185] 1
SHAI Short Health Anxiety Inventory [186] 1
SI-happy Measuring happiness with a single-item scale [187] 1
SCTS Social Cohesion and Trust Scale [188] 1
SPS Social Provisions Scale [189] 1
SWS Stress at Work Scale by the Behavioural Science Institute, Korea university (1999), occupational stress 1
SRI-MF Stress response inventory-modified form [190] 1
SRS-18 Stress Response Scale [191] 1
TAP Taylor Aggression Paradigm [192] 1
TMM Model of mood [193] 1
CMMS Current mood measurement scale (The best/worst ever; scale taken from [194] 1
TFI-CS Therapeutic Factors Inventory–Cohesiveness Scale [195] 1
VQ Volitional Questionnaire [196] 1
WSRI Workers Stress Response Inventory; an extended version of the Stress Response Inventory-Modified from [190,197] 1
WOS Workplace Ostracism Scale [198] 1
WUS Wildernism-Urbanism Scale [199] 1
ZSDS Zung self-rating depression scale [200] 1

Table 8 gives an overview of the different tools and what health endpoints the tools have been used to investigate. Some studies have not used tools such as questionnaires, surveys, scales, or inventories, but have instead used interviews, focus groups, observations, or similar methodologies. These studies are not included in Table 8. To measure the ten most used health endpoints (Table 6), the following tools have been most frequently used (Table 9): The GHQ was used in 15.4% of studies investigating mental health; 11.9% of studies developed their own questionnaires (DOQ) when investigating wellbeing; the PSS was used in 13.6% of studies investigating stress; the PRS was used in 32.4% of studies investigating restorativeness; 13.9% of studies used BDI when investigating depression; 16.7% of the studies developed their own questionnaires (DOQ) when investigating quality of life; 8.7% of studies DOQ when investigation psychological wellbeing; the GHQ was used in 17.6% of studies investigating general health; the WEMWBS was used in 21.4% of studies investigating mental wellbeing; and 75% of studies DOQ when investigating life satisfaction. Some studies have used several tools to measure one health endpoint.

Table 8.

Health outcome measure; the most commonly used tools included in studies assessing the associations between GS and mental HWB.

Paper Number Primary Health Outcome Health Outcome Measure
1 [201] Affect BDI, SCL-90-R,
2 [202] Affect STAI, RRQ, PANAS
3 [203] Affect QEACL
4 [204] Aggression WOS
5 [205] Aggression TAP, BPAQ, BSCS, PRS; PANAS
6 [206] Anxiety CSAI-2,
7 [207] Anxiety STAI
8 [208] Chronic stress PSS
9 [209] Depression BDI
10 [210] Depression PHQ
11 [211] Depression BRFSS, PHQ
12 [60] Depression BDI, HAM-17, STAI
13 [212] Depression DOQ
14 [39] Depression BDI, AFI, BS, PRS
15 [213] Depression BDI, STAI, PANAS, PSS, TFI-CS
16 [214] Depression IBD, ROS, WEMWBS
17 [215] Depression GDS
18 [216] Depression GDS
19 [217] Depression GDS
20 [218] Depression GHQ
21 [219] Depression CES-D, DOQ
22 [220] Depression MINI, ICD, PSS, WHOQOL
23 [221] Depression EPDS
24 [222] Depression ZSDS
25 [223] Depression CES-D
26 [224] Depression BDI
27 [225] Depression CES-D
28 [226] Depression PHQ
29 [227] Emotion POMS
30 [228] Emotional wellbeing Urban HEART-2
31 [229] General health GHQ
32 [230] General health GHQ, POPS
33 [231] General health HS SF-36, PSS, DOQ
34 [232] General health HS SF-36, PSS
35 [233] General health POMS, PANAS, ROS, SVS
36 [24] General health POPS, HS SF-36, GHQ,
37 [234] General health POPS
38 [235] General health HS SF-12,
39 [236] General wellbeing RVP, RQE, SWLS, SPNE
40 [237] General wellbeing OHI
41 [28] General wellbeing EQ-5D, GHQ, DOQ, RSE, POMS
42 [238] Happiness SWLS, PANAS
43 [239] Health anxiety SHAI
44 [240] Job stress SWS
45 [241] Life satisfaction DOQ
46 [242] Life satisfaction DOQ
47 [243] Life satisfaction DOQ
48 [244] Life Satisfaction LSIA
49 [245] Mental health GHQ
50 [25] Mental health GHQ
51 [246] Mental health HS SF-36, K10
52 [247] Mental health DOQ, CES-D, BAI, RCAS
53 [248] Mental health GHQ
54 [249] Mental health POPS, GHQ
55 [1] Mental health HPLP-II, BDI
56 [250] Mental health DASS
57 [251] Mental health DASS, MANSA
58 [252] Mental health PHQ, PSS, BSI
59 [253] Mental health DASS,
60 [254] Mental health PANAS, RSE
61 [255] Mental health GHQ
62 [256] Mental health CES-D
63 [257] Mental health PANAS, HAM-17
64 [258] Mental health DOQ
65 [259] Mental health PS, GHQ, WEMWBS
66 [260] Mental health PSQ, GSES, MAAS
67 [261] Mental health PS
68 [262] Mental health IPA
69 [41] Mental health VQ
70 [263] Mental health RSE, PSS, POMS
71 [264] Mental health MHI-5
72 [265] Mental health MHI-5
73 [266] Mental health HS SF-12,
74 [267] Mental health PS, K10,
75 [268] Mental health HS SF-36, DOQ,
76 [269] Mental health HS SF-36
77 [270] Mental health GHQ
78 [271] Mental health DASS
79 [272] Mental health GHQ, DOQ
80 [273] Mental health WEMEBS, HS SF-12
81 [274] Mental health IPA
82 [275] Mental health WEMWBS
83 [52] Mental stress SRS-18
84 [276] Mental wellbeing PS, HS SF-36, K10, BF, DOQ
85 [277] Mental wellbeing REP, PAQ
86 [278] Mental wellbeing DOQ, WEMWBS,
87 [279] Mental wellbeing PSS, WEMWBS
88 [280] Mental wellbeing WEMWBS
89 [281] Mental wellbeing QOLI, BDI
90 [282] Mood RSE, TMD
91 [283] Mood TBES, DOQ
92 [284] Personal development PGIS, QLS-ACI
93 [285] Psychological distress K10
94 [286] Psychological distress DOQ
95 [54] Psychological health MMSE, GDS, PS
96 [287] Psychological restoration ES
97 [288] Psychological stress PRS, CN
98 [289] Psychological wellbeing ABS
99 [290] Psychological wellbeing FS, FAS, MSS
100 [291] Psychological wellbeing PRS, PANAS, PSS, CES-D, MUNSH, SPW
101 [292] Psychological wellbeing DOQ
102 [293] Psychological wellbeing CN, WHOQOL
103 [294] Psychological wellbeing STAI, PWB
104 [295] Psychological wellbeing SRSMS
105 [55] Psychological wellbeing DOQ, GHQ, SCTS, SHCI
106 [296] Psychological wellbeing CN, MAAS, FS, SPNE, SVS
107 [297] Quality of life PRQOL
108 [298] Quality of life EQ-5D
109 [299] Quality of life QLCELQ
110 [300] Quality of life CRC-QOL
111 [301] Quality of life DOQ
112 [302] Quality of life LSIA
113 [61] Quality of life QOLT
114 [303] Quality of life DOQ
115 [62] Quality of life QLS
116 [43] Quality of life DEMQOL, CS-DD, CMAI, MMSE
117 [304] Recovery REQ, DOQ
118 [305] Restorativeness EFI, NMS,
119 [306] Restorativeness POMS, PRS, NCPC
120 [50] Restorativeness ZIPERS, NCPC
121 [50] Restorativeness SRRS
122 [307] Restorativeness WUS, ZIPERS, OHS
123 [308] Restorativeness DOQ, PRS
124 [59] Restorativeness DOQ, SRSA
125 [309] Restorativeness NCPC, STAI
126 [310] Restorativeness PRS, SMBQ, HAD, NCPC, DOQ
127 [142] Restorativeness ISS, PRS,
128 [311] Restorativeness DOQ
129 [33] Restorativeness PRS
130 [312] Restorativeness PRS, INS
130 [313] Restorativeness PRS, GEBS, MC-SDS,
131 [314] Restorativeness WHOQOL, PRS,
132 [315] Restorativeness DOQ
133 [316] Restorativeness PRS
134 [317] Restorativeness DOQ
135 [318] Restorativeness PRS
135 [306] Restorativeness POMS, PRS, NCPC
136 [319] Rumination RRQ
137 [320] Self-esteem DOQ
138 [321] Self-esteem RSE, POMS, GHQ
139 [63] Stress ICD, SMBQ
140 [322] Stress PS
141 [323] Stress DOQ
142 [53] Stress PSS, SPS, HS SF-36
143 [57] Stress SRI-MF
144 [58] Stress MBI-GS, WSRI, REQ
145 [32] Stress CES-D
146 [324] Stress PSQ
147 [154] Stress PSQ, BRFSS
148 [325] Stress DOQ
149 [326] Stress MMS-SF, STAI
150 [327] Stress DSI, MOS SF-20
151 [328] Stress PSS, WEMWBS
152 [329] Stress PSS, WEMWBS, PS
153 [37] Stress DOQ, SMBQ
154 [330] Stress ICD, BM
155 [331] Stress SCI-93, EQ-5D,
156 [332] Stress PSS, WEMWBS, PS
157 [333] Stress PSS, WEMWBS, PS
158 [334] Stress ROS, PRS, PANAS
159 [335] Stress PANAS
160 [333] Stress PSS, WEMWBS, PS
161 [336] Stress reduction PANAS, NCPC
162 [337] Stress reduction NCQ, QPS, JSS-N, SHCI, DOQ
163 [36] Stress related mental illness SMBQ, BDI, BAI, PGWB
164 [338] Stress restoration TMM
165 [56] Stress restoration PS
166 [246] Wellbeing HS SF-36, DOQ
167 [339] Wellbeing HS SF-36, K10
168 [340] Wellbeing PS
169 [45] Wellbeing DOQ
170 [341] Wellbeing PRS
171 [342] Wellbeing MDBF, SWLS, TPI, HS SF-36
172 [343] Wellbeing SVS, UWES, QPS
173 [344] Wellbeing SVS, UWES, QPS
174 [345] Wellbeing DOQ
175 [346] Wellbeing PS
176 [347] Wellbeing WEMWBS, CD-RS, SOC, PS, DOQ
177 [348] Wellbeing CN, PANAS, SEES
178 [349] Wellbeing DOQ, IWG-2006, CN
179 [350] Wellbeing MDI, PSS, PANAS, WEMWBS, ISEL
180 [351] Wellbeing PWB, PANAS, SWLS, ES-SF
181 [352] Wellbeing PANAS, EES, SMS, CN
182 [353] Wellbeing DOQ, OHI, INS
183 [354] Wellbeing CN, PRS, PAQ, PANAS, PPWB
184 [355] Wellbeing STAI, PRS
185 [356] Wellbeing CN, QEWB, WHOQOL
186 [357] Wellbeing DOQ
187 [358] Wellbeing PANAS, INS

Table 9.

Overview of the ten most used health endpoints and the tools most commonly used to assess them – the number next to the tool is the number of studies where it was used.

Mental Health Wellbeing Stress Restorativeness Depression Quality of Life Psychological Wellbeing General Health Mental Wellbeing Life Satisfaction
GHQ 8 DOQ 7 PSS 6 PRS 11 BDI 5 DOQ 2 DOQ 2 GHQ 3 WEMWBS 3 DOQ 3
DASS 4 PANAS 6 PS 5 DOQ 6 CES-D 3 CS-DD 1 CN 2 POPS 3 DOQ 2 LSIA 1
DOQ 4 CN 5 WEMWBS 5 NCPC 5 GDS 3 CMAI 1 FS 2 HS SF-36 3 BDI 1
HS SF-36 3 HS SF-36 3 DOQ 3 POMS 2 PHQ 3 CRC-QOL 1 ABS 1 PSS 2 BF 1
PS 3 PRS 3 ICD 2 ZIPERS 2 DOQ 2 DEMQOL 1 CES-D 1 DOQ 1 HS SF-36 1
RSE 2 PS 3 PANAS 2 EFI 1 PSS 2 EQ-5D 1 FAS 1 HS SF-12 1 K10 1
WEMWBS 2 INS 2 PSQ 2 HAD 1 PHQ 2 LSIA 1 GHQ 1 PANAS 1 PAQ 1
CES-D 2 QPS 2 SMBQ 2 ISS 1 STAI 2 MMSE 1 MSS 1 POMS 1 PS 1
HS SF-12 2 SVS 2 BM 1 NMS 1 AFI 1 QLCELQ 1 MUNSH 1 ROS 1 PSS 1
IPA 2 SWLS 2 BRFSS 1 SMBQ 1 BRFSS 1 QLS 1 PANAS 1 SVS 1 QOLI 1
K10 2 UWES 2 CES-D 1 SRRS 1 BS 1 QOLT 1 PRS 1 REP 1
MHI-5 2 WEMWBS 2 DSI 1 SRSA 1 EPDS 1 PSS 1
PANAS 2 CD-RS 1 EQ-5D 1 STAI 1 GHQ 1 PWB 1
PSS 2 EES 1 HS SF-36 1 IBD 1 SCTS 1
BAI 1 ES-SF 1 MBI-GS 1 ICD 1 SHCI 1
POMS 1 ISEL 1 MMS-SF 1 MINI 1 SPW 1
BSI 1 IWG-2006 1 MOS SF-20 1 PANAS 1 SRSMS 1
GSES 1 K10 1 PRS 1 PRS 1 STAI 1
HPLP-II 1 MDBF 1 REQ 1 ROS 1 SVS 1
MANSA 1 MDI 1 ROS 1 WEMWBS 1 WHOQOL 1
MAAS 1 OHI 1 SCI-93 1 WHOQOL 1
PHQ 1 PAQ 1 SPS 1 ZSDS 1
POPS 1 PPWB 1 SRI-MF 1
PSQ 1 PSS 1 STAI 1
RCAS 1 PWB 1 WSRI 1
VQ 1 QEWB 1
SEES 1
SMS 1
SOC 1
STAI 1
TPI 1
WHOQOL 1

3.3. Analysis of Study Design

When testing a research hypothesis, an RCT is the most scientifically rigorous method available [359]. In an RCT, the participants are randomly assigned to one of at least two groups; a design that specifically reduces selection bias and is often considered the gold standard for research designs, when considering the efficacy of different treatments compared to a control.

There were 30 RCTs identified; 11.4% of the total number of papers selected for review. Nine (30%) of these studies included a crossover element: eight had a 2-arm design and one study had a 4-arm design. Out of the 21 RCT without a crossover element, ten had a 2-arm design, seven had a 3-arm design, and four had a 4-arm design. Eight studies used a non-randomized Controlled Trial (CT), with a 2-arm design. Two of these studies used a crossover element, and six studies had no crossover element.

It is not always convenient or possible to introduce randomization. In their study, Sung and colleagues [61] evaluated the health effects of a forest therapy program using what they call a ‘convenient assignment’ and not true randomization, which considers the subjects’ preference and suitability to the intervention or the control group. Bang et al. [1] investigated the effects of a forest-walking program on physical and psychological health using a quasi-experimental design. The participants were assigned to the experimental or control group based on the participants’ preference, to boost motivation. Dewi et al. [52] also used a quasi-experimental design, investigating already existing community garden activities. Beute and de Kort [201] investigated if lower mental health makes an individual more or less responsive to the positive health effects of GS. Accordingly, the participants were not randomized, but split into groups based on their obtained score from the BDI-II, which was an appropriate design to answer their particular research question. Non-randomized study designs like these [1,52,61] may say something about the effect of an intervention or activity on people with a predisposition for the environment chosen, which might not represent a result that is transferable to the general population.

There may be other practicalities preventing the use of randomization. Park et al. [54] used a quasi-experimental design with a non-equivalent control group; the groups being two senior community centres, with one participating in a gardening intervention, while the other one did not. Wood and colleagues [321] investigated the health and wellbeing benefits of allotment gardening, using a case-control study to compare allotment gardeners with non-gardeners. In many real-life situations, such methods [54,321] will be the only possible way to evaluate an intervention and randomization is not an option. However, if the process, context, and delivery of the intervention are considered, this type of evaluation may produce meaningful results.

Another aspect that can increase the rigidity of a study design is the incorporation of a crossover element [360]. In a study with a crossover design, all participants receive both the intervention treatment and the control treatment. The different treatments are given at different times and with a sufficient washout period in between to insure there is no carryover effect from one treatment into the next. The order of the treatments is randomized. When using a crossover design, the between-subject variability is significantly reduced as each participant serves as their own control. This results in a reduction of the variation in factors not related to the treatment, which in turn allows for the detection of smaller effect sizes using a reduced sample size [360]. However, crossover designs need careful design to minimize potential bias.

Barnicle and Midden [289] investigated the effects of a horticultural activity program on psychological wellbeing among older people in two care homes. As randomization would not be practical at the individual level, the randomization took place at the site level. It would, however, have strengthened the study design if a crossover had been introduced and participants from both care homes had been exposed to the intervention and the control treatment. The authors give no explanation as to why they chose not to include a crossover. However, this often comes down to time, funding, and the likelihood of being able to secure participation and retention for an extended period of data collection. A number of studies fall into this category; an RCT study design that would have benefitted significantly from a crossover element introduced to the design (see, for example, [202,306,319,320,324,335]).

Seven studies identified for this review used a 3-arm design; typically, two intervention treatments and one control treatment [304], or three different types of intervention treatment [282,336]. None of these 3-arm RCTs have incorporated a crossover element. This is not unexpected, as adding more arms to a study design will increase the complexity of the study and put strain on resources, such as time, money, and by no means least the participants.

Five studies used a 4-arm RCT. Sonntag-Ostrom and colleagues [310] investigated the restorative effect of visits to one urban area and three different forest environments, with each participant visiting all four outdoor environments. The authors highlight the difficulties in carrying out a study with such a complex design, e.g., a long data collection period and difficulties in recruiting participants. These difficulties resulted in a 3-year project and only 20 participants [310].

Based on the studies included in this review, the strongest design appears to be RCTs with a crossover element, a finding which is also supported in other literature [359,360]. In addition, the results from this review highlight that unless answers to very specific research questions are sought, increasing the complexity of the study design does not necessarily improve the quality of the data collected as constraints and limitations increase with increasing complexity.

There were eight studies using a 2-arm RCT with a crossover element (Table 10). Three of the studies focused on urban GS, four on natural GS, and one study on virtual/indoor GS. Six of the studies were qualitative, and only two studies used qualitative as well as quantitative methods. Seven of the studies predominantly used questionnaires as the main tool to assess the changes in the investigated health outcome.

Table 10.

Randomized controlled trials investigating the effects of GS on mental health.

Paper Number Country/Green Space Participant Type # of Subjects/Male/Female Mean Age/min/max Positive/Negative Health Outcome Health Assessment Green Space Assessment Quantitative/Qualitative Study Methods Intervention/Control Group Comments
2-arm randomised controlled crossover design
[50] Study 2 * UK/Natural University students 17/7/10 23.18 (±6.23)/18/43 Mixed/Negative Restorativeness Questionnaire, heart rate Photos, videos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Quantitative Walk through low prospect-high refuge natural environment/Walk through high prospect-low refuge environment Small study with indication the GS is restorative only when there is an open aspect and few places where someone might hide.
[361] USA/Urban General public 12/8/4 x/x/x Positive Stress, Health Heart rate Observations. No quantitative assessment Quantitative Self-paced walk in local neighbourhood past sites receiving greening treatment/Self-paced walk in local neighbourhood past sites not receiving greening treatment Small study only measuring heart rate. Based on heart rate only, the results indicated that in-view proximity to a greened vacant lot decreased heart rate compared to in-view proximity to a non-greened vacant lot.
[57] Korea/Natural University students 41/14/27 x/18/35 Positive Stress Cytokine serum levels, questionnaire No quantitative or qualitative assessment Quantitative/Qualitative 2 h exposure to a forest environment/2 h exposure to an urban environment Small study indicating the level of somatic and depressive symptoms decrease significantly after exposure to forest environments. Weak design; no before-and-after measurements allowing for comparison.
[57] Japan/Natural Male university students 12/12/0 21.3 (±1.1)/20/23 Mixed/Negative Restorativeness Cortisol, blood pressure, pulse rate, questionnaire No quantitative or qualitative assessment Quantitative/Qualitative 15 min visits to forest environments/15 min visits to urban environments Small study with no clear conclusion from the quantitative data about the effect of GS. Subjective evaluation data showed significantly more positive responses after exposure to forest environments.
[326] Japan/Natural General public 498/244/254 56.2 (±10.6)/20/x Positive Stress, Mental health Questionnaire No quantitative or qualitative assessment Qualitative 2 x forest walks/2 days where a forest was not visited Relatively large number of participants, but no quantitative objective data. The study concluded that a forest environment significantly reduces hostility and depression. The largest benefit was seen for the most stressed participants.
[318] Sweden/Virtual, indoor Undergraduates 74/x/x x/x/x Positive Restorativeness Questionnaire 852 colour photos of two gardens were sampled. Final sample consisted of 12 photos for each garden. Qualitative 2 ha large spacious garden with large as well as small garden rooms and many views without buildings/Small and detailed courtyard garden of 13 × 17 m. Views at eye-level always include buildings. A study using only one qualitative measure and no quantitative data. The data showed that gardens are likely to be restorative to varying degrees, depending on the design and the surroundings of the garden.
[233] Japan/Urban Undergraduates 45/45/0 21.13 (±1.25)/x/x Positive General health Questionnaire Photos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative A 15 min walk in a forest environment/A 15 min walk in an urban environment Relatively small study looking only at young men. Four different validated questionnaires were used; some revealed significant positive effects of the forest environment, some did not. The results indicated the combination of activity and GS results in greater psychological benefits. The feelings of vigour, positive effects, subjective recovery, and vitality were stronger in the forest environment.
[209] USA/Urban People with depression 20/8/12 26/x/x Mixed/Negative Cognition, Affect Interview, questionnaire Satellite GPS images. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative 50 min walk in natural setting/50 min walk in urban setting Small study with no clear conclusions about the effect of GS.
2-arm randomised controlled design, no crossover
Author Country/Green space Participant type # of subjects/male/female Mean age/min/max Positive/Negative Health outcome Health assessment Green space assessment Quantitative/Qualitative Intervention/Control group Comments
[60] Korea/Natural Patients with history of stroke 59/40/19 60.8 (±9.1)/36/79 Positive Depression. anxiety Questionnaire, physiological measurement (Reactive oxygen metabolites (dROM). Biological antioxidant potential (BAPs)) No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Quantitative/Qualitative Patients randomly assigned to a forest therapy group or an urban control group. The study found that forest therapy can significantly lower oxidative stress and improve anti-oxidative capacity for patients with a history of stroke. High levels of oxidative stress and reduced anti-oxidative capacity are indicative of depression and anxiety.
[306] China/Urban College students 32/16/16 20.6 (±1.6)/x/x Positive Cognition, restorativeness Questionnaire, EEG Photos. Quantification of green elements, buildings and paved areas of the two environments used. Quantitative/Qualitative 20 min exposure to one of two environments: A wooded campus garden/A traffic island under an elevated highway Positive EEG results identified from a brief exposure to photographs of nature compared to urban environment (20 min).
[319] USA/Urban General public 30/14/16 26.6/x/x Positive Rumination Questionnaire, neural activity in the sgPFC Detailed description of the two walk. Quantitative/Qualitative 5.3 km nature walk/5.3 km urban walk The study found a significant reduction in self-reported rumination driven by a decreased cerebral blood flow in the sgPFC for the nature group, but not for the urban group.
[335] Netherlands/Horticulture, garden General public 30/8/22 57.6 (±8.49)/38/79 Positive/mixed Stress Questionnaire, salivary cortisol Very brief description of allotment complex but no in depth description or quantitative measures of the GS investigated Quantitative/Qualitative After stress induction: 30 min of outdoor gardening in own allotment/30 min of indoor reading in allotment home with no view of nature (popular magazines chosen by researcher) The small study found that both reading and gardening showed a significant reduction in cortisol levels after stress. Cortisol levels were lower after gardening compared to reading, but the difference was not significant. Positive mood was significantly higher after gardening compared to reading. There were indications that gardening is more restorative after stress than reading.
[204] – study 1 USA/Virtual, indoor General public 86/22/64 35.47 (±14.05)/x/x Positive Aggression Questionnaire Qualitative Ostracised individuals exposed to urban or nature pictures/Non-ostracised individuals exposed to urban or nature pictures The qualitative study found that among participants with a high feeling of ostracism, those who viewed nature pictures reported a significantly lower level of aggression than those who viewed urban pictures. The authors concluded that nature exposure can counteract the relationship between ostracism and aggression.
[289] USA/Virtual, indoor Older adults 62/6/56 x/x/x Mixed, negative Physiological wellbeing Questionnaire No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative Horticultural activity program in a care home, once a week for 7 weeks/Normal daily activities in a care home, over 7 weeks The study found no statistically significant differences in the effect of a horticultural activity program on physiological wellbeing of older adults in a care home. However, there were some indications that horticultural activities may have a positive effect on wellbeing.
[202] USA/Urban General public 60/27/33 22.9/x/x Mixed Affect, cognition Questionnaire Photos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative A nature walk/An urban walk The study found significant evidence that a nature walk improves affect, but no clear evidence that it improves cognition.
[324] USA/Urban University office staff 37/34/3 48.8/x/x Positive Stress Questionnaire No description or quantitative measures of the GS investigated. Qualitative Work breaks over 4 weeks: 10–15 min outdoor booster break/standard work break The small qualitative study found that a 10–15 min outdoor booster break during the work day results in a significantly greater reduction in stress than an indoor work break.
[320] USA/Horticulture, garden Undergraduates 32/x/x x/18/32 Mixed/Negative Self-esteem Questionnaire No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative 4 h of gardening work over a period of 3 weeks/No gardening activities This small qualitative study found no significant differences regarding ethnocentrism and self-esteem, in relation to the effects of GS. There were indications that gardening can positively affect self-esteem.
[271] Serbia/Horticulture, garden Psychiatric patients 30/9/21 45.35 (±10.16)/25/65 Mixed Mental health Questionnaire Map, photos and short description. No in-depth quality assessment or quantitative measures Qualitative Four weeks (12 sessions) of horticultural therapy/Four weeks of occupational art therapy The small qualitative study found a significantly larger reduction in stress after horticultural therapy compared to occupational art therapy. However, no significant differences were identified for anxiety or depression after the two treatments.
3-arm randomised controlled design, no crossover
Author Country/Green space Participant type # of subjects/male/female Mean age/min/max Positive/Negative Health outcome Health assessment Green space assessment Quantitative/Qualitative Intervention/Control group Comments
[307]-Study 2 ** USA/Urban College students 34/17/17 20/x/x Positive Restorativeness Questionnaire, physiological measurements (blood pressure and pulse) Very brief description of GS Quantitative/Qualitative College students were randomly assigned to a nature walk, an urban walk, or a relaxation condition Small study showing that happiness and positive affect significantly increase and anger and aggression significantly decreased after being in a natural environment compared to an urban environment.
[304] Finland/Urban Office workers 153/137/20 47.2/x/x Mixed Stress, wellbeing Questionnaire (paper format and mobile text messages) No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative Park walk/relaxation exercises/usual break activities The study found no clear conclusions about the effect of park walks on employees’ wellbeing. The effects on wellbeing were of a small magnitude and short duration.
[338] USA/Urban Park visitors 108/48/60 22/x/x Mixed, negative Stress reduction, restorativeness Questionnaire No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative
[282] UK/Urban Adults with mental health issues 53/20/33 53 (±15.4)/21/83 Mixed Mood, self-esteem Questionnaire No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative Three health-promoting interventions: Walking in GS/Swimming/Quizzes, bingo, games, crafts and music The study found that green exercise was as health-promoting for people experiencing mental ill health as existing non-green interventions. There was no conclusive evidence that GS activity was more health-promoting than other activities.
[217] USA/Horticulture, garden Older adults with mild to moderate depression 39/16/23 74.3 (±6.40)/x/x Positive Depression Questionnaire, focus groups No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative The participants were randomly assigned to one of 3 treatments; walk alone, guided imagery, or art therapy. Small study surmising that GS as well as art interventions were helpful in improving mood and overall attitude. However, only subjective, anecdotal evidence was explored.
[215] USA/Horticulture, garden Older adults with mild to moderate depression 39/x/x 75/x/x Positive/Mixed Depression Questionnaire, focus groups No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative The participants were randomly assigned to one of 3 treatments; walk alone, group walking, or art therapy. The study found that assisted and unassisted GS walks as well as art therapy interventions can significantly reduce symptoms of depression.
[336] Iceland/Urban University students 18/9/9 *** x/x/x Mixed/negative Stress Interviews, observations Photos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative Three treatments for alleviation of stress: Walking in the gym/Walking in nature/Watching nature on TV The very small study using personal narratives involving restoration found no clear conclusions about the effect of GS on stress.
4-arm randomised controlled crossover design
Author Country/Green space Participant type # of subjects/male/female Mean age/min/max Positive/Negative Health outcome Health assessment Green space assessment Quantitative/Qualitative Intervention/Control group Comments
[310] Sweden/Natural Females diagnosed with exhaustion disorder 20/0/20 41.6 (±7.3)/24/55 Positive Restorativeness Questionnaire, heart rate, blood pressure, heart rate recovery Photos, detailed description. No quantitative assessment. Quantitative/Qualitative 90 min test procedure in 3 different forest environments/and in 1 city environment Small study indicating significantly higher perceived restorativeness in the forest environments compared to the city.
4-arm randomised controlled design, no crossover
Author Country/Green space Participant type # of subjects/male/female Mean age/min/max Positive/Negative Health outcome Health assessment Green space assessment Quantitative/qualitative Intervention/Control group Comments
[227] Taiwan/Urban College students 116/52/64 20.85 (±1.14)/x/x Mixed/Negative Emotion, attention Questionnaire Photos used to quantify the level of greenness, aerial maps Quantitative/Qualitative Walking or jogging in natural environment/Walking or jogging in built environment The study found no clear conclusions about the effect of GS and exercise on emotion and attention. The key finding is the indication that walking in a setting with at least 40% visible greenness elicits the largest benefits.
[204] – study 2 USA/Virtual, indoor General public 150/48/102 36.87 (±13.30)/x/x Mixed Aggression Questionnaire Photos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative Ostracised individuals exposed to urban or nature pictures/Non-ostracised individuals exposed to urban or nature pictures The study found no clear conclusions about the effect of viewing nature photos to moderate the relationship between ostracism and aggression. There were some indications that viewing nature photos can alleviate aggressive responses following ostracism.
[204] – study 3 USA/Virtual, indoor General public 144/47/97 35.47 (±11.99)/x/x Mixed Aggression Questionnaire Photos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative Ostracised individuals exposed to urban or nature pictures/Non-ostracised individuals exposed to urban or nature pictures The study found no clear conclusions about the effect of viewing nature photos to moderate the relationship between ostracism and aggression. There were some indications that viewing nature photos can alleviate aggressive responses following ostracism.
[205] China/Virtual, indoor Undergraduate students 118/25/93 21.23 (±2.26) Mixed, negative Aggression, mood Questionnaire Video. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measures Qualitative Depleted individuals exposed to a natural or urban video/Non-depleted individuals exposed to a natural or urban video The study found no clear conclusions about the effect of viewing a natural video to counteract aggression after depletion. The study suggests that watching a natural video helps to restore self-control after depletion.

x = data missing. ± = standard deviation around the mean. * This paper consists of three small studies; only one of which is presented in this table (study 2). ** This paper consists of two studies; only study 2 is presented in this table. *** Only three participants are described in the results; one for each treatment.

Berman et al. [209] used a 2-arm RCT with a crossover to show that participants exhibited a significantly increased memory span after a walk in the park compared to an urban walk. The PANAS (positive affect) revealed a significant effect of location (nature vs. urban) but not time (pre-walk vs. post-walk); for a negative effect, there was no significant effect of location and the negative effect did not decrease more for the park walk than for the urban walk. The authors were therefore not able to show conclusively that GS positively affects the mood of individuals with depression. Gatersleben and Andrews [50] found that exposure to GS with high levels of prospect (clear field of vision) and low levels of refuge (places to hide) generated a restorative effect. However, the authors also found that exposure to GS with low levels of prospect and high levels of refuge did not create a restorative effect. Such a scenario was proposed to increase stress levels and reduce attention. Im et al. [57] found that the levels of somatic and depressive symptoms, and of stress responses, were significantly reduced after exposure to a forest environment, when compared to exposure to an urban environment. The authors also found a significant reduction of immunological inflammation and an increase in the antioxidant effect after the forest exposure. However, due to the design of the study (no before-and-after measurements allowing for comparison), it is not clear if the positive changes are related to a reduction in air pollution (or other harmful urban exposures), rather than the presence of the forest environment. Lee et al. [59] found that the salivary cortisol concentration, diastolic blood pressure, and pulse rate were all significantly lower in participants after exposure to a forest environment. Self-reported subjective measures revealed that participants felt more comfortable, soothed, and refreshed when viewing a forest landscape, when compared to an urban environment. Morita and colleagues [326] investigated the psychological effects of exposure to a forest environment, when compared to exposure to a control environment. Co-exposures and contextual factors were considered, such as conditions during the forest visit and on the control day (weather, duration of visit, previous visits, accompanying people, activities undertaken, walking course and distance walked, degree of exercise, subjective feelings, objective activities undertaken). The authors found that exposure to a forest environment significantly decreased feelings of hostility and depression, and increased the feeling of liveliness, when compared to exposure to a control environment. It was also seen that the positive effect of exposure to a forest environment was greater the higher the stress level of the subject. Despite a high number of participants and a generally stringent study design, the study only used qualitative data and would have benefited from the inclusion of quantitative data. South et al. [361] found that when subjects were in view of a green vacant lot, their heart rate decreased significantly, when compared to being in view of a non-greened vacant lot or not in view of any vacant lot. The authors conclude that remediating neighborhood blight can reduce stress and improve health. Takayama et al. [233] investigated the emotional, restorative, and vitalizing effects related to forest and urban exposures and concluded that exposure to a forest environment improved mood and positive affect, and induced a feeling of subjective restoration and subjective vitality. Tenngart Ivarsson and Hagerhall [318] investigated the perceived restorativeness of gardens. Two gardens with differing levels of build and natural elements were photographed, and a set of 12 photos were selected to represent each garden. The PRS was used to examine the perceived restorativeness of the two gardens. The study also aimed to evaluate the ability of the PRS to distinguish between two different gardens with a mix of build and natural elements, rather than to distinguish a contrast between built and natural scene types. The authors found that both gardens were perceived as restorative, and the PRS can be used to discriminate between two gardens from the same scene type. Hence, one garden can be perceived as more restorative than another although they both have the same type of scene. This highlights the importance of considering the contribution of contextual factors and co-exposures to the overall health effect caused by a GS environment.

Out of the eight studies included here, with a 2-arm RCT crossover design, seven had a positive outcome. Only two of the studies included quantitative measures [57,59], with both studies having a low participant number. All studies heavily rely on qualitative subjective data (Table 10), on which it is difficult to draw comparative conclusive interpretations. However, it is evident that in the included RCTs, there is clear agreement of a positive association between GS exposure and mental HWB. Despite the lack of high-quality studies and methodological rigor between studies, the accumulated strength of these findings highlights the importance of the positive associations between GS and mental HWB.

4. Discussion

The effects of GS on mental HWB is relevant to city planning and public health policy, which is becoming increasingly important as the world’s urban population grows. The published research generally shows positive associations between GS and mental HWB. However, this review has identified great diversity in study designs, GS definitions, outcome measures, inclusion of co-exposures and contextual factors, and reporting of results. This makes it difficult to aggregate the evidence to identify the underlying mechanisms for this positive association or to provide advice to help construct GS that is beneficial for mental HWB.

Based on the diversity of research available on the subject, it is not possible to unequivocally answer all of the four research questions we initially posited. However, based on the weight of evidence of the research reviewed, it is possible to conclude the following with reasonable certainty:

  • Different types of GS in many contexts and different environments have a positive effect on mental HWB (RQ 1 & 3);

  • For a variety of different groups of people (RQ 4), GS does have a positive effect on mental HWB;

  • Different types of GS affect the HWB of individuals in different ways (RQ 1 & 4).

However, based on the analysed literature it is clear that there is no universally agreed definition for GS or mental HWB and in many studies, a definition and/or detailed description of the two has been omitted. Only a few studies have attempted to quantify the GS investigated and/or the amount of GS needed for health improvement (RQ 1 & 2).

RQ 1:

How do different types of GS (recreational, residential, urban, rural) affect HWB and how much green space is needed for health improvement?

There are suggestions that different types of GS may affect mental HWB in different ways and that different age groups and population subgroups benefit differently from exposure to GS. There is also limited evidence that some threshold amount of GS is needed to generate positive health outcomes. However, there is insufficient coherence in the evidence to generalize the results.

RQ 2:

How can we best define, measure, and quantify GS?

Often, the description of the GS is limited to simple text descriptors, e.g., allotment garden, urban park, or private garden. There are some good examples of studies that have attempted to quantify the GS investigated and assess the GS quality. For example, Tilley et al. [362] included graphic Ordnance Survey maps clearly depicting the urban environments investigated, giving a clear overview of the settings and contexts. A written overview and typology was included, of quartiles of urban green and urban busy areas, derived from a Geographic Information System (GIS). The authors also used photographs giving visual evidence of the different environments, which would make it easy to replicate the study in other cities and countries. Our findings highlight the necessity to investigate further how best to define, measure, and quantify GS. With a systematic review, it would be possible to explore in more detail what types of measurements are used most efficiently to quantify GS, the accuracy of the different methods, and the reproducibility.

RQ 2: How can we best define, measure, and quantify mental HWB? The World Health Organisation (1948) has defined health as “A state of complete physical, mental and social wellbeing and not merely the absence of disease and infirmity”. However, wellbeing is difficult to define. Fleuret and Atkinson [363] reviewed the various ways in which wellbeing has been used in research and policy contexts. They note that the term ‘wellbeing’ mainly originates from Anglophone countries and in many languages, it is difficult to find and appropriate comparable terms. Often, a number of different terms are used interchangeably to describe wellbeing, such as quality of life, happiness, welfare, pleasure, wealth, and subjective and objective wellbeing [363]. These terms are rarely specified, and it is therefore impossible to know if they are synonymous. Additionally, different stakeholders in different countries adhere to the wellbeing concept in various ways and it is a matter of practice amongst stakeholders that determines how a term is defined. As far as possible, it would be an advantage to harmonize definitions of HWB and to at least explicitly describe the definition used in a research study. The definition proposed by The UK Faculty of Public Health is perhaps a good starting point:

  • ‘Realise our abilities, live a life with purpose and meaning, and make a positive contribution to our communities;

  • Form positive relationships with others, and feel connected and supported;

  • Experience peace of mind, contentment, happiness and joy;

  • Cope with life’s ups and downs and be confident and resilient;

  • Take responsibility for oneself and for others as appropriate.’

(Faculty of Public Health, 2010: https://www.fph.org.uk/policy-campaigns/special-interest-groups/special-interest-groups-list/public-mental-health-special-interest-group/better-mental-health-for-all/concepts-of-mental-and-social-wellbeing/).

This holistic definition of wellbeing incorporates a more social aspect, highlighting a change in focus from looking more at physical health to looking at the realization of the individuals’ potential [364]. It is more inclusive and relevant to more diverse population subgroups, such as people with learning disabilities, who in many cases experience chronic conditions on a daily basis [365]. Furthermore, we propose that the quality of the environment, i.e., built or natural, is also taken into consideration when assessing wellbeing in such a holistic way, in line with the GS exposome.

RQ 3:

Do different co-exposures or contextual factors affect the mental HWB outcome?

Very few studies included in this review have taken contextual factors and co-exposures into account; they were generally poorly described and so it is difficult to replicate studies. The importance of this is highlighted in a study by McMahan and Estes [46], who aimed to synthesize research on the effect of exposure to natural environments on positive and negative affect, using a meta-analysis technique. The authors only included studies with an RCT design including a comparison group and a self-report assessment of the current emotional state; 32 papers were identified. Study and design-related characteristics, such as the year of publication, location of study, mean age of sample, percent female, and instrument used to measure affective wellbeing, were examined to reveal if they had a moderating effect on the investigated outcome. The type of exposure was also addressed (i.e., real or laboratory simulations of nature), as was the type of natural environment (i.e., manicured or wild nature). The review concluded that exposure to natural environments was associated with a moderate increase in positive affect and a small decrease in negative affect. The authors found that study location, type of assessment used to measure emotion, and type of exposure moderated the effect of nature on positive affect. This indicates that co-exposures and contextual factors may play a role in mediating positive as well as negative health effects associated with GS exposure. The attempt in this review, to look at context and co-exposures, has highlighted a gap in the available literature; our knowledge on contextual factors and co-exposures in relation to the GS experience (GS exposome) is insufficient and research is needed to investigate the totality and combination of exposures related to GS that affects mental HWB.

RQ 4:

Do different age groups and population subgroups benefit differently from exposure to GS?

Participant type varied greatly between studies and in many cases, the subjects were very specifically specified, e.g., park users, allotment gardeners, or active walkers. These groups may have an affinity for the GS being investigated. This makes it difficult to compare study results and hinders the interpretation of whether a finding can be generalized to other groups within the population. However, based on the weight of evidence, it can be concluded with reasonable certainty that different population subgroups will benefit differently to a variety of GS exposures.

Based on the analysis in this review, we suggest a number of key points that should be assessed and reported when investigating GS exposures:

  1. Quantity of greenery or natural elements;

  2. Type of vegetation (creating shade or not/natural daylight);

  3. Whether the environment is natural or managed;

  4. Quantity of built elements;

  5. Traffic noise and air pollution levels;

  6. General soundscape;

  7. Number of people present in the environment;

  8. Setting and context.

The majority of studies rely on qualitative data collection methods and there is limited methodological consistency between studies. There is a need for more robust quantitative data collection methods, e.g., using vegetation cover maps from airborne hyperspectral and light detection and ranging (LiDAR) data to derive measures of GS [253], or measurement of stress hormones (cortisol) for the quantification of changes in stress levels after exposure to different urban and natural environments [58,59,61,62,334,335], or in relation to neighborhood GS and long-term exposure [208,329,332,366]. Ng, et al. [367] recently published the findings from an RCT (waitlist-control randomized controlled trial) investigating the effects of horticultural therapy on Asian older adults. Qualitative measures (MOCA, Zung Self-Rating Depression and Anxiety Scales) were used to investigate cognitive functioning, depression, anxiety, psychological wellbeing, and positive relations with others. Quantitative measures were used to measure nine plasma biomarkers ranging from interleukins and chemokines to hormones. Ng, et al. [367] found no significant changes in conventional psychological subjective measures of health and wellbeing after 6 months of horticultural therapy. However, there was a significant reduction in pro-inflammatory cytokines after the intervention; high levels of these cytokines are associated with depression [368]. This highlights the importance of including objective quantitative methods to underpin and clarify any subjective findings.

Recommendations

Overall, we suggest a number of key points that should be included when planning and reporting on findings from research investigating GS and mental HWB:

  1. Description of aim and research question(s);

  2. Description of the study design;

  3. Description of participant type (incl. sex, mean age, min/max age, population subgroup characteristics and other relevant socioeconomic characteristics);

  4. Description of recruitment process;

  5. Careful description and quantification of the GS investigated (study sites);

  6. Clear definition of the mental HWB endpoint(s);

  7. Justification of the choice of tools to assess the health endpoint;

  8. Measurement of contextual factors and co-exposures.

We advocate that, in future research, the entire GS exposome should be considered when investigating the impact on mental HWB. There is a need for large well-designed randomized controlled crossover trails that reliably measure a range of environmental and personal exposures associated with GS. Future studies should include standardized quantitative data collection methods to describe and define the GS investigated and to quantify the changes in mental HWB. By also including standardized qualitative data collection methods, a meaningful comparison and pooling of data across studies would be possible. This will allow a better understanding of the underlying factors responsible for positive associations between GS and mental HWB.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to colleagues in the Institute of Occupational Medicine and Heriot Watt University for their helpful discussions about the work.

Author Contributions

C.W.-N. and J.W.C. conceived the study and carried out the planning and initial screening. C.W.-N. undertook the detailed review of the literature, created the initial synthesis of the evidence, and prepared the first draft of the manuscript. S.K. and M.K. advised on the literature search strategy and data management and assisted in preparation of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the text.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Bang K.S., Lee I., Kim S., Lim C.S., Joh H.K., Park B.J., Song M.K. The effects of a campus forest-walking program on undergraduate and graduate students’ physical and psychological health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14 doi: 10.3390/ijerph14070728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Oh B., Lee K.J., Zaslawski C., Yeung A., Rosenthal D., Larkey L., Back M. Health and well-being benefits of spending time in forests: Systematic review. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2017;22 doi: 10.1186/s12199-017-0677-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Elmendorf W. The importance of trees and nature in community: A review of the relative literature. Arboric. Urban For. 2008;34:152–156. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gascon M., Triguero-Mas M., Martinez D., Dadvand P., Forns J., Plasencia A., Nieuwenhuijsen M.J. Mental health benefits of long-term exposure to residential green and blue spaces: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2015;12:4354–4379. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120404354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jorgensen A., Gobster P.H. Shades of green: Measuring the ecology of urban green space in the context of human health and well-being. Nat. Cult. 2010;5:338–363. doi: 10.3167/nc.2010.050307. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.van den Bosch M., Ode Sang Å. Urban natural environments as nature-based solutions for improved public health—A systematic review of reviews. Environ. Res. 2017;158:373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.05.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kondo M.C., Fluehr J.M., McKeon T., Branas C.C. Urban green space and its impact on human health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2018;15 doi: 10.3390/ijerph15030445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Douglas O., Lennon M., Scott M. Green space benefits for health and well-being: A life-course approach for urban planning, design and management. Cities. 2017;66:53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.cities.2017.03.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hartig T., Mitchell R., de Vries S., Frumkin H. Nature and health. Annu. Rev. Public Health. 2014;35:207–228. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032013-182443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Okvat H.A., Zautra A.J. Community gardening: A parsimonious path to individual, community, and environmental resilience. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2011;47:374–387. doi: 10.1007/s10464-010-9404-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Whear R., Coon J.T., Bethel A., Abbott R., Stein K., Garside R. What is the impact of using outdoor spaces such as gardens on the physical and mental well-being of those with dementia? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative evidence. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 2014;15:697–705. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2014.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wright S.D., Wadsworth A.M. Gray and green revisited: A multidisciplinary perspective of gardens, gardening, and the aging process. J. Aging Res. 2014;2014:13. doi: 10.1155/2014/283682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Detweiler M.B., Sharma T., Detweiler J.G., Murphy P.F., Lane S., Carman J., Chudhary A.S., Halling M.H., Kim K.Y. What is the evidence to support the use of therapeutic gardens for the elderly? Psychiatry Investig. 2012;9:100–110. doi: 10.4306/pi.2012.9.2.100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lee I., Choi H., Bang K.S., Kim S., Song M., Lee B. Effects of forest therapy on depressive symptoms among adults: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14 doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.van den Berg M., Wendel-Vos W., van Poppel M., Kemper H., van Mechelen W., Maas J. Health benefits of green spaces in the living environment: A systematic review of epidemiological studies. Urban For. Urban Green. 2015;14:806–816. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2015.07.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Tzoulas K., Korpela K., Venn S., Yli-Pelkonen V., Kazmierczak A., Niemela J., James P. Promoting ecosystem and human health in urban areas using green infrastructure: A literature review. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2007;81:167–178. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2007.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sandifer P.A., Sutton-Grier A.E., Ward B.P. Exploring connections among nature, biodiversity, ecosystem services, and human health and well-being: Opportunities to enhance health and biodiversity conservation. Ecosyst. Serv. 2015;12:1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.ecoser.2014.12.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Jennings V., Larson L., Yun J. Advancing sustainability through urban green space: Cultural ecosystem services, equity, and social determinants of health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2016;13 doi: 10.3390/ijerph13020196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Taylor L., Hochuli D.F. Defining greenspace: Multiple uses across multiple disciplines. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017;158:25–38. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.09.024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lederbogen F., Kirsch P., Haddad L., Streit F., Tost H., Schuch P., Wust S., Pruessner J.C., Rietschel M., Deuschle M., et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature. 2011;474:498–501. doi: 10.1038/nature10190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wang J.L. Rural-urban differences in the prevalence of major depression and associated impairment. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2004;39:19–25. doi: 10.1007/s00127-004-0698-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Peen J., Schoevers R.A., Beekman A.T., Dekker J. The current status of urban-rural differences in psychiatric disorders. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2010;121:84–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01438.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mitchell R.J., Richardson E.A., Shortt N.K., Pearce J.R. Neighborhood environments and socioeconomic inequalities in mental well-being. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2015;49:80–84. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2015.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Triguero-Mas M., Dadvand P., Cirach M., Martínez D., Medina A., Mompart A., Basagaña X., Gražulevičienė R., Nieuwenhuijsen M.J. Natural outdoor environments and mental and physical health: Relationships and mechanisms. Environ. Int. 2015;77:35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2015.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Alcock I., White M.P., Lovell R., Higgins S.L., Osborne N.J., Husk K., Wheeler B.W. What accounts for ‘england’s green and pleasant land’? A panel data analysis of mental health and land cover types in rural England. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015;142:38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.05.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Butler S.S., Cohen A.L. The importance of nature in the well-being of rural elders. Nat. Cult. 2010;5:150–174. doi: 10.3167/nc.2010.050203. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hansen-Ketchum P.A., Marck P., Reutter L., Ha’penny E. Strengthening access to restorative places: Findings from a participatory study on engaging with nature in the promotion of health. Health Place. 2011;17:558–571. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2010.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Pretty J., Peacock J., Hine R., Sellens M., South N., Griffin M. Green exercise in the UK countryside: Effects on health and psychological well-being, and implications for policy and planning. J. Environ. Plan. Manag. 2007;50:211–231. doi: 10.1080/09640560601156466. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gilbert A., Colley K., Roberts D. Are rural residents happier? A quantitative analysis of subjective wellbeing in Scotland. J. Rural Stud. 2016;44:37–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jrurstud.2016.01.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Weich S., Twigg L., Lewis G. Rural/non-rural differences in rates of common mental disorders in Britain —Prospective multilevel cohort study. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2006;188:51–57. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.008714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Verheij R.A., Maas J., Groenewegen P.P. Urban-rural health differences and the availability of green space. Eur. Urban Reg. Stud. 2008;15:307–316. doi: 10.1177/0969776408095107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kohlleppel T., Bradley J.C., Jacob S. A walk through the garden: Can a visit to a botanic garden reduce stress? HortTechnology. 2002;12:489–492. doi: 10.21273/HORTTECH.12.3.489. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Carrus G., Scopelliti M., Panno A., Lafortezza R., Colangelo G., Pirchio S., Ferrini F., Salbitano F., Agrimi M., Portoghesi L., et al. A different way to stay in touch with ‘urban nature’: The perceived restorative qualities of botanical gardens. Front. Psychol. 2017;8 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kreski B. Healing and empowering veterans in a botanic garden. J. Mus. Educ. 2016;41:110–115. doi: 10.1080/10598650.2016.1169734. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Adevi A.A., Lieberg M. Stress rehabilitation through garden therapy a caregiver perspective on factors considered most essential to the recovery process. Urban For. Urban Green. 2012;11:51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2011.09.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Sahlin E., Ahlborg G., Tenenbaum A., Grahn P. Using nature-based rehabilitation to restart a stalled process of rehabilitation in individuals with stress-related mental illness. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2015;12:1928–1951. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120201928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Sahlin E., Ahlborg G., Matuszczyk J.V., Grahn P. Nature-based stress management course for individuals at risk of adverse health effects from work-related stress-effects on stress related symptoms, workability and sick leave. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2014;11:6586–6611. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110606586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Adevi A.A., Mårtensson F. Stress rehabilitation through garden therapy: The garden as a place in the recovery from stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 2013;12:230–237. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2013.01.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Gonzalez M.T., Hartig T., Patil G.G., Martinsen E.W., Kirkevold M. Therapeutic horticulture in clinical depression: A prospective study of active components. J. Adv. Nurs. 2010;66:2002–2013. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2010.05383.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Grabbe L., Ball J., Goldstein A. Gardening for the mental well-being of homeless women. J. Holist. Nurs. 2013;31:258–266. doi: 10.1177/0898010113488244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Parkinson S., Lowe C., Vecsey T. The therapeutic benefits of horticulture in a mental health service. Br. J. Occup. Ther. 2011;74:525–534. doi: 10.4276/030802211X13204135680901. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Baybutt M., Chemlal K. Health-promoting prisons: Theory to practice. Glob. Health Promot. 2016;23:66–74. doi: 10.1177/1757975915614182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Edwards C.A., McDonnell C., Merl H. An evaluation of a therapeutic garden’s influence on the quality of life of aged care residents with dementia. Dementia. 2013;12:494–510. doi: 10.1177/1471301211435188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hawkins J.L., Mercer J., Thirlaway K.J., Clayton D.A. ‘Doing’ gardening and ‘being’ at the allotment site: Exploring the benefits of allotment gardening for stress reduction and healthy aging. Ecopsychology. 2013;5:110–125. doi: 10.1089/eco.2012.0084. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Burton E., Mitchell L., Stride C. Bed of roses? The role of garden space in older people’s well-being. Proc. Inst. Civ. Eng.—Urban Des. Plan. 2015;168:164–173. doi: 10.1680/udap.14.00030. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.McMahan E.A., Estes D. The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: A meta-analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 2015;10:507–519. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.994224. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wild C.P. Complementing the genome with an “exposome”: The outstanding challenge of environmental exposure measurement in molecular epidemiology. Cancer Epidemiol. Biomark. 2005;14:1847–1850. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-05-0456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Arksey H., O’Malley L. Scoping studies: Towards a methodological framework. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 2005;8:19–32. doi: 10.1080/1364557032000119616. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Bingley A. Woodland as working space: Where is the restorative green idyll? Soc. Sci. Med. 2013;91:135–140. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.02.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gatersleben B., Andrews M. When walking in nature is not restorative—The role of prospect and refuge. Health Place. 2013;20:91–101. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2013.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wahrborg P., Petersson I.F., Grahn P. Nature-assisted rehabilitation for reactions to severe stress and/or depression in a rehabilitation garden: Long-term follow-up including comparisons with a matched population-based reference cohort. J. Rehabil. Med. 2014;46:271–276. doi: 10.2340/16501977-1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Dewi N.S., Komatsuzaki M., Yamakawa Y., Takahashi H., Shibanuma S., Yasue T., Okayama T., Toyoda A., Shimonishi H., Sasaki S. Community gardens as health promoters: Effects on mental and physical stress levels in adults with and without mental disabilities. Sustainability. 2017;9 doi: 10.3390/su9010063. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hawkins J.L., Thirlaway K.J., Backx K., Clayton D.A. Allotment gardening and other leisure activities for stress reduction and healthy aging. HortTechnology. 2011;21:577–585. doi: 10.21273/HORTTECH.21.5.577. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Park S.A., Lee A.Y., Son K.C., Lee W.L., Kim D.S. Gardening intervention for physical and psychological health benefits in elderly women at community centers. HortTechnology. 2016;26:474–483. doi: 10.21273/HORTTECH.26.4.474. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Soga M., Cox D.T.C., Yamaura Y., Gaston K.J., Kurisu K., Hanaki K. Health benefits of urban allotment gardening: Improved physical and psychological well-being and social integration. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14 doi: 10.3390/ijerph14010071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.van den Berg A.E., Maas J., Verheij R.A., Groenewegen P.P. Green space as a buffer between stressful life events and health. Soc. Sci. Med. 2010;70:1203–1210. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Im S.G., Choi H., Jeon Y.H., Song M.K., Kim W., Woo J.M. Comparison of effect of two-hour exposure to forest and urban environments on cytokine, anti-oxidant, and stress levels in young adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2016;13 doi: 10.3390/ijerph13070625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Jung W.H., Woo J.M., Ryu J.S. Effect of a forest therapy program and the forest environment on female workers’ stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 2015;14:274–281. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2015.02.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Lee J., Park B.J., Tsunetsugu Y., Kagawa T., Miyazaki Y. Restorative effects of viewing real forest landscapes, based on a comparison with urban landscapes. Scand. J. For. Res. 2009;24:227–234. doi: 10.1080/02827580902903341. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Chun M.H., Chang M.C., Lee S.-J. The effects of forest therapy on depression and anxiety in patients with chronic stroke. Int. J. Neurosci. 2017;127:199–203. doi: 10.3109/00207454.2016.1170015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sung J., Woo J.-M., Kim W., Lim S.-K., Chung E.-J. The effect of cognitive behavior therapy-based “forest therapy” program on blood pressure, salivary cortisol level, and quality of life in elderly hypertensive patients. Clin. Exp. Hypertens. 2012;34:1–7. doi: 10.3109/10641963.2011.618195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Yu Y.M., Lee Y.J., Kim J.Y., Yoon S.B., Shin C.S. Effects of forest therapy camp on quality of life and stress in postmenopausal women. For. Sci. Technol. 2016;12:125–129. doi: 10.1080/21580103.2015.1108248. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Eriksson T., Westerberg Y., Jonsson H. Experiences of women with stress-related ill health in a therapeutic gardening program. Can. J. Occup. Ther. 2011;78:273–281. doi: 10.2182/cjot.2011.78.5.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Korpela K., Hartig T. Restorative qualities of favorite places. J. Environ. Psychol. 1996;16:221–233. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1996.0018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hartig T., Korpela K., Evans G.W., Gärling T. A measure of restorative quality in environments. Scand. Hous. Plan. Res. 1997;14:175–194. doi: 10.1080/02815739708730435. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Watson D., Clark L.A., Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect—The panas scales. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1988;54:1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Cohen S., Kamarck T., Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983;24:385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Goldberg D.P., Hillier V.F. Scaled version of the general health questionnaire. Psychol. Med. 1979;9:139–145. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700021644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Stewart-Brown S., Tennant A., Tennant R., Platt S., Parkinson J., Weich S. Internal construct validity of the warwick-edinburgh mental well-being scale (WEMWBS): A Rasch analysis using data from the scottish health education population survey. Health Qual. Life Outcomes. 2009;7 doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-7-15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Ware J.E., Sherbourne C.D. The mos 36-item short-form health survey (sf-36):1. Conceptual-framework and item selection. Med. Care. 1992;30:473–483. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199206000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Beck A.T., Rial W.Y., Rickels K. Short form of depression inventory: Cross-validation. Psychol. Rep. 1974;34:1184–1186. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Beck A.T., Steer R.A. Internal consistencies of the original and revised beck depression inventory. J. Clin. Psychol. 1984;40:1365–1367. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198411)40:6<1365::AID-JCLP2270400615>3.0.CO;2-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Beck A.T., Ward C.H., Mendelson M., Mock J., Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Grove J.R., Prapavessis H. Preliminary evidence for the reliability and validity of an abbreviated profile of mood states. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 1992;23:93–109. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Mcnair D.M. Citation classic-manual for the profile of mood states. Curr. Contents/Soc. Behav. Sci. 1984;27:20. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.McNair D.M. Profile of mood states (poms) in evaluation of antianxiety and antidepressant drugs. J. Pharmacol. 1974;5:10. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Mayer F.S., Frantz C.M. The connectedness to nature scale: A measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004;24:503–515. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Spielberger C.D., Gorsuch R.L., Lushene R.E. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologist Press; Mountain View, CA, USA: 1970. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Radloff L.S. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1977;1:385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Cimprich B. Development of an intervention to restore attention in cancer-patients. Cancer Nurs. 1993;16:83–92. doi: 10.1097/00002820-199304000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Rosenberg M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton University Press; Princeton, NJ, USA: 1965. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Kessler R.C., Andrews G., Colpe L.J., Hiripi E., Mroczek D.K., Normand S.L.T., Walters E.E., Zaslavsky A.M. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol. Med. 2002;32:959–976. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702006074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Diener E., Emmons R.A., Larsen R.J., Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. J. Personal. Assess. 1985;49:71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.The Whoqol Group The World Health Organization quality of life assessment (WHOQOL): Development and general psychometric properties. Soc. Sci. Med. 1998;46:1569–1585. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(98)00009-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Lovibond P.F., Lovibond S.H. The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 1995;33:335–343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Spitzer R.L., Kroenke K., Williams J.B.W., Primary P.H.Q. Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD—The PHQ primary care study. JAMA. 1999;282:1737–1744. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.18.1737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Melamed S., Ugarten U., Shirom A., Kahana L., Lerman Y., Froom P. Chronic burnout, somatic arousal and elevated salivary cortisol levels. J. Psychosom. Res. 1999;46:591–598. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00007-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Ryan R.M., Frederick C. On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. J. Personal. 1997;65:529–565. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00326.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Sheikh J.I., Yesavage J.A. Geriatric depression scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. J. Aging Ment. Health. 1986;5:165–173. [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Jenkinson C., Layte R., Coulter A., Wright L. Evidence for the sensitivity of the SF-36 health status measure to inequalities in health: Results from the Oxford healthy lifestyles survey. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 1996;50:377–380. doi: 10.1136/jech.50.3.377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Jenkinson C., Layte R., Jenkinson D., Lawrence K., Petersen S., Paice C., Stradling J. A shorter form health survey: Can the SF-12 replicate results from the SF-36 in longitudinal studies? J. Public Health Med. 1997;19:179–186. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.pubmed.a024606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Williams A. Euroqol—A new facility for the measurement of health-related quality-of-life. Health Policy. 1990;16:199–208. doi: 10.1016/0168-8510(90)90421-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Schultz P.W. Inclusion with nature: The psychology of human-nature relations. In: Schmuck P., Schultz W.P., editors. Psychology of Sustainable Development. Kluwer Academic Publishers; Norwell, MA, USA: 2002. pp. 61–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Ryff C.D. Happiness is everything, or is it—Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1989;57:1069–1081. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Elo A.-L., Dallner M., Gamberale F., Hottinen V., Knardahl S., Lindström K., Skogstad A., Ørhede E. Validation of the nordic questionnaire for psychological and social factors at work—QPSNordic. In: Vartiainen M., Avallone F., Anderson N., editors. Innovative Theories, Tools, and Practices in Work and Organizational Psychology. Hogrefe & Huber Publishers; Ashland, OH, USA: 2000. pp. 47–57. [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Beck A.T., Brown G., Epstein N., Steer R.A. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety—Psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1988;56:893–897. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Rumpf H.J., Meyer C., Hapke U., John U. Screening for mental health: Validity of the MHI-5 using DSM-IV Axis I psychiatric disorders as gold standard. Psychiatry Res. 2001;105:243–253. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00329-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Pangman V.C., Sloan J., Guse L. An examination of psychometric properties of the mini-mental state examination and the standardized mini-mental state examination: Implications for clinical practice. Appl. Nurs. Res. 2000;13:209–213. doi: 10.1053/apnr.2000.9231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Sonnentag S., Fritz C. The recovery experience questionnaire: Development and validation of a measure for assessing recuperation and unwinding from work. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2007;12:204–221. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.12.3.204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Trapnell P.D., Campbell J.D. Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999;76:284–304. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Schaufeli W.B., Bakker A.B., Salanova M. The measurement of work engagement with a short questionnaire—A cross-national study. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2006;66:701–716. doi: 10.1177/0013164405282471. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Brown K.W., Ryan R.M. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003;84:822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Hardy C.J., Rejeski W.J. Not what, but how one feels—The measurement of affect during exercise. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1989;11:304–317. doi: 10.1123/jsep.11.3.304. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Smith J.A. Beyond the divide between cognition and discourse: Using interpretative phenomenological analysis in health psychology. Psychol. Health. 1996;11:261–271. doi: 10.1080/08870449608400256. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Eriksen H.R., Ihlebaek C., Ursin H. A scoring system for Subjective Health Complaints (SHC) Scand. J. Public Health. 1999;27:63–72. doi: 10.1177/14034948990270010401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Neugarten B.L., Havighurst R.J., Tobin S.S. The measurement of life satisfaction. J. Gerontol. 1961;16:134–143. doi: 10.1093/geronj/16.2.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Diener E., Biswas-Diener R. Happiness: Unlocking the Mysteries of Psychological Wealth. Blackwell Publishing; Malden, MA, USA: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Zuckerman M. Development of a situation-specific trait-state test for prediction and measurement of affective responses. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1977;45:513–523. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.45.4.513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Russell J.A., Ward L.M., Pratt G. Affective quality attributed to environments—A factor analytic study. Environ. Behav. 1981;13:259–288. doi: 10.1177/0013916581133001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Kyle G., Graefe A., Manning R. Testing the dimensionality of place attachment in recreational settings. Environ. Behav. 2005;37:153–177. doi: 10.1177/0013916504269654. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Abele-Brehm A., Brehm W. Zur konzeptualisierung und messung von befindlichkeit. Die entwicklung der “befindlichkeitsskalen” (BFS) [the conceptualization and measurement of mood: The development of the ‘Mood Survey.’] Diagnostica. 1986;32:209–228. [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Zhang J.X., Schwarzer R. Measuring optimistic self-beliefs—A Chinese adaptation of the general self-efficacy scale. Psychologia. 1995;38:174–181. [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Clifton R.A., Etcheverry E., Hasinoff S., Roberts L.W. Measuring the cognitive domain of the quality of life of university students. Soc. Indic. Res. 1996;38:29–52. doi: 10.1007/BF00293785. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Roberts L.W., Clifton R.A. Measuring the affective quality-of-life of university-students—The validation of an instrument. Soc. Indic. Res. 1992;27:113–137. doi: 10.1007/BF00300557. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Becker P. Der Trier Personlichkeitsfragebogen [Trier Personality Questionnaire] Hogrefe; Göttingen, Germany: 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Bradburn N.M. Bradburn Scale of Psychological Well-Being. Aldine; Chicago, IL, USA: 1969. [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Cimprich B., Visovatti M., Ronis D.L. The attentional function index-a self-report cognitive measure. Psychooncology. 2011;20:194–202. doi: 10.1002/pon.1729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Cosco N.G., Moore R.C., Islam M.Z. Behavior mapping: A method for linking preschool physical activity and outdoor design. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2010;42:513–519. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181cea27a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Saucier G. Mini-markers: A brief version of goldberg’s unipolar Big-Five markers. J. Personal. Assess. 1994;63:506–516. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6303_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Derogatis L.R., Melisaratos N. The brief symptom inventory: An introductory report. Psychol. Med. 1983;13:595–605. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700048017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Treynor W., Gonzalez R., Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognit. Ther. Res. 2003;27:247–259. doi: 10.1023/A:1023910315561. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Buss A.H., Perry M. The aggression questionnaire. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1992;63:452–459. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Cohen-Mansfield J., Lipson S., Werner P., Billig N., Taylor L., Woosley R. Withdrawal of haloperidol, thioridazine, and lorazepam in the nursing home—A controlled, double-blind study. Arch. Intern. Med. 1999;159:1733–1740. doi: 10.1001/archinte.159.15.1733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Cox R.H., Martens M.P., Russell W.D. Measuring anxiety in athletics: The revised competitive state anxiety inventory-2. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2003;25:519–533. doi: 10.1123/jsep.25.4.519. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Connor K.M., Davidson J.R.T. Development of a new resilience scale: The Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC) Depress. Anxiety. 2003;18:76–82. doi: 10.1002/da.10113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Alexopoulos G.S., Abrams R.C., Young R.C., Shamoian C.A. Cornell scale for depression in dementia. Biol. Psychiatry. 1988;23:271–284. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(88)90038-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Brantley P.J., Waggoner C.D., Jones G.N., Rappaport N.B. A daily stress inventory—Development, reliability, and validity. J. Behav. Med. 1987;10:61–74. doi: 10.1007/BF00845128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Brod M., Stewart A.L., Sands L., Walton P. Conceptualization and measurement of quality of life in dementia: The dementia quality of life instrument (DQOL) Gerontologist. 1999;39:25–35. doi: 10.1093/geront/39.1.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Steer R.A. Modal profile analyses of the SCL-90-r for 7 mental-disorders. Multivar. Exp. Clin. Res. 1983;6:55–67. [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Maloney M.P., Ward M.P., Braucht G.N. Psychology in action—Revised scale for measurement of ecological attitudes and knowledge. Am. Psychol. 1975;30:787–790. doi: 10.1037/h0084394. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Vincenti G.E. Edinburgh post-natal depression scale. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1987;151:865. doi: 10.1192/S0007125000137171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Huta V., Ryan R.M. Pursuing pleasure or virtue: The differential and overlapping well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. J. Happiness Stud. 2010;11:735–762. doi: 10.1007/s10902-009-9171-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Gauvin L., Rejeski W.J. The exercise-induced feeling inventory—Development and initial validation. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1993;15:403–423. doi: 10.1123/jsep.15.4.403. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Gauvin L., Szabo A. Application of the experience sampling method to the study of the effects of exercise withdrawal on weil-being. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1992;14:361–374. doi: 10.1123/jsep.14.4.361. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Svebak S., Murgatroyd S. Metamotivational dominance: A multimethod validation of reversal theory constructs. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1985;48:107–116. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.48.1.107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Gagnon Thompson S.C., Barton M.A. Ecocentric and anthropocentric attitudes toward the environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 1994;14:149–157. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80168-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Kaiser F.G. General measure of ecological behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1998;28:395–422. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01712.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Kozma A., Stones M.J. The measurement of happiness—Development of the memorial-university-of-newfoundland scale of happiness (munsh) J. Gerontol. 1980;35:906–912. doi: 10.1093/geronj/35.6.906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Zigmond A.S., Snaith R.P. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1983;67:361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Seo H.M., Hah Y.S. A study of factors influencing on health promiting lifestyle in the elderly-application of pender’s health promotion model. J. Korean Acad. Nurs. 2004;34:1288–1297. doi: 10.4040/jkan.2004.34.7.1288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Thake C.L., Bambling M., Edirippulige S., Marx E. A psychoevolutionary approach to identifying preferred nature scenes with potential to provide restoration from stress. HERD—Health Environ. Res. Des. J. 2017;10:111–124. doi: 10.1177/1937586717705085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Cohen S., Mermelstein R., Kamarck T., Hoberman H. Measuring the functional components of social support. In: Sarason I.G., Sarason B.R., editors. Social Support: Theory, Research and Applications. Martinus Nijhoff Publishers; Dordrecht, The Netherlands: 1985. pp. 73–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Vagg P.R., Spielberger C.D. The job stress survey: Assessing perceived severity and frequency of occurrence of generic sources of stress in the workplace. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 1999;4:288–292. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.4.3.288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Olsen L.R., Mortensen E.L., Bech P. The SCL-90 and SCL-90R versions validated by item response models in a Danish community sample. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2004;110:225–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00399.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Forsell Y. The major depression inventory versus schedules for clinical assessment in neuropsychiatry in a population sample. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2005;40:209–213. doi: 10.1007/s00127-005-0876-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Priebe S., Huxley P., Knight S., Evans S. Application and results of the Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life (MANSA) Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry. 1999;45:7–12. doi: 10.1177/002076409904500102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Crowne D.P., Marlowe D. A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. J. Consult. Psychol. 1960;24:349–354. doi: 10.1037/h0047358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Maslach C., Jackson S.E., Leiter M.P. Maslach burnout inventory. In: Zalaquett C.P., Wood R.J., editors. Evaluating Stress: A Book of Resources. 3rd ed. Scarecrow Press; Lanham, MD, USA: 1997. pp. 191–218. [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Sheehan D.V., Janavs J., Baker R., Harnett-Sheehan K., Knapp E., Sheehan M., Lecrubier Y., Weiller E., Hergueta T., Amorim P., et al. MINI—Mini international neuropsychiatric interview—English version 5.0.0—DSM-IV. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 1998;59:34–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Sheehan D.V., Lecrubier Y., Sheehan K.H., Amorim P., Janavs J., Weiller E., Hergueta T., Baker R., Dunbar G.C. The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 1998;59:22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Steyer R., Schwenkmezger P., Notz P., Eid M. Der Mehrdimensionale Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF) [the Multidimensional Comfort Questionnaire (MDBF)] Hogrefe; Göttingen, Germany: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Terasaki M., Kishimoto Y., Koga A. Construction of a multiple mood scale. Jpn. J. Psychol. 1992;62:350–356. doi: 10.4992/jjpsy.62.350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Largo-Wight E., Chen W.W., Dodd V., Weiler R. The nature contact questionnaire: A measure of healthy workplace exposure. Work. 2011;40:411–423. doi: 10.3233/Wor-2011-1253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Scott B., Brandberg M., Ohman A. Measuring the negative mood component of stress experiences: Description and psychometric properties of a short adjective check-list of stress responses. Scand. J. Psychol. 2001;42:1–7. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Francis L.J., Brown L.B., Lester D., Philipchalk R. Happiness as stable extraversion: A cross-cultural examination of the reliability and validity of the Oxford happiness inventory among students in the UK, USA, Australia, and Canada. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1998;24:167–171. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00170-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Campbell A., Converse P.E., Rodgers W.L. The Quality of American Life: Perceptions, Evaluations, and Satisfactions. Russell Sage Foundation; New York, NY, USA: 1976. [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Robitschek C. Personal growth initiative: The construct and its measure. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 1998;30:183–198. [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Lafortezza R., Carrus G., Sanesi G., Davies C. Benefits and well-being perceived by people visiting green spaces in periods of heat stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 2009;8:97–108. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2009.02.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Dupuy H. The psychological general wellbeing (PGWB) index. In: Wenger N.K., Mattson M.E., Furberg C.D., Elinson J., editors. Assessment of Quality of Life in Clinical Trials of Cardiovascular Therapies. Le Jacq Publishing; New York, NY, USA: 1984. pp. 170–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Andereck K.L., Nyaupane G. Development of a tourism and quality-of-life instrument. Soc. Indic. Res. Ser. 2011;43:95–113. doi: 10.1007/978-90-481-9861-0_6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Andereck K.L., Nyaupane G.P. Exploring the nature of tourism and quality of life perceptions among residents. J. Travel Res. 2011;50:248–260. doi: 10.1177/0047287510362918. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Pang S.M.C., Chan K.S., Chung B.P.M., Lau K.S., Leung E.M.F., Leung A.W.K., Chan H.Y.L., Chan T.M.F. Assessing quality of life of patients with advanced chronic obstructive pulmonary disease in the end of life. J. Palliat. Care. 2005;21:180–187. doi: 10.1177/082585970502100311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Pang S.M.C., Tse C., Chan K., Chung B.P.M., Leung A.K.A., Leung E.M.F., Ko S.K.K. An empirical analysis of the decision-making of limiting life-sustaining treatment for patients with advanced chronic obstructive pulmonary disease in Hong Kong, China. J. Crit. Care. 2004;19:135–144. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrc.2004.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Burckhardt C.S., Woods S.L., Schultz A.A., Ziebarth D.M. Quality of life of adults with chronic illness—A psychometric study. Res. Nurs. Health. 1989;12:347–354. doi: 10.1002/nur.4770120604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Dunbar G.C., Stoker M.J., Hodges T.C.P., Beaumont G. The development of SBQUL: A unique scale for measuring quality of life. Br. J. Med. Econ. 1992;2:65–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Kim K.Y., Chun B.Y., Kam S., Lee S.W., Park K.S., Chae S.C. Development of measurement scale for the quality of life in hypertensive patients. J. Prev. Med. Public Health. 2005;38:61–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Waterman A.S., Schwartz S.J., Zamboanga B.L., Ravert R.D., Williams M.K., Agocha V.B., Kim S.Y., Donnellan M.B. The questionnaire for eudaimonic well-being: Psychometric properties, demographic comparisons, and evidence of validity. J. Posit. Psychol. 2010;5:41–61. doi: 10.1080/17439760903435208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Hays R.D., Sherbourne C.D., Mazel R.M. User’s Manual for the Medical Outcomes Study (MOS) Core Measures of Health-Related Quality of Life. RAND Corporation; Santa Monica, CA, USA: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 170.McDowell I., Newell C. Measuring Health: A Guide to Rating Scales and Questionnaires. 2nd ed. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 171.Crompton J.L. Motivations for pleasure vacation. Ann. Tour. Res. 1979;6:408–424. doi: 10.1016/0160-7383(79)90004-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Mill R.C.M., Morrison A.M. The Tourism System: An Introductory Text. Prentice-Hall; Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA: 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Pearce P.L., Lee U.-I. Developing the travel career approach to tourist motivation. J. Travel Res. 2005;43:226–237. doi: 10.1177/0047287504272020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Tinsley H.E.A., Kass R.A., Driver B.L. Reliability and concurrent validity of the recreation experience preference scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1981;41:897–907. doi: 10.1177/001316448104100333. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Korpela K.M., Ylen M., Tyrvainen L., Silvennoinen H. Determinants of restorative experiences in everyday favorite places. Health Place. 2008;14:636–652. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2007.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Korpela K.M., Ylen M.P. Effectiveness of favorite-place prescriptions a field experiment. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2009;36:435–438. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.01.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Rizzo J.R., House R.J., Lirtzman S.I. Role conflict and ambiguity in complex organizations. Adm. Sci. Q. 1970;15:150–162. doi: 10.2307/2391486. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Ryan R.M., Weinstein N., Bernstein J., Brown K.W., Mistretta L., Gagne M. Vitalizing effects of being outdoors and in nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010;30:159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.10.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Ericsson A., Nystrom C., Mannerkorpi K. Psychometric properties of the stress and crisis inventory (SCI-93) in females with fibromyalgia and chronic widespread pain. Nord. J. Psychiatry. 2015;69:28–34. doi: 10.3109/08039488.2014.915580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Nyström C., Nyström O. Skattad stress verifierar autonom dysfunktion [estimated stress verifies autonomic dysfunction] Lakartidningen. 1996;93:2583–2584. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Mcauley E., Courneya K.S. The Subjective Exercise Experiences Scale (sees)—Development and preliminary validation. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1994;16:163–177. [Google Scholar]
  • 182.Tangney J.P., Baumeister R.F., Boone A.L. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Personal. 2004;72:271–324. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Han K.T. A reliable and valid self-rating measure of the restorative quality of natural environments. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2003;64:209–232. doi: 10.1016/S0169-2046(02)00241-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Mackay C., Cox T., Burrows G., Lazzerini T. Inventory for measurement of self-reported stress and arousal. Br. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 1978;17:283–284. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1978.tb00280.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Lundberg O., Peck N.M. A simplified way of measuring sense of coherence experiences from a population survey in Sweden. Eur. J. Public Health. 1995;5:56–59. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/5.1.56. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Salkovskis P.M., Rimes K.A., Warwick H.M.C., Clark D.M. The health anxiety inventory: Development and validation of scales for the measurement of health anxiety and hypochondriasis. Psychol. Med. 2002;32:843–853. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702005822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Abdel-Khalek A.M. Measuring happiness with a single-item scale. Soc. Behav. Personal. 2006;34:139–149. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2006.34.2.139. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.Sampson R.J., Raudenbush S.W., Earls F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science. 1997;277:918–924. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Cutrona C.E., Russell D.W. Advances in Personal Relationships. JAI Press; Greenwich, CT, USA: 1987. The provisions of social relationships and adaptation to stress; pp. 37–67. [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Koh K.B., Park J.K., Kim C.H., Cho S. Development of the stress response inventory and its application in clinical practice. Psychosom. Med. 2001;63:668–678. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200107000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Suzuki S.-I., Shimada H., Miura M., Katayanagi K., Umano R., Sakano Y. Development of a new psychological Stress Response Scale (SRS-18) and investigation of the reliability and the validity. Jpn. J. Behav. Med. 1997;4:22–29. doi: 10.11331/jjbm.4.22. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Taylor S.P. Aggressive behavior and physiological arousal as a function of provocation and tendency to inhibit aggression. J. Personal. 1967;35:297. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1967.tb01430.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 193.Thayer R.E. The Biopsychology of Mood and Arousal. Oxford University Press; New York, NY, USA: 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 194.Sherman G.D., Haidt J., Coan J.A. Viewing cute images increases behavioral carefulness. Emotion. 2009;9:282–286. doi: 10.1037/a0014904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 195.Lese K.P., MacNair-Semands R.R. The therapeutic factors inventory: Development of a scale. Group. 2000;24:303–317. doi: 10.1023/A:1026616626780. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 196.Chern J.S., Kielhofner G., delasHeras C.G., Magalhaes L.C. The volitional questionnaire: Psychometric development and practical use. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 1996;50:516–525. doi: 10.5014/ajot.50.7.516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 197.Choi S.M., Kang T.Y., Woo J.M. Development and validation of a modifiedform of the stress response inventory for workers. J. Korean Neuropsychiatr. Assoc. 2006;45:541–553. [Google Scholar]
  • 198.Ferris D.L., Brown D.J., Berry J.W., Lian H.W. The development and validation of the workplace ostracism scale. J. Appl. Psychol. 2008;93:1348–1366. doi: 10.1037/a0012743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 199.Hendee J.C., Catton W.R., Marlow L.D., Brockman C.F. Wilderness Users in the Pacific Northwest—Their Characteristics, Values, and Management Preferences. Pacific Northwest Forest and Range Experiment Station; Portland, OR, USA: 1968. USDA Forest Service research paper PNW-61. [Google Scholar]
  • 200.Zung W.W.K. A self-rating depression scale. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1965;12:63–70. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1965.01720310065008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 201.Beute F., de Kort Y.A.W. The natural context of wellbeing: Ecological momentary assessment of the influence of nature and daylight on affect and stress for individuals with depression levels varying from none to clinical. Health Place. 2018;49:7–18. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 202.Bratman G.N., Daily G.C., Levy B.J., Gross J.J. The benefits of nature experience: Improved affect and cognition. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015;138:41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.02.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 203.Hinds J., Sparks P. The affective quality of human-natural environment relationships. Evol. Psychol. 2011;9:147470491100900314. doi: 10.1177/147470491100900314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 204.Poon K.T., Teng F., Wong W.Y., Chen Z.S. When nature heals: Nature exposure moderates the relationship between ostracism and aggression. J. Environ. Psychol. 2016;48:159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.10.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 205.Wang Y., She Y., Colarelli S.M., Fang Y., Meng H., Chen Q., Zhang X., Zhu H. Exposure to nature counteracts aggression after depletion. Aggress. Behav. 2018;44:89–97. doi: 10.1002/ab.21727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 206.DeWolfe J., Waliczek T.M., Zajicek J.M. The relationship between levels of greenery and landscaping at track and field sites, anxiety, and sports performance of collegiate track and field athletes. HortTechnology. 2011;21:329–335. doi: 10.21273/HORTTECH.21.3.329. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 207.Mackay G.J., Neill J.T. The effect of “green exercise” on state anxiety and the role of exercise duration, intensity, and greenness: A quasi-experimental study. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2010;11:238–245. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2010.01.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 208.Gidlow C.J., Randall J., Gillman J., Smith G.R., Jones M.V. Natural environments and chronic stress measured by hair cortisol. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2016;148:61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.12.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 209.Berman M.G., Kross E., Krpan K.M., Askren M.K., Burson A., Deldin P.J., Kaplan S., Sherdell L., Gotlib I.H., Jonides J. Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2012;140:300–305. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 210.Beyer K.M.M., Szabo A., Nattinger A.B. Time spent outdoors, depressive symptoms, and variation by race and ethnicity. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2016;51:281–290. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2016.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 211.Christensen K.M., Holt J.M., Wilson J.F. The relationship between outdoor recreation and depression among older adults. World Leisure J. 2013;55:72–82. doi: 10.1080/04419057.2012.759143. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 212.Farber M.E., Hall T.E. Emotion and environment: Visitors’ extraordinary experiences along the dalton highway in Alaska. J. Leisure Res. 2007;39:248–270. doi: 10.1080/00222216.2007.11950107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 213.Gonzalez M.T., Hartig T., Patil G.G., Martinsen E.W., Kirkevold M. A prospective study of group cohesiveness in therapeutic horticulture for clinical depression. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 2011;20:119–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0349.2010.00689.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 214.Korpela K.M., Stenggård E., Jussila P. Nature walks as a part of therapeutic intervention for depression. Ecopsychology. 2016;8:8–15. doi: 10.1089/eco.2015.0070. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 215.McCaffrey R. The effect of healing gardens and art therapy on older adults with mild to moderate depression. Holist. Nurs. Pract. 2007;21:79–84. doi: 10.1097/01.HNP.0000262022.80044.06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 216.McCaffrey R., Hanson C., McCaffrey W. Garden walking for depression: A research report. Holist. Nurs. Pract. 2010;24:252–259. doi: 10.1097/HNP.0b013e3181f1acd7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 217.McCaffrey R., Liehr P., Gregersen T., Nishioka R. Garden walking and art therapy for depression in older adults: A pilot study. Res. Gerontol. Nurs. 2011;4:237–242. doi: 10.3928/19404921-20110201-01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 218.McEachan R.R.C., Prady S.L., Smith G., Fairley L., Cabieses B., Gidlow C., Wright J., Dadvand P., van Gent D., Nieuwenhuijsen M.J. The association between green space and depressive symptoms in pregnant women: Moderating roles of socioeconomic status and physical activity. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2016;70:253–259. doi: 10.1136/jech-2015-205954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 219.Min K.B., Kim H.J., Kim H.J., Min J.Y. Parks and green areas and the risk for depression and suicidal indicators. Int. J. Public Health. 2017;62:647–656. doi: 10.1007/s00038-017-0958-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 220.Mukherjee D., Safraj S., Tayyab M., Shivashankar R., Patel S.A., Narayanan G., Ajay V.S., Ali M.K., Narayan K.M.V., Tandon N., et al. Park availability and major depression in individuals with chronic conditions: Is there an association in urban India? Health Place. 2017;47:54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 221.Nichani V., Dirks K., Burns B., Bird A., Grant C. Green space and depression during pregnancy: Results from the growing up in New Zealand study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14 doi: 10.3390/ijerph14091083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 222.Rappe E., Kivelä S.-L. Effects of garden visits on long-term care residents as related to depression. HortTechnology. 2005;15:298–303. doi: 10.21273/HORTTECH.15.2.0298. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 223.Reklaitiene R., Grazuleviciene R., Dedele A., Virviciute D., Vensloviene J., Tamosiunas A., Baceviciene M., Luksiene D., Sapranaviciute-Zabazlajeva L., Radisauskas R., et al. The relationship of green space, depressive symptoms and perceived general health in urban population. Scand. J. Public Health. 2014;42:669–676. doi: 10.1177/1403494814544494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 224.Shin W., Shin C., Yeoun P. The influence of forest therapy camp on depression in alcoholics. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2012;17:73–76. doi: 10.1007/s12199-011-0215-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 225.Torres E.R., Sampselle C.M., Ronis D.L., Neighbors H.W., Gretebeck K.A. Gardening/yard work and depressive symptoms in African Americans. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 2016;30:155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2015.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 226.Wilson J.F., Christensen K.M. The relationship between outdoor recreation and depression among individuals with disabilities. J. Leisure Res. 2012;44:486–506. doi: 10.1080/00222216.2012.11950275. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 227.Han K.-T. The effect of nature and physical activity on emotions and attention while engaging in green exercise. Urban For. Urban Green. 2017;24:5–13. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2017.03.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 228.Marselle M.R., Irvine K.N., Lorenzo-Arribas A., Warber S.L. Moving beyond green: Exploring the relationship of environment type and indicators of perceived environmental quality on emotional well-being following group walks. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2015;12:106–130. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120100106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 229.Ambrey C.L., Shahni T.J. Greenspace and wellbeing in Tehran: A relationship conditional on a neighbourhood’s crime rate? Urban For. Urban Green. 2017;27:155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2017.08.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 230.Dadvand P., Bartoll X., Basagaña X., Dalmau-Bueno A., Martinez D., Ambros A., Cirach M., Triguero-Mas M., Gascon M., Borrell C., et al. Green spaces and general health: Roles of mental health status, social support, and physical activity. Environ. Int. 2016;91:161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2016.02.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 231.de Vries S., van Dillen S.M.E., Groenewegen P.P., Spreeuwenberg P. Streetscape greenery and health: Stress, social cohesion and physical activity as mediators. Soc. Sci. Med. 2013;94:26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 232.Stigsdotter U.K., Ekholm O., Schipperijn J., Toftager M., Kamper-Jørgensen F., Randrup T.B. Health promoting outdoor environments—Associations between green space, and health, health-related quality of life and stress based on a Danish national representative survey. Scand. J. Public Health. 2010;38:411–417. doi: 10.1177/1403494810367468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 233.Takayama N., Korpela K., Lee J., Morikawa T., Tsunetsugu Y., Park B.J., Li Q., Tyrvainen L., Miyazaki Y., Kagawa T. Emotional, restorative and vitalizing effects of forest and urban environments at four sites in Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2014;11:7207–7230. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110707207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 234.Weimann H., Rylander L., Albin M., Skärbäck E., Grahn P., Östergren P.-O., Björk J. Effects of changing exposure to neighbourhood greenness on general and mental health: A longitudinal study. Health Place. 2015;33:48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2015.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 235.Willis K., Crabtree B., Osman L.M., Cathrine K. Green space and health benefits: A QALY and CEA of a mental health programme. J. Environ. Econ. Policy. 2016;5:163–180. doi: 10.1080/21606544.2015.1058195. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 236.Cini F., Kruger S., Ellis S. A model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivations on subjective well-being: The experience of overnight visitors to a national park. Appl. Res. Qual. Life. 2013;8:45–61. doi: 10.1007/s11482-012-9173-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 237.Frash R.E., Jr., Blose J.E., Norman W.C., Patience M. Healthy parks, happy people: An exploratory study of a county park system. J. Park Recreat. Adm. 2016;34:84–102. [Google Scholar]
  • 238.Saw L.E., Lim F.K.S., Carrasco L.R. The relationship between natural park usage and happiness does not hold in a tropical city-state. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0133781. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 239.Dzhambov A.M., Dimitrova D.D. Elderly visitors of an urban park, health anxiety and individual awareness of nature experiences. Urban For. Urban Green. 2014;13:806–813. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2014.05.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 240.Shin W.S. The influence of forest view through a window on job satisfaction and job stress. Scand. J. For. Res. 2007;22:248–253. doi: 10.1080/02827580701262733. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 241.Ambrey C., Fleming C. Public greenspace and life satisfaction in urban Australia. Urban Stud. 2014;51:1290–1321. doi: 10.1177/0042098013494417. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 242.Biedenweg K., Scott R.P., Scott T.A. How does engaging with nature relate to life satisfaction? Demonstrating the link between environment-specific social experiences and life satisfaction. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017;50:112–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.02.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 243.Fleming C.M., Manning M., Ambrey C.L. Crime, greenspace and life satisfaction: An evaluation of the New Zealand experience. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2016;149:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.12.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 244.Waliczek T.M., Zajicek J.M., Lineberger R.D. The influence of gardening activities on consumer perceptions of life satisfaction. Hortscience. 2005;40:1360–1365. doi: 10.21273/HORTSCI.40.5.1360. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 245.Alcock I., White M.P., Wheeler B.W., Fleming L.E., Depledge M.H. Longitudinal effects on mental health of moving to greener and less green urban areas. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2014;48:1247–1255. doi: 10.1021/es403688w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 246.Ambrey C.L. Urban greenspace, physical activity and wellbeing: The moderating role of perceptions of neighbourhood affability and incivility. Land Use Policy. 2016;57:638–644. doi: 10.1016/j.landusepol.2016.06.034. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 247.An M., Colarelli S.M., O’Brien K., Boyajian M.E. Why we need more nature at work: Effects of natural elements and sunlight on employee mental health and work attitudes. PLoS ONE. 2016;11 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0155614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 248.Annerstedt M., Ostergren P.-O., Björk J., Grahn P., Skärbäck E., Währborg P. Green qualities in the neighbourhood and mental health—Results from a longitudinal cohort study in Southern Sweden. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:337. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 249.Astell-Burt T., Mitchell R., Hartig T. The association between green space and mental health varies across the lifecourse. A longitudinal study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2014;68:578–583. doi: 10.1136/jech-2013-203767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 250.Beyer K.M.M., Kaltenbach A., Szabo A., Bogar S., Nieto F.J., Malecki K.M. Exposure to neighborhood green space and mental health: Evidence from the survey of the health of wisconsin. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2014;11:3453–3472. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110303453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 251.Bos E.H., van der Meulen L., Wichers M., Jeronimus B.F. A primrose path? Moderating effects of age and gender in the association between green space and mental health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2016;13 doi: 10.3390/ijerph13050492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 252.Cohen-Cline H., Turkheimer E., Duncan G.E. Access to green space, physical activity and mental health: A twin study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2015;69:523–529. doi: 10.1136/jech-2014-204667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 253.Cox D.T.C., Shanahan D.F., Hudson H.L., Plummer K.E., Siriwardena G.M., Fuller R.A., Anderson K., Hancock S., Gaston K.J. Doses of neighborhood nature: The benefits for mental health of living with nature. BioScience. 2017;67:147–155. doi: 10.1093/biosci/biw173. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 254.Crust L., Henderson H., Middleton G. The acute effects of urban green and countryside walking on psychological health: A field-based study of green exercise. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 2013;44:160–177. doi: 10.7352/ijsp.2013.44.160. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 255.Dzhambov A., Hartig T., Markevych I., Tilov B., Dimitrova D. Urban residential greenspace and mental health in youth: Different approaches to testing multiple pathways yield different conclusions. Environ. Res. 2018;160:47–59. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 256.Gubbels J.S., Kremers S.P.J., Droomers M., Hoefnagels C., Stronks K., Hosman C., de Vries S. The impact of greenery on physical activity and mental health of adolescent and adult residents of deprived neighborhoods: A longitudinal study. Health Place. 2016;40:153–160. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 257.Iwata Y., Dhubháin Á.N., Brophy J., Roddy D., Burke C., Murphy B. Benefits of group walking in forests for people with significant mental ill-health. Ecopsychology. 2016;8:16–26. doi: 10.1089/eco.2015.0045. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 258.Liu H., Li F., Li J., Zhang Y. The relationships between urban parks, residents’ physical activity, and mental health benefits: A case study from beijing, China. J. Environ. Manag. 2017;190:223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2016.12.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 259.Mitchell R. Is physical activity in natural environments better for mental health than physical activity in other environments? Soc. Sci. Med. 2013;91:130–134. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 260.Mutz M., Mueller J. Mental health benefits of outdoor adventures: Results from two pilot studies. J. Adolesc. 2016;49:105–114. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 261.Nutsford D., Pearson A.L., Kingham S. An ecological study investigating the association between access to urban green space and mental health. Public Health. 2013;127:1005–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2013.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 262.Pálsdóttir A.M., Persson D., Persson B., Grahn P. The journey of recovery and empowerment embraced by nature—Clients’ perspectives on nature-based rehabilitation in relation to the role of the natural environment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2014;11:7094–7115. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110707094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 263.Rogerson M., Brown D.K., Sandercock G., Wooller J.J., Barton J. A comparison of four typical green exercise environments and prediction of psychological health outcomes. Perspect. Public Health. 2016;136:171–180. doi: 10.1177/1757913915589845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 264.Ruijsbroek A., Mohnen S.M., Droomers M., Kruize H., Gidlow C., Grazuleviciene R., Andrusaityte S., Maas J., Nieuwenhuijsen M.J., Triguero-Mas M., et al. Neighbourhood green space, social environment and mental health: An examination in four european cities. Int. J. Public Health. 2017;62:657–667. doi: 10.1007/s00038-017-0963-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 265.Sturm R., Cohen D. Proximity to urban parks and mental health. J. Ment. Health Policy Econ. 2014;17:19–24. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 266.Sugiyama T., Leslie E., Giles-Corti B., Owen N. Associations of neighbourhood greenness with physical and mental health: Do walking, social coherence and local social interaction explain the relationships? J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2008;62:e9. doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.064287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 267.Sugiyama T., Villanueva K., Knuiman M., Francis J., Foster S., Wood L., Giles-Corti B. Can neighborhood green space mitigate health inequalities? A study of socio-economic status and mental health. Health Place. 2016;38:16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 268.van den Berg M., van Poppel M., Smith G., Triguero-Mas M., Andrusaityte S., van Kamp I., van Mechelen W., Gidlow C., Gražulevičiene R., Nieuwenhuijsen M.J., et al. Does time spent on visits to green space mediate the associations between the level of residential greenness and mental health? Urban For. Urban Green. 2017;25:94–102. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2017.04.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 269.van den Berg M., van Poppel M., van Kamp I., Andrusaityte S., Balseviciene B., Cirach M., Danileviciute A., Ellis N., Hurst G., Masterson D., et al. Visiting green space is associated with mental health and vitality: A cross-sectional study in four european cities. Health Place. 2016;38:8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 270.van den Bosch M.A., Östergren P.-O., Grahn P., Skärbäck E., Währborg P. Moving to serene nature may prevent poor mental health-results from a Swedish longitudinal cohort study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2015;12:7974–7989. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120707974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 271.Vujcic M., Tomicevic-Dubljevic J., Grbic M., Lecic-Tosevski D., Vukovic O., Toskovic O. Nature based solution for improving mental health and well-being in urban areas. Environ. Res. 2017;158:385–392. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 272.White M.P., Alcock I., Wheeler B.W., Depledge M.H. Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data. Psychol. Sci. 2013;24:920–928. doi: 10.1177/0956797612464659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 273.Wilson N.W., Jones R., Fleming S., Lafferty K., Knifton L., Cathrine K., McNish H. Branching out: The impact of a mental health ecotherapy program. Ecopsychology. 2011;3:1–7. doi: 10.1089/eco.2010.0049. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 274.Wilson N.W., Fleming S., Jones R., Lafferty K., Cathrine K., Seaman P., Knifton L. Green shoots of recovery: The impact of a mental health ecotherapy programme. Ment. Health Rev. J. 2010;15:4–14. doi: 10.5042/mhrj.2010.0366. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 275.Wood L., Hooper P., Foster S., Bull F. Public green spaces and positive mental health—Investigating the relationship between access, quantity and types of parks and mental wellbeing. Health Place. 2017;48:63–71. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 276.Ambrey C.L., Cartlidge N. Do the psychological benefits of greenspace depend on one’s personality? Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017;116:233–239. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 277.Budruk M., Lee W. Importance of managing for personal benefits, hedonic and utilitarian motivations, and place attachment at an urban natural setting. Environ. Manag. 2016;58:504–517. doi: 10.1007/s00267-016-0723-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 278.Gilchrist K., Brown C., Montarzino A. Workplace settings and wellbeing: Greenspace use and views contribute to employee wellbeing at pen-urban business sites. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015;138:32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.02.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 279.Henderson-Wilson C., Sia K.L., Veitch J., Staiger P.K., Davidson P., Nicholls P. Perceived health benefits and willingness to pay for parks by park users: Quantitative and qualitative research. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14 doi: 10.3390/ijerph14050529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 280.Houlden V., Weich S., Jarvis S. A cross-sectional analysis of green space prevalence and mental wellbeing in England. BMC Public Health. 2017;17 doi: 10.1186/s12889-017-4401-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 281.Jakubec S.L., Den Hoed D.C., Ray H., Krishnamurthy A. Mental well-being and quality-of-life benefits of inclusion in nature for adults with disabilities and their caregivers. Landsc. Res. 2016;41:616–627. doi: 10.1080/01426397.2016.1197190. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 282.Barton J., Griffin M., Pretty J. Exercise-, nature- and socially interactive-based initiatives improve mood and self-esteem in the clinical population. Perspect. Public Health. 2012;132:89–96. doi: 10.1177/1757913910393862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 283.Joye Y., Bolderdijk J.W. An exploratory study into the effects of extraordinary nature on emotions, mood, and prosociality. Front. Psychol. 2015;5 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 284.McCaffrey R., Liehr P. The effect of reflective garden walking on adults with increased levels of psychological stress. J. Holist. Nurs. 2016;34:177–184. doi: 10.1177/0898010115594934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 285.Astell-Burt T., Feng X., Kolt G.S. Mental health benefits of neighbourhood green space are stronger among physically active adults in middle-to-older age: Evidence from 260,061 australians. Prev. Med. 2013;57:601–606. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2013.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 286.Hansmann R., Hug S.-M., Seeland K. Restoration and stress relief through physical activities in forests and parks. Urban For. Urban Green. 2007;6:213–225. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2007.08.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 287.Hartig T., Kaiser F.G., Strumse E. Psychological restoration in nature as a source of motivation for ecological behaviour. Environ. Conserv. 2007;34:291–299. doi: 10.1017/S0376892907004250. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 288.Calogiuri G., Evensen K., Weydahl A., Andersson K., Patil G., Ihlebæk C., Raanaas R.K. Green exercise as a workplace intervention to reduce job stress. Results from a pilot study. Work. 2016;53:99–111. doi: 10.3233/WOR-152219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 289.Barnicle T., Midden K.S. The effects of a horticulture activity program on the psychological well-being of older people in a long-term care facility. HortTechnology. 2003;13:81–85. doi: 10.21273/HORTTECH.13.1.0081. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 290.Fruhauf A., Niedermeier M., Elliott L.R., Ledochowski L., Marksteiner J., Kopp M. Acute effects of outdoor physical activity on affect and psychological well-being in depressed patients—A preliminary study. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2016;10:4–9. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2016.02.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 291.Herzog T.R., Strevey S.J. Contact with nature, sense of humor, and psychological well-being. Environ. Behav. 2008;40:747–776. doi: 10.1177/0013916507308524. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 292.Hug S.-M., Hansmann R., Monn C., Krütli P., Seeland K. Restorative effects of physical activity in forests and indoor settings. Int. J. Fit. 2008;4:25–37. [Google Scholar]
  • 293.Kamitsis I., Francis A.J.P. Spirituality mediates the relationship between engagement with nature and psychological wellbeing. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013;36:136–143. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.07.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 294.Lawton E., Brymer E., Clough P., Denovan A. The relationship between the physical activity environment, nature relatedness, anxiety, and the psychological well-being benefits of regular exercisers. Front. Psychol. 2017;8 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 295.Martens D., Gutscher H., Bauer N. Walking in “wild” and “tended” urban forests: The impact on psychological well-being. J. Environ. Psychol. 2011;31:36–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.11.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 296.Wolsko C., Lindberg K. Experiencing connection with nature: The matrix of psychological well-being, mindfulness, and outdoor recreation. Ecopsychology. 2013;5:80–91. doi: 10.1089/eco.2013.0008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 297.Bricker K.S., Hendricks W.W., Aschenbrenner C.A. Californians’ perceptions of the influence of parks and recreation on quality of life. J. Park Recreat. Adm. 2016;34:64–82. doi: 10.18666/JPRA-2016-V34-I3-7441. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 298.Camargo D.M., Ramirez P.C., Fermino R.C. Individual and environmental correlates to quality of life in park users in Colombia. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14 doi: 10.3390/ijerph14101250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 299.Lai C.K.Y., Lau C.K.Y., Kan W.Y., Lam W.M., Fung C.Y.Y. The effect of horticultural therapy on the quality of life of palliative care patients. J. Psychosoc. Oncol. 2017;35:278–291. doi: 10.1080/07347332.2017.1286699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 300.McFarland A.L., Waliczek T.M., Zajicek J.M. The relationship between student use of campus green spaces and perceptions of quality of life. HortTechnology. 2008;18:232–238. doi: 10.21273/HORTTECH.18.2.232. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 301.Shafer C.S., Lee B.K., Turner S. A tale of three greenway trails: User perceptions related to quality of life. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2000;49:163–178. doi: 10.1016/S0169-2046(00)00057-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 302.Sommerfeld A.J., Waliczek T.M., Zajicek J.M. Growing minds: Evaluating the effect of gardening on quality of life and physical activity level of older adults. HortTechnology. 2010;20:705–710. doi: 10.21273/HORTTECH.20.4.705. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 303.Tiyarattanachai R., Hollmann N.M. Green campus initiative and its impacts on quality of life of stakeholders in green and non-green campus universities. SpringerPlus. 2016;5:84. doi: 10.1186/s40064-016-1697-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 304.de Bloom J., Sianoja M., Korpela K., Tuomisto M., Lilja A., Geurts S., Kinnunen U. Effects of park walks and relaxation exercises during lunch breaks on recovery from job stress: Two randomized controlled trials. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017;51:14–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.03.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 305.Bodin M., Hartig T. Does the outdoor environment matter for psychological restoration gained through running? Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2003;4:141–153. doi: 10.1016/S1469-0292(01)00038-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 306.Chen Z., He Y., Yu Y. Enhanced functional connectivity properties of human brains during in-situ nature experience. PeerJ. 2016;2016 doi: 10.7717/peerj.2210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 307.Hartig T., Mang M., Evans G.W. Restorative effects of natural environment experiences. Environ. Behav. 1991;23:3–26. doi: 10.1177/0013916591231001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 308.Korpela K.M., Hartig T., Kaiser F.G., Fuhrer U. Restorative experience and self-regulation in favorite places. Environ. Behav. 2001;33:572–589. doi: 10.1177/00139160121973133. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 309.Weng P.-Y., Chiang Y.-C. Psychological restoration through indoor and outdoor leisure activities. J. Leisure Res. 2014;46:203–217. doi: 10.1080/00222216.2014.11950320. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 310.Sonntag-Ostrom E., Nordin M., Lundell Y., Dolling A., Wiklund U., Karlsson M., Carlberg B., Jarvholm L.S. Restorative effects of visits to urban and forest environments in patients with exhaustion disorder. Urban For. Urban Green. 2014;13:344–354. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2013.12.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 311.von Lindern E., Bauer N., Frick J., Hunziker M., Hartig T. Occupational engagement as a constraint on restoration during leisure time in forest settings. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013;118:90–97. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2013.03.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 312.Cervinka R., Schwab M., Schönbauer R., Hämmerle I., Pirgie L., Sudkamp J. My garden—My mate? Perceived restorativeness of private gardens and its predictors. Urban For. Urban Green. 2016;16:182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2016.01.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 313.Hartig T., Kaiser F.G., Bowler P.A. Psychological restoration in nature as a positive motivation for ecological behavior. Environ. Behav. 2001;33:590–607. doi: 10.1177/00139160121973142. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 314.Hipp J.A., Gulwadi G.B., Alves S., Sequeira S. The relationship between perceived greenness and perceived restorativeness of university campuses and student-reported quality of life. Environ. Behav. 2016;48:1292–1308. doi: 10.1177/0013916515598200. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 315.Twedt E., Rainey R.M., Proffitt D.R. Designed natural spaces: Informal gardens are perceived to be more restorative than formal gardens. Front. Psychol. 2016;7 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 316.Abkar M., Kamal M.S.M., Maulan S., Mariapan M., Davoodi S.R. Relationship between the preference and perceived restorative potential of urban landscapes. HortTechnology. 2011;21:514–519. doi: 10.21273/HORTTECH.21.5.514. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 317.Nordh H., Alalouch C., Hartig T. Assessing restorative components of small urban parks using conjoint methodology. Urban For. Urban Green. 2011;10:95–103. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2010.12.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 318.Tenngart Ivarsson C., Hagerhall C.M. The perceived restorativeness of gardens—Assessing the restorativeness of a mixed built and natural scene type. Urban For. Urban Green. 2008;7:107–118. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2008.01.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 319.Bratman G.N., Hamilton J.P., Hahn K.S., Daily G.C., Gross J.J. Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual prefrontal cortex activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:8567–8572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1510459112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 320.Hoffman A.J., Knight L.F.M., Wallach J. Gardening activities, education, and self-esteem—Learning outside the classroom. Urban Educ. 2007;42:403–411. doi: 10.1177/0042085907304909. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 321.Wood C.J., Pretty J., Griffin M. A case-control study of the health and well-being benefits of allotment gardening. J. Public Health. 2016;38:E336–E344. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdv146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 322.Fan Y., Das K.V., Chen Q. Neighborhood green, social support, physical activity, and stress: Assessing the cumulative impact. Health Place. 2011;17:1202–1211. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2011.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 323.Grahn P., Stigsdotter U.A. Landscape planning and stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 2003;2:1–18. doi: 10.1078/1618-8667-00019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 324.Largo-Wight E., Wlyudka P.S., Merten J.W., Cuvelier E.A. Effectiveness and feasibility of a 10-minute employee stress intervention: Outdoor booster break. J. Workplace Behav. Health. 2017;32:159–171. doi: 10.1080/15555240.2017.1335211. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 325.Lottrup L., Grahn P., Stigsdotter U.K. Workplace greenery and perceived level of stress: Benefits of access to a green outdoor environment at the workplace. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013;110:5–11. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2012.09.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 326.Morita E., Fukuda S., Nagano J., Hamajima N., Yamamoto H., Iwai Y., Nakashima T., Ohira H., Shirakawa T. Psychological effects of forest environments on healthy adults: Shinrin-yoku (forest-air bathing, walking) as a possible method of stress reduction. Public Health. 2007;121:54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2006.05.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 327.Orsega-Smith E., Mowen A.J., Payne L.L., Godbey G. The interaction of stress and park use on psycho-physiological health in older adults. J. Leisure Res. 2004;36:232–256. doi: 10.1080/00222216.2004.11950021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 328.Roe J.J., Aspinall P.A., Thompson C.W. Coping with stress in deprived urban neighborhoods: What is the role of green space according to life stage? Front. Psychol. 2017;8 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 329.Roe J.J., Thompson C.W., Aspinall P.A., Brewer M.J., Duff E.I., Miller D., Mitchell R., Clow A. Green space and stress: Evidence from cortisol measures in deprived urban communities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2013;10:4086–4103. doi: 10.3390/ijerph10094086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 330.Sidenius U., Stigsdotter U.K., Poulsen D.V., Bondas T. “I look at my own forest and fields in a different way”: The lived experience of nature-based therapy in a therapy garden when suffering from stress-related illness. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Health Well-Being. 2017;12 doi: 10.1080/17482631.2017.1324700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 331.Stigsdotter U.K., Grahn P. Stressed individuals’ preferences for activities and environmental characteristics in green spaces. Urban For. Urban Green. 2011;10:295–304. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2011.07.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 332.Thompson C.W., Roe J., Aspinall P., Mitchell R., Clow A., Miller D. More green space is linked to less stress in deprived communities: Evidence from salivary cortisol patterns. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2012;105:221–229. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.12.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 333.Thompson C.W., Aspinall P., Roe J., Robertson L., Miller D. Mitigating stress and supporting health in deprived urban communities: The importance of green space and the social environment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2016;13 doi: 10.3390/ijerph13040440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 334.Tyrvainen L., Ojala A., Korpela K., Lanki T., Tsunetsugu Y., Kagawa T. The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014;38:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.12.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 335.Van Den Berg A.E., Custers M.H.G. Gardening promotes neuroendocrine and affective restoration from stress. J. Health Psychol. 2010;16:3–11. doi: 10.1177/1359105310365577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 336.Olafsdottir G., Cloke P., Vogele C. Place, green exercise and stress: An exploration of lived experience and restorative effects. Health Place. 2017;46:358–365. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 337.Bjornstad S., Patil G.G., Raanaas R.K. Nature contact and organizational support during office working hours: Benefits relating to stress reduction, subjective health complaints, and sick leave. Work—J. Prev. Assess. Rehabilit. 2016;53:9–20. doi: 10.3233/WOR-152211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 338.Hull R.B., Michael S.E. Nature-based recreation, mood change, and stress restoration. Leisure Sci. 1995;17:1–14. doi: 10.1080/01490409509513239. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 339.Ambrey C.L. An investigation into the synergistic wellbeing benefits of greenspace and physical activity: Moving beyond the mean. Urban For. Urban Green. 2016;19:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2016.06.020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 340.Bertram C., Rehdanz K. The role of urban green space for human well-being. Ecol. Econ. 2015;120:139–152. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2015.10.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 341.Carrus G., Scopelliti M., Lafortezza R., Colangelo G., Ferrini F., Salbitano F., Agrimi M., Portoghesi L., Sernenzato P., Sanesi G. Go greener, feel better? The positive effects of biodiversity on the well-being of individuals visiting, urban and peri-urban green areas. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015;134:221–228. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2014.10.022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 342.Cervinka R., Röderer K., Hefler E. Are nature lovers happy? On various indicators of well-being and connectedness with nature. J. Health Psychol. 2011;17:379–388. doi: 10.1177/1359105311416873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 343.Korpela K., De Bloom J., Sianoja M., Pasanen T., Kinnunen U. Nature at home and at work: Naturally good? Links between window views, indoor plants, outdoor activities and employee well-being over one year. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017;160:38–47. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.12.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 344.Korpela K., Nummi T., Lipiainen L., De Bloom J., Sianoja M., Pasanen T., Kinnunen U. Nature exposure predicts well-being trajectory groups among employees across two years. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017;52:81–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.06.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 345.Kothencz G., Kolcsar R., Cabrera-Barona P., Szilassi P. Urban green space perception and its contribution to well-being. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14 doi: 10.3390/ijerph14070766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 346.Krekel C., Kolbe J., Wuestemann H. The greener, the happier? The effect of urban land use on residential well-being. Ecol. Econ. 2016;121:117–127. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2015.11.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 347.Leck C., Upton D., Evans N. Growing well-beings: The positive experience of care farms. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2015;20:745–762. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 348.Loureiro A., Veloso S. Outdoor exercise, well-being and connectedness to nature. PSICO. 2014;45:299–304. doi: 10.15448/1980-8623.2014.3.19180. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 349.Luck G.W., Davidson P., Boxall D., Smallbone L. Relations between urban bird and plant communities and human well-being and connection to nature. Conserv. Biol. 2011;25:816–826. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2011.01685.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 350.Marselle M.R., Irvine K.N., Warber S.L. Examining group walks in nature and multiple aspects of well-being: A large-scale study. Ecopsychology. 2014;6:134–147. doi: 10.1089/eco.2014.0027. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 351.Nisbet E.K., Zelenski J.M., Murphy S.A. Happiness is in our nature: Exploring nature relatedness as a contributor to subjective well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 2011;12:303–322. doi: 10.1007/s10902-010-9197-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 352.Holli-Anne P., Andrew J.H. Nature involvement increases hedonic and eudaimonic well-being: A two-week experimental study. Ecopsychology. 2014;6:148–154. doi: 10.1089/eco.2014.0023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 353.Richardson M., Cormack A., McRobert L., Underhill R. 30 days wild: Development and evaluation of a large-scale nature engagement campaign to improve well-being. PLoS ONE. 2016;11 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 354.Scopelliti M., Carrus G., Adinolfi C., Suarez G., Colangelo G., Lafortezza R., Panno A., Sanesi G. Staying in touch with nature and well-being in different income groups: The experience of urban parks in bogota. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2016;148:139–148. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.11.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 355.Wang X., Rodiek S., Wu C., Chen Y., Li Y. Stress recovery and restorative effects of viewing different urban park scenes in Shanghai, China. Urban For. Urban Green. 2016;15:112–122. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2015.12.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 356.Webber J., Hinds J., Camic P.M. The well-being of allotment gardeners: A mixed methodological study. Ecopsychology. 2015;7:20–28. doi: 10.1089/eco.2014.0058. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 357.McClimens A., Doel M., Ibbotson R., Partridge N., Muscroft E., Lockwood L. How do the ‘peace gardens’ make you feel? Public space and personal wellbeing in city centre Sheffield. J. Urban Des. 2012;17:117–133. doi: 10.1080/13574809.2011.593961. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 358.Nisbet E.K., Zelenski J.M. Underestimating nearby nature: Affective forecasting errors obscure the happy path to sustainability. Psychol. Sci. 2011;22:1101–1106. doi: 10.1177/0956797611418527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 359.Akobeng A.K. Understanding randomised controlled trials. Arch. Dis. Child. 2005;90:840–844. doi: 10.1136/adc.2004.058222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 360.Stoney C.M., Johnson L.L. Design of clinical studies and trials. In: Gallin J.I., Ognibene F.P., editors. Principles and Practice of Clinical Research. 3rd ed. Academic Press; London, UK: 2012. pp. 225–242. [Google Scholar]
  • 361.South E.C., Kondo M.C., Cheney R.A., Branas C.C. Neighborhood blight, stress, and health: A walking trial of urban greening and ambulatory heart rate. Am. J. Public Health. 2015;105:909–913. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 362.Tilley S., Neale C., Patuano A., Cinderby S. Older people’s experiences of mobility and mood in an urban environment: A mixed methods approach using electroencephalography (EEG) and interviews. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14 doi: 10.3390/ijerph14020151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 363.Fleuret S., Atkinson S. Wellbeing, health and geography: A critical review and research agenda. N. Z. Geogr. 2007;63:106–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-7939.2007.00093.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 364.Gesler W.M., Kearns R.A. Culture/Place/Health. Routledge; New York, NY, USA: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 365.Heslop P., Blair P., Flemming P., Hoghton M., Marriot A., Russ L. Confidential Inquiry into Premature Deaths for People with Learning Disabilities (CIPOLD) Norah Fry Research Centre, University of Bristol; Bristol, UK: 2013. Final Report. [Google Scholar]
  • 366.Honold J., Lakes T., Beyer R., van der Meer E. Restoration in urban spaces: Nature views from home, greenways, and public parks. Environ. Behav. 2016;48:796–825. doi: 10.1177/0013916514568556. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 367.Ng K.S.T., Sia A., Ng M.K.W., Tan C.T.Y., Chan H.Y., Tan C.H., Rawtaer I., Feng L., Mahendran R., Larbi A., et al. Effects of horticultural therapy on asian older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2018;15 doi: 10.3390/ijerph15081705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 368.Liu Y., Ho R.C.-M., Mak A. Interleukin (il)-6, tumour necrosis factor alpha (tnf-α) and soluble interleukin-2 receptors (sil-2r) are elevated in patients with major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis and meta-regression. J. Affect. Disord. 2012;139:230–239. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES