Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Child Dev. 2019 Jan 10;91(3):754–768. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13213

Role Fulfillment Mediates the Association Between Daily Family Assistance and Cortisol Awakening Response in Adolescents

Emma Armstrong-Carter a,b, Susannah Ivory a,c, Lynda C Lin a,d, Keely A Muscatell a,e, Eva Telzer a
PMCID: PMC6620160  NIHMSID: NIHMS1003592  PMID: 30629290

Abstract

Family assistance (helping the family), is associated with both positive and negative psychological and biological outcomes during adolescence. However, the association between family assistance on the HPA axis remains unstudied. Thus, we assess how helping the family relates to adolescents’ diurnal cortisol, an index of HPA activity, and psychological outcomes. 370 ethnically diverse adolescents (ages 11–18) reported daily helping behaviors and psychological experiences for 14 days and provided 4 saliva samples per day for 4 days. Multi-level modeling revealed that cortisol awakening response was lower the day after adolescents helped their families more. This association was explained, in part, by perceived role fulfillment (feeling like a good son, daughter and sibling). Results highlight a possible psychological and biological benefit of assisting the family during adolescence.

Keywords: Adolescence, family assistance, cortisol, role fulfillment, HPA axis, daily diary


Adolescents provide significant assistance to their family members, including helping around the house, taking care of siblings, and running errands for their parents. Prior research and theory regarding youths’ help in the household suggest that it can be both detrimental and beneficial for their mental and physical health (Telzer, Gonzales & Fuligni, 2014; Fuligni & Telzer, 2013). In particular, when adolescents’ assistance to their family provides them with a sense of role fulfillment, or feeling like a good son, daughter or sibling, family assistance tends to be associated with positive psychological and biological outcomes at both the level of behavior and brain (Telzer, Masten, Berkman, Lieberman & Fuligni, 2010). Although understanding how family assistance “gets under the skin” has recently been of interest (Fuligni & Telzer, 2013), the impact of family assistance on adolescents’ HPA axis function, an important indicator of physical health (Chida & Steptoe, 2009) remains unknown. Thus, in the current study, we utilize daily diary methods to examine whether adolescents’ daily family assistance behaviors predict variations in diurnal patterns of cortisol secretion, and whether those variations are explained by adolescents’ daily psychological experiences.

Family Assistance

Although helping behaviors may be more common among some sociocultural groups than others, providing some help to the family is a typical feature of children and adolescents’ daily lives around the world (Weisner, 2001). Family assistance can take many forms, including cleaning, cooking, helping siblings with homework, and providing emotional support to a sibling or parent. Family assistance during the teenage years impacts adolescents in divergent ways, as it is associated with both positive and negative biological and psychological outcomes (Aldridge & Becker, 1993; Telzer, Gonzales & Fuligni, 2014; Fuligni, Telzer, Bower, Irwin, Kiang & Cole, 2009; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a). For example, contributing to the family can help maintain family connectedness for adolescents (Beach, 1997), increasing their happiness (Weisner, 2001; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b) but it can also be burdensome, particularly if it conflicts with other social and academic demands (Cohen, Greene, Toyinbo & Siskowski, 2012; Siskowski, 2006; Bridgeland, DiIulio, & Morison, 2006; East, 2010; Suarez-Orozco & Suárez-Orozco, 1995). Research has also chronicled associations between family assistance and a number of biological processes and outcomes during adolescence, indicating that more frequent or intense family assistance is associated with higher BMI (Fuligni et al., 2009), higher risk for substance use (Telzer et al., 2014), and elevated circulating levels of the inflammatory markers C-reactive protein and interleukin-6 two years later (Fuligni et al., 2009). This latter effect is buffered among adolescents reporting greater role fulfillment (Fuligni et al., 2009), suggesting that finding meaning and purpose from helping the family can be protective, benefiting adolescents even when it also feels burdensome (Fuligni & Telzer, 2013, Fuligni et al., 2009). While role fulfillment may moderate the associations between family assistance and chronic long-term outcomes, research looking at daily level outcomes finds that role fulfillment also mediates or partially explains the link between family assistance and daily happiness (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b). This suggests that role fulfillment may be a key mechanism through which family assistance relates to child proximal daily outcomes.

HPA Axis Activity

Despite growing interest in examining how family assistance predicts biological and psychological outcomes, no known studies have examined the link between family assistance and the functioning of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis. The HPA axis is an important biological mechanism linking psychological stress and physical health conditions (McEwen, 1998). One indicator of HPA axis output measured in human research is salivary cortisol. Cortisol is a hormone that regulates a number of important bodily functions, including those involved with the metabolic, cardiovascular and immune systems (Chida & Steptoe, 2009). Cortisol levels fluctuate naturally throughout the day, typically peaking 30–45 minutes after waking in the morning and subsequently declining from afternoon to evening (Stalder et al., 2016; Gunnar, Doom & Esposito, 2015). Despite this normative pattern, individuals vary in their daily cortisol patterns (Adam & Gunnar 2001; Adam, 2006) and social and psychological experiences have been shown to impact its release and daily fluctuations (Adam, Quinn, Tavernier, McQuillan, Dahlke, & Gilbert, 2017).

Three indices of HPA functioning are typically examined in psychological research. The first indicator, diurnal slope, represents the decrease in secreted cortisol from morning to evening. Deviations from the typical diurnal slope, indicated by a flatter decline over the day, have been associated with greater risk for physical illness (Adam, Doane, Zinbarg, Mineka, Craske, & Griffith, 2010). The second indicator, area under the curve (AUC) is a measure of total daily cortisol output (Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid & Hellhammer, 2003) and is positively associated with chronic stress (Miller, Chen & Zhou 2009). Both AUC and diurnal slope are impacted by events occurring throughout the course of the day and are typically examined the same day as experiences of interest (Chida & Steptoe, 2009; Adam et al., 2006). Finally, cortisol awakening response (CAR), is the steep increase in cortisol from wake-up to 30–45 minutes after awakening (Stalder et al., 2016). CAR is considered an indication of the body mobilizing energy to meet the anticipated demands of the upcoming day (Fries, Dettenborn, & Kirschbaum, 2009). Since CAR occurs shortly after waking and before the events of the day, it is impacted largely by events the previous day, and thus is commonly and most effectively examined the day following events or experiences of interest (Chida & Steptoe, 2009; Adam, Hawkley, Kudielka, & Cacioppo, 2006). Thus, CAR reflects both an anticipatory stress response based on anticipated demands of the upcoming day and also a reflection of prior-day life events. Deviations from typical CAR can be either amplified (i.e., a very large increase in cortisol upon wake-up), which is associated with depression and chronic life stress (Chida & Steptoe, 2009) or blunted (i.e., a small increase upon wake-up), which is associated with fatigue, burnout and posttraumatic stress (Chida & Steptoe, 2009; Boggero, Hostinar, Haak, Murphy, & Segerstrom, 2017). Since moderate levels of CAR are associated with optimal functioning and outcomes, one way to illuminate whether reductions in CAR are negative (i.e. blunting to an unhealthy extent) or positive (i.e. lowering to normative, healthy levels), is to examine potential psychological mediators that shed light on the quality of the adolescent’s daily experience.

Psychological and social experiences influence HPA functioning and the biological rhythm of cortisol, likely by prompting signals from the limbic system to the hypothalamus and pituitary gland, which trigger cortisol release from the adrenal cortex (Adam, Hawkley, Kudielka, & Cacioppo, 2006). Cortisol levels in the body are then regulated by feedback to and from multiple brain regions including the pituitary, hypothalamus, and hippocampus (Adam et a., 2006). Operating through these mechanisms, family assistance may impact HPA axis activity in divergent ways. On the one hand, if assisting the family is taxing for adolescents, it may affect the HPA axis output much like a chronic stressor. For example, family assistance may predict higher AUC, blunted or heightened CAR, or flatter diurnal slopes, all of which are associated with increased risk for poor psychological and physical health (Adam et al., 2006; Adam et al., 2010; Boggoro et al., 2017; Ruttle, Javaras, Klein, Armstrong, Burk, & Essex, 2013; Williams, Hayman, Daniels, Robinson, Steinberger, Paridon, & Bazzarre, 2002). Supporting this idea, past research has shown that family demands (i.e., tasks at home that are perceived as demanding or stressful) are associated with stress and blunted CAR, but only when those demands also interfere with adolescents’ sleep (Chiang, Tsai, Park, Bower, Almeida, Dahl, & Fuligni, 2016). On the other hand, if family assistance provides emotional and social benefits to adolescents (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b), it may be associated with a healthier cortisol profile (e.g., lower AUC or lowered CAR). As such, family assistance may provide a sense of meaning and purpose for adolescents (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b), enabling the HPA axis to optimally adapt and cope with stressors. Thus, previous research and theory suggest that family assistance could be related to either adaptive or maladaptive cortisol patterns, and this likely varies based on whether family assistance is experienced as demanding and burdensome, or if instead it provides adolescents with a sense of happiness, meaning and role fulfillment.

Role Fulfillment as a Possible Psychological Mechanism Linking Family Assistance and HPA Axis Activity

Understanding the psychological mechanisms by which family assistance relates to cortisol profiles may help to elucidate if such associations are likely to contribute to detrimental or beneficial effects on current and future health. For example, if a positive psychological experience such as happiness or role fulfillment mediates the link between family assistance and cortisol outcomes, this would suggest that family assistance is linked with a more adaptive HPA-axis function and a lower-risk profile. Conversely, if negative affect states such as distress or burnout (i.e., stress) mediate the link, this would suggest that family assistance is associated with maladaptive HPA-axis function and a higher-risk profile. Prior research has examined the psychological mediators of the link between adolescents’ daily family assistance and adjustment in this way, highlighting role fulfillment rather than distress as a mediator (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a). This work suggests that adolescent’s role fulfillment, or derived meaning from helping their families, is part of the reason that family assistance may be associated with positive outcomes. Despite this intriguing prior research, no known studies have examined if positive psychological processes (e.g., role fulfillment, happiness) or negative psychological processes (e.g., distress, burnout) are mediators of the link between family assistance and cortisol profiles.

The Present Study

The present study used a daily diary method to examine whether family assistance is associated with diurnal cortisol patterns in a culturally diverse sample of adolescents. Specifically, we sought to answer three key questions: (1) How does family assistance relate to cortisol patterns, as indicated by cortisol awakening response (CAR), area under the curve (AUC) and diurnal slope? (2) Is family assistance related to adolescents’ psychological experiences, as indicated by daily reports of distress, burnout, happiness and role fulfillment? We separately assessed both positive (i.e., happiness and role fulfillment) and negative (i.e., distress and burnout) psychological experiences to allow for the possibility that both may simultaneously be related to family assistance. (3) Are any observed links between family assistance and cortisol patterns mediated by a sense of role fulfillment, happiness, distress or burnout? Finally, we conducted exploratory analyses to examine whether the association between family assistance and cortisol patterns varies by race, age, sex, or socio-economic status (SES; operationalized as maternal education). Past research is inconsistent, suggesting that demographic characteristics may moderate associations between family assistance and adolescent functioning (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a Telzer et al., 2010; Weisner, 2001), but also that family assistance operates similarly across groups of adolescents (Fuligni et al., 2009; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b).

The Daily Diary Method

The daily diary method is a uniquely useful method for examining questions related to adolescents’ daily family assistance behaviors and psychological experiences (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b), particularly related to cortisol, which has a unique diurnal pattern that varies day-by-day. Youth reports of their daily activities, behaviors, and feelings are more reliable and accurate than when these processes are assessed using traditional retrospective accounts from a single questionnaire (Bolger, Davis, & Rafaeli, 2003). Daily diary methods also allow researchers to examine whether specific events, behaviors, and feelings that occur on one day are associated with psychological and biological processes measured the same day or the following day. For example, do adolescents exhibit lower CAR following days that they help their family more? While not causal, data of this nature allows us to gain more confidence in the likely directionality of effects. In addition, we are able to examine individual and group differences in the associations between daily-level processes. For example, is the association between family assistance and CAR the same for male and female youth? In contrast to our method, prior literature that examines links between social experiences and HPA axis functioning has averaged cortisol across days, which precludes the ability to examine diurnal variations and day-to-day associations with familial and psychological experiences (e.g Adam et al., 2010; DeSantis, Adam, Doane, Mineka, 2007). More recent studies emphasize the importance of examining cortisol day-by-day to capture how it varies in response to daily activities, experiences and behaviors (Myin-Germeys, Oorschot, Collip, Lataster, Delespaul, & Van, 2009; Sladek, & Doane, 2015; Deer, Shields, Ivory, Hostinar, & Telzer, in press). Accordingly, the current study capitalized on daily diary methods to investigate whether adolescents’ daily family assistance predicts their diurnal cortisol, and whether that is explained by their daily psychological experiences.

Methods

Participants

Participants included 370 adolescents (57.3% female) between the ages of 11 and 18 years (M age = 14.63 years, SD = 1.39 years). The sample was racially and ethnically diverse: 39.46% non-Hispanic White (from here on referred to as White, N = 146), 25.4% Asian (N = 94, 11 of whom were mixed [i.e., Asian and White], 17.8% Hispanic (N = 66, 11 of whom were mixed [i.e., Hispanic and White]), 10.8% African American (N = 40, 9 of whom were mixed [i.e., African American and White]), and 6.5% other race (N = 24). The sample was also socioeconomically diverse. Using maternal education as an indicator of socioeconomic status, approximately 10% of mothers had less than an 8th grade education, 13% did not complete high school, 24% completed high school, 27% completed post-secondary education (college, trade, or vocational school), and 23% completed graduate school (3% declined to answer). Participants were recruited from the community using convenience sampling, including posting flyers at schools, posting on listservs serving ethnic minority families, recruiting participants from previous studies who agreed to be contacted for other research studies, and word of mouth. Participants were compensated $10 in total for completing the daily diaries and $10 for completing the saliva samples. In addition, adolescents were told that they would receive a $20 bonus if inspection of the data indicated that they had completed all the diaries and saliva samples correctly and on time. All participants provided written consent or assent and all procedures were approved by the sponsoring institution’s Committee on Human Subjects.

Procedure

Participants were provided with 14 days of diary checklists and a saliva collection kit to complete on days 2 through 5 of the diaries. These days included both weekdays and weekends. The order of days differs between participants depending on the day of the week that they started, however all participants had the same proportion of weekday to weekend data if they completed all of the diaries (completion rate 93.8%, with 345 adolescents completing all diaries across all four days of saliva collection). Participants were given the choice of completing the diaries with paper and pencil or via a secure website. In order to verify the time that checklists were completed, participants who chose to respond with paper and pencil responses were given 14 manila envelopes and an electronic time stamper (Dymo Corporation, Stamford, CT). The time stamper is a small device that imprints the current date and time and is programmed with a security code so that the correct date and time cannot be changed. Participants were instructed to place their completed checklists into a sealed envelope each night and to stamp the seal of the envelope with the time stamper. Participants who completed surveys online were sent an email with the link to each daily diary survey, and the time and date of completion were recorded via the website. The daily diary checklists were 3 pages long and each took approximately 5–10 min to complete.

Measures

Diurnal cortisol.

Adolescents provided saliva samples via passive drool method on days 2 through 5 of the daily diaries. These days were selected in order to ensure consistency across participants, and to minimize error by completing samples as soon as possible after the initial instructions. As with the diaries, saliva sample days included both weekdays and weekends, depending on the day of the week that the participant started their diaries. On saliva sample days, participants provided saliva at 4 time-points each day for a total of 16 samples: (1) immediately upon wake up, (2) 30 min after waking up, (3) 5 pm (or before dinner), and (4) 8 pm (or before bed). Participants were advised to take their samples before or no less than 30 minutes after brushing their teeth, drinking, eating, or using tobacco. Participants recorded the timing of each sample using a log-card and stamped with an electronic time stamper (Dymo Corporation, Stamford, CT), which printed the current date and time and was programmed with a security code so that participants could not change the accurate and correct date and time. Participants stamped the card beside the heading for each sample and immediately placed the sample in their fridge. At the end of the saliva collection days, participants or research staff brought the samples to the research laboratory, where samples were stored in a −80ºC freezer. At the end of the data collection period, the samples were shipped to the Laboratory of Biological Psychology at the Technical University of Dresden, Germany where they were assayed in duplicate using high-sensitivity chemiluminescence-immunoassays (IBL International, Hamburg, Germany). The inter-assay coefficient of variation (CV) was less than 8%.

We computed diurnal slope, CAR, and AUC using standard formulas. Specifically, diurnal slopes were computed as the difference between the fourth (bedtime) cortisol sample and the first morning sample, divided by the time elapsed between these two samples (Pruessner et al., 2003). We computed the cortisol awakening response as the increase in cortisol from wake to 30 min post-wake (Pruessner et al., 2003). For samples one and two, which were used to compute cortisol awakening response, 15– 45 minutes between the two samples was considered to be on time (Adam & Kumari, 2009). According to this metric, 98% of samples were collected on time. Seven samples were collected more than 45 minutes apart. Four additional samples were not collected in the morning, but upon waking in the afternoon. The total cortisol area under the curve (AUC) was computed using the trapezoid method (Pruessner et al., 2003). Cortisol values exceeding 60 nmol/l were flagged as outliers and excluded from analyses.

Family assistance.

Daily family assistance data from the four days that salivary cortisol was collected (and one day prior for CAR analyses) were used for our analyses in order to best capture potential associations between daily family assistance and diurnal cortisol. Daily family assistance was measured by a set of questions on the daily checklist that asked participants to indicate whether they did any of the following activities to help their family each day: helped to clean your apartment or house, took care of your brothers or sisters, ran an errand for your parents or family, helped your brothers or sisters with their schoolwork, helped to cook a meal for your family, provided emotional support to your family, (i.e. listened, gave advice, comforted). The list of activities was derived from focus group studies of adolescents and has been used successfully in previous studies with these populations (Fuligni et al., 2009; Telzer & Fuligni 2009a; Hardway & Fuligni, 2006). A Family Assistance variable was created by summing the number activities that the adolescent performed each day, ranging from 0–7.

Daily psychological experience.

Daily psychological experience was assessed with items on the daily checklist that were drawn from the Profile of Mood States (McNair, Lorr, & Droppleman, 1971). Adolescents used a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely) to indicate the extent to which they felt a number of emotions each day. A Distress variable was calculated from nine items (sad, hopeless, discouraged, on edge, unable to concentrate, uneasy, nervous, stressed, worried), overall Cronbach’s α = .92, daily α = .858- .942. A Happiness variable was calculated from two items (joyful and happy), overall α = .80, daily α = .740-.836. A Burnout variable was calculated from three items (fatigue, exhausted, and worn-out), overall α = .84, daily α = .803-.881. A Role Fulfillment variable was calculated from two additional items in the daily diary in which participants responded on a seven-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Not at all) to 7 (Extremely) to report the extent to which they felt like “a good son or daughter” and “a good brother or sister” that day. Role fulfillment was calculated as the mean of the available data from these two items, overall α = .80, daily α = .664-.856.

Data Analysis

All analyses performed were linear mixed effects models that nested days (level 1) within participants (level 2). Fixed effects were tested at the level of participants (i.e., level 2). This statistical approach accounts for dependency within participants and introduces less bias due to missing data compared to traditional statistical analyses, such as repeated measures ANOVA (Finch, Bolin & Kelley 2014; Bryk & Raudenbush 1992). To manage missing data, we used listwise deletion for person-day elimination. All analyses were conducted using Statistical Analysis Software (SAS, Version 9.4).

To examine whether family assistance relates to cortisol, we conducted linear mixed effects models that included family assistance as a level 1 predictor of cortisol parameters (diurnal slope, CAR, AUC), each examined independently in separate analyses. To examine whether family assistance relates to daily psychological experience, we conducted linear mixed effects models that included family assistance as a level 1 predictor of four variables of daily psychological experience: distress, happiness, burnout and role fulfillment. To examine whether daily psychological experiences explain the link between family assistance and cortisol outcomes, we modeled CAR as a function of family assistance and role fulfillment in a linear mixed mediation model. This allowed us to test whether family assistance-related differences in CAR persist after accounting for role fulfillment, when controlling for race, sex and age. We conducted a mediation of lower level effect (1 1 1 mediation), which is recommended for multi-level data (Bauer, Preacher, & Gil, 2006). We used Soble’s test to determine the indirect effect.

Age, sex, and race or ethnicity were included as covariates in all analyses, given that preliminary analyses indicated that each was related to cortisol, family assistance or both (see Table 1). Sex was coded Male = 0 and Female = 1. Race was dummy coded within each race (i.e., African American = 1, not African American = 0). Age was entered as numeric age. Because SES (as measured by maternal education) was not significantly related to family assistance (p = .33) or cortisol outcomes (ps = .3-.7), we did not include it as a covariate in analyses.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for primary constructs, overall and by sample demographics.

M(SD)
r
Overall Female Male White African American Asian Hispanic Mixed/Other Age
AUC 160.28 (88.14) 165.94 (97.70)1 152.51 (72.35)0 167.49 (89.59)B 155.16 (99.48)B 169.08 (70.92)B 114.74 (83.11)C 193.22 (94.04)A −0.20***
CAR 4.68 (13.74) 5.10 (14.79)0 4.13 (12.20)0 5.81 (13.93)A 5.82 (12.27)A 3.79 (15.25)A,B 1.35 (9.23)B 6.50 (15.21)A −0.07*
Cortisol Slope −1.27 (1.04) −1.30 (1.14)0 −1.22 (0.85)0 −1.39 (1.04)C −1.07 (1.10)A,B −1.36 (0.81)C −0.91 (1.31)A −1.25 (0.93)C,B 0.18***
Family Assistance 1.63 (1.45) 1.86 (1.52)1 1.30 (1.28)0 1.82 (1.41)B 1.77 (1.34)B 1.24 (1.38)D 1.49 (1.32)C 2.20 (1.82)A 0.08***
Role Fulfillment 4.91 (1.34) 4.94 (1.35)0 4.87 (1.32)0 5.07 (1.32)A 5.07 (1.45)A 4.61 (1.23)B 4.96 (1.44)A 5.03 (1.36)A −0.01
Distress 1.64 (0.78) 1.72 (0.86)1 1.53 (0.65)0 1.66 (0.77)A 1.70 (0.84)A 1.60 (0.77)B 1.31 (0.56)C 1.65 (0.90)A,B 0.09***
Burnout 2.09 (1.05) 2.19 (1.11)1 1.94 (0.94)0 2.19 (1.06)B 2.36 (1.13)A 1.90 (0.98)C 1.89 (0.97)C 2.13 (1.07)B 0.12***
Happiness 3.27 (1.07) 3.27 (1.08)0 3.28 (1.06)0 3.43 (1.04)A 3.14 (1.24)B 3.11 (0.99)B 3.39 (1.07)A 3.14 (1.19)B −0.05**

Note. For race, means with the same letter are not significantly different. For sex, means with the same number are not significantly different. For age correlations,

*

p < .05,

**

p <.01,

***

p <.001

Results

Sample Characteristics

Table 1 displays sample characteristics for primary constructs, overall and by sample demographics. Table 2 displays bivariate correlations among the major constructs of interest, which represent the averages across the diary days. As indicated in Table 2, cortisol indices showed expected correlations with each other, and daily psychological experience variables also showed expected correlations with each other. The only cortisol outcome showing significant associations with family assistance was the next day CAR, which was inversely correlated with family assistance.

Table 2.

Partial correlations among major constructs controlling for sex, age and race.

AUC CAR Slope Family
Assistance
Role
Fulfillment
Distress Burnout
CAR −0.01
Slope −0.45*** 0.07*
Family Assistance 0.00 −0.07** −0.05
Role Fulfillment 0.00 −0.08** −0.02 0.25***
Distress 0.01 0.04 0.05 0.03 −0.30***
Burnout 0.02 −0.01 0.07* 0.04* −0.16*** 0.58***
Happiness −0.03 −0.01 0.03 0.18*** 0.46*** −0.25*** −0.11***

Note.

*

p < .05,

**

p <.01,

***

p <.001

Family Assistance and Cortisol

To examine the association between family assistance and diurnal cortisol, we modeled the daily level association between cortisol outcomes (diurnal slope, CAR, AUC) as a function of family assistance in three separate models. Each outcome on a particular day for a particular adolescent was modeled as a function of the average of that outcome of the adolescent across days and their daily family assistance, controlling for their race, age and sex. A random intercept was included for each subject.

Results indicate that daily family assistance was significantly associated with CAR, such that on days when adolescents assisted their families more, they had lower CAR the next day compared to days when they assisted their families less (see Table 3a). There was no association between family assistance on AUC or diurnal slope (ps > .11).

Table 3a:

Daily level associations between adolescents’ family assistance and cortisol outcomes.

CAR B (SD) AUC B (SE) Diurnal Slope B (SE)
Intercept 11.66 (5.71)* 314.24 (40.47)*** −3.19 (0.43)***
Sex 1.65 (0.98) 15.95 (6.96)* −0.10 (0.07)
Age −0.34 (0.39) −10.41 (2.80)*** 0.13 (0.03)***
Race (Reference=White)
  African American −0.26 (1.55) −7.56 (11.21) 0.31 (0.12)**
  Asian −2.65 (1.21)* −2.88 (8.61) 0.04 (0.09)
  Hispanic −4.07 (1.56)** −35.58 (11.19)** 0.22 (0.12)
Other/Mixed 2.74 (2.38) 6.36 (16.77) 0.10 (0.18)
Family Assistance −0.99 (0.32)** −2.30 (2.08) −0.04 (0.02)

Note.

*

p < .05,

**

p <.01,

***

p <.001. Sex was coded Male=0 and Female=1. Race was dummy coded within each race (i.e., African American=1, not African American=0).

Family Assistance and Daily Psychological Experience

To examine associations between family assistance and daily psychological experience, we next modeled happiness, burnout, distress and role fulfillment as a function of family assistance, in a linear mixed model, again controlling for race, age and sex. We used a similar model as with our first aim but replaced the cortisol outcomes with each daily psychological experience.

Overall, participants reported fewer negative and more positive psychological outcomes on days that they assisted their families more (see Table 3b). Specifically, family assistance was negatively associated with distress and burnout, such that on days that participants helped their families more, they reported feeling less distress and less burnout compared to days when they helped their families less. Conversely, family assistance was positively association with happiness and role fulfillment, such that on days that participants helped their families more, they reported more happiness and role fulfillment compared to days when they helped their families less. Overall, these results suggest that daily family assistance is associated with decreased negative psychological outcomes and increased positive psychological outcomes.

Table 3b:

Daily level associations between adolescents’ family assistance and psychological outcomes.

Role Fulfillment B (SE) Happiness B (SE) Distress B (SE) Burnout B (SE)
Intercept 5.59 (0.70)*** 4.35 (0.51)*** 0.74 (0.42) 0.72 (0.51)
Sex −0.04 (0.12) −0.08 (0.09) 0.20 (0.07)** 0.21 (0.09)*
Age −0.05 (0.05) −0.07 (0.04) 0.06 (0.03)* 0.10 (0.04)**
Race (Reference=White)
 African American 0.07 (0.20) −0.30 (0.15)* −0.03 (0.12) 0.12 (0.14)
 Asian −0.37 (0.15)* −0.32 (0.11)** −0.04 (0.09) −0.24 (0.11)*
 Hispanic 0.08 (0.19) 0.17 (0.14) −0.46 (0.29) −0.40 (0.14)**
 Other/Mixed −0.23 (0.24) −0.34 (0.18) −0.04 (0.14) −0.15 (0.17)
Family Assistance 0.12 (0.01)*** 0.05 (0.01)*** −0.04 (0.01)*** −0.03 (0.01)**

Note.

*

p < .05,

**

p <.01,

***

p <.001. Sex was coded Male=0 and Female=1. Race was dummy coded within each race (i.e., African American=1, not African American=0).

Daily Psychological Experience and Cortisol

To examine associations between daily psychological experience and cortisol, we next modeled each cortisol outcome (diurnal slope, CAR, AUC,) as a function of each psychological variable (distress, burnout, happiness and role fulfillment) in a mixed linear model, again controlling for age, sex and race. We used a similar model as in our first and second aims but replaced the independent and dependent variables.

We first modeled CAR as a function of each psychological variable. Role fulfillment was negatively associated with CAR, such that when participants experienced more role fulfillment, they had lower CAR the next day compared to days when they experienced less role fulfilment (Table 3c). Burnout was marginally negatively associated with CAR, and distress and happiness were not related to next day CAR (p > .726).

Table 3c:

Daily level associations between adolescents’ CAR and psychological outcomes.

Role Fulfillment B (SD) Happiness B (SD) Distress B (SD) Burnout B (SD)
Intercept 5.50 (0.72)*** 4.11 (0.55)*** 0.97 (0.43)* 0.85 (0.55)
Sex 0.03 (0.12) −0.11 (0.09) 0.18 (0.08)* 0.23 (0.09)*
Age −0.04 (0.05) −0.04 (0.04) 0.04 (0.03) 0.09 (0.04)*
Race (Reference=White)
 African American −0.02 (0.20) −0.40 (0.15)** 0.03 (0.11) 0.17 (0.15)
 Asian −0.29 (0.15) −0.28 (0.12)* 0.00 (0.09) −0.15 (0.12)
 Hispanic 0.08 (0.20) −0.00 (0.15) −0.40 (0.27) −0.42 (0.15)**
 Other/Mixed −0.10 (0.30) −0.38 (0.23) 0.06 (0.18) −0.06 (0.23)
CAR −0.89 (0.34)* −0.047 (0.41) 0.23 (0.66) −0.75 (0.41)

Note.

*

p < .05,

**

p <.01,

***

p <.001. Sex was coded Male=0 and Female=1. Race was dummy coded within each race (i.e., African American=1, not African American=0).

We also modeled diurnal slope and AUC each as a function of each psychological variable. Burnout was positively associated with diurnal slope, (Β = 0.06, SE = 0.03, p = .042), whereas distress, happiness, and role fulfillment were not related to diurnal slope (p > .122). Distress was marginally and positively associated with AUC (Β = 7.00, SE = 4.10, p = .089), whereas burnout, happiness, and role fulfillment were not related to AUC (p > .167).

Does Role Fulfillment Explain Differences in Cortisol Awakening Response?

Since daily family assistance was negatively associated with next day CAR, we examined potential psychological mechanisms that could explain this association and unpack whether this cortisol response likely represents a low-risk or high-risk profile. Of our psychological variables of interest, only role fulfillment was related to next day CAR and family assistance and so was a potential mediator. Mediation analysis indicated that the total effect of family assistance on CAR was significant (estimate = −0.932, p = .002, 95% CI [−0.148, −0.155]). The effect of family assistance on role fulfillment was also significant (estimate = 0.156, p <.001, 95% CI [0.103, 0.210]), as was the effect of role fulfillment on CAR (estimate = −0.730, p = .032, 95% CI [−1.398, −0.062]). The direct effect of family assistance on CAR was also significant (estimate = −0.818, p = .015, 95% CI [−1.534, −0.329]). Finally, the mediation effect of role fulfillment was significant (estimate = −0.114, p = .048, 95% CI [−0.227, −0.001]). These results suggest that role fulfillment explains, in part, the association between family assistance and CAR.

We reran all models with instrumental support (6 items) and emotional support (1 item) separately as predictors, given previous work suggesting that instrumental support is linked with differential outcomes compared to emotional support (Tsai, Gonzales & Fuligni, 2016). The results remained the same. We also reran all models excluding participants who were taking steroid medications (N = 4) and the results remained the same.

Exploratory Analysis with Demographics

Finally, we conducted exploratory analyses to examine whether the associations between family assistance and outcomes varies by race, age sex, and SES. We modeled CAR and psychological outcomes (i.e., role fulfillment, happiness, distress and burnout) each as a function of family assistance with race, age sex, and SES entered as cross-level interactions (see Table 4). Male and White youth served as the reference group. The association between family assistance on CAR did not vary by age, sex, or SES. We did find significant differences for Hispanic and other and mixed race youth, who differed from White youth. For both groups, their CAR was higher than White youth following days when they assisted their family more. See Table 4 for cross-level interaction for family assistance on role fulfillment, happiness, distress, and burnout.

Table 4:

Cross level interactions in the association between family assistance and outcomes.

CAR B (SD) Role Fulfillment B (SD) Happiness B (SD) Distress B (SD) Burnout B (SD)
Individual Level
Intercept 9.47 (5.87) 5.59 (0.70)*** 4.35 (0.52)*** 0.72 (0.43) 0.76 (0.52)
  Sex 1.32 (1.01) −0.04 (0.12) −0.08 (0.09) 0.18 (0.08)* 0.19 (0.09)*
  Age −0.27 (0.40) −0.05 (0.05) −0.07 (0.04) 0.07 (0.03)* 0.10 (0.04)**
  SES 0.05 (0.32) 0.04 (0.04) 0.02 (0.03) −0.03 (0.02) −0.02 (0.03)
 Race (Reference=White)
  African American −0.21 (1.61) 0.08 (0.20) −0.25 (0.14) −0.08 (0.12) 0.01 (0.14)
  Asian −2.63 (1.31)* −0.39 (0.16)* −0.34 (0.11)** 0.00 (0.09) −0.21 (0.11)
  Hispanic −3.92 (1.80)* 0.12 (0.21) 0.18 (0.16) −0.25 (0.26) −0.46 (0.16)**
  Other/Mixed 1.44 (2.47) −0.16 (0.29) −0.35 (0.22) 0.02 (0.18) −0.08 (0.22)
Family Assistance −1.08 (0.67) 0.16 (0.03)*** 0.06 (0.02)* −0.04 (0.02)* −0.02 (0.02)
  Sex −1.02 (0.70) −0.04 (0.03) 0.01 (0.03) −0.05 (0.02)* −0.06 (0.03)*
  Age −0.19 (0.28) −0.01 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01) −0.00 (0.01)
  SES 0.40 (0.22) 0.02 (0.01)* 0.01 (0.01) −0.01 (0.01)* −0.02 (0.01)*
 Race (Reference=White)
  African American 1.62 (1.09) −0.03 (0.04) −0.03 (0.04) 0.08 (0.03)** 0.05 (0.04)
  Asian 0.78 (0.87) −0.01 (0.04) −0.02 (0.03) 0.03 (0.02) 0.02 (0.03)
  Hispanic 2.48 (1.24)* 0.08 (0.08) 0.03 (0.06) 0.13 (0.08) 0.15 (0.06)*
  Other/Mixed 2.91 (1.36)* −0.05 (0.06) −0.05 (0.05) 0.04 (0.04) −0.04 (0.05)

Note.

*

p < .05,

**

p <.01,

***

p <.001. Sex was coded Male=0 and Female=1. Race was dummy coded within each race (i.e., African American=1, not African American=0).

Discussion

The present study examined family assistance, the act of providing instrumental and emotional support to one’s family, which is a significant yet understudied aspect of family interactions during adolescence. Results from the present study contribute to a growing body of research that has highlighted both positive and negative psychological, social and biological consequences of family assistance during adolescence; here, we provide the first known evidence of how helping the family during the teenage years relates to diurnal cortisol rhythm, an index of HPA activity, as well as psychological outcomes. Our results indicate that family assistance is associated with lower next day CAR, which is mediated by greater role fulfillment.

The primary goal of this study was to examine the association between family assistance and diurnal cortisol outcomes. Analyses indicated that greater family assistance predicts lower next day CAR, suggesting that providing more assistance to the family on one day is associated with lower cortisol reactivity upon waking the following day. Interestingly, daily family assistance was not related to diurnal slope or AUC. Why might family assistance be related to CAR and not to diurnal slope or AUC? Part of the explanation might be that CAR is relatively independent from cortisol output during the rest of the day and is only loosely associated with later diurnal cortisol (Schmidt-Reinwald, Pruessner, Hellhammer, Federenko, Rohleder, Schürmeyer, & Kirschbaum, 1999; Edwards, Clow, Evans, & Hucklebridge, 2001). As such, it may operate on different regulatory mechanisms compared to diurnal slope and AUC (Chida & Steptoe, 2009). Indeed, previous meta-analyses indicate that psychosocial experiences have unique and significant associations with CAR, suggesting that it may be particularly sensitive to psychosocial processes relative to other cortisol indicators (Chida & Steptoe, 2009). A second part of the explanation might be because of temporal delay, or daily experiences taking a day to translate into biological profiles to “get under the skin”. Prior literature suggests that CAR reflects both an anticipatory mobilization of the stress response system for the upcoming day, and also a reflection of prior-day life events (Chida & Steptoe, 2009; Adam, et al., 2006). These results provide further evidence of the association between CAR and previous day experiences, in contrast to other cortisol indices (Chida & Steptoe, 2009).

Understanding the psychological mechanisms by which family assistance relates to cortisol helps to elucidate if such associations are positive or negative, given that both blunted and heightened cortisol are related to negative outcomes (Chida & Steptoe, 2009). As such, we examined links between daily family assistance, positive and negative daily psychological experiences, and cortisol output. We found that adolescents reported less distress and burnout, and more happiness and role fulfillment, on days that they helped their families more. A reduction in distress and burnout and an increase in happiness and role fulfillment as a function of greater family assistance are meaningful outcomes in and of themselves, as they are key indices of psychological well-being (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b). Furthermore, these psychological experiences are important because they contribute to a variety of other positive outcomes in youth, ranging from school achievement and school behaviors (Quinn & Duckworth, 2007) to physical health, both in the short- and long-term (Mahon, Yarcheski & Yarcheski 2005; Trzesniewski, Donnellan, Moffitt, Robins, Poulton, & Caspi, 2006). These results are consistent with prior research documenting how helping the family is associated with meaningful psychological benefits for adolescents (Fuligni & Telzer, 2013; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b). Further, they are also consistent with work demonstrating that, generally, providing daily assistance to the family is not uniformly associated with stress (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b, Fuligni, Yip, & Tseng, 2002) but rather can contribute to feelings of happiness and role fulfillment (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b, Walsh, Shulman, Bar‐On, & Tsur 2006). Together, these results replicate and reiterate the beneficial psychological outcomes associated with assisting the family during adolescence.

Since moderate levels of CAR are associated with optimal functioning and outcomes, it was important to examine whether reductions in CAR were negative (i.e. blunting to an unhealthy degree) or positive (i.e. lowering to normal, healthy levels; Chida & Steptoe, 2009). We chose to examine role fulfillment as a mediator given previous evidence that role fulfillment mediates the association between family assistance and happiness on a daily level (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b). If the association between family assistance and CAR was mediated by distress or burnout (or other negative psychological constructs), then that would suggest that lower CAR is potentially signaling a maladaptive stress response. However, the association between family assistance and CAR was explained, in part, by a positive psychological experience (i.e., role fulfillment). This suggests that family assistance is associated with a positive reduction in CAR, rather than negative blunting. Given the inconsistency of prior literature in terms of distinguishing whether relatively lower or higher CAR responses are adaptive or maladaptive, we chose to utilize this approach of examining shared variance (i.e., with role fulfillment) to untangle the potential mechanism at play. However, we also acknowledge that this is somewhat exploratory and cannot definitively indicate that lower CAR is healthier. With this acknowledgment, our findings are consistent with prior research showing that role fulfillment, in conjunction with family assistance, is associated with positive health outcomes in youth. Prior research indicates for example that role fulfillment partly explains the link between family assistance and happiness on a daily level (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b) and adolescents who feel greater role fulfillment from helping their family show increased activity in reward-related brain regions when contributing to their family (Telzer, Fuligni, Lieberman, & Galván, 2013). Taken together, these findings highlight the importance of adolescents’ feelings of role fulfillment when helping the family for predicting well-being. The present findings build on existing literature by demonstrating that role fulfillment may also be an important mechanism linking adolescents’ daily family experience and HPA axis functioning.

Finally, we conducted exploratory analyses to see if the associations between family assistance and CAR varied by race, age, SES, and sex (Table 4). The association between family assistance and lower CAR did not vary by these variables, with the exception of the Hispanic and Other and Mixed race group, who showed a higher CAR response following days when they assisted their family more. Given the small sample and heterogeneity of the Mixed race youth, we do not interpret this further. The effect for the Hispanic youth is consistent with previous research suggesting that Hispanic youth value family obligation and engage in more family assistance compared to other groups (Telzer & Fuligni 2009b). Although family assistance may be valued more among Hispanic youth, it has also been linked to poorer school performance in Mexican compared to European youth, suggesting that it is a more demanding or stressful daily activity in Hispanic families, perhaps due to higher overall levels of family assistance behaviors (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a). Indeed, the current study also shows more burnout on days of greater family assistance among Hispanic youth compared to White youth. Future research should further examine how the association between family assistance and cortisol responses are moderated by race and ethnicity. Future research should also examine other contextual variables as potential moderators, family conflict, parental distress, or extreme economic hardship (Telzer, Tsai, Gonzales, & Fuligni, 2015; Tsai, Telzer, Gonzales, & Fuligni, 2013).

In understanding the implications of helping the family during adolescence, it is important to draw a distinction between normative levels of family assistance, such as those studied here, and intense family caregiving responsibilities. This study does not differentiate between family assistance that is perceived as excessively demanding versus that which is experienced positively, but future research should examine that distinction. Indeed, data suggest that over 1 million youth in the U.S. between ages 8 and 18 provide high levels of daily care for a parent, sibling or other relative in need of assistance at home (Hunt, Levine, & Naiditch, 2005), which is distinct from the moderate levels of family assistance that are the focus of the present study. This intensity of care provided by children and adolescents may be present more often in family contexts that are more difficult, for example characterized by parental illness or poverty (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a; Tsai, Gonzales, & Fuligni, 2016). Intense levels of family care during childhood and adolescence may be associated with more negative psychological outcomes (Siskowski, 2006; Shifren & Chong, 2012; Burton, 2007) compared to the family assistance studied here. Future research should establish a clear distinction between moderate levels of family assistance and intense caregiving and investigate potential differential impacts on psychological and physiological functioning.

Several limitations in the current study should be acknowledged. First, we were unfortunately unable to control for sleep, physical activity, smoking or alcohol, all of which could impact cortisol. We were also unable to control completely for medication use as we only collected this information from a small subset of participants. Future research should carefully control for these variables. Due to the rich nature of our daily diary and cortisol assessments, there was also a level of missing data. Days that adolescents did not provide daily dairy or cortisol data might represent some of their most difficult days, and thus we may be missing important days that were more stressful for adolescents. Further, there may be selection biases, as youth who did not participate in the study may come from the most difficult home contexts, for example those experiencing economic hardship, parental illness or absence. Thus, we may be missing an important subset of adolescents. In addition, our measure of family assistance was a count measure of the number of different family assistance activities adolescents completed, and it did not take into consideration the amount of time spent on each activity. As such, it is unclear from the current study how timing or intensity may affect the association between family assistance, cortisol and psychological experience. Future work should measure the timing and chronicity of family assistance to provide a more nuanced index of this type of activity. In addition, many other variables could impact if, how, and when adolescents help the family. For example, this study does not address whether family assistance is internally or externally motivated, although prior evidence suggests that family assistance increases with family need (Tsai et al., 2016). Unpacking the motivation and context of family assistance behaviors in adolescence can further disentangle the complex relation between helping behaviors and psychobiological responses. Future work should also continue to distinguish between emotional support and instrumental support (Tsai et al., 2016), and examine potentially different associations between these two types of family assistance and biological and psychological outcomes. Finally, although the present study utilizes daily diary methods that can account for individual differences and thus offer robust predictive power (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009b), the direction of causality between family assistance, cortisol awakening response, and role fulfillment cannot be determined in this study. Future experimental work could address this issue by randomly assigning adolescents to assist their families (or not) and examine the psychological and biological outcomes of such a manipulation.

Conclusions

The results of this study highlight a possible biological and psychological benefit of assisting the family during the teenage years. This is the first known study to examine how daily family assistance relates to daily variations in cortisol output, thus providing greater insight into how cortisol varies and fluctuates in connection with normative daily behaviors in adolescents’ lives. The strength of our findings lies in daily diary methodology and daily saliva sample collection, and in the diverse and relatively large sample of adolescents. This work builds on a growing body of literature chronicling positive associations with family assistance and role fulfillment, ranging from academic success (Telzer & Fuligni, 2009a) to a reduction in risk-taking behaviors (Telzer et al., 2013) and extends these findings to cortisol and the HPA axis, an important indicator of short-term and long-term physical and mental health, for the first time. Overall, our findings highlight how assisting the family may be a valuable activity for adolescents that contributes to a sense of role fulfillment, thus increasing their psychological and physiological well-being.

Acknowledgements:

This manuscript was prepared with support from the National Institutes of Health Grant R01DA039923 and National Science Foundation Grant SES 1459719 provided to EHT, the Department of Psychology at the University of Illinois, and the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. We greatly appreciate Chris Weissen and the Odum Institute for assistance with data analysis.

References

  1. Adam EK & Gunnar MR (2001). Relationship functioning and home and work demands predict individual differences in diurnal cortisol patterns in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26, 189–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adam EK (2006). Transactions among adolescent trait and state emotion and diurnal and momentary cortisol activity in naturalistic settings. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31(5), 664–679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Adam EK, Hawkley LC, Kudielka BM, & Cacioppo JT (2006). Day-to-day dynamics of experience-cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(45), 17058–17063. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605053103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Adam EK, & Kumari M (2009). Assessing salivary cortisol in large-scale, epidemiological research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(10), 1423–1436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Adam EK, Doane LD, Zinbarg RE, Mineka S, Craske MG, & Griffith JW (2010). Prospective prediction of major depressive disorder from cortisol awakening responses in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(6), 921–931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Adam EK, Quinn ME, Tavernier R, McQuillan MT, Dahlke KA, & Gilbert KE (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 83, 25–41. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Aldridge J, & Becker S (1993). Children who care: inside the world of young carers. Loughborough: Young Carers Research Group. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bauer DJ, Preacher KJ, & Gil KM (2006). Conceptualizing and testing random indirect effects and moderated mediation in multilevel models: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 11(2), 142–163. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Beach DL (1997). Family caregiving: The positive impact on adolescent relationships. The Gerontologist, 37(2), 233–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Boggero IA, Hostinar CE, Haak EA, Murphy MLM, & Segerstrom SC (2017). Psychosocial functioning and the cortisol awakening response: Meta-analysis, P-curve analysis, and evaluation of the evidential value in existing studies. Biological Psychology, 129, 207–230. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.08.058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bolger N, Davis A, & Rafaeli E (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 579–616. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bridgeland JM, DiIulio JJ Jr., & Morison KB (2006). The Silent Epidemic. Retrieved 9/22/18 from www.civicenterprises.net/pdfs/thesilentepidemic3–06.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bryk AS, & Raudenbush SW (1992). Hierarchical linear models for social and behavioral research: Applications and data analysis methods. [Google Scholar]
  14. Burton L (2007). Childhood adultification in economically disadvantaged families: A conceptual model. Family Relations, 56(4), 329–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2007.00463.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chiang JJ, Tsai KM, Park H, Bower JE, Almeida DM, Dahl RE, . . . Fuligni AJ (2016). Daily family stress and HPA axis functioning during adolescence: The moderating role of sleep. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 71, 43–53. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.05.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chida Y, & Steptoe A (2009). Cortisol awakening response and psychosocial factors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 80(3), 265–278. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cohen D, Greene JA, Toyinbo PA, & Siskowski CT (2012). Impact of family caregiving by youth on their psychological well-being: A latent trait analysis. The journal of behavioral health services & research, 39(3), 245–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Deer LK, Shields GS, Ivory SL, Hostinar, &Telzer (in press). Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. de Kloet ER (1991). Brain corticosteroid receptor balance and homeostatic control. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology., 12, 95–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. DeSantis AS, Adam EK, Doane LD, Mineka S, Zinbarg RE, & Craske MG (2007). Racial/Ethnic differences in cortisol diurnal rhythms in a community sample of adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41(1), 3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. East PL, & Weisner TS (2009). Mexican american adolescents’ family caregiving: Selection effects and longitudinal associations with adjustment. Family Relations, 58(5), 562–577. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2009.00575.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. East PL (2010). Children’s provision of family caregiving: Benefit or burden? Child Development Perspectives, 4(1), 55–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00118.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Finch WH, Bolin JE, & Kelley K (2014). Multilevel modeling using R. Baton Rouge: CRC Press. [Google Scholar]
  24. Fries E, Dettenborn L, & Kirschbaum C (2009). The cortisol awakening response (CAR): Facts and future directions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72(1), 67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fuligni AJ, Yip T, & Tseng V (2002). The impact of family obligation on the daily activities and psychological well-being of chinese american adolescents. Child Development, 73(1), 302–314. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fuligni AJ, Telzer EH, Bower J, Irwin MR, Kiang L, & Cole SW (2009). Daily family assistance and inflammation among adolescents from latin american and european backgrounds. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 23(6), 803–809. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.02.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fuligni AJ, & Telzer EH (2013). Another way family can get in the head and under the skin: The neurobiology of helping the family. Child Development Perspectives, 7(3), 138–142. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gunnar MR, Doom JR, & Esposito EA (2015). Psychoneuroendocrinology of stress: Normative development and individual differences. Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, 1–46. [Google Scholar]
  29. Hardway C, & Fuligni AJ (2006). Dimensions of family connectedness among adolescents with mexican, chinese, and european backgrounds. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1246–1258. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kavanaugh MS, Stamatopoulos V, Cohen D, & Zhang L (2016). Unacknowledged caregivers: A scoping review of research on caregiving youth in the United States. Adolescent Research Review, 1(1), 29–49. [Google Scholar]
  31. Mahon NE, Yarcheski A, & Yarcheski TJ (2005). Happiness as related to gender and health in early adolescents. Clinical Nursing Research, 14(2), 175–190. doi: 10.1177/1054773804271936 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. McEwen BS. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York academy of sciences 840, 33–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. McNair DM, Lorr M, & Droppleman LF (1971). Profile of mood states, Educational and Industrial Testing Service. San Diego, CA. [Google Scholar]
  34. Miller GE, Chen E, & Zhou ES (2007). If it goes up, must it come down? chronic stress and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in humans. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 25–45. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.25 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Myin-Germeys I, Oorschot M, Collip D, Lataster J, Delespaul P, & van Os J (2009). Experience sampling research in psychopathology: Opening the black box of daily life. Psychological Medicine, 39(9), 1533–1547. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004947 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pruessner JC, Kirschbaum C, Meinlschmid G, & Hellhammer DH (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28(7), 916–931. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00108-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Quinn PD, & Duckworth AL (2007). Happiness and academic achievement: Evidence for reciprocal causality. The Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Society; 24:27.5. [Google Scholar]
  38. Ruttle PL, Javaras KN, Klein MH, Armstrong JM, Burk LR, & Essex MJ (2013). Concurrent and longitudinal associations between diurnal cortisol and body mass index across adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 52(6), 731–737. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.11.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Sladek MR, & Doane LD (2015;2014;). Daily diary reports of social connection, objective sleep, and the cortisol awakening response during adolescents’ first year of college. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44(2), 298–316. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0244-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Schmidt-Reinwald A, Pruessner JC, Hellhammer DH, Federenko I, Rohleder N, Schürmeyer TH, & Kirschbaum C (1999). The cortisol response to awakening in relation to different challenge tests and a 12-hour cortisol rhythm. Life Sciences, 64(18), 1653–1660. doi: 10.1016/S0024-3205(99)00103-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Shifren K, & Chong A (2012). Health-related behaviors: A study among former young caregivers. Journal of Adult Development, 19(2), 111–121. doi: 10.1007/s10804-011-9140-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Siskowski C (2006). Young caregivers: Effect of family health situations on school performance. The Journal of School Nursing, 22(3), 163–169. doi: 10.1177/10598405060220030701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Stalder T, Kirschbaum C, Kudielka BM, Adam EK, Pruessner JC, Wüst S, Dockray S, Smyth N, Evans P, Hellhammer DH and Miller R, (2016). Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 63, 414–432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Suarez-Orozco C, & Suarez-Orozco M (1995). Transformations: Immigration, family life, and achievement motivation among latino adolescents. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  45. Telzer EH, & Fuligni AJ (2009a). A longitudinal daily diary study of family assistance and academic achievement among adolescents from mexican, chinese, and european backgrounds. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38(4), 560–571. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9391-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Telzer EH, & Fuligni AJ (2009b). Daily family assistance and the psychological well-being of adolescents from latin american, asian, and european backgrounds. Developmental Psychology, 45(4), 1177–1189. doi: 10.1037/a0014728 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Telzer EH, Masten CL, Berkman ET, Lieberman MD, & Fuligni AJ (2010). Gaining while giving: An fMRI study of the rewards of family assistance among white and latino youth. Social Neuroscience, 5(5), 508–518. doi: 10.1080/17470911003687913 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Telzer EH, Fuligni AJ, Lieberman MD, & Galván A (2013). Meaningful family relationships: Neurocognitive buffers of adolescent risk taking. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(3), 374–387. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Telzer EH, Gonzales N, & Fuligni AJ (2014). Family obligation values and family assistance behaviors: Protective and risk factors for Mexican–American adolescents’ substance use. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43(2), 270–283. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-9941-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Telzer EH, Tsai KM, Gonzales N, & Fuligni AJ (2015). Mexican American adolescents’ family obligation values and behaviors: Links to internalizing symptoms across time and context. Developmental psychology, 51(1), 75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tsai KM, Gonzales NA, & Fuligni AJ (2016). Mexican american adolescents’ emotional support to the family in response to parental stress. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26(4), 658–672. doi: 10.1111/jora.12216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tsai KM, Telzer EH, Gonzales NA, & Fuligni AJ (2013). Adolescents’ daily assistance to the family in response to maternal need. Journal of Marriage and Family, 75(4), 964–980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Trzesniewski KH, Donnellan MB, Moffitt TE, Robins RW, Poulton R, & Caspi A (2006). Low self-esteem during adolescence predicts poor health, criminal behavior, and limited economic prospects during adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 42(2), 381–390. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Walsh S, Shulman S, Bar‐On Z, & Tsur A (2006). The role of parentification and family climate in adaptation among immigrant adolescents in Israel. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16(2), 321–350. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2006.00134.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Weisner TS (2001). Children investing in their families: The importance of child obligation in successful development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development (94), 77–83. doi: 10.1002/cd.32 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Williams CL, Hayman LL, Daniels SR, Robinson TN, Steinberger J, Paridon S, & Bazzarre T (2002). Cardiovascular health in childhood: A statement for health professionals from the committee on atherosclerosis, hypertension, and obesity in the young (AHOY) of the council on cardiovascular disease in the young, american heart association. Circulation, 106(1), 143–160. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000019555.61092.9E [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES