Abstract
Psychedelic drugs produce profound changes in consciousness, but the underlying neurobiological mechanisms for this remain unclear. Spontaneous and induced oscillatory activity was recorded in healthy human participants with magnetoencephalography after intravenous infusion of psilocybin—prodrug of the nonselective serotonin 2A receptor agonist and classic psychedelic psilocin. Psilocybin reduced spontaneous cortical oscillatory power from 1 to 50 Hz in posterior association cortices, and from 8 to 100 Hz in frontal association cortices. Large decreases in oscillatory power were seen in areas of the default-mode network. Independent component analysis was used to identify a number of resting-state networks, and activity in these was similarly decreased after psilocybin. Psilocybin had no effect on low-level visually induced and motor-induced gamma-band oscillations, suggesting that some basic elements of oscillatory brain activity are relatively preserved during the psychedelic experience. Dynamic causal modeling revealed that posterior cingulate cortex desynchronization can be explained by increased excitability of deep-layer pyramidal neurons, which are known to be rich in 5-HT2A receptors. These findings suggest that the subjective effects of psychedelics result from a desynchronization of ongoing oscillatory rhythms in the cortex, likely triggered by 5-HT2A receptor-mediated excitation of deep pyramidal cells.
Introduction
Psychedelics have been used in various human cultures for thousands of years. Recent studies have shown that a single dose of a psychedelic can produce remarkable effects, such as profound spiritual experiences and long-term changes in personality (MacLean et al., 2011). Other research suggests that psychedelics may be useful in treating alcohol dependence (Krebs and Johansen, 2012), obsessive-compulsive disorder (Moreno et al., 2006), and anxiety related to terminal illness (Grob et al., 2011). It has also been proposed that psychedelics may model certain psychotic symptoms (Vollenweider et al., 1998; Carhart-Harris et al., 2012b). The neural mechanisms mediating all of these effects remain largely unknown, but it is known that all psychedelics are 5-HT2A receptor agonists (Glennon et al., 1984; Vollenweider et al., 1998).
Electroencephalographic (EEG) studies in humans have found decreased cortical synchrony after ingestion of the psychedelic plant brew ayahuasca (Riba et al., 2004). Local field potential recordings in rats have demonstrated that both serotonin (Puig et al., 2010) and the 5-HT2A receptor agonist 1-(2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodophenyl-2-aminopropane) (DOI; Wood et al., 2012) reduce high-frequency brain activity. However, the analysis of high-frequency time-series components is difficult with EEG recordings (Whitham et al., 2007, 2008). Magnetoencephalography (MEG), the magnetic counterpart of EEG, is more robust to the tissue conduction effects present in EEG and provides both more accurate source localization and better characterization of high-frequency (>20 Hz) activity due to the decreased influence of muscle artifacts (Whitham et al., 2007). For these reasons, here, for the first time, we used MEG to directly record broadband (1–100 Hz) neural activity during the human psychedelic state.
Recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in healthy participants found decreased blood flow and venous oxygenation in the thalamus, putamen, and association cortices after psilocybin administration, implying decreased neural activity in these regions (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012a). However, fMRI measures changes in neural activity indirectly via changes in brain blood flow, which can be problematic when studying a drug that might directly affect the cerebral vasculature. Moreover, the poor temporal resolution of the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal of fMRI provides limited information about the higher-frequency neural dynamics underlying different states of consciousness. Thus, to directly assess the effects of a psychedelic on neural activity in the human brain, we performed MEG recordings.
Fifteen healthy volunteers with prior experience with psychedelics received a bolus infusion of 10 ml of saline in one MEG session and 2 mg of psilocybin in 10 ml of saline in a second session, in a within-subjects single-blind design. Each resting-state recording lasted 11 min. There was a 5 min preinfusion baseline period, followed by a 60 s infusion of psilocybin, and a 5 min postinfusion period. The subjective effects of psilocybin begin within seconds when administered in this way, allowing the capture of the transition from normal waking consciousness to the psychedelic state. Participants were instructed to relax and keep their eyes open for the duration of the scan, and a fixation point was presented throughout. Approximately 6 min after the infusion, participants performed a visuomotor task designed to elicit stimulus-induced gamma-band oscillations in the primary visual and motor cortices. After exiting the scanner, participants completed a 23-item questionnaire scoring specific aspects of their psychedelic experience.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Fifteen healthy male participants (mean age, 34.5 years, SD, 2.3 years) gave informed consent to participate in the study. The study was approved by a National Health Service research ethics committee. A physical examination, including electrocardiogram (ECG), routine blood tests, and urine test for drugs of abuse was carried out. A psychiatric assessment was conducted, and participants disclosed their drug-taking histories. Exclusion criteria were as follows: <21 years of age, pregnancy, personal or immediate family history of psychiatric disorder, substance dependence, cardiovascular disease, claustrophobia, blood or needle phobia, or a significant adverse response to a hallucinogenic drug. All participants had previous experience with a hallucinogenic drug but not within 6 weeks of the study.
Drug infusion and protocol
Because psilocybin is known to cause long-term psychological changes (Griffiths et al., 2006, 2008, 2011; MacLean et al., 2011; of unknown biological substrate), which far outlive the drug half-life, we opted for a fixed-order design. Participants underwent two scans on each day, one using a placebo saline infusion, and one using psilocybin. The psilocybin was always received second, but participants were blinded to the order of infusion. To minimize any potential order effect (see Discussion), we compared the postinfusion data with a preinfusion baseline. For stimulus-induced activity, we have already demonstrated that the protocols used are extremely robust to time-of-day and repeated-exposure effects (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2010, 2013a,b). Participants were seated in the MEG with their heads partly stabilized by a plastic chin rest. In each scan session, 5 min of resting MEG was recorded, and then the participant was infused over 60 s (2 mg in 10 ml of saline). The subjective effects of psilocybin begin within seconds (Carhart-Harris et al., 2011, 2012a) of infusion, and hence we recorded 5 min of resting MEG data immediately after infusion. At this point, participants were asked to provide a rating of their subjective “high” on a scale between 0 and 100 using button press in the scanner. Following this, the 10 min visuomotor stimulation task was performed (described below). Participants provided another rating after completion of this task. Participants' pulse rate and blood oxygenation level were continually monitored throughout the experiment. After scanning, participants were asked to complete a 23-item subjective rating scale pertaining to characteristic effects of psychedelics. The rating scale structure was informed by the five-dimension altered states of consciousness rating scale of Dittrich (1998), but the items were selected based on our own previous empirical work (Carhart-Harris et al., 2011, 2012a), and were reduced to 22 items (plus 1 control item) for concision and to reflect subjective phenomena that are most characteristic of the psychedelic state.
Visuomotor paradigm
Following the resting recordings, participants were presented with a visual stimulus consisting of a vertical, stationary, maximum-contrast, 3 cycles/°, square-wave grating presented on a mean luminance background. The stimulus subtended 8° both horizontally and vertically with a small red fixation square located at the center of the stimulus, which remained on for the entire stimulation protocol (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2009; Swettenham et al., 2009). The stimulus was presented on a Mitsubishi Diamond Pro 2070 monitor controlled by the Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). The screen size was 1024 × 768 pixels, and the monitor frame rate was 100 Hz. The monitor was outside the magnetically shielded room and was viewed directly from within, at 2.15 m, through a cutaway portal in the shield. The duration of each stimulus was 1.5–2 s followed by 2 s of the fixation square only. Participants were instructed to fixate for the entire experiment and to maintain attention where instructed to abduct their index finger at the termination of each stimulation period. This recording took ∼10 min.
MEG acquisition and analysis
Whole-head MEG recordings were made using a CTF 275-channel radial gradiometer system sampled at 600 Hz (0–150 Hz bandpass filter). An additional 29 reference channels were recorded for noise cancellation purposes, and the primary sensors were analyzed as synthetic third-order gradiometers (Vrba and Robinson, 2001). Three of the 275 channels were turned off due to excessive sensor noise. At the onset of each stimulus presentation, a transistor–transistor logic pulse was sent to the MEG system. Participants were fitted with three electromagnetic head coils (nasion and preauriculars), which were localized relative to the MEG system immediately before and after the recording session. Additional vertical and horizontal electroculograms, an ECG, and an electromyogram (EMG) from the right first dorsal interosseus were also recorded with the MEG. Additionally, finger movements of the right index finger during the visuomotor task were recorded with an optical displacement system (Muthukumaraswamy, 2010).
For source localization, a 3D Fast Spoiled Gradient Recalled (FSPGR) anatomical MRI scan was obtained using a 3 T General Electric HDx scanner with an eight-channel receive-only head receptive-field coil from each participant. Fast spoiled gradient echo scans were acquired in an oblique–axial orientation, with 1 mm isotropic voxel resolution. To achieve MRI/MEG coregistration, for source localization, the fiduciary markers were placed at fixed distances from anatomical landmarks identifiable in participants' anatomical MRIs (tragus, eye center). Fiduciary locations were verified afterward using digital photographs.
MEG analysis
Preprocessing of resting data.
Five minutes of data obtained immediately before the beginning of infusion and immediately after the cessation of infusion were extracted from the continuous recording, high-pass filtered at 1 Hz, and segmented into epochs of 2 s in length. We discarded the MEG data during infusion as it was potentially confounded, and as participants could sometimes detect the solution entering their arm or hear the physician move as they dispensed the solution (the magnetically shielded room is nearly silent). Hence, there were 150 epochs both preinfusion and postinfusion for analysis. Each epoch was visually inspected, and those with gross artifacts (e.g., head movements, jaw clenches) were removed from the analysis. One participant was removed from all MEG analyses due to excessive artifacts (head movements and jaw clenching).
Source localization of resting oscillatory changes.
To localize drug-induced changes in oscillatory power, we used the beamformer algorithm synthetic aperture magnetometry (SAM; Robinson and Vrba, 1999). In addition to localizing activity, beamformers are effective in detecting and suppressing a number of potential artifacts, including muscle (Carl et al., 2012), ocular (Bardouille et al., 2006), dental (Cheyne et al., 2007), and stimulator artifacts (Litvak et al., 2010). Global covariance matrices were calculated for the following six bandpass-filtered versions of the datasets: delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), low gamma (30–50 Hz), and high gamma (50–100 Hz). These frequency bands were based on previous investigations of resting-state MEG data (Brookes et al., 2011; Hall et al., 2013). Based on these covariance matrices, using the beamformer algorithm (Robinson and Vrba, 1999), a set of beamformer weights was computed for all voxels in the brain at 4 mm isotropic voxel resolution. A multiple local-spheres (Huang et al., 1999) volume conductor model was derived by fitting spheres to the brain surface extracted by the FSL Brain Extraction Tool (Smith, 2002). For SAM imaging, virtual sensors were constructed at each beamformer voxel and Student's t images of source power changes computed for postinfusion versus preinfusion epochs.
Group statistical analysis was conducted using software from the FSL software library (www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/). Individual SAM images were first spatially normalized onto the MNI (T1) average brain using the FMRIB Linear Affine Registration Tool (FLIRT). This was done by first obtaining a set of warping parameters by registering the participant's anatomical MRI with the average brain and then applying these parameters to the SAM source power maps. Statistical group analysis of the normalized SAM images was performed on each condition and frequency band image using nonparametric permutation testing (Nichols and Holmes, 2002; Singh et al., 2003). Five thousand permutations were calculated for each statistical test conducted with a 5 × 5 × 5 mm Gaussian smoothing kernel applied to the variance maps. We computed a paired t test for the placebo versus psilocybin after and before images to reveal the drug interaction effect. t images were thresholded at p < 0.05 (corrected, two-tailed). For all bands, we used the statistical omnibus to correct for multiple comparisons, except in the 50–100 Hz band where we used a less conservative (Chau et al., 2004) cluster-based threshold with an initial cluster-forming threshold of 2.3 (Nichols and Holmes, 2002).
Based on the source reconstruction images, we reconstructed sources from the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)/precuneus using the Harvard-Oxford Atlas. For frequency analysis of the virtual sensors, we computed power spectral density estimates using Hanning window FFTs. Statistical analysis of these was conducted using 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA using a resampling approach in the FSL randomize (5000 permutations).
Independent component analysis network analysis.
For analysis of oscillatory networks, we used the methodology described in Brookes et al. (2011) on the preprocessed MEG data. For each participant and frequency band, beamformer weights were computed on an 8 mm grid based on the preprocessed dataset. Beamformer time courses were then generated at every voxel and normalized by an estimate of the projected noise amplitude at that voxel. The Hilbert transform was applied to each voxel time course, and the absolute value was computed to generate an amplitude envelope of the oscillatory signals in each frequency band. The data at each voxel was downsampled to an effective sampling rate of 1 Hz, transformed to standard (MNI) space using FLIRT in FSL, and data from all subjects were concatenated in the time dimension across subjects. Temporal independent component analysis (ICA) was applied to the concatenated datasets (separately for all six frequency bands) using the fast ICA (research.ics.tkk.fi/ica/fastica) algorithm. Prewhitening was applied to reduce the dimensionality of the source space Hilbert envelope signals to 20 principal components before ICA (Hyvärinen and Oja, 2000; Brookes et al., 2011; Hall et al., 2013). Fifteen independent components were derived for each frequency band. To reduce the number of independent components subsequently analyzed, the following strategy was applied: each component was classified as follows: (1) an artifact due to either physiological or beamformer errors (deep brainstem sources); (2) a unimodal component; or (3) a network. Since we had already localized the changes in source power, we specifically focused on components in category 3. In total, 11 such components were identified. For these components, we computed the SD of the component time course for each subject and condition. Differences in the SD of the independent component time course between placebo and psilocybin were assessed using 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVAs in those where p < 0.01 was reported.
Dynamic causal modeling.
The purpose of the dynamic causal modeling (DCM)—in the context in which we have used it here—is to link the spectral changes we observed in the PCC/precuneus with underlying synaptic changes that occurred with the drug. We focused on reconstructed source activity from the PCC/precuneus as convergent evidence from fMRI, arterial spin labeling (ASL), and positron emission tomography (PET), and now MEG, suggests that the posterior parietal lobe is a key area involved in the human psychedelic experience (Erritzoe et al., 2009; Carhart-Harris et al., 2012a). To do this, we used dynamic causal modeling for steady-state responses previously described in a number of articles (Moran et al., 2007, 2009, 2011).
The canonical microcircuit (neural-mass) model we used here was composed of four subpopulations consisting of spiny stellate cells, inhibitory interneurons, deep pyramidal cells, and superficial pyramidal cells. The connectivity of each neural subpopulation is summarized in Figure 1. The behavior of each subpopulation is modeled with pairs of first-order differential equations of the following form (Moran et al., 2009):
The column vectors xv and xI correspond to the mean voltages and currents, where each element corresponds to the hidden state of the subpopulation. These differential equations implement a convolution of a subpopulation's presynaptic input (U) to produce a postsynaptic response (Fig. 1), where the rate constants (κ) represent an inverse of a lumped parameter that accounts for all dendritic and membrane time delays. Delays between the layers are accounted for through a Jacobian expansion (Kiebel et al., 2007) and have a prior value of 1 ms. The presynaptic input to the subpopulations comprises an endogenous component, E, which is a mixture of white and pink noise perturbations (these enter only the stellate cells; Table 1, see αu and βu in Parameters), and a scaled input of ensemble firing from each prescribed presynaptic population (the signs of these inputs are shown in Fig. 1 and implement effective excitability or gain increases and decreases). Firing rates from presynaptic subpopulations are prescribed through a sigmoidal transform (slope = 2/3) dependent on their membrane depolarization (Moran et al., 2009).
The output of the model is a mixture of the depolarization of each subpopulation. Since pyramidal dendrites are oriented tangentially to the cortical surface in a more “open field” configuration (Murakami and Okada, 2006), the contribution of each subpopulation is weighted and optimized during inversion. This is augmented with a parameterized channel noise component and amplifier gain to comprise the full observer function. Here we used prior weights of 20% for deep pyramidal cells (x_V_DP), 80%for superficial pyramidal cells (x_V_SP), and 20% for the stellates (x_V_SC).
Table 1.
Parameters | Prior |
Interpretation | |
---|---|---|---|
Mean (π) | Variance (σ) | ||
Observation model | |||
αu | 0 | 1/16 | Exogenous white input |
αs | 0 | 1/16 | Channel white noise |
βu | 0 | 1/16 | Exogenous pink input |
βs | 0 | 1/16 | Channel pink noise |
ϴ | 1 | 1 | Lead-field gain |
[Deep, super, stellate] | [0.2, 0.8, 0.2] | [1/16] | [Subpopulation contributions] |
Neuronal sources | |||
1/κ1 | 2 ms | 1/16 | Time constant (s.s.) |
1/κ2 | 2 ms | 1/16 | Time constant (s.p.) |
1/κ3 | 16 ms | 1/16 | Time constant (i.i.) |
1/κ4 | 28 ms | 1/16 | Time constant (d.p.) |
γ1 | 4 | 1/16 | Intrinsic connection (s.s. to >s.s.) |
γ2 | 4 | 1/16 | Intrinsic connection (s.p. to >s.s.) |
γ3 | 4 | 1/16 | Intrinsic connection (i.i. to >s.s.) |
γ4 | 4 | 1/16 | Intrinsic connection (i.i. to >i.i.) |
γ5 | 4 | 1/16 | Intrinsic connection (s.s. to >i.i.) |
γ6 | 2 | 1/16 | Intrinsic connection (d.p. to >i.i.) |
γ7 | 4 | 1/16 | Intrinsic connection (s.p. to >s.p.) |
γ8 | 4 | 1/16 | Intrinsic connection (s.s. to >s.p.) |
γ9 | 2 | 1/16 | Intrinsic connection (i.i. to >d.p.) |
γ10 | 1 | 1/16 | Intrinsic connection (d.p. to >d.p.) |
D | 1 ms | 1/16 | Laminar delay |
Experimental design | |||
β1 | 0 | 1/8 | Modulation of γ1 |
β2 | 0 | 1/8 | Modulation of γ7 |
β3 | 0 | 1/8 | Modulation of γ4 |
β4 | 0 | 1/8 | Modulation of γ10 |
s.s., Spiny stellate; s.p., superficial pyramidal; i.i., inhibitory interneuron; d.p., deep pyramidal.
The generative model created above was then inverted (fitted to the data) to obtain the posterior densities of the parameters. In DCM, a variational Bayesian scheme (variational Laplace) is used to approximate the posterior density over parameters by maximizing the negative free energy, a bound on the log-model evidence (Friston et al. (2007)). The parameters operate as scaling values on prior expectations. The full list of parameters and the sufficient statistics of their priors is given in Table 1, and the full mass model with equations is represented in Figure 1. Most of the intrinsic connectivity (γ) parameters that connect the different neuronal masses would represent glutamatergic or GABAergic synapses. Psilocybin acting on serotonin receptors would modulate the excitability of neuronal masses; that is, it would modulate the gain of the populations that contain these receptor types. That is, psilocybin has a neuromodulatory influence (i.e., rather than being directly GABAergic or glutamatergic). Therefore, we specifically modeled prespectral versus postspectral changes as a set of parameters (β1 … 4). These four parameters effectively modeled drug-induced modulations of the gain of each population in the neuronal mass (Table 1). Parameters β1-modeled changes in γ1 (stellate cell gain), β2-modeled changes in γ7 superficial pyramidal cell gain, β3-modeled inhibitory interneuron gain in γ4, and β4-modeled deep pyramidal cell gain in γ10. Having obtained these β1…4 parameter estimates, we then performed paired t tests comparing them between placebo and psilocybin infusion over subjects.
Visual and motor responses.
Analysis of motor cortical responses to the offset of the visual stimulus was conducted in a very similar fashion to those we (Muthukumaraswamy, 2010, 2011; Hamandi et al., 2011; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013b) and others have previously reported (Jurkiewicz et al., 2006; Cheyne et al., 2008; Gaetz et al., 2010, 2011). From the continuous MEG recordings, EMG onsets were marked using an automated algorithm that marked increases in the rectified EMG signal by 3 SDs above the noise floor (Cheyne et al., 2008), subject to the constraint that they occurred within 1 s of the tone pip. Data were epoched from 1.5 s before to 3 s after the EMG markers. Each trial was then visually inspected and discarded for any of the following reasons: (1) excessive MEG signal artifacts (e.g., head movements/jaw clenches); (2) irregular movement displacements (e.g., double movements); or (3) irregular EMG activity. Analysis of visual cortical responses to the onset of the grating stimulus was conducted in a very similar fashion to those we (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2009, 2010, 2013a; Gaetz et al., 2012; Muthukumaraswamy and Singh, 2013; Perry et al., 2013; Saxena et al., 2013) and others (Adjamian et al., 2004; Brookes et al., 2005; Hall et al., 2005; Hadjipapas et al., 2007; Schwarzkopf et al., 2012) have previously reported. Data were first epoched from −1.5 to 1.5 s around stimulus onset, and each trial was visually inspected for data quality. Epochs with gross artifacts, such as head movements and muscle clenching, were excluded from further analysis.
For source localization of the data, we used SAM (Robinson and Vrba, 1999) with a set of beamformer weights (4 mm resolution) calculated from global covariance matrices in the gamma band (60–90 Hz for motor, 35–80 Hz for visual). For SAM image reconstruction, virtual sensors were constructed for each beamformer voxel and Student's t test images of source power changes were computed (Table 2). Using parameters from Cheyne et al. (2008), Muthukumaraswamy (2010), and Hamandi et al. (2011) for motor gamma localization, we used a baseline period of −1.3 to 1 s, and an active period of 0–0.3 s, while for visual cortex localization we used a baseline period of −1.5 to 0 s, and an active period of 0–1.5 s. After generating the source reconstruction images, the voxels with the strongest change in visual and motor cortices were located, and virtual sensors were constructed at these peak locations. To reveal the time–frequency response at a virtual sensor location, the virtual sensor was repeatedly bandpass filtered at 0.5 Hz frequency step intervals using an 8-Hz-wide third-order Butterworth filter from 1 to 150 Hz (Le Van Quyen et al., 2001; Muthukumaraswamy, 2010; Saxena et al., 2013). At each frequency step, the Hilbert transform was used to obtain the time-varying amplitude envelope, and the envelopes were then averaged across trials. In a first iteration of the analysis, we computed time–frequency spectra as the percentage change from baseline values. In a second iteration, unbaselined maps were calculated to check for differences in the prestimulus baseline spectra (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013b; Saxena et al., 2013). From the baseline time–frequency spectra, amplitude envelopes were calculated for beta (15–30 Hz) and gamma (60–90 Hz; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013b) bands for the motor cortex, and for alpha (8–13 Hz) and gamma (35–80 Hz) bands in the visual cortex. We searched for differences in the frequency band-limited envelopes with paired t tests at each sample point using a resampling approach (Nichols and Holmes, 2002) in FSL Randomize (5000 permutations). Permuted t statistics were corrected for multiple comparisons using cluster-based techniques (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007) with an initial cluster-forming threshold of t = 2.3. This approach allowed us to examine the temporal profile of oscillatory spectral modulations and, in the case of alpha activity, to study changes after 300 ms that were not confounded by the evoked response.
Table 2.
Talairach coordinates |
t | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | ||
Delta | ||||
Precuneus (R) | 25 | −63 | 31 | −8.14 |
Posterior cingulate gyrus (L) | −23 | −45 | 37 | −7.55 |
Insula (L) | −55 | −33 | 19 | −6.88 |
Precentral gyrus (L) | −39 | −19 | 35 | −6.38 |
Postcentral gyrus (R) | 55 | −11 | 53 | −5.08 |
Anterior cingulate gyrus (L) | −19 | 25 | 33 | −4.74 |
Postcentral gyrus (R) | 51 | −17 | 25 | −4.60 |
Superior frontal gyrus (L) | −15 | 11 | 53 | −4.50 |
Theta | ||||
Superior parietal lobule (R) | 29 | −57 | 61 | −6.24 |
Postcentral gyrus (L) | −27 | −37 | 51 | −5.42 |
Superior temporal gyrus (L) | −49 | −55 | 15 | −5.35 |
Superior parietal lobule (L) | −15 | −67 | 65 | −5.11 |
Precuneus (L) | −5 | −47 | 53 | −4.98 |
Paracentral lobule (L) | −5 | −41 | 77 | −4.83 |
Precentral gyrus (R) | 39 | −15 | 69 | −4.30 |
Supramarginal gyrus (R) | 53 | −47 | 31 | −4.19 |
Alpha | ||||
Superior parietal lobule (L) | −13 | −53 | 61 | −8.95 |
Precuneus (L) | −7 | −61 | 33 | −8.94 |
Supramarginal gyrus (R) | 59 | −43 | 29 | −8.65 |
Transverse temporal gyrus (L) | −55 | −19 | 11 | −8.11 |
Superior temporal gyrus (R) | 63 | −27 | 7 | −7.18 |
Insula (R) | 33 | −11 | 9 | −7.16 |
Supramarginal gyrus (L) | −51 | −51 | 25 | −6.68 |
Anterior cingulate gyrus (R) | 5 | 35 | 11 | −5.48 |
Inferior frontal gyrus (L) | −55 | 21 | 21 | −5.33 |
Middle frontal gyrus (L) | −55 | 5 | 47 | −5.28 |
Cingulate gyrus (L) | −1 | 3 | 31 | −5.15 |
Middle frontal gyrus (L) | −21 | −1 | 45 | −5.07 |
Medial frontal gyrus (L) | −5 | −3 | 51 | −5.06 |
Middle frontal gyrus (R) | 29 | 9 | 57 | −4.70 |
Beta | ||||
Precuneus (R) | 25 | −53 | 33 | −9.15 |
Insula (L) | −43 | −19 | 15 | −7.73 |
Superior temporal gyrus (L) | −35 | −51 | 17 | −7.71 |
Medial frontal gyrus (L) | −21 | 27 | 23 | −6.87 |
Superior frontal gyrus (R) | 19 | 65 | 5 | −6.69 |
Superior Frontal gyrus (R) | 5 | 59 | 27 | −6.50 |
Superior parietal lobule (L) | −21 | −69 | 63 | −6.31 |
Inferior frontal gyrus (L) | −57 | 13 | 35 | −6.26 |
Postcentral gyrus (L) | −59 | −33 | 53 | −5.74 |
Superior frontal gyrus (R) | 17 | 17 | 65 | −4.83 |
Low gamma | ||||
Precentral gyrus (R) | 37 | −25 | 71 | −5.28 |
Superior parietal lobule (L) | −19 | −61 | 65 | −5.17 |
Precentral gyrus (L) | 35 | −7 | 69 | −5.04 |
Middle frontal gyrus (L) | −47 | 33 | 31 | −4.91 |
Superior frontal gyrus (L) | −43 | 15 | 55 | −4.91 |
Postcentral gyrus (R) | 15 | −59 | 69 | −4.72 |
Middle frontal gyrus (L) | −23 | 15 | 57 | −4.71 |
Middle frontal gyrus (R) | 43 | 23 | 49 | −4.55 |
Superior parietal lobule (R) | 41 | −59 | 57 | −4.29 |
Precentral gyrus (L) | −55 | −19 | 37 | −4.28 |
High gamma | ||||
Middle frontal gyrus (L) | −43 | 15 | 57 | −3.22 |
Middle frontal gyrus (R) | 31 | 23 | 59 | −3.18 |
Superior frontal gyrus (R) | 23 | −1 | 71 | −2.89 |
Precentral gyrus (L) | −63 | −15 | 41 | −2.84 |
L, Left; R, right.
Results
Subjective effects
All participants reported marked changes in their normal conscious experience after psilocybin infusion. The most common effects included altered visual, spatial, and temporal perception; loosened associations; and vivified imagination. However, more profound effects such as depersonalization or “ego disintegration” were also scored highly by some participants. Expressed as a percentage, with 100% signaling “extremely intense effects,” participants rated the subjective strength of their experience at 78.2% (SE, 5.5%) 5 min after infusion of psilocybin, decreasing to 65.4% (SE, 4.7%) after 15 min; placebo had negligible effects (both time points, 0.7%; SE, 2.5%). Items rated 15 min postscanning are shown in Figure 2.
Spectral analysis and source localization: spontaneous activity
To localize corresponding spectral changes in cortical activity, we used the beamformer source localization technique SAM. Figure 3 displays the contrast of psilocybin after versus before infusion compared with placebo after versus before infusion. Results revealed decreased oscillatory power across a broad frequency range after psilocybin infusion, mainly localized to association cortices—with marked decreases in areas of the default-mode network (DMN; Raichle et al., 2001) such as the PCC. In posterior areas, the decreases were observed in the delta (1–4 Hz), theta (48 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and low gamma (30–50 Hz) frequency bands. In bilateral prefrontal cortices, the decreased power (“desynchronization”) ranged from alpha (8–13 Hz) to high gamma (50–100 Hz). The result of simple contrasts between after and before psilocybin infusion showed very similar spatial localizations and direction of effect compare with those of the interaction contrast in Figure 3 (i.e., decreased power). No increases in oscillatory power were observed in any region. As such, the interaction effects in Figure 3 can be interpreted as a decrease of power following administration of psilocybin. A comparison of head position from the beginning to the end of the resting MEG recordings showed no significant difference in movement between psilocybin and placebo sessions (t = 0.63, p = 0.53).
Resting-state MEG networks
Techniques have recently been developed to identify spatiotemporally coherent brain networks in MEG data using independent component analysis (Brookes et al., 2011). Applying this to our resting-state data, we recovered 11 functional brain networks, 7 of which showed post-psilocybin infusion decreases in oscillatory power in the frequency bands from which they were derived (p < 0.01, uncorrected; Fig. 4). A further four networks were identified that did not pass the significance criterion. These networks were a bilateral parietal network in the delta band (p = 0.065), a bilateral extrastriate cortex network in the alpha band (p = 0.061), a bilateral frontal cortex network in the low gamma band (p = 0.013), and a bilateral extrastriate cortex network in the high gamma band (p = 0.057). Despite not reaching significance, activity in these four networks was consistently decreased under the effects of psilocybin.
Dynamic causal modeling
Recently, mathematical modeling techniques have been developed to test hypotheses about the underlying cell populations responsible for steady-state changes in spectral power recorded with EEG and MEG, termed DCM for steady-state responses (Moran et al., 2009). Thus, to identify the most likely synaptic mechanism underlying the psilocybin-induced desynchronization, we constructed a model based on the canonical microcircuit thought to recapitulate the intrinsic circuitry of individual cortical regions (Bastos et al., 2012). This model encompasses four cell types and their respective connections (Fig. 5b). We used the beamformer to estimate spectral activity in the PCC, which was significantly decreased in the alpha band after psilocybin infusion (Fig. 5a). We set up a model where drug effects could be accounted for by differential gain (or excitability) of each of the four cell types (β parameters; Fig. 5b). Paired t tests using a Bonferroni-adjusted significance level of p = 0.0125 (0.05/4 parameters) showed a psilocybin-dependent increase in the excitability of deep-layer pyramidal cells (Fig. 5c). These modeling results in humans are consistent with the results of the animal studies that directly recorded 5-HT2A receptor-mediated increases in EPSPs in layer 5 pyramidal cells (Andrade, 2011). Moreover, excitation of deep-layer pyramidal cells is consistent with decreased brain activity (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012a) and oscillatory power because the net effect of feedback connections is thought to be inhibitory (Bastos et al., 2012). Individual data and model fits for each participant are presented in Figure 6.
Visually induced and motor-induced gamma oscillations
Psilocybin had a profound effect on participants' subjective state, as reflected in the marked changes in spontaneous brain activity. When asked to perform a simple visuomotor task, participants performed reasonably well, being marginally slower (t = 2.56, p = 0.023) after psilocybin (295 ms; SE, 15 ms) than placebo (260 ms; SE, 22 ms) infusion. Analyses of EMG onset time-locked spectral responses are presented in Figure 7. Time–frequency responses (Fig. 7b) show the typical component morphology seen during simple movement tasks. This morphology includes a short-lasting burst of gamma-band activity accompanied by a desynchronization, and later rebound of the beta rhythm (Jurkiewicz et al., 2006; Cheyne et al., 2008). No significant differences were seen between placebo and psilocybin for either the gamma (60–90 Hz) or beta (15–30 Hz) band. For the motor cortex source locations, we repeated the time–frequency analysis with no baseline correction (raw source amplitude estimates) applied to examine the baseline spectra in the frequency windows of interest for each task. For the motor cortex virtual sensors, no significant differences were seen in the prestimulus baseline in either the gamma (t = 0.60, p = 0.56) or beta (t = 1.69, p = 0.11) frequency bands.
Analyses of visual cortical responses time locked to stimulus onset are presented in Figure 8. The time–frequency response plots show the typical component morphology seen during visual stimulation with a grating patch. This morphology includes a short-lasting burst of broadband gamma-band activity accompanied by a long-duration gamma response, together with alpha desynchronization. No significant differences were seen in either the gamma (35–80 Hz) or alpha (8–13 Hz) band. For the virtual sensors from the visual cortex, no difference in baseline gamma-band activity was seen (t = −0.23, p = 0.82). There were baseline alpha power differences (t = 4.02, p = 0.0015). This is consistent with the resting-state analysis and might account for the tendency for the slight (nonsignificant) reduction in alpha desynchronization seen during visual stimulation. Together, the analyses in Figures 7 and 8 suggest that some low-level induced elements of oscillatory brain activity are relatively preserved during the psychedelic experience. A comparison of head position from the beginning to the end of the task-related MEG recordings showed no significant difference in movement between psilocybin and placebo sessions (t = 1.23, p = 0.23).
Psychometric correlations
We conducted exploratory correlations between resting post-psilocybin infusion changes (decreases) in alpha power extracted from a PCC mask for each participant versus the visual analog scale-style items rated after the experience (Fig. 1). Analyses were restricted to the alpha frequency band to minimize multiple testing and because the PCC alpha-power decreases were especially marked (Fig. 3). There were strong ceiling and floor effects for the highest and lowest rated items in the 23-item rating scale (Fig. 1), thus making meaningful correlations with these items unlikely. However, scores for other items were more broadly distributed. Using a Bonferroni-adjusted significance level of p = 0.0022 (0.05/23 items), two items showed positive significant correlations with PCC alpha-power decreases. These were, “I experienced a disintegration of my ‘self’ or ‘ego’” (R2 = 0.66, p = 0.00016) and “the experience had a supernatural quality” (R2 = 0.53, p = 0.00018). Positive correlations were also found between deep-layer pyramidal cell excitation and the magnitude of the decreases in PCC alpha power (R2 = 0.42, p = 0.0056) and ratings of ego disintegration (R2 = 0.42, p = 0.005). The psychometric correlations presented in this section will require subsequent replication given their exploratory nature.
Discussion
The present study examined the neural effects of a classic psychedelic drug using MEG. We observed a broadband desynchronization of cortical oscillatory rhythms after psilocybin infusion and decreased brain network integrity. The results are consistent with those of previous EEG (Riba et al., 2004) and fMRI studies (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012a) in humans and electrophysiological recordings in animals (Wood et al., 2012), and they significantly extend our understanding of the mechanisms by which psychedelics alter oscillatory brain activity. By exploring the neuronal source of the decreases in oscillatory power with dynamic causal modeling, we present a model of the mechanism of action of psychedelics in the PCC. This begins at the cellular level with excitation of layer 5 pyramidal neurons, and extends to the macroscopic level with cortical desynchronization and decreased brain network integrity. We postulate that this excitation occurs via stimulation of 5-HT2A receptors, which are densely expressed there (Erritzoe et al., 2009).
Previous pharmacological EEG and MEG studies have found increases and decreases in oscillatory power in different frequency bands post-drug infusion (Fink, 1969, 2010). That the decreases here occurred in all of the frequency bands implies a general collapse of the normal rhythmic structure of cortical activity. This supports the claim that psychedelics decrease, or, more accurately, disorganize, spontaneous brain activity (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012a). Moreover, the selective localization of the decreases to association cortices is consistent with previous fMRI work (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012a) and suggests that the reduced blood flow reported therein may relate to desynchronized neural activity. Indeed, the MEG source localization results we find here are highly consistent with previous fMRI results using the same pharmacological intervention (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012a). Some of the key areas of overlap include the PCC, precuneus, superior and middle frontal gyri, anterior cingulate gyri, and supramarginal and precentral gyri. These mostly represent hub areas of association cortex rather than primary sensory cortex. The PCC showed especially marked effects in both studies, and these correlated with the drug's subjective effects (e.g., note the highly significant correlation between PCC alpha decreases and ratings of ego disturbance observed here). It is significant also that the spatial location of the effects observed with fMRI and MEG are consistent with the known distribution of 5-HT2A receptors. Explicitly, in a PET study of 5-HT2A receptor binding using 18F-altanserin in 136 participants, association cortices had the highest binding potentials, and the PCC was highest of all (Erritzoe et al., 2009). The posterior association cortices showed desynchonization in a lower frequency range (1–50 Hz), whereas frontal association cortices showed higher frequency changes (8–100 Hz). These frequency differences may reflect the different resting rhythms that these areas spontaneously generate (Buzsaki, 2006). It is important not to overinterpret the specificity of MEG source localization data, as these data have inferior spatial resolution compared with both fMRI and PET. We did note, in some single-participant data, strong high-frequency desynchronizations in posterior association areas. However, these were quite inconsistent across both individuals and cortical locations, and did not survive at the group-level analysis. Further investigations will be needed to explain these individual differences in observed neural responses to psychedelic drugs.
A number of previous studies have investigated the relationship between blood flow metrics and oscillatory activity. These have generally found an inverse relationship between BOLD activity and alpha/beta rhythms (Goldman et al., 2002; Singh et al., 2002; Laufs et al., 2003; Scheeringa et al., 2011), while a positive relationship is found between gamma-band activity and BOLD activity (Logothetis et al., 2001; Scheeringa et al., 2011). With psilocybin, however, we observe a decrease in BOLD activity and a general decrease in oscillatory activity during resting-state conditions. The hypercapnic challenge delivered by Carhart-Harris et al. (2012a) suggests that the BOLD decreases seen in that study were not a confound of psilocybin working directly on the cerebral vasculature. Consistent with our human data, administration of the relatively selective 5-HT2A receptor agonist DOI in rats resulted in dose-dependent decreases in broadband (including high-frequencies) oscillatory power and multiunit activity in the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortices (Wood et al., 2012). This suggests a generalized reduction in neural activity (at least in terms of oscillations and hemodynamics) with 5-HT2A receptor activation and a decoupling of the usual relationship between BOLD and neural oscillations.
In the present study, some of the largest decreases in oscillatory power were found in areas of the DMN (Fig. 3) consistent with recent fMRI studies revealing decreased blood flow in these regions after psilocybin infusion (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012a). The DMN consumes more energy (Raichle and Snyder, 2007), receives more perfusion (Zou et al., 2009), and is more widely connected (Hagmann et al., 2008) than other cortical regions. It undergoes significant ontogenetic development (Supekar et al., 2010) and underwent significant evolutionary expansion (Van Essen and Dierker, 2007), and its connectivity has been found to relate to personality (Adelstein et al., 2011) and psychopathology (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford, 2012). Several recent intracranial studies (Jerbi et al., 2010; Ossandón et al., 2011) have demonstrated that broadband, high-frequency, task-related suppression of DMN areas correlates with improved task performance. In these studies (Ossandón et al., 2011), during task performance, activity in task-positive networks emerges, whereas in the psychedelic state studied here a pharmacologically driven suppression of all observed networks was seen, suggesting a general disorganization of network-level brain activity during resting conditions.
Psilocin is a mixed serotonin receptor agonist (Halberstadt and Geyer, 2011). However, the potency of psychedelics correlates positively with their affinity for the 5-HT2A receptor (Glennon et al., 1984), and pretreatment with the 5-HT2A receptor antagonist ketanserin blocks the hallucinogenic effects of psilocybin in humans (Vollenweider et al., 1998). 5-HT2A receptors are densely expressed in cortical regions (Erritzoe et al., 2009), particularly in cortical layer 5 (Weber and Andrade, 2010). The present modeling results, indicating increased excitation of deep-layer pyramidal cells, are entirely consistent with previous research. Stimulation of the 5-HT2A receptor has been shown to enhance spontaneous EPSPs/EPSCs in neocortical layer 5 pyramidal cells by reducing outward potassium currents (Aghajanian and Marek, 1997; Lambe et al., 2000) and to cause increased glutamatergic recurrent activity in layer 5 of the cortex (Béïque et al., 2007). It is noteworthy that, while the decreases in spectral power seen here were broad, decreases in the alpha frequency were especially marked—particularly in the PCC where 5-HT2A receptors are densely expressed (Erritzoe et al., 2009). Layer 5 pyramidal cells fire with an intrinsic alpha rhythm (Silva et al., 1991; Sun and Dan, 2009) and spontaneous alpha oscillations are closely linked to perceptual processing, including processing of space and time (Lorincz et al., 2009; Klimesch, 2012). Thus, the marked decreases in alpha power observed here with psilocybin were likely due to interference with the intrinsic alpha oscillations of deep-layer pyramidal neurons via stimulation of 5-HT2A receptors, although we observed marked changes in spontaneous brain activity after psilocybin, but no effects on low-level induced gamma-band responses.
There are some limitations to the present work. Participants all had previous experience with psychedelic drugs. This was to minimize the risk of adverse responses by excluding volunteers who had a history of reacting negatively to psychedelics. However, that we used experienced participants limits the generalizability of the present results to psychedelic-naive individuals. Also, we used a fixed-order scanning protocol to avoid contaminating the placebo condition with subacute drug effects. A balanced-order design with a prolonged period between scans may be an alternative experimental design, although it would be difficult to perform in practice. However, we suggest that any potential order effects would likely be subtle relative to the marked neuronal and subjective effects of psilocybin. Reinforcing this view, in our previous fMRI work with psilocybin, similar results were observed with balanced (BOLD) and fixed (ASL) order scanning (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012a). This was also a single-blind design. However, in experiments such as this, comparing the acute effects of potent psychoactive drugs with those of a (necessarily) inert placebo, participants quickly become “unblinded” to the experimental manipulation, thus diminishing the value of blinding. Finally, it was necessary to remove periods of MEG data that were contaminated by large-muscle artifacts/head movements in the preprocessing stage. If these artifacts were correlated with particular aspects of the psychedelic state, then this (necessary) preprocessing step precluded their detection.
Previous theoretical accounts of positive psychotic symptoms suggest that delusional thinking represents a failure of neural mechanisms to accurately encode predictions and prediction error or “surprise” (Corlett et al., 2010). In one prediction-coding model of global brain function based on the free-energy principle (Friston, 2010), activity in deep-layer projection neurons encodes top-down inferences about the world. Speculatively, if deep-layer pyramidal cells were to become hyperexcitable during the psychedelic state, information processing would be biased in the direction of inference—such that implicit models of the world become spontaneously manifest—intruding into consciousness without prior invitation from sensory data. This could explain many of the subjective effects of psychedelics.
Finally, the therapeutic potential of psychedelics is currently attracting much interest (Grob et al., 2011; Krebs and Johansen, 2012). Frontoparietal connectivity has been found to correlate with rumination in depression (Berman et al., 2011), and deficient 5-HT2A receptor stimulation have been found to be associated with trait neuroticism (Frokjaer et al., 2008) and pessimism (Meyer et al., 2003). Here we found decreased frontoparietal connectivity after psilocybin infusion, consistent with previous fMRI findings (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012a). Thus, the ability of serotonergic psychedelics to disrupt pathological patterns of brain activity via stimulation of 5-HT2A receptors may underlie their therapeutic potential in psychiatric settings (Grob et al., 2011; Krebs and Johansen, 2012), and breakdown of brain networks may be a fundamental feature of the psychedelic state.
Footnotes
This research received financial and intellectual support from the Beckley Foundation and was conducted as part of the Beckley-Imperial Psychedelic Research Programme. The authors thank Alison Diaper and the research volunteers.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Adelstein JS, Shehzad Z, Mennes M, Deyoung CG, Zuo XN, Kelly C, Margulies DS, Bloomfield A, Gray JR, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Personality is reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional architecture. PLoS One. 2011;6:e27633. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adjamian P, Holliday IE, Barnes GR, Hillebrand A, Hadjipapas A, Singh KD. Induced visual illusions and gamma oscillations in human primary visual cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;20:587–592. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03495.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aghajanian GK, Marek GJ. Serotonin induces excitatory postsynaptic potentials in apical dendrites of neocortical pyramidal cells. Neuropharmacology. 1997;36:589–599. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(97)00051-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrade R. Serotonergic regulation of neuronal excitability in the prefrontal cortex. Neuropharmacology. 2011;61:382–386. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bardouille T, Picton TW, Ross B. Correlates of eye blinking as determined by synthetic aperture magnetometry. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117:952–958. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastos AM, Usrey WM, Adams RA, Mangun GR, Fries P, Friston KJ. Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron. 2012;76:695–711. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Béïque JC, Imad M, Mladenovic L, Gingrich JA, Andrade R. Mechanism of the 5-hydroxytryptamine 2A receptor-mediated facilitation of synaptic activity in prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:9870–9875. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700436104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berman MG, Peltier S, Nee DE, Kross E, Deldin PJ, Jonides J. Depression, rumination and the default network. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011;6:548–555. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10:433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brookes MJ, Gibson AM, Hall SD, Furlong PL, Barnes GR, Hillebrand A, Singh KD, Holliday IE, Francis ST, Morris PG. GLM-beamformer method demonstrates stationary field, alpha ERD and gamma ERS co-localisation with fMRI BOLD response in visual cortex. Neuroimage. 2005;26:302–308. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brookes MJ, Woolrich M, Luckhoo H, Price D, Hale JR, Stephenson MC, Barnes GR, Smith SM, Morris PG. Investigating the electrophysiological basis of resting state networks using magnetoencephalography. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:16783–16788. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112685108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G. Rhythms of the brain. Oxford: Oxford UP; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Carhart-Harris RL, Williams TM, Sessa B, Tyacke RJ, Rich AS, Feilding A, Nutt DJ. The administration of psilocybin to healthy, hallucinogen-experienced volunteers in a mock-functional magnetic resonance imaging environment: a preliminary investigation of tolerability. J Psychopharmacol. 2011;25:1562–1567. doi: 10.1177/0269881110367445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carhart-Harris RL, Erritzoe D, Williams T, Stone JM, Reed LJ, Colasanti A, Tyacke RJ, Leech R, Malizia AL, Murphy K, Hobden P, Evans J, Feilding A, Wise RG, Nutt DJ. Neural correlates of the psychedelic state as determined by fMRI studies with psilocybin. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012a;109:2138–2143. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1119598109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carhart-Harris RL, Leech R, Erritzoe D, Williams TM, Stone JM, Evans J, Sharp DJ, Feilding A, Wise RG, Nutt DJ. Functional connectivity measures after psilocybin inform a novel hypothesis of early psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2012b doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs117. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs117. Advance online publication. Retrieved August 21, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carl C, Açik A, König P, Engel AK, Hipp JF. The saccadic spike artifact in MEG. Neuroimage. 2012;59:1657–1667. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chau W, McIntosh AR, Robinson SE, Schulz M, Pantev C. Improving permutation test power for group analysis spatically filtered MEG data. Neuroimage. 2004;23:983–996. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheyne D, Bostan AC, Gaetz W, Pang EW. Event-related beamforming: a robust method for presurgical functional mapping using MEG. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007;118:1691–1704. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.05.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheyne D, Bells S, Ferrari P, Gaetz W, Bostan AC. Self-paced movements induce high-frequency gamma oscillations in primary motor cortex. Neuroimage. 2008;42:332–342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett PR, Taylor JR, Wang XJ, Fletcher PC, Krystal JH. Toward a neurobiology of delusions. Prog Neurobiol. 2010;92:345–369. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dittrich A. The standardized psychometric assessment of altered states of consciousness (ASCs) in humans. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1998;31(Suppl 2):80–84. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-979351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erritzoe D, Frokjaer VG, Haugbol S, Marner L, Svarer C, Holst K, Baaré WF, Rasmussen PM, Madsen J, Paulson OB, Knudsen GM. Brain serotonin 2A receptor binding: relations to body mass index, tobacco and alcohol use. Neuroimage. 2009;46:23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fink M. EEG and human psychopharmacology. Annu Rev Pharmacol. 1969;9:241–258. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pa.09.040169.001325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fink M. Remembering the lost neuroscience of pharmaco-EEG. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;121:161–173. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01467.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K, Mattout J, Trujillo-Barreto N, Ashburner J, Penny W. Variational free energy and the Laplace approximation. Neuroimage. 2007;34:220–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frokjaer VG, Mortensen EL, Nielsen FA, Haugbol S, Pinborg LH, Adams KH, Svarer C, Hasselbalch SG, Holm S, Paulson OB, Knudsen GM. Frontolimbic serotonin 2A receptor binding in healthy subjects is associated with personality risk factors for affective disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:569–576. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaetz W, Macdonald M, Cheyne D, Snead OC. Neuromagnetic imaging of movement-related cortical oscillations in children and adults: age predicts post-movement beta rebound. Neuroimage. 2010;51:792–807. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaetz W, Edgar JC, Wang DJ, Roberts TP. Relating MEG measured motor cortical oscillations to resting gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) concentration. Neuroimage. 2011;55:616–621. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaetz W, Roberts TPL, Singh KD, Muthukumaraswamy SD. Functional and structural correlates of the aging brain: relating visual cortex (V1) gamma band responses to age related structural change. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33:2035–2046. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glennon RA, Titeler M, McKenney JD. Evidence for 5-HT2 involvement in the mechanism of action of hallucinogenic agents. Life Sci. 1984;35:2505–2511. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(84)90436-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman RI, Stern JM, Engel J, Jr, Cohen MS. Simultaneous EEG and fMRI of the alpha rhythm. Neuroreport. 2002;13:2487–2492. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200212200-00022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths RR, Richards WA, McCann U, Jesse R. Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;187:268–283. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0457-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths RR, Johnson MW, Richards WA, Richards BD, McCann U, Jesse R. Psilocybin occasioned mystical-type experiences: immediate and persisting dose-related effects. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011;218:649–665. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2358-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths R, Richards W, Johnson M, McCann U, Jesse R. Mystical-type experiences occasioned by psilocybin mediate the attribution of personal meaning and spiritual significance 14 months later. J Psychopharmacol. 2008;22:621–632. doi: 10.1177/0269881108094300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grob CS, Danforth AL, Chopra GS, Hagerty M, McKay CR, Halberstadt AL, Greer GR. Pilot study of psilocybin treatment for anxiety in patients with advanced-stage cancer. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:71–78. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadjipapas A, Adjamian P, Swettenham JB, Holliday IE, Barnes GR. Stimuli of varying spatial scale induce gamma activity with distinct temporal characteristics in human visual cortex. Neuroimage. 2007;35:518–530. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagmann P, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Meuli R, Honey CJ, Wedeen VJ, Sporns O. Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol. 2008;6:e159. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halberstadt AL, Geyer MA. Multiple receptors contribute to the behavioral effects of indoleamine hallucinogens. Neuropharmacology. 2011;61:364–381. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall EL, Woolrich MW, Thomaz CE, Morris PG, Brookes MJ. Using variance information in magnetoencephalography measures of functional connectivity. Neuroimage. 2013;67:203–212. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall SD, Holliday IE, Hillebrand A, Singh KD, Furlong PL, Hadjipapas A, Barnes GR. The missing link: analogous human and primate cortical gamma oscillations. Neuroimage. 2005;26:13–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamandi K, Singh KD, Muthukumaraswamy S. Reduced movement-related beta desynchronisation in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy: a MEG study of task specific cortical modulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122:2128–2138. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.04.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang MX, Mosher JC, Leahy RM. A sensor-weighted overlapping-sphere head model and exhaustive head model comparison for MEG. Phys Med Biol. 1999;44:423–440. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/44/2/010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyvärinen A, Oja E. Independent component analysis: algorithms and applications. Neural Netw. 2000;13:411–430. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(00)00026-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jerbi K, Vidal JR, Ossandon T, Dalal SS, Jung J, Hoffmann D, Minotti L, Bertrand O, Kahane P, Lachaux JP. Exploring the electrophysiological correlates of the default-mode network with intracerebral EEG. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010;4:27. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jurkiewicz MT, Gaetz WC, Bostan AC, Cheyne D. Post-movement beta rebound is generated in motor cortex: evidence from neuromagnetic recordings. Neuroimage. 2006;32:1281–1289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiebel SJ, Garrido MI, Friston KJ. Dynamic causal modelling of evoked responses: the role of intrinsic connections. Neuroimage. 2007;36:332–345. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16:606–617. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krebs TS, Johansen PØ. Lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) for alcoholism: meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Psychopharmacol. 2012;26:994–1002. doi: 10.1177/0269881112439253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambe EK, Goldman-Rakic PS, Aghajanian GK. Serotonin induces EPSCs preferentially in layer V pyramidal neurons of the frontal cortex in the rat. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:974–980. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.10.974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laufs H, Kleinschmidt A, Beyerle A, Eger E, Salek-Haddadi A, Preibisch C, Krakow K. EEG-correlated fMRI of human alpha activity. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1463–1476. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00286-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Van Quyen M, Foucher J, Lachaux J, Rodriguez E, Lutz A, Martinerie J, Varela FJ. Comparison of Hilbert transform and wavelet methods for the analysis of neuronal synchrony. J Neurosci Methods. 2001;111:83–98. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(01)00372-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litvak V, Eusebio A, Jha A, Oostenveld R, Barnes GR, Penny WD, Zrinzo L, Hariz MI, Limousin P, Friston KJ, Brown P. Optimized beamforming for simultaneous MEG and intracranial local field potential recordings in deep brain stimulation patients. Neuroimage. 2010;50:1578–1588. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logothetis NK, Pauls J, Augath M, Trinath T, Oeltermann A. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature. 2001;412:150–157. doi: 10.1038/35084005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorincz ML, Kékesi KA, Juhász G, Crunelli V, Hughes SW. Temporal framing of thalamic relay-mode firing by phasic inhibition during the alpha rhythm. Neuron. 2009;63:683–696. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacLean KA, Johnson MW, Griffiths RR. Mystical experiences occasioned by the hallucinogen psilocybin lead to increases in the personality domain of openness. J Psychopharmacol. 2011;25:1453–1461. doi: 10.1177/0269881111420188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maris E, Oostenveld R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;164:177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer JH, McMain S, Kennedy SH, Korman L, Brown GM, DaSilva JN, Wilson AA, Blak T, Eynan-Harvey R, Goulding VS, Houle S, Links P. Dysfunctional attitudes and 5-HT2 receptors during depression and self-harm. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:90–99. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moran RJ, Kiebel SJ, Stephan KE, Reilly RB, Daunizeau J, Friston KJ. A neural mass model of spectral responses in electrophysiology. Neuroimage. 2007;37:706–720. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moran RJ, Stephan KE, Seidenbecher T, Pape HC, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ. Dynamic causal models of steady-state responses. Neuroimage. 2009;44:796–811. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moran RJ, Symmonds M, Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ. An in vivo assay of synaptic function mediating human cognition. Curr Biol. 2011;21:1320–1325. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.06.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreno FA, Wiegand CB, Taitano EK, Delgado PL. Safety, tolerability, and efficacy of psilocybin in 9 patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67:1735–1740. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v67n1110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murakami S, Okada Y. Contributions of principal neocortical neurons to magnetoencephalography and electroencephalography signals. J Physiol. 2006;575:925–936. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.105379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthukumaraswamy SD, Singh KD. Visual gamma oscillations: the effects of stimulus type, visual field coverage and stimulus motion on MEG and EEG recordings. Neuroimage. 2013;69:223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthukumaraswamy SD. Functional properties of human primary motor cortex gamma oscillations. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104:2873–2885. doi: 10.1152/jn.00607.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthukumaraswamy SD. Temporal dynamics of primary motor cortex gamma oscillation amplitude and piper corticomuscular coherence changes during motor control. Exp Brain Res. 2011;212:623–633. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2775-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthukumaraswamy SD, Edden RA, Jones DK, Swettenham JB, Singh KD. Resting GABA concentration predicts peak gamma frequency and fMRI amplitude in response to visual stimulation in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:8356–8361. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900728106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthukumaraswamy SD, Singh KD, Swettenham JB, Jones DK. Visual gamma oscillations and evoked responses: variability, repeatability and structural MRI correlates. Neuroimage. 2010;49:3349–3357. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthukumaraswamy SD, Myers JF, Wilson SJ, Nutt DJ, Hamandi K, Lingford-Hughes A, Singh KD. Elevating endogenous GABA levels with GAT-1 blockade modulates evoked but not induced responses in human visual cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013a;38:1105–1112. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthukumaraswamy SD, Myers JF, Wilson SJ, Nutt DJ, Lingford-Hughes A, Singh KD, Hamandi K. The effects of elevated endogenous GABA levels on movement-related network oscillations. Neuroimage. 2013b;66:36–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;15:1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ossandón T, Jerbi K, Vidal JR, Bayle DJ, Henaff MA, Jung J, Minotti L, Bertrand O, Kahane P, Lachaux JP. Transient suppression of broadband gamma power in the default-mode network is correlated with task complexity and subject performance. J Neurosci. 2011;31:14521–14530. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2483-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis. 1997;10:437–442. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry G, Hamandi K, Brindley LM, Muthukumaraswamy SD, Singh KD. The properties of induced gamma oscillations in human visual cortex show significant individual variability in their dependence on stimulus size. Neuroimage. 2013;68:83–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puig MV, Watakabe A, Ushimaru M, Yamamori T, Kawaguchi Y. Serotonin modulates fast-spiking interneuron and synchronous activity in the rat prefrontal cortex through 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors. J Neurosci. 2010;30:2211–2222. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3335-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle ME, Snyder AZ. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1083–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riba J, Anderer P, Jané F, Saletu B, Barbanoj MJ. Effects of the South American psychoactive beverage ayahuasca on regional brain electrical activity in humans: a functional neuroimaging study using low-resolution electromagnetic tomography. Neuropsychobiology. 2004;50:89–101. doi: 10.1159/000077946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson SE, Vrba J. Functional neuroimaging by synthetic aperture manetometry (SAM) In: Yoshimoto T, Kotani M, Kuriki S, Karibe H, Nakasato N, editors. Recent advances in biomagnetism. Sendai, Japan: Tohoku UP; 1999. pp. 302–305. [Google Scholar]
- Saxena N, Muthukumaraswamy SD, Diukova A, Singh K, Hall J, Wise R. Enhanced stimulus-induced gamma activity in humans during propofol-induced sedation. PLoS One. 2013;8:e57685. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheeringa R, Fries P, Petersson KM, Oostenveld R, Grothe I, Norris DG, Hagoort P, Bastiaansen MC. Neuronal dynamics underlying high- and low-frequency EEG oscillations contribute independently to the human BOLD signal. Neuron. 2011;69:572–583. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzkopf DS, Robertson DJ, Song C, Barnes GR, Rees G. The frequency of visually induced gamma-band oscillations depends on the size of early human visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2012;32:1507–1512. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4771-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silva LR, Amitai Y, Connors BW. Intrinsic oscillations of neocortex generated by layer 5 pyramidal neurons. Science. 1991;251:432–435. doi: 10.1126/science.1824881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh KD, Barnes GR, Hillebrand A, Forde EM, Williams AL. Task-related changes in cortical synchronization are spatially coincident with the hemodynamic response. Neuroimage. 2002;16:103–114. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh KD, Barnes GR, Hillebrand A. Group imaging of task-related changes in cortical synchronisation using nonparametric permutation testing. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1589–1601. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00249-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;17:143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun W, Dan Y. Layer-specific network oscillation and spatiotemporal receptive field in the visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:17986–17991. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903962106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Supekar K, Uddin LQ, Prater K, Amin H, Greicius MD, Menon V. Development of functional and structural connectivity within the default mode network in young children. Neuroimage. 2010;52:290–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swettenham JB, Muthukumaraswamy SD, Singh KD. Spectral properties of induced and evoked gamma oscillations in human early visual cortex to moving and stationary stimuli. J Neurophysiol. 2009;102:1241–1253. doi: 10.1152/jn.91044.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC, Dierker DL. Surface-based and probabilistic atlases of primate cerebral cortex. Neuron. 2007;56:209–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vollenweider FX, Vollenweider-Scherpenhuyzen MF, Bäbler A, Vogel H, Hell D. Psilocybin induces schizophrenia-like psychosis in humans via a serotonin-2 agonist action. Neuroreport. 1998;9:3897–3902. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199812010-00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vrba J, Robinson SE. Signal processing in magnetoencephalography. Methods. 2001;25:249–271. doi: 10.1006/meth.2001.1238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weber ET, Andrade R. Htr2a gene and 5-HT(2A) receptor expression in the cerebral cortex studied using genetically modified mice. Front Neurosci. 2010;4:36. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Ford JM. Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012;8:49–76. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitham EM, Pope KJ, Fitzgibbon SP, Lewis T, Clark CR, Loveless S, Broberg M, Wallace A, DeLosAngeles D, Lillie P, Hardy A, Fronsko R, Pulbrook A, Willoughby JO. Scalp electrical recording during paralysis: quantitative evidence that EEG frequencies above 20 Hz are contaminated by EMG. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007;118:1877–1888. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitham EM, Lewis T, Pope KJ, Fitzgibbon SP, Clark CR, Loveless S, DeLosAngeles D, Wallace AK, Broberg M, Willoughby JO. Thinking activates EMG in scalp electrical recordings. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119:1166–1175. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood J, Kim Y, Moghaddam B. Disruption of prefrontal cortex large scale neuronal activity by different classes of psychotomimetic drugs. J Neurosci. 2012;32:3022–3031. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6377-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zou Q, Wu CW, Stein EA, Zang Y, Yang Y. Static and dynamic characteristics of cerebral blood flow during the resting state. Neuroimage. 2009;48:515–524. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]