Abstract
Operant control of a prosthesis by neuronal cortical activity is one of the successful strategies for implementing brain-machine interfaces (BMI), by which the subject learns to exert a volitional control of goal-directed movements. However, it remains unknown if the induced brain circuit reorganization affects preferentially the conditioned neurons whose activity controlled the BMI actuator during training. Here, multiple extracellular single-units were recorded simultaneously in the motor cortex of head-fixed behaving rats. The firing rate of a single neuron was used to control the position of a one-dimensional actuator. Each time the firing rate crossed a predefined threshold, a water bottle moved toward the rat, until the cumulative displacement of the bottle allowed the animal to drink. After a learning period, most (88%) conditioned neurons raised their activity during the trials, such that the time to reward decreased across sessions: the conditioned neuron fired strongly, reliably and swiftly after trial onset, although no explicit instruction in the learning rule imposed a fast neuronal response. Moreover, the conditioned neuron fired significantly earlier and more strongly than nonconditioned neighboring neurons. During the first training sessions, an increase in firing rate variability was seen only for the highly conditionable neurons. This variability then decreased while the conditioning effect increased. These findings suggest that modifications during training target preferentially the neuron chosen to control the BMI, which acts then as a “master” neuron, leading in time the reconfiguration of activity in the local cortical network.
Introduction
Understanding the neuronal code in motor cortical areas has long been a key issue in neuroscience. It should allow scientists to extract the relevant brain firing patterns preceding movement execution (Evarts, 1968) to move a prosthetic device (Humphrey et al., 1970; Schmidt et al., 1978). Multiple demonstrations of neuronal control of a robotic limb or of a cursor in real-time have been achieved in the last decade, giving rise to the new field of brain-machine interfaces (BMI). These pioneering studies are principally based on the decoding of a large neural ensemble activity, i.e., activity from dozens to hundreds of neurons (Taylor et al., 2002; Carmena et al., 2003; Velliste et al., 2008). An alternative way, envisioned already a long time ago (Olds, 1965; Fetz, 1969; Fetz and Baker, 1973; Schmidt, 1980) but transposed to a real robotic device only recently (Moritz et al., 2008), consists in associating prosthesis movement rules with the neural output of a small number of neurons, from 1 to 10. Repeated associations between a self-generated neuronal pattern and an actuator that controls access to reward (such as a prosthetic arm bringing the reward to the mouth) result in the reactivation of the neural process, which thus becomes operant in maximizing reward probability. This strategy, i.e., imposing a predefined association rule between neural activity and reward accessibility, does not require knowing beforehand the exact role and function of the conditioned neurons in terms of motor control; instead it relies on behavioral adaptation and learning—and sometimes on the forced coadaptation of the algorithm rule itself and brain circuits (Gage et al., 2005; Marzullo et al., 2006).
Previous implementations of prosthetic control by one or a few neurons mostly concentrated on optimizing the spatial precision with which a target could be reached. Less importance has been given to the speed of neuronal reaction or success rate during a session (Schmidt, 1980; Gage et al., 2005; Moritz et al., 2008). However, the level of performance of a brain-machine interface should also include measures of speed control and reliability, two parameters important for the design of BMIs useful in everyday life.
In the present study, motor cortex neurons of awake behaving rats underwent operant conditioning one at a time, using a simple activity-based rule to drive a one-dimensional prosthetic actuator toward a fixed reward delivery position. We subjected the same selected neuron to several successive sessions, allowing us to estimate the best performance that can be expected for control of a BMI using the firing rate of a single-unit. After asymptotic training, neuronal reaction times of the conditioned units were fast, mostly under 200 ms. Because we always recorded multiple single neurons simultaneously, we examined the activity of neighboring nonconditioned units as well, and found that the activity patterns underlying control of the BMI were specific to the successfully conditioned neurons.
Materials and Methods
Animal handling and pretraining.
Eight male Wistar rats weighing 250–350 g were obtained from the in-house animal facility of the CNRS Campus of Gif-sur-Yvette (French Agriculture Ministry Authorization B91-272-105). Maintenance, manipulations, and surgery were performed in conformity with French (JO 2001-464) and European legislation (86/609/CEE) on animal experimentation. Before surgery, animals were gently handled by the experimenter, and progressively trained to stay quiet in a harness and drink from a bottle containing a solution of water and glucose (strawberry syrup). While attached in the harness, the posterior limbs laid on a platform, and the forelimbs were free to move. Animals were kept at 85% of their free-feeding weight. The bottle was mounted on a one-dimensional linear actuator (Festo) perpendicular to the rat body, and moved to and away from the rat mouth on a left-right axis (Fig. 1A). During preoperative training, the bottle followed four successive steps: (1) a waiting period of randomized duration from 8–12 s, in the dark, and during which the bottle was kept away from the animal; (2) a fast displacement of the bottle to the mouth position, during which a green light-emitting diode (LED) placed close to the animal was “on”; (3) a period of 3 s of drinking during which a blue LED was “on”; and (4) a return travel back to the initial position. A new waiting period started simultaneously with the start of the bottle return. Two sessions of 10–15 min occurred each day, consisting each of up to 50 repetitions approximately. The LEDs were switched “on” and “off” by a microcontroller (Arduino Diecimila). The whole preconditioning period lasted several weeks, after which the animal was well accustomed to the setup and showed no sign of stress throughout an entire session.
Surgical procedure.
Two days before surgery, the rat received subcutaneous injections of 0.1 ml of the anti-inflammatory drug meloxicam (Metacam 0.5 mg.kg−1) and 0.1 ml of the antibiotics cevofecin (Convenia 25 mg.kg−1) to prevent pain and infections, respectively. On the day of surgery, we placed the animal in a ventilated box and induced anesthesia with isoflurane at 3%. The animal was then transferred to a stereotaxic frame. Anesthesia was maintained throughout surgery with isoflurane, the level of which was progressively decreased down to ∼1.5%. The ear bars were covered with lidocaine gel (Xylocaine) and we injected 0.3 ml of lidocaine 2% under the head skin before incision. Once the skull was exposed, seven to eight screws were inserted, both to ensure a strong contact between skull and implant, and for electrical grounding (see below). A craniotomy was drilled above the forelimb or hindlimb region of the motor cortex, and the dura was resected. We implanted arrays of 32 electrodes: for six rats, we used microwire arrays of 8 rows and 4 columns with a grid spacing of 0.25 mm (Microprobes for Life Sciences) and for two rats, we used custom-made tetrodes distributed over an area of 0.25 mm2. The electrodes were lowered to a cortical depth of ∼1300 μm. We verified in two instances that the electrode tip was indeed in deep layers of the primary motor cortex, by performing electrolytic lesions and Nissl staining of brain slices. Once the microwire array was in place, a ground wire was coiled around one or several of the ground screws (A-M Systems). Gelfoam was applied around the upper part of the electrodes outside the brain to help prevent excessive bleeding. Drops of cyanoacrylate were sparsely spread on the dry skull. The electrode array was then fixed in place with dental acrylic (Henry Schein). Finally, a piece of polyvinyl chloride (PVC, custom-made) was embedded in the dental cement to allow head-fixation in subsequent training sessions (see below). The rat received a saline injection intraperitoneally before the anesthesia was stopped. Food was accessible ad libitum for 5 d during which the rat was closely looked after to check proper recovery. Drops of an oral solution of meloxicam were given if signs of pain or disturbance were noticed.
Head fixation.
The rat was then submitted to the same food control protocol than before surgery. The training sessions were similar and the rat was trained to stay quiet during a strict head-fixation ensured with a 3D articulated arm (NMG700030, Noga). The arm extremity (NFA1100) was designed to match the PVC piece glued to the skull (Fig. 1A). With that device, the rat snout and mouth could be positioned precisely by the experimenter in front of the bottle in the drinking position. The four limbs were still free to move as before.
Data acquisition and control of the behavioral setup during training.
Neuronal activity was recorded and processed in real-time (Cerebus hardware, BlackRock Microsystems). Each electrode output was filtered between 250 Hz and 7.5 kHz and sampled at 30 kHz. Spikes were sorted online (Central software, BlackRock Microsystems). Spike sorting was performed at the beginning of each session, using a template-matching method: assignment of a waveform to a unit depended on whether it crossed all the criterion windows drawn by the experimenter (Fig. 1B, inset). A putative unit was considered as well isolated if less than one percentage of spikes was contained in the first bin (2 ms) of its autocorrelogram. Spikes were considered to be emitted by the same unit from one session to the next when their waveform remained invariant withstanding the precision of the measure. Spikes of nonconditioned neurons were not always successfully isolated throughout all successive sessions for the currently conditioned neuron.
All information was sent to a computer (Dell Intel QuadCore at 2.66 GHz, 3.24 Gb of RAM, OS Windows XP) via a fiber-optic data link. A custom-made software (Eclipse Qt C++) read in the spike information in real-time and commanded the linear actuator holding the bottle through a serial 56k bauds communication. The instantaneous bottle position was recorded on the same file through another serial port whenever the bottle crossed from one spatial bin to another (10 bins spanning the bottle course).
Neuronal control of the bottle position.
Training during neural control sessions broke down into repetitions of the same four steps described above for the pretraining period: waiting, bottle travel to the mouth, drinking, bottle travel back. However, during the second step (the trial), the bottle did not move automatically toward the rat mouth as before electrode implantation, but was now submitted to neural rules on the basis of the ongoing recorded activity. A single unit was chosen as the operantly conditioned neuron for controlling the bottle position. Criteria for selection were stability of recording over days, high signal-to-noise ratio, wide firing rate distribution, and modulation with limb movement. During the experiment, spiking activity was computed every 62.5 ms, and was smoothed over 500 ms by convolving each spike with a continuous filter: h(t) = 2 * (0.5 − t) between 0 and 0.5 s and h(t) = 0 otherwise. When the green LED was turned on (playing the role of a “GO” signal and marking the trial onset), the neuron had to increase the smoothed firing rate above a high threshold fhigh to make the bottle move toward the mouth (Fig. 1B). The speed increased when the firing rate f increased according to the relation: v(f) = v0.(f − flow)/(fhigh − flow) if f ≥ fhigh, v(f) = 0 otherwise. Note that there was no movement of the bottle when the firing rate f crossed the low threshold; flow only affected the slope of the v(f) function. The low and high thresholds flow and fhigh were re-evaluated every block of three successive trials. Their value was set respectively to 10% and 90–94% of the firing distribution (see below). Once the bottle entered the drinking zone, it was automatically stabilized in the mouth position and the 3 s drinking period started. We carried on the conditioning of the same neuron in the next session unless one of three conditions occurred: (1) the neuron was trained successfully up to the highest level of difficulty (see below); (2) the recording was lost, or the waveform or firing rate changed so that we could no longer ascertain that it was the same neuron; (3) 10 successive sessions were not sufficient to induce successful conditioning.
Task difficulty was gradually increased across sessions to reach the final parameters of neuronal control. Maximum trial length was set to 30 s for the first session and was progressively decreased to 7 s. The speed factor of the bottle v0 also decreased, from ∼3 cm.s−1 to ∼1 cm.s−1. These parameters were fixed before starting the session. When in the course of one session, a sudden drop in performance or motivation threatened to greatly lower the number of trials, the task could then be made easier or more difficult by modifying the threshold fhigh inside the range 90–94% of the firing rate distribution.
Conditioning criterion and conditioning effect.
All spiking activities were analyzed with a custom-made program (Eclipse Qt C++). Results were displayed with the same software or in Matlab (MathWorks).
Performance during a session was measured by counting the number of trials during which reinforcement was obtained. However, we always kept the task difficulty sufficiently low so that the animal received frequent rewards during the session. This was necessary to avoid stress of the head-fixed rat and maintain its behavioral motivation (Schwarz et al., 2010). Thus, we devised a measure of the effect of the conditioning protocol during one session based on the comparison of test and control trials completed under the same set of session parameters. Accordingly, we compared the movements of the bottle during the trials to the virtual movements of the bottle that would have been produced if the neural activity of the waiting period had been used, together with the same activity-to-speed rule as described above for trials. For these reconstructed trajectories, we took the neural activity between 3 and 6 s of each waiting period and concatenated these episodes to obtain periods of activity with a duration as long as the real trials. To confirm the validity of our reconstruction algorithm, we applied it also to the real trial periods and compared the real trajectories and time-to-rewards with the reconstructed ones using trial activity (Fig. 2A,B, magenta vs green curves). Trajectories were most often indistinguishable. We observed small discrepancies due to the fact that whereas the offline algorithm indeed calculates the smoothed neuronal activity precisely every 62.5 ms, the online algorithm was sometimes delayed by a computer clock increment so that the change in bottle speed was delayed as well. Thus, time-to-reward values were consistently slightly longer during the real trials than when simulated offline (Fig. 2B). To eliminate this bias in the rest of our analysis, we always compared the waiting period time-to-reward distribution to the offline (reconstructed) trial time-to-reward distribution, and not the online (real) trial time-to-reward distribution.
The neuron was considered successfully conditioned for a session if the distributions of time-to-reward of the waiting and trial periods were significantly different (two-tailed unpaired Student's t test, p < 0.01). To estimate the strength of the conditioning effect, we used the normalized mean difference to capture the distance between the time-to-reward distributions: d′ = (μW − μT)/√(σW2 + σT2), with μ and σ representing the mean and SD of the times-to-reward for waiting (W) and trial (T) reconstructions. This measure gets larger when the activity of the neuron is modulated quickly and strongly after trial onset. For analyses requiring only one session per neuron, we defined the best session of the neuron as the session with the highest conditioning effect d′.
The three parameters defining the task difficulty, i.e., fhigh, the maximum trial length, and the velocity v0 (see above), were the same for the real trials and for the simulated (reconstructed) trials using the waiting period activity.
Latency of individual trials, mean session latency, and rank of activation.
To compute the latencies of firing rate increases for the individual trials, we convolved the instantaneous firing rate with a filter pattern designed to signal an increase in activity hinc = [0 1 2 2.5]. The bin width was 100 ms with 20 ms sliding steps. To detect high values, we converted the filtered profile to a z-score waveform, using the mean and SD in the window [−2 s 0 s] during the last five waiting periods. The trial latency was then computed in two steps, by finding: (1) the time after trial onset at which the z-score was first above a value of 3 (p < 0.00135 for a one sided t test), and (2) within that 100 ms window, the time abscissa of the first z-score >3 in the filtered profile calculated with 20 ms bins. If no such variation of discharge was found in 10 s, it meant either that no increase and no reward occurred or that reward occurred without a measurable onset of increase, and the trial latency was left unassigned. We defined the neuronal reaction time of the neuron during one session by finding the mode of the distribution of trial latencies, or the mean of the modes if there were several local maxima.
Response latencies were sometimes not detected on individual trials, even though the neuron exhibited a clear increase in activity when averaged across trials. To quantify the delay of this increase, we defined a mean session latency. A perievent time histogram (PETH) between −2 s and +2 s relative to the trial start was constructed with a sliding window of 100 and 20 ms step. The mean of the PETH was computed within [−2 s; 0 s]. We transformed the raw PETH into an equivalent normalized PETH of z-score values using this mean and the corresponding variance value of a Poisson process. We first searched for the earliest 100 ms duration bins where six successive z-scores were found >2 (p < 0.023). Within this selected time window, the mean session latency was given by the earliest point in time where the first z-score was found >1 using a discretization of 20 ms bins. A second set of z-score thresholds, 5 (p < 1.10−4) and 2.5 (p < 0.00625), was used to detect high increases in firing rate. If, during a session, a latency could be defined for at least one neuron, we looked at the order of activation of all other neurons simultaneously recorded during that session based on their mean session latency. If several neurons had the same latency, the rank of activation was defined by the rounded mean position of those neurons.
Variability.
For each session, we studied the evolution of the firing variability of a neuron in the waiting periods between the end of one trial and the beginning of the next one. These waiting periods were of randomized duration, between 8 and 12 s. To quantify the trial-to-trial variability, we computed the spike count within each 200 ms bin in the waiting period, with time bins locked to the previous trial end. We calculated the Fano Factor (FF) by dividing for each time bin the variance of the spike count by its mean. This measure gives 1 for an ideal Poisson process, as often reported for cortical neurons during ongoing activity (for review, see Churchland et al., 2006).
Distances between recording sites were assessed from the known topology of the microwire rectangular array. The minimum distance between two recording wires was 250 μm (grid spacing) and the maximal distance was 1.9 mm. Data from the two rats implanted with tetrodes were discarded for analyses involving these distances (see Figs. 5D,E, 6A2,B2), as they could not be estimated accurately. We rarely recorded activity from more than one neuron per electrode per session, so that we could not estimate the average firing properties of nonconditioned neurons at the same location site as conditioned neurons.
Results
General features of neural-based operant conditioning
One hundred seventy-eight single units were recorded from the primary motor cortex of eight rats. Seventeen neurons were trained, one at a time, for neuronal operant conditioning, from 1 to 6 per animal.
Before surgery, the animals were accustomed to sit in a harness and drink from a bottle containing a sweet liquid, while their head was maintained fixed (Fig. 1A). For each trial of this preconditioning period, the bottle moved automatically from its initial position to the drinking zone in front of the mouth. The animal was then allowed to drink during 3 s, after which the bottle returned to the start position and a waiting period of 8–12 s elapsed before the next trial. Once a unit was chosen for neuronal control, this conditioned unit had to increase its firing rate above a high threshold to move the bottle toward the animal until it reached the drinking zone (Fig. 1B, blue area). Liquid reinforcement was then allowed during 3 s. Above the threshold, the speed of the bottle increased linearly with the smoothed firing rate of the unit (see Materials and Methods). Animals were submitted to two training sessions per day, each session consisting of ∼50–100 trials both in the preconditioning and the conditioning period. Task difficulty was progressively adjusted so that a sufficient number of completed trials could be collected during any given session, both for data analysis purposes, and for maintaining a high reward level and motivation of the animal (Schwarz et al., 2010).
To quantify the performance of the conditioned neuron during one session, we compared the spiking activity patterns during the trials to those recorded during the same session in the waiting periods between trials (see Materials and Methods). This comparison was based on the ability of spike patterns to successfully bring the bottle to the drinking position. Figure 2A shows the action potentials of a conditioned neuron during one trial and during a waiting period of same duration, as well as the virtual control trajectories of the bottle calculated with the neural control algorithm using these two firing patterns as inputs (green and black lines, respectively). The firing rate of the unit increased 500 ms after trial onset, resulting in a rapid movement of the bottle toward the drinking zone until it entered it at 1 s. By contrast, the firing activity of the waiting period remained low and resulted in a simulated trajectory of the bottle that was still outside the drinking zone after 3 s. We defined the time-to-reward as the time after onset when the reconstructed position of the bottle first entered the drinking zone. For this conditioned neuron, the average time-to-reward varied little for the first three sessions (Fig. 2B, green curve) and decreased progressively in the following sessions, whereas the average time-to-reward calculated on the waiting period activity remained relatively unchanged (black curve). We defined a successful conditioning session as one for which the average time-to-reward was significantly reduced during trials compared with the one obtained with reconstructed values from the waiting period (two-tailed unpaired Student's t test, p < 0.01; Fig. 2B, green stars). This neuron was thus successfully conditioned from session 4 to session 8, which was its last conditioning session. On Figure 2C, all the trajectories of the second (left) and seventh (right) session are plotted for the trial (top) and waiting (bottom) periods. Bottle movements were similar for both periods during the second session, whereas in the seventh session, many more trajectories entered the drinking zone during trial periods than waiting periods activity (40% vs 4% in the first second of the trials). This analysis was applied to all neurons selected for operant conditioning (n = 17). All but two neurons were successfully conditioned in at least one session.
In addition to determining for each session whether the conditioning protocol had a significant effect, we quantified the magnitude of the associated changes. For example, for the fifth session of the data displayed in Figure 3A (same neuron as Fig. 2B), we plotted the histograms of time-to-reward for the two reconstruction datasets (Fig. 3B). As defined above, a conditioning session was successful when these two distributions were significantly different. We further defined the conditioning effect (d′—see Materials and Methods) as the distance (d) between the means of these two histograms, normalized by the combined SD. At the population level, the percentage of neurons that were successfully conditioned increased with the number of sessions (Fig. 3C, r2 = 0.69, p < 7.10−5). Figure 3D shows that in parallel, the average conditioning effect d′ increased significantly (r2 = 0.82, p < 2.10−6). On average, 896 trials were necessary to reach the maximal task difficulty. This level was successfully achieved in 4 neurons and required 10–25 consecutive sessions per conditioned neuron.
Furthermore, we tested whether there was an improvement of performance within sessions. We divided each session in two equal successive blocks of trials and compared the conditioning effect in the two halves. On average, 15 of 16 sessions of conditioning showed an improvement in the second half of the session compared with the first half (Fig. 3E).
We conclude that neuronal control resulted in rapid movements of the bottle to the drinking zone after trial onset, and that this was learnt progressively from session to session for a given neuron.
Reliable neuronal reaction time at each trial onset
The strength of the conditioning effect does not indicate whether the modulation of activity took place late or early during the trial or even in anticipation of the trial onset. We determined when the activity of the neuron was modulated relative to the trial start, on a trial-by-trial basis, and how regular and consistent were the fast modulations. To that end we selected for each neuron the session with the highest conditioning effect, to estimate a lower bound of best performance that can be achieved with this protocol with sufficient training. For such a session, we constructed a raster plot centered on each trial start and indicated the latency of the neuronal activity increase for each trial (see Materials and Methods), if measurable (Fig. 4A1, magenta dots). On a few trials, the neuron failed to show a significant increase and the rat missed the reward. The average bottle movement and increase of activity during trials for the whole session are displayed below the raster plot (Fig. 4A2,A3), confirming that the activity rose in the first few hundreds of milliseconds. We estimated the neuronal reaction time for that session by determining the mode of the distribution of individual trial latencies (150 ms; Fig. 4A3). Figure 4B shows the distribution of those modes for the population of conditioned neurons, and this for the best session of each neuron as defined above. We observed that the discharge rate of 13 of the 17 conditioned neurons had a peak reaction time shorter than 500 ms, and 8 among them consistently fired in the first 200 ms after trial onset. Among these 8 fastest neurons, 7 had 100% successful trials during the session, and the last one 98%. In total this corresponds to 1 failure per 500 trials. This shows that for nearly half of the neurons that were conditioned, the increase in firing was very reliable and fast, despite the absence of any requirements on the neuronal reaction time during the trial. This performance was only reached in sessions with the highest conditioning effect. When averaging over all sessions and all neurons, the percentage of trials for which the animal obtained the reward was 85%.
The conditioned neurons are the first activated in the local network
The conditioned neuron and many neighboring neurons were recorded simultaneously with electrodes arranged in a 250 μm grid around the conditioned neuron electrode. In total, 161 neighboring neurons were recorded in the eight rats (∼15–20 per session). We often observed increases in the firing rate of nonconditioned neurons after trial onset, as displayed by the PETHs of Figure 5A for one conditioned neuron (green line) and three neighboring neurons (blue and red lines). To assess the temporal organization of activity in the local network, we computed the latency of activation of each neuron for each session, if measurable (see Materials and Methods; Fig. 5A, filled and open circles for two levels of sensitivity). Each neuron was assigned a rank of activation based on the response latencies of all the neurons recorded simultaneously. We divided the rank data for all sessions into three categories, depending on whether the neuron was conditioned at that session, had been submitted to operant conditioning previously, or had never been submitted to operant conditioning.
Across all sessions, the conditioned neurons responded faster than neurons from the two other categories. The distribution of ranks for conditioned neurons was significantly shifted toward lower values compared with the distributions for the two other groups (Fig. 5B; Mann–Whitney U test, p < 10−8 and p < 10−28, n = 165 sessions). This was especially true for ranks 1 and 2, which indicates that the conditioned neuron was often among the first neurons to respond. To confirm that the conditioned neuron was reacting both fast and strongly, we raised the threshold of latency detection from 2 to 5 SD above the mean (see Materials and Methods), so that only neurons with highly significant increases in firing rate were now considered for determining the activation order in the network. As expected, fewer neurons responded according to that criterion, and the number of sessions where we could measure the latency of at least one neuron decreased (Fig. 5C, n = 109 sessions). However, taking into account only those sessions, the proportion of conditioned neurons with rank 1 or rank 2 increased from 33% to 42%, whereas it decreased for the two other categories of neurons. This confirmed that the conditioned neuron tended to react more often, faster and more strongly than the surrounding nonconditioned neurons.
We also quantified directly the strength of activation of each neuron by integrating the firing rate above the baseline value in the first second after trial onset, whenever it exceeded the mean by two times the SD value (Fig. 5D, inset). For this analysis, the conditioned neurons were classified in two categories: those that showed significant conditioning in at least half of the sessions, called “highly conditionable” (n = 10), and those that failed in more than half of the sessions, called “weakly conditionable” (n = 7). During all training sessions, conditioned neurons (green) associated with a strong conditioning efficacy (highly conditionable) exhibited on average a larger response integral than nonconditioned neighboring neurons (Fig. 5D, red). The response integral of nonconditioned neurons did not depend on the distance to the conditioned neurons. During training sessions with weakly conditionable neurons, all neurons displayed similar response integrals (Fig. 5E).
Firing rate variability of the conditioned neuron increases before trial onset
Trial-to-trial variability of spiking activity in a neuronal network has been previously proposed to be inversely related to the degree of preparation before a movement (Churchland et al., 2006). In addition, it can facilitate learning and adaptation in a dynamic environment by favoring the exploration of more network states (Faisal et al., 2008). In particular, it is exacerbated at the time and location where sensorimotor learning is supposed to happen (Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2009). To establish a possible relation between variability in the ongoing activity and success of the conditioning, we quantified the trial-to-trial variability of the firing rate of each neuron during the waiting period using the FF index (see Materials and Methods). At the beginning of the waiting period, there was no significant difference between the conditioned and nonconditioned neurons (Fig. 6A1,B1, early). However, the highly conditionable neurons showed a strong increase of their mean variability index at the end of the waiting period (Fig. 6A1, green curve) compared with the nonconditioned population average (Fig. 6A1, red curve; Mann–Whitney U test, p < 0.01 are noted with stars). No trend was noticeable for the weakly conditionable category (Fig. 6B1). Also, the increase in variability was not accompanied by a concomitant trend of the mean firing rate (Fig. 6A1, gray line).
To assess whether this increase was due to a bias in the selection of conditioned neurons, we looked at the mean variability index during the waiting period for neurons that had been submitted to a conditioning protocol in previous sessions. Indeed, if we just happened to select neurons with a propensity for high variability to start with, we expect all conditioned neurons—currently or previously conditioned—to exhibit the same variability profile in any session. In contrast, the curve for the previously conditioned neurons did not differ significantly from that of the nonconditioned neurons (Mann–Whitney U test, p < 0.01, data not shown). It is thus unlikely that the high variability that we observed was due to a selection bias of the neurons. Similarly, we wanted to assess the possibility that the high trial-to-trial variability emerges from correct patterns of activity triggered in the local network in anticipation of trial start. We averaged the variability for the group of nonconditioned neurons that were responsive after trial onset, i.e., neurons for which the session latency could be measured and thus are most susceptible to participate in patterns of activity related to the task. Again, there was no significant difference with the nonconditioned average (Mann–Whitney U test, p < 0.01, data not shown). This implies that task-related increases in firing did not generate by themselves increased variability before trial onset, because in that case the increased variability should have been visible for these neurons as well. These results suggest that the increase in Fano Factor index before trial onset is a correlate of the learning specific to the currently conditioned neuron.
This was further confirmed by plotting the variability index as a function of the distance between the recorded neuron and the conditioned neuron. Again, the variability increase was found to be selective for the conditioned neuron. The mean variability computed for the neurons located at each distance did not significantly differ from the average variability of the population (Fig. 6A2, late; Mann–Whitney U test, stars indicate significance at the p < 0.01 level). For sessions involving weakly conditionable neurons, no significant difference from the population level was noticeable at any distance either (Fig. 6B2, late). Overall, these data indicates no observable spatial trend of the variability increase depending on the distance to the conditioned neuron. This observation does not contradict the fact that neurons other than the one used to control behavior also modified their activity during the learning process (Fig. 5D), but it excludes a possible gradient of variability, or at least indicates that it is very localized with a length constant smaller than 250 μm. In summary, an increase in the trial-to-trial variability of the firing rate of the conditioned neuron develops during the waiting period, for highly conditionable neurons, but this modification does not seem to involve any specific motor cortex region around it, except possibly a small one.
Finally, to examine how the transient increase in variability before trial onset changes in the course of learning, we computed the variability of the highly conditionable neurons at the end of the waiting period as a function of session number and confronted it with the learning curve of those neurons. The late variability was high during the first sessions of the training and progressively decreased throughout the sessions (Fig. 7, solid green curve). During the last sessions, the variability was close to the variability of a Poisson process (FF = 1), that is, it was at the same level than during the early part of the waiting period, and at the same level than that of nonconditioned or weakly conditionable neurons. Interestingly, the curve of performance (Fig. 7, dashed green curve) increased in the first sessions as long as the variability was high, and stabilized in the last sessions when the variability returned to its intrinsic level, confirming that the variability decrease time course was indeed related to the learning process. The time course of the variability throughout sessions for the nonconditioned neurons did not follow the same trend, as it slightly increased over days (Fig. 7, red curve), possibly demonstrating a refocus of learning (Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2009).
Discussion
We applied operant conditioning on single neurons in the rat motor cortex, to assess whether the reactivity and response reliability of a single-unit provides an output signal efficient enough for controlling brain-machine interfaces. Our results support the concept that using one neuron at a time may be suitable for BMI control. Most neurons tested (88%) successfully learnt the operant task at least in one of the sessions during which they were submitted to conditioning. The conditioning effect strengthened over successive sessions with the same neuron. The conditioned neurons displayed a number of specific functional modifications compared with simultaneously recorded neurons, notably a larger increase in firing rate, a shorter neuronal reaction time, and a transient change in discharge variability in anticipation of the trial onset.
Improvement in the operant task often required up to 5–10 sessions with the same neuron. This was similar to results reported from the few other operant conditioning studies conducted with rats (Gage et al., 2005; Marzullo et al., 2006). By contrast, operant conditioning of neurons in the primate motor cortex can be achieved much faster, within minutes (Moritz et al., 2008).
Previous operant conditioning studies focused on mean firing rate changes (Olds, 1965; Fetz, 1969) or mean changes at trial onset (Gage et al., 2005; Marzullo et al., 2006). Only a few works reported single trial reaction times, but without quantification for all neurons and trials (Evarts, 1966; Schmidt et al., 1978). Here, we examined the kinetics of activity for each single trial, and focused for each conditioned neuron on the session with the highest conditioning effect. Despite the fact that units had potentially the whole trial of several seconds to increase their firing rate, most of the conditioned units consistently responded within 500 ms, and nearly half of them below 200 ms. These latencies compare to the ones found by Schmidt et al. (1978) when conditioning motor cortex neurons in the hand-arm area or by Evarts (1966) when monkeys performed a fast movement of grasping, while recording pyramidal tract cells in motor cortex.
In the eight neurons exhibiting a reduced latency of the operant response output, we observed a high reliability across trials, such that the percentage of successful trials reached 99.8% for the best sessions recorded for these neurons. This might appear unexpected, in light of recent studies suggesting that the motor representation may be continuously changing at the single cell level, even for a given stable output (Cohen and Nicolelis, 2004; Carmena et al., 2005; Rokni et al., 2007). However, other studies have argued on the contrary for a strong stability of functional properties of motor cortex neurons (Chestek et al., 2007; Stevenson et al., 2011). In our experiments, the decoding rule of the prosthesis was fixed and only one neuron was used. This could have forced the emergence of a stable and precise map, in which the conditioned neuron was the sole operant output. For the sessions that were less reliable, it remains to be determined whether the failures were caused by changes in the internal states like attention or motivational drops, intrinsic stochasticity of some of the conditioned neurons, or a motor strategy limiting the precision of the firing rate.
Most remarkably, we found that the conditioned neuron fired on average before neighboring neurons recorded simultaneously, and exhibited stronger modulations. This was unexpected, because our protocol did not require the conditioned neuron to increase its firing rate first or more strongly. Indeed, if conditioned responses had occurred in synchrony with increased activity in the network a few hundred milliseconds later, this would essentially have produced the same number of drinking reinforcements by the end of the session. Our results show that the neuron responsible for the instrumental control acquires the functional role of a “master” cell, taking the lead of activation of the local cortical network.
We wondered whether conditioned neurons responded to bottle movements, so that a positive sensory feedback might be responsible for the conditioned neurons modulation of activity. PETHs aligned with the onset of bottle movement of each trial showed a phasic peak of response around the movement in the conditioned master neurons only, which was absent in nonconditioned neurons. We cannot exclude that a positive feedback loop triggered by the bottle displacement participates to later phases of maintained activity, but the differential patterns observed between conditioned and nonconditioned cells suggests that the time course of the activity does not reflect simply visual reafference.
Several observations strengthen the concept of a specificity of the conditioned cells developing progressively during learning of the operant task. First, master cells exhibited a surprisingly large firing rate variability in the waiting periods between trials. This was in contrast with the smaller variability observed for nonconditioned neurons and for conditioned neurons that failed to reach a high performance threshold (weakly conditionable neurons). For those categories, the variability was at the level of a Poisson process, as reported in other cortical studies (for review, see Churchland et al., 2006). Examination of the time course showed that the variability of the master cells started at baseline level and increased progressively throughout the waiting period, until it became significantly higher than control values before trial onset. High variability has been reported in the literature in situations involving uncertainty of the action to be executed and a related lack of preparation of the motor plan (Churchland et al., 2006; Afshar et al., 2011). Here, all trials have identical constraints for completion. However, because the duration of the waiting periods was variable, the high variability in firing rate could reflect the building up of expectancy of the next trial start. Alternatively, the high variability of master cells could reflect intrinsic mechanisms of learning. Similar observations have been reported during trace eye blink conditioning (Disterhoft et al., 1988) and in motor cortex during Pavlovian learning (Woody et al., 1991; Saar and Barkai, 2003). Both studies report the same temporal dissociation between an early phase of increased excitability and its extinction during the asymptotic phase of learning (Moyer et al., 1996). Indeed, such excitability process could facilitate learning by exploring patterns of discharge that are not commonly triggered (Rokni et al., 2007; Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2009). This is further supported by the temporal relationship between the master cell variability and the conditioning effect (Fig. 7), showing that this mechanism is necessary for learning but not for performance afterward. The transient nature of increased excitability also suggests that some regulatory processes restore the normal baseline state, which is an important property for a mechanism supporting memory consolidation (Byrne, 1987). In our case, the fact that the mean variability of the nonconditioned population slowly increases in late sessions additionally suggests that once the master cell has been established, plasticity mechanisms could occur in the local network and consolidate task-related modifications of nonconditioned neurons (Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2009).
Another indication that the master cell occupies a unique position comes from the unexpected spatial selectivity of both the firing rate response and the increase in variability between trials. Whereas most studies of learning-related changes noticed widespread phenomena (Pascual-Leone et al., 1994), or modifications that are common to all cells tuned for the task (Churchland et al., 2006; Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2009), in our study the functional modifications are largely restricted to the successfully conditioned neurons. Of course, we do not exclude the possibility of nondetected plastic modifications since our recording sample is limited, but they would be necessarily sparsely distributed in the network, or very close to the master neuron (<250 μm).
Two mechanisms could lead the master neuron to fire earlier and more strongly than others: a change in intrinsic excitability and/or a change in the synaptic circuitry of the pathways that ultimately activate the conditioned neuron. In the motor cortex, enhanced excitability and signal transduction are dependent on neuromodulatory dopamine projections, known to be active during learning and reward (Yasumoto et al., 2002; Schultz, 2007; Hosp et al., 2009; Hosp et al., 2011). A possible interpretation of our results is that dopamine levels may change progressively and affect selectively the neurons of the motor cortex that are involved actively in the task. Other processes could come into play such as the unmasking of latent priming effects revealed when the learning context reappears, tagging (Redondo and Morris, 2011), and state-dependent learning (Shulz et al., 2000). However, it is difficult to explain how such modifications could be restricted only to the master neuron. Alternatively, plasticity changes of the master neuron may reflect synaptic modifications distributed upstream in the sensory and sensorimotor networks recruited by the task (Meftah and Rispal-Padel, 1994). Again, dopamine release triggered by the task reinforcements could be an important factor for the induction of such modifications (Rioult-Pedotti et al., 2000; Bao et al., 2001; Molina-Luna et al., 2009).
In the context of the development of efficient brain-machine interfaces, the demonstration of operant conditioning of the firing rate of a single neuron, almost in real time, should help building bottom-up strategies for higher dimensional control, in particular n-D as required for sophisticated actuators. To design a full BMI with our paradigm, each of several conditioned neurons—or small groups of neurons, to increase reliability even more—should be assigned to control different state variables (kinematic, dynamic, or even higher level parameters) or different task-related values of a prosthetic device (Snyder et al., 1997; Musallam et al., 2004). This requires that neurons can function independently. Such independence has been previously observed for motoneurons (Smith et al., 1974), motor units (Basmajian, 1963), and between motor cortex neurons and muscles (Fetz and Finocchio, 1971; Fetz and Baker, 1973; Moritz et al., 2008). The operant conditioning approach, exploiting the plasticity properties of cortical neurons, should be pursued in parallel to the fruitful advances obtained with the neural ensemble decoding approach. Initially based on the recording of very large populations of neurons, this strategy was recently implemented using a few tens of cortical motor neurons in order for tetraplegic patients to successfully control a robotic arm (Hochberg et al., 2012).
The functional changes induced in the cortical network by operant conditioning may be explained by different but not exclusive scenarios. A first scheme posits that the primary changes are not restricted to the conditioned cell and that the operant control of the bottle is established through dynamic restructuration of correlations in the premotor and motor networks. Such network changes may result both from synaptic and excitability changes, as often observed during classical behavioral (Disterhoft et al., 1986) or cellular (Daoudal and Debanne, 2003) conditioning. Unfortunately in our experiments, the sampling of nonconditioned cells was not dense enough to have a chance to reveal coupling changes in a distributed assembly.
However, the comparison between simultaneously recorded cells in our experiments showed that (1) the conditioning does not result in a spatial gradient centered in the vicinity of the conditioned cell whose activity changes drove the bottle displacement, and (2) that activation latencies point to a temporal reordering of the network activity. This latter observation suggests a second interpretative scheme, where the conditioned neurons would become master units encoding for the causality established through operant conditioning between the cell's firing rate and the bottle movement. Note that this concept, popular in electronics and robotics (e.g., tinkertrons) and invertebrate literature [“orchestra leaders” in the study by Meyrand et al. (1994)], share strong similarities to that of “grand-mother” cells (for review, see Bowers, 2009) and “iconic memory” cells (Sakai et al., 1994), except that the emergence of these highly specialized neurons through learning applies here to anticipation of action/decision rather than perception.
Footnotes
This work was supported by CNRS, European Union Sixth Framework Programme under the grant no. 15879 (FACETS), and European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreement no 243914 (Brain-i-Nets) and no. 269921 (BrainScaleS). We also thank the NERF (Neuropôle de Recherche Francilien) for cofinancing the multielectrode recording system (Grant 2009.22). P.-J.A.'s thesis was supported by ENP (Ecole des Neurosciences Paris Ile de France) and an FRM (Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale) grant was awarded to Y.F. (“Brain Machine Interface”). V.E.-S. was supported by a Career Development Award from the Human Frontier Science Program (HFSP) Organization. We thank Jean-Yves Tiercelin and Patrick Parra for mechanical work, Aurélie Daret and Guillaume Hucher for technical help, Kossi Agbeviade, Mathieu Benoît, and Jacques Giovanola at EPFL (Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne) for the design and implementation of the actuator, and Isabelle Férézou for helpful comments on this manuscript. We thank Dr. Miguel Nicolelis for his support during the initial phase of this project at EPFL, before the present protocol was performed at UNIC.
References
- Afshar A, Santhanam G, Yu BM, Ryu SI, Sahani M, Shenoy KV. Single-trial neural correlates of arm movement preparation. Neuron. 2011;71:555–564. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bao S, Chan VT, Merzenich MM. Cortical remodelling induced by activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons. Nature. 2001;412:79–83. doi: 10.1038/35083586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basmajian JV. Control and training of individual motor units. Science. 1963;141:440–441. doi: 10.1126/science.141.3579.440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowers JS. On the biological plausibility of grandmother cells: implications for neural network theories in psychology and neuroscience. Psychol Rev. 2009;116:220–251. doi: 10.1037/a0014462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne JH. Cellular analysis of associative learning. Physiol Rev. 1987;67:329–439. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1987.67.2.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmena JM, Lebedev MA, Crist RE, O'Doherty JE, Santucci DM, Dimitrov DF, Patil PG, Henriquez CS, Nicolelis MA. Learning to control a brain-machine interface for reaching and grasping by primates. PLoS Biol. 2003;1:E42. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0000042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmena JM, Lebedev MA, Henriquez CS, Nicolelis MA. Stable ensemble performance with single-neuron variability during reaching movements in primates. J Neurosci. 2005;25:10712–10716. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2772-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chestek CA, Batista AP, Santhanam G, Yu BM, Afshar A, Cunningham JP, Gilja V, Ryu SI, Churchland MM, Shenoy KV. Single-neuron stability during repeated reaching in macaque premotor cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27:10742–10750. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0959-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Churchland MM, Yu BM, Ryu SI, Santhanam G, Shenoy KV. Neural variability in premotor cortex provides a signature of motor preparation. J Neurosci. 2006;26:3697–3712. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3762-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen D, Nicolelis MA. Reduction of single-neuron firing uncertainty by cortical ensembles during motor skill learning. J Neurosci. 2004;24:3574–3582. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5361-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daoudal G, Debanne D. Long-term plasticity of intrinsic excitability: learning rules and mechanisms. Learn Mem. 2003;10:456–465. doi: 10.1101/lm.64103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Disterhoft JF, Coulter DA, Alkon DL. Conditioning-specific membrane changes of rabbit hippocampal neurons measured in vitro. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1986;83:2733–2737. doi: 10.1073/pnas.83.8.2733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Disterhoft JF, Golden DT, Read HL, Coulter DA, Alkon DL. AHP reductions in rabbit hippocampal neurons during conditioning correlate with acquisition of the learned response. Brain Res. 1988;462:118–125. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90593-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evarts EV. Pyramidal tract activity associated with a conditioned hand movement in the monkey. J Neurophysiol. 1966;29:1011–1027. doi: 10.1152/jn.1966.29.6.1011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evarts EV. Relation of pyramidal tract activity to force exerted during voluntary movement. J Neurophysiol. 1968;31:14–27. doi: 10.1152/jn.1968.31.1.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faisal AA, Selen LP, Wolpert DM. Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fetz EE. Operant conditioning of cortical unit activity. Science. 1969;163:955–958. doi: 10.1126/science.163.3870.955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fetz EE, Baker MA. Operantly conditioned patterns on precentral unit activity and correlated responses in adjacent cells and contralateral muscles. J Neurophysiol. 1973;36:179–204. doi: 10.1152/jn.1973.36.2.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fetz EE, Finocchio DV. Operant conditioning of specific patterns of neural and muscular activity. Science. 1971;174:431–435. doi: 10.1126/science.174.4007.431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gage GJ, Ludwig KA, Otto KJ, Ionides EL, Kipke DR. Naive coadaptive cortical control. J Neural Eng. 2005;2:52–63. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/2/2/006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hochberg LR, Bacher D, Jarosiewicz B, Masse NY, Simeral JD, Vogel J, Haddadin S, Liu J, Cash SS, van der Smagt P, Donoghue JP. Reach and grasp by people with tetraplegia using a neurally controlled robotic arm. Nature. 2012;485:372–375. doi: 10.1038/nature11076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hosp JA, Molina-Luna K, Hertler B, Atiemo CO, Luft AR. Dopaminergic modulation of motor maps in rat motor cortex: an in vivo study. Neuroscience. 2009;159:692–700. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.12.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hosp JA, Hertler B, Atiemo CO, Luft AR. Dopaminergic modulation of receptive fields in rat sensorimotor cortex. Neuroimage. 2011;54:154–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humphrey DR, Schmidt EM, Thompson WD. Predicting measures of motor performance from multiple cortical spike trains. Science. 1970;170:758–762. doi: 10.1126/science.170.3959.758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mandelblat-Cerf Y, Paz R, Vaadia E. Trial-to-trial variability of single cells in motor cortices is dynamically modified during visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci. 2009;29:15053–15062. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3011-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marzullo TC, Miller CR, Kipke DR. Suitability of the cingulate cortex for neural control. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2006;14:401–409. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2006.886730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meftah EM, Rispal-Padel L. Synaptic plasticity in the thalamo-cortical pathway as one of the neurobiological correlates of forelimb flexion conditioning: electrophysiological investigation in the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1994;72:2631–2647. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.6.2631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyrand P, Simmers J, Moulins M. Dynamic construction of a neural network from multiple pattern generators in the lobster stomatogastric nervous system. J Neurosci. 1994;14:630–644. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-02-00630.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molina-Luna K, Pekanovic A, Röhrich S, Hertler B, Schubring-Giese M, Rioult-Pedotti MS, Luft AR. Dopamine in motor cortex is necessary for skill learning and synaptic plasticity. PLoS One. 2009;4:e7082. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moritz CT, Perlmutter SI, Fetz EE. Direct control of paralysed muscles by cortical neurons. Nature. 2008;456:639–642. doi: 10.1038/nature07418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moyer JR, Jr, Thompson LT, Disterhoft JF. Trace eyeblink conditioning increases ca1 excitability in a transient and learning-specific manner. J Neurosci. 1996;16:5536–5546. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-17-05536.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Musallam S, Corneil BD, Greger B, Scherberger H, Andersen RA. Cognitive control signals for neural prosthetics. Science. 2004;305:258–262. doi: 10.1126/science.1097938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olds J. Operant conditioning of single unit responses. Excerpta Med Int Cong Series. 1965;87:372–380. [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Leone A, Grafman J, Hallett M. Modulation of cortical motor output maps during development of implicit and explicit knowledge. Science. 1994;263:1287–1289. doi: 10.1126/science.8122113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redondo RL, Morris RG. Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:17–30. doi: 10.1038/nrn2963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rioult-Pedotti MS, Friedman D, Donoghue JP. Learning-induced LTP in neocortex. Science. 2000;290:533–536. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5491.533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rokni U, Richardson AG, Bizzi E, Seung HS. Motor learning with unstable neural representations. Neuron. 2007;54:653–666. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.04.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saar D, Barkai E. Long-term modifications in intrinsic neuronal properties and rule learning in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:2727–2734. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02699.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakai K, Naya Y, Miyashita Y. Neuronal tuning and associative mechanisms in form representation. Learn Mem. 1994;1:83–105. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt EM. Single neuron recording from motor cortex as a possible source of signals for control of external devices. Ann Biomed Eng. 1980;8:339–349. doi: 10.1007/BF02363437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt EM, McIntosh JS, Durelli L, Bak MJ. Fine control of operantly conditioned firing patterns of cortical neurons. Exp Neurol. 1978;61:349–369. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(78)90252-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:203–210. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz C, Hentschke H, Butovas S, Haiss F, Stüttgen MC, Gerdjikov TV, Bergner CG, Waiblinger C. The head-fixed behaving rat–procedures and pitfalls. Somatosens Mot Res. 2010;27:131–148. doi: 10.3109/08990220.2010.513111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulz DE, Sosnik R, Ego V, Haidarliu S, Ahissar E. A neuronal analogue of state-dependent learning. Nature. 2000;403:549–553. doi: 10.1038/35000586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith HM, Jr, Basmajian JV, Vanderstoep SF. Inhibition of neighboring motoneurons in conscious control of single spinal motoneurons. Science. 1974;183:975–976. doi: 10.1126/science.183.4128.975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder LH, Batista AP, Andersen RA. Coding of intention in the posterior parietal cortex. Nature. 1997;386:167–170. doi: 10.1038/386167a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson IH, Cherian A, London BM, Sachs NA, Lindberg E, Reimer J, Slutzky MW, Hatsopoulos NG, Miller LE, Kording KP. Statistical assessment of the stability of neural movement representations. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:764–774. doi: 10.1152/jn.00626.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor DM, Tillery SI, Schwartz AB. Direct cortical control of 3D neuroprosthetic devices. Science. 2002;296:1829–1832. doi: 10.1126/science.1070291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Velliste M, Perel S, Spalding MC, Whitford AS, Schwartz AB. Cortical control of a prosthetic arm for self-feeding. Nature. 2008;453:1098–1101. doi: 10.1038/nature06996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woody CD, Gruen E, Birt D. Changes in membrane currents during Pavlovian conditioning of single cortical neurons. Brain Res. 1991;539:76–84. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90688-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yasumoto S, Tanaka E, Hattori G, Maeda H, Higashi H. Direct and indirect actions of dopamine on the membrane potential in medium spiny neurons of the mouse neostriatum. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1234–1243. doi: 10.1152/jn.00514.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]