Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2012 Jan 4;32(1):4–11. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3261-11.2012

Topographic Contribution of Early Visual Cortex to Short-Term Memory Consolidation: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study

Vincent van de Ven 1,, Christianne Jacobs 1, Alexander T Sack 1
PMCID: PMC6621312  PMID: 22219265

Abstract

The neural correlates for retention of visual information in visual short-term memory are considered separate from those of sensory encoding. However, recent findings suggest that sensory areas may play a role also in short-term memory. We investigated the functional relevance, spatial specificity, and temporal characteristics of human early visual cortex in the consolidation of capacity-limited topographic visual memory using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Topographically specific TMS pulses were delivered over lateralized occipital cortex at 100, 200, or 400 ms into the retention phase of a modified change detection task with low or high memory loads. For the high but not the low memory load, we found decreased memory performance for memory trials in the visual field contralateral, but not ipsilateral to the side of TMS, when pulses were delivered at 200 ms into the retention interval. A behavioral version of the TMS experiment, in which a distractor stimulus (memory mask) replaced the TMS pulses, further corroborated these findings. Our findings suggest that retinotopic visual cortex contributes to the short-term consolidation of topographic visual memory during early stages of the retention of visual information. Further, TMS-induced interference decreased the strength (amplitude) of the memory representation, which most strongly affected the high memory load trials.

Introduction

The visual short-term memory (VSTM) system effectively retains visual sensory information well after the sensory stimulation has ended. Short-term retention may include the consolidation of visual information into the neural system, in which the information can be manipulated and used to guide our future behavior (Jonides et al., 2008). Neurophysiological and brain imaging studies suggest that the neural correlates of VSTM are separate from those that encode the sensory information (Goldman-Rakic, 1995; Miller et al., 1996; Courtney et al., 1997; D'Esposito et al., 2000; Munk et al., 2002; Linden et al., 2003). However, there is increasing evidence that sensory cortex may play an important role in STM (Fuster, 1995; Pasternak and Greenlee, 2005). Several animal neurophysiology studies reported increased activity in higher-level (Fuster and Jervey, 1982; Miyashita and Chang, 1988) as well as low-level (Supèr et al., 2001) visual areas during visual short-term retention. Further, while most functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of VSTM did not reveal increased activity in sensory brain areas during retention (Courtney et al., 1997; Munk et al., 2002; Linden et al., 2003), recent fMRI studies showed that visual cortex may contribute to retention of visual information in other ways than increased activity (Harrison and Tong, 2009; Tambini et al., 2010).

The temporal window for visual short-term consolidation may last ∼500 ms into the retention interval (Shapiro et al., 1994; Chun and Potter, 1995; Giesbrecht and Di Lollo, 1998; Jolicoeur and Dell'Acqua, 1998; Lalonde and Chaudhuri, 2002; Vogel et al., 2006). Earlier studies using the attentional blink paradigm showed that identification of a target stimulus is impaired if it is presented 200–500 ms after an initial stimulus (Shapiro et al., 1994; Chun and Potter, 1995). More recent behavioral experiments showed impaired memory performance if a distractor, or memory mask, is presented up to 500 ms after presentation of the memory item (Magnussen et al., 1996; Jolicoeur and Dell'Acqua, 1998; Vogel et al., 2006). The distractor may interfere with the memory representation of the target stimulus, thereby affecting the window of consolidation more directly (Magnussen and Greenlee, 1999; Enns and Di Lollo, 2000).

It remains unclear how sensory cortex contributes to this consolidation window. Here, we investigated the functional role of human early visual areas in VSTM consolidation using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). TMS can be used to manipulate activity in retinotopic visual cortex (Amassian et al., 1989; Kastner et al., 1998; Kammer et al., 2005a,b; Sack et al., 2009a; Thielscher et al., 2010) and chart the time course of behavioral relevance of the stimulated area (Pascual-Leone et al., 2000). In our study, participants saw a visual stimulus array in either their left or right visual field and had to report whether a subsequent test array differed from the previously presented memory array. We administered TMS to one visual field (spatial specificity) at different time points (temporal specificity) during the consolidation window, which allowed us to directly test the functional relevance, topography, and temporal characteristics of early visual cortex contribution in VSTM.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

We ran a behavioral pilot experiment, for which we recruited 11 participants. Nine of the 11 participants reached the performance threshold during training [five females; one left-handed; mean (SD) age of 24.8 (2.2) years].

For the TMS experiment, we recruited 19 participants. On 14 participants, we positioned the TMS coil over the scalp using phosphene localization; six of these participants were later excluded (two because of failing to reach the performance threshold during training and four because phosphenes could not be elicited reliably). In the other five participants, we used fMRI-based neuronavigation to position the coil; the data of one participant were excluded because of seeing phosphenes during the main part of the experiment. The remaining 12 participants (six females; one left-handed) had a mean (SD) age of 25.3 (3.8) years. The TMS protocol followed published safety guidelines (Wassermann, 1998; Rossi et al., 2009) and was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the Academic Hospital of Maastricht.

For the memory-masking experiment, we recruited 12 participants. All participants reached the required performance threshold during training [seven females; four left-handed; mean (SD) age of 20.8 (1.3) years].

Participants were recruited from the academic environment of Maastricht University, The Netherlands. All participants provided written informed consent before participation, and TMS participants also filled out and signed health screening forms that were approved by a medical supervisor and an independent physician. All participants received financial compensation or course credits for their participation.

Stimulus material and presentation.

Pictures were gray surfaces (luminance, 68.7 cd/m2) of nonnatural shapes (BORTS: blurred outlines of random Tetris shapes; courtesy of N. Kriegeskorte, MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK), which could not be easily verbalized or conceptualized (Linden et al., 2003; Peters et al., 2009). Pictures spanned 1.5° in width and height and were drawn on a white background (227 cd/m2) that spanned the entire computer screen (34° × 27° visual angle). For the pilot and memory-masking experiments (see below), we created a distractor stimulus (memory mask) from the average of all the abstract shapes used in the experiments. Participants were seated in front of a PC-controlled computer screen (refresh rate, 60 Hz) at an eye–screen distance of ∼57 cm. Stimulus presentation and timing, and logging of individual responses were controlled using the Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems).

The experimental task was a variant of the change detection task. A memory trial (Fig. 1) started with a brief presentation (500 ms) of an arrow (< or > symbol) at the fixation location to indicate in which visual field the trial would be presented. The arrow was briefly replaced by a fixation symbol (+, duration 500 ms), after which the remainder of the trial started, including presentation of the sample stimulus array, retention of the sample in working memory, presentation of the test stimulus array, and the participant's response.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Experimental design. The memory task was a modified version of the change detection task that contained a sample presentation phase, a retention phase and a test phase. Top left, Sample and test stimulus arrays contained sample and test arrays of one (low memory load) or three (high load) simultaneously presented abstract shapes, which were always presented at either the left or the right side of the fixation point, at 6° visual angle, and dispersed at equal distances of 20° polar angle steps. Right, A memory trial started with a short presentation of a visual field cue (< or >) that designated the visual field in which the trial would be presented (cue validity = 100%). Afterward, the sample array was briefly shown and was followed by a retention interval of fixed duration (1500 ms). Finally, the retention interval was followed by presentation of the test array and participants were required to judge whether the test array was the same as the sample array (i.e., match) or not (nonmatch). For TMS trials, a single TMS pulse was delivered at 100, 200, or 400 ms into the retention interval (ISI). For baseline measurements, no pulses were delivered. Bottom left, TMS pulses were delivered over lateral occipital cortex, using the ipsilateral visual field as TMS-naive control. LVF, Left visual field.

The sample array contained one or three gray-scale abstract pictures, which were presented simultaneously. Pictures were centered at an eccentricity of 6° visual angle in the lower visual fields on either the left or right side of the fixation symbol. The respective polar angle of the pictures were 25°, 45°, and 65° from the horizontal meridian in the left and right lower quadrants (for stimulus presentation for the lower left visual field, see Fig. 1, top left). The sample array was presented for 150 ms and was followed by a blank screen and superimposed fixation symbol for 1500 ms, during which the sample array had to be retained in memory. A test array followed the retention period at the same location as the sample array. The test array contained either the same pictures as the sample array or had one different picture. For the three-picture stimulus arrays, the altered picture had an equal probability to be presented at any of the three positions. Participants were required to judge whether the test array was the same as the sample array (i.e., SAME response, pressing the Z key with the left hand) or not (DIFFERENT response, pressing the/key with the right hand). Participants were instructed that accuracy was more important than response speed, but that there was a time limit of 5 s.

General procedure.

Participants first completed a number of practice sessions, in which 24 change detection trials were presented (four blocks of six trials, two blocks in each visual field). Participants were required to attain a minimum accuracy of 20/24 trials, which was commonly attained within two to five sessions. For each practice trial, participants received feedback about their performance (green or red fixation cross indicated correct or false judgment, respectively). Afterward, participants completed a baseline session (60 trials across two hemifields and two memory load conditions; six blocks of 10 trials each) during which no TMS pulses or masking stimuli were delivered. Finally, participants completed the main memory experiment, in which a TMS pulse or memory mask was presented at one of three time points during the retention interval [i.e., after offset of the sample array; interstimulus intervals (ISI) of 100, 200, and 400 ms; 60 trials per time point; 18 blocks of 10 trials each]. Consecutive trials in each block were separated by 3000 ms during which participants only saw the fixation cross at the center of the screen. Visual field position of the memory trials varied in a block-by-block fashion (i.e., left-right-left). During the training and baseline experiments, memory load was randomized within and across blocks. During the TMS and memory-masking experiments, memory load and time point of interference (TMS pulse or memory mask) were randomized within and across blocks, with equal probability for each Load × Time point combination. In the pilot experiment, the location of the memory mask was always spatially congruent with the memory trial. In the TMS and the memory-masking experiment, the pulse or memory mask was presented at a fixed visual field location so that it was spatially congruent to 50% of the memory trials. Specifically, in the TMS experiment, pulses were delivered to the left or right visual field in 11 or one participants, respectively. In the memory masking experiment, distractor stimuli were presented in the left or right visual field in eight or four participants, respectively.

Transcranial magnetic stimulation.

Biphasic TMS pulses were delivered over lateral visual cortex using a figure-of-eight coil (MC-B70, the inner and outer radii of the two coil loops were 1.2 and 5.4 cm, respectively Fig. 1, bottom left) and a MagPro R 30 stimulator (maximum stimulator output, 1.9 T; Medtronic Functional Diagnostics).

We used phosphene localization to determine the coil position over the scalp in eight participants (Pascual-Leone and Walsh, 2001; Campana et al., 2002; Silvanto et al., 2005; Romei et al., 2008; Cattaneo et al., 2009; Sack et al., 2009a). Here, the location and intensity of pulse delivery was determined by localizing TMS-induced phosphenes as close as possible to the visual field location of the center stimulus of the sample array. TMS-induced phosphenes have been associated with local transient changes in activity of early visual areas (Amassian et al., 1989; Meyer et al., 1991; Kammer et al., 2005a).

Phosphene localization was performed with a black computer screen, which facilitated phosphene detection and localization. Phosphene localization started with 60% of maximum stimulator output intensity and the center of the coil was positioned ∼2 cm above the inion. The coil was then moved laterally and single TMS pulses were applied until a clear phosphene was perceived in the contralateral visual field. Phosphene perception had to depend on stimulated hemisphere and retinotopic organization to be considered a valid TMS-induced percept (Meyer et al., 1991; Sack et al., 2009a). This procedure aimed at positioning the TMS coil in such a way that the induced phosphenes overlapped with or were in close proximity to the spatial location of the sample and test arrays. We used custom-written software to allow participants to monitor and register phosphene locations in the visual field. After localization, the TMS coil was fixed in a mechanical arm coil holder and placed tangentially on the skull. Three phosphenes were then elicited to ensure that phosphene location was not altered. In seven of the eight participants, we administered TMS pulses to the right hemisphere (left visual field), thereby using the left hemisphere as control. In one participant, reliable phosphenes could only be elicited in the left hemisphere (right visual field), with the right hemisphere used as control.

Phosphene localization is inherently based on subjective reports of phosphene perceptions, in which phosphenes may not be reliably produced in some participants. Therefore, we used fMRI-based neuronavigation (Sack et al., 2009b) to position the coil over the V1/V2 complex of the upper convex of the calcarine fissure (lower visual field) of the right hemisphere in four participants. Neuronavigation was based on the results of an fMRI localizer of the stimulus position in the lower left visual field (6° eccentricity), which was acquired in a separate, unrelated study (De Weerd et al., in press). Note that for these participants, the coil position was independent of whether or how reliably phosphenes could be induced. TMS neuronavigation followed procedures described previously (Sack et al., 2009b). After coil positioning, we attempted to induce phosphenes. Two participants saw phosphenes in the same part of the visual field as the participants in whom coil position was based on phosphene localization did. We could not elicit phosphenes in the other two participants.

During the main experiment, single-pulse TMS was applied at stimulator output intensity equal to 110% of phosphene threshold to correct for the white background of the screen during presentation of the memory experiment. Visual exposure to dimmed or dark luminance markedly decreases phosphene thresholds (Boroojerdi et al., 2000). Stimulus presentation and pulse triggering were controlled using Presentation (Neurobehavioral Systems). For the two participants who did not see phosphenes, stimulation intensity during the remainder of the experiment was set at 45% machine output.

Analysis.

The dependent variable in all experiments was accuracy, which was estimated according to signal detection theory rationale that a decision criterion to correctly judge the presence of a signal results from the deviation between signal and noise distributions (MacMillan and Creelman, 2005). Hit rate (H) was calculated as the number of hits (correctly judging that sample and probe were the same) divided by the sum of number of hits and misses (i.e., H = hits/(hits + misses)). False alarm rate (FA) was calculated as the number of false alarms (erroneously judging that sample and probe were the same) divided by the sum of the number of false alarms and correct rejections (i.e., FA = false alarms/(false alarms + correct rejections)). We used the nonparametric A′ (A-prime) index (Snodgrass and Corwin, 1988; Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999; MacMillan and Creelman, 2005) as a measure of accuracy independent of response bias. Originally, A′ is estimated using two formulas:

graphic file with name zns00112-1076-m01.jpg

when H > FA, and

graphic file with name zns00112-1076-m02.jpg

when FA > H.

These equations can be rewritten into a single formula, as follows (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999):

graphic file with name zns00112-1076-m03.jpg

A′ presents a nonparametric alternative to the more often used d′ (d-prime) when assumptions of normality that are required for d′ are suspect to violation (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999; MacMillan and Creelman, 2005). In our case, we used A′ rather than d′ because we obtained a relatively small sample of responses per cell, and because some participants showed perfect performance (i.e., H = 1) in some conditions. We also ran the main analyses using d′, and results were similar.

Accuracy was analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVA (RMANOVA) models. For the pilot experiment, we first verified that responses in the two visual fields were not significantly different, using a Field (left, right) × Load (1, 3) × ISI (100, 200, 400) RMANOVA model with first- and second-order interaction terms. We then proceeded with a Load (1, 3) × Mask (100, 200, 400) RMANOVA model that also included the interaction term. For the TMS experiment, we used a Field (TMS, Control) × Load (1, 3) × TMS (100, 200, 400) RMANOVA model with first- and second-order interaction terms. We used a similar model for analysis of the memory-masking experiment [i.e., Field (Mask, Control) × Load (1, 3) × ISI (100, 200, 400), with first- and second-order interaction terms]. Effects with p values at or below the alpha level of 0.05 were considered significant. Post hoc comparisons of mean differences were performed using two-tailed one-sample t tests (Holm-Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons). Effect sizes for RMANOVA were further estimated for significant effects [partial η squared (η2) for ANOVA effects and Cohen's d for t test effects].

Results

Pilot experiment of short-term memory and visual masking

In this experiment, we presented lateralized short-term memory trials and memory masks that were always spatially congruent with the memory trials to investigate the effect of memory masking independent of visual field. In an initial analysis, we verified that memory masks presented in each visual field provided similar response patterns. We analyzed percentage accuracy of memory performance using a 2 × 2 × 3 RMANOVA with factors Visual Field (left or right), Load (1 or 3), and ISI (100, 200, or 400). We found a significant main effect of Load (F(1,8) = 238.2, p < 0.001), but no significant effects related to Field (F(1,8) = 1.02, p = 0.34), which confirmed that memory masking did not differentially affect performance of the two visual fields. Further, we found trend effects for ISI (F(1,8) = 3.0, p = 0.079) and for the Load × ISI interaction (F(2,16) = 3.1, p = 0.073).

To obtain more power, we pooled the results across the two visual fields and recalculated a 2 × 3 RMANOVA (Load × ISI). We found a significant main effect of Load (F(1,8) = 241.0, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.97) and a significant Load × ISI interaction effect (F(2,16) = 3.6, p = 0.052, η2 = 0.31). Figure 2 shows the mean accuracies across all conditions (data pooled across Field; Table 1). The interaction effect was based on a decrease of accuracy when the mask was presented 200 ms into the retention interval (mean A′ = 0.71), compared with the other two time points (mean A′ ISI 100 = 0.78; ISI 400 = 0.78), only during high memory load trials (Table 1). These results indicated that presentation of a memory mask ∼200 ms into the retention interval interfered with subsequent recognition in high memory load trials.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Pilot results. Plots show accuracy (A′) of memory trials with memory masks at 100, 200, or 400 ms into the retention interval. Here, memory masks were always spatially congruent to the memory trial. A, Mean accuracies are shown for the two memory loads (solid lines, load 1; dashed lines, load 3) and for the two visual fields [red, left visual field (LVF); blue, right visual field (RVF)]. ANOVA showed that responses were similar across the two visual fields. B, Mean accuracies are shown for the two memory loads collapsed across visual fields. Error bars represent 1 SEM.

Table 1.

Memory performance

Pilot Load 1
Load 3
100 ms 200 ms 400 ms 100 ms 200 ms 400 ms
Experiment 1
    Mask (L + R) 0.94 (0.03) 0.94 (0.03) 0.94 (0.02) 0.78 (0.05) 0.71 (0.06) 0.78 (0.08)
Experiment 2
    TMS 0.89 (0.09) 0.90 (0.13) 0.93 (0.05) 0.70 (0.20) 0.54 (0.26) 0.74 (0.18)
    Control 0.92 (0.07) 0.90 (0.11) 0.91 (0.06) 0.75 (0.22) 0.76 (0.17) 0.63 (0.21)
Experiment 3
    Mask 0.95 (0.11) 0.94 (0.13) 0.95 (0.09) 0.75 (0.13) 0.68 (0.10) 0.77 (0.14)
    Control 0.94 (0.05) 0.96 (0.13) 0.94 (0.14) 0.79 (0.16) 0.79 (0.14) 0.74 (0.14)

Mean (SD) accuracies (A′) of the three experiments according to memory load (one or three items) and TMS or masking time point (100, 200, or 400 ms into the retention interval). For the pilot study (Experiment 1), performance in the left (L) and right (R) visual fields were pooled because memory masks were always spatially congruent with the memory trials.

Short-term memory and TMS

In this experiment, we presented lateralized short-term memory trials while TMS pulses were always delivered over retinotopic occipital cortex (affecting one visual field location). Thus, TMS pulses were spatially congruent with 50% of the memory trials.

Accuracy was analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 3 RMANOVA model (Field × Load × TMS). To verify that the results were not biased by the two different methods of coil positioning, we added Positioning Method (phosphenes, neuronavigation) as between-subject factor.

With respect to positioning method, we found a significant Load × Method interaction effect (F(1,10) = 5.2, p = 0.046, η2 = 0.34). Participants of the neuronavigation method showed slightly better performance for low memory load trials [mean (SD) A′ = 0.94 (0.04)] compared with those of with phosphene localization [mean (SD) A′ = 0.89 (0.04)], but slightly worse performance for high memory load trials [mean (SD) A′ = 0.64 (0.11)] than those with the phosphene localization [mean (SD) A′ = 0.71 (0.12)]. All other interactions with Method were not significant (ps > 0.22). Thus, localization method did not affect the pattern of results. We pooled the data of participants from both positioning methods in the following analyses.

For the within-subject factors, we found a significant main effect of Load (F(1,11) = 57.8, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.84), a significant Field × TMS interaction effect (F(2,22) = 3.9, p = 0.037, η2 = 0.26), and a significant Field × Load × TMS interaction effect (F(2,22) = 4.3, p = 0.026, η2 = 0.28). To parcel out these effects, we calculated 2 × 3 RMANOVAs for each of the two memory loads. For Load 1, we found no significant main or interaction effects (ps > 0.59). For Load 3, we found a significant Field × TMS interaction effect (F(2,22) = 5.0, p = 0.016, η2 = 0.31). Figure 3 shows the mean accuracies across the conditions (Table 1).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

TMS and memory-masking results. A–D, Plots show accuracy (A′) of memory trials with TMS pulses (A, B) or memory masks (C, D) at 100, 200, or 400 ms into the retention interval. Here, TMS pulses and memory masks were spatially congruent to the memory trial in 50% of the trials. A, C, Mean accuracies are shown for the two memory loads and two visual fields after presentation of a TMS pulse (A) or memory mask (C). B, D, Mean interference effect of accuracy after presentation of a TMS pulse (B, TMS visual field—Control visual field) or a memory mask (D, Mask visual field—Control visual field). Error bars represent 1 SEM. TMS/Mask, TMS or memory-mask visual field; Ctrl, control visual field.

These results corroborated and extended the pilot memory-masking results. The interactions were based on a decrease of accuracy when the spatially congruent TMS pulse was presented 200 ms into the retention interval, compared with the spatially incongruent pulse, only for the high memory load (Table 1). Post hoc contrasts between TMS and control visual fields for the time points during high memory load trials showed a significant TMS-induced impairment in performance at the 200 ms time point (mean decrease A′ = −0.21; t(11) = −2.9, p = 0.013, Cohen's d = 0.85), but not at the other two time points. Further post hoc one-tailed comparisons in the TMS condition of the decrease in accuracy at 200 ms compared with the other two time points were significant ([A′100 > A′200]: t(11) = −1.9, p = 0.044; [A′400 > A′200]: t(11) = −2.3, p = 0.021). We found no significant differences within the control condition.

Short-term memory and visual masking

In this experiment, we presented lateralized short-term memory trials while memory masks were always presented in one visual field location. Thus, memory masks were spatially congruent with 50% of the trials. Accuracy was analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 3 RMANOVA model (Field × Load × ISI). We verified that the choice of control visual field (right in eight participants, left in four) did not affect the results by adding Mask Location (left, right) as between-subject factor. Mask Location did not show interaction effects with any of the within-subject factors (ps > 0.32), which further corroborated the finding from the pilot study that memory performance did not differ between visual fields.

For the within-subject factors, we found a significant main effect of Load (F(1,11) = 239.1, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.96) and a significant Field × ISI interaction effect (F(2,22) = 5.5, p = 0.012, η2 = 33). To parcel out these effects, we recalculated 2 × 3 RMANOVAs for each of the two memory loads (i.e., Field × ISI). For Load 1, we found no significant main or interaction effects (ps > 0.31). For Load 3, we found a significant Field × TMS interaction effect (F(2,22) = 4.1, p = 0.031, η2 = 0.27). Figure 3 shows the mean accuracies across the conditions (see also Table 1).

These results corroborated and extended the pilot and TMS results. The interactions were based on a decrease of accuracy when the spatially congruent mask was presented 200 ms into the retention interval, compared with the spatially incongruent mask (Table 1). This effect was present only for the high memory load. Post hoc contrasts between mask and control visual fields for the 200 ms time point showed that the mask-induced performance impairment was close to significant at the uncorrected p value (mean decrease A′ = −0.12; t(11) = −2.1, p = 0.056, Cohen's d = 0.62), but not at the corrected p value. The other two masking time points showed no significant differences (ps > 0.7). Further post hoc comparisons in the TMS condition of the decrease in accuracy at 200 ms compared with the other two time points were significant ([A′100 > A′200]: t(11) = −2.1, p = 0.028; [A′400 > A′200]: t(11) = −2.4, p = 0.016). We found no significant differences within the control condition. These results suggest that participants showed a similar pattern of interference at 200 ms compared with the TMS experiment.

Discussion

To investigate the functional relevance, topography, and temporal characteristics of early visual cortex contribution to VSTM consolidation, we presented memory masks or administered topographically specific TMS pulses affecting the visual field contralateral to the side of TMS at different time points within 100–400 ms into the retention interval of a change detection task with low and high memory loads, and used the other visual field as within-subject control. We found a very strong correspondence in memory interference between the TMS and masking experiments. Specifically, memory masks and TMS pulses decreased accuracy only in the targeted visual field, revealing the topographic specificity of our TMS-induced memory consolidation interference in early visual cortex. Furthermore, accuracy in the targeted visual field decreased only when the memory mask or TMS pulse was administered at 200 ms into the retention window (temporal specificity), and most strongly for the high memory load (capacity limit). These findings not only provide direct evidence for the functional necessity of intact neural processing in early visual cortex at 200 ms into the retention phase, but also suggest a load dependence of this functional relevance in memory consolidation. The similar findings of both experiments suggest that interference of activity in visual cortex reliably impaired memory consolidation, thereby affecting memory performance. Our findings are in line with previous interference studies that showed a critical window for VSTM consolidation up to 500 ms after stimulus offset (Chun and Potter, 1995; Magnussen et al., 1996; Giesbrecht and Di Lollo, 1998; Jolicoeur and Dell'Acqua, 1998; Lalonde and Chaudhuri, 2002; Vogel et al., 2006), and reveal a neurophysiological correlate of this consolidation window that includes activation states of topographic early visual cortex, which suggests that VSTM representations may be formed or maintained in sensory brain areas that encoded the stimuli. To our knowledge, we are the first to show that topographic early visual cortex is functionally relevant for short-term consolidation of sensory visual information.

To date, very few studies have investigated the role of occipital cortex in VSTM. In a previous study (Beckers and Hömberg, 1991), participants completed a delayed match-to-sample (DMS) task of face stimuli, during which TMS was applied over the occipital pole before (i.e., during the retention phase) or after (i.e., during memory scanning) presentation of the memory probe. The authors found no significant change in reaction times after pulses were delivered during the memory retention phase. However, pulses delivered during the memory scanning phase significantly decreased the scanning rate. A recent TMS study found that TMS administered at the onset of the DMS retention phase increased reaction times, whereas TMS at the end of the retention phase decreased reaction times (Cattaneo et al., 2009). Crucially, in these studies, effects of TMS on memory performance were found in altered reaction times, but not in accuracy, which suggests that their effects may be mediated by neural mechanisms other than those studied here.

TMS may interfere with signal processing by decreasing signal strength, rather than by adding random noise (Harris et al., 2008). Therefore, TMS may decrease the strength of or overwrite the neural representation of the memory trace in visual cortex. This suggestion is in line with propositions of object substitution in visual memory after presentation of a competing stimulus (Giesbrecht and Di Lollo, 1998; Enns and Di Lollo, 2000). Furthermore, our finding of a lateralized TMS effect on memory performance suggested that the neural memory representation was retinotopically organized, with limited transfer of memorized information to other parts of the visual field (Karni and Sagi, 1993; Schoups et al., 1995; Dill and Fahle, 1997). This spatial specificity of our findings further supports the notion of a local neural representation of memory in visual cortex. Importantly, the interference of TMS on memory performance was not due to impaired visual awareness of the to-be-memorized items. Visual awareness is commonly impaired if a visual mask is presented 60–120 ms after a very brief presentation of the target stimulus (Breitmeyer and Ogmen, 2000). A number of TMS studies that presented pulses in a similar temporal window reported similar impairments of awareness (Amassian et al., 1989; Beckers and Hömberg, 1991; Kammer, 2007; Sack et al., 2009a). In our study, participants were not impaired in their memory performance if TMS or the mask was presented at 100 ms into the retention interval. Instead, TMS impaired memory performance at a later time window, and only for the high memory load, which suggests that TMS pulses interfered with poststimulus processes beyond those of initial encoding.

Also, it is unlikely that TMS decreased accuracy through phosphenes acting as visual masks. Most participants reported seeing no or few phosphenes during the memory experiment, similarly to previous studies (Silvanto et al., 2005; Cattaneo et al., 2009). Furthermore, even if participants saw phosphenes but failed to report them, phosphene masking cannot explain the larger TMS-induced impairment compared with the smaller impairment in the memory-masking experiment. Finally, TMS-induced phosphenes could even increase memory performance. Silvanto and colleagues showed that TMS-induced phosphenes may contain previously learnt or memorized information (Silvanto et al., 2007; Silvanto and Cattaneo, 2010). Participants could possibly use such phosphenes to rehearse the information during retention and increase recognition accuracy.

Alternatively, TMS may have interfered with ongoing communication between visual cortex and higher-level areas that may occur during periods of consolidation, as is shown in recent fMRI studies (Lewis et al., 2009; Tambini et al., 2010). Furthermore, the time point of impairment may be associated with dynamically altering states of brain activity in sensory and higher-level areas. For example, Lamme and Roelfsema (2000) proposed that visual perception is the result of sweeps of activity running up and down the visual hierarchy. Incoming visual sensory information is initially processed in a feedforward sweep up the hierarchy that occurs within 40 and 70 ms after stimulus onset. Higher-level brain areas than feed information back to early visual areas within 80 and 120 ms after stimulus onset. The feedforward sweep may be associated with initial stimulus encoding, while the feedback sweep may be associated with visual awareness or attention. In our study, the time point of interference may indicate that alteration of activity in early visual cortex may have affected a later feedback sweep to early visual cortex during short-term memory consolidation, suggesting that memory consolidation requires additional forward and backward sweeps beyond those needed for visual awareness.

Finally, we found that the functional relevance of early visual cortex to memory consolidation depended on the capacity constraints of VSTM (Luck and Vogel, 1997; Cowan, 2000). Retaining memory loads that approach the capacity limit is more easily or more profoundly impaired by the processing of distractors (Cowan, 2000; Vogel et al., 2006). Previous fMRI studies showed a neural correlate for the capacity limitation of VSTM in frontal and parietal cortex (Linden et al., 2003; Todd and Marois, 2004), in which brain activity increased monotonically with higher memory loads until the capacity limit was reached. Furthermore, retention of higher memory loads may be associated with increased functional coupling between these brain areas (Honey et al., 2002; Woodward et al., 2006). Thus, higher memory loads may require more neural resources for memory retention, which may leave limited resources available to protect against interfering signals. This suggestion could further be relevant when considering that we used complex memory items. Previous studies have shown that increased object complexity may constitute a higher information load, which thereby limits the object capacity of short-term memory (Alvarez and Cavanagh, 2004; Eng et al., 2005; Luria et al., 2010). Therefore, complexity-imposed shrinkage in VSTM capacity could be particularly costly when the number of memory items approaches the capacity limit. In this sense, trials of high memory and information load should be more prone to interfering signals, which is exactly what we found. However, the nature of the TMS-induced interference on the memory representation in early visual cortex may be different for simple stimuli, such as color or gratings.

The larger TMS-induced impairment compared with that after memory masking may indicate that the nature of the TMS interference is different from that of memory masking. TMS pulses may have directly affected relevant neural populations, interfering with cortical activity as the memory was processed. In contrast, the memory mask was presented to the retina, and traversed the same neural pathways as did the to-be-remembered stimuli. Here, interference effects may be much more subtle. Alternatively, the memory mask may not have provided enough overlap with relevant stimulus dimensions of the abstract shapes, thereby limiting its interference on memory-related processing of the target stimuli (Magnussen and Greenlee, 1999; Lalonde and Chaudhuri, 2002).

In conclusion, we showed that early visual cortex topographically contributes to consolidation of visual information in VSTM early within the retention window. Furthermore, TMS likely decreased the signal strength of the memory representation, which affects performance most strongly when the memory load approaches the capacity limit of VSTM. Finally, the specific time point of interference appears in line with a sequential pattern of discrete temporal windows during which visual cortex contributes to visual perception.

Footnotes

This work was supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (Grant 451-07-014 to V.V.) and by the European Research Council under the European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013)/European Research Council Grant Agreement Number 263472 (to A.T.S.). We kindly thank Drs. David E. J. Linden and Christoph Bledowski for very helpful comments on a previous version of this manuscript, and Peter Schubert and Liselot Kerpershoek for assistance in data acquisition. We thank our medical supervisor Cees van Leeuwen and independent physician Martin van Boxtel.

References

  1. Alvarez GA, Cavanagh P. The capacity of visual short-term memory is set both by visual information load and by number of objects. Psychol Sci. 2004;15:106–111. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502006.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amassian VE, Cracco RQ, Maccabee PJ, Cracco JB, Rudell A, Eberle L. Suppression of visual perception by magnetic coil stimulation of human occipital cortex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1989;74:458–462. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(89)90036-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beckers G, Hömberg V. Impairment of visual perception and visual short term memory scanning by transcranial magnetic stimulation of occipital cortex. Exp Brain Res. 1991;87:421–432. doi: 10.1007/BF00231859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Boroojerdi B, Bushara KO, Corwell B, Immisch I, Battaglia F, Muellbacher W, Cohen LG. Enhanced excitability of the human visual cortex induced by short-term light deprivation. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:529–534. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.5.529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Breitmeyer BG, Ogmen H. Recent models and findings in visual backward masking: a comparison, review, and update. Percept Psychophys. 2000;62:1572–1595. doi: 10.3758/bf03212157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Campana G, Cowey A, Walsh V. Priming of motion direction and area V5/MT: a test of perceptual memory. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:663–669. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.6.663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cattaneo Z, Vecchi T, Pascual-Leone A, Silvanto J. Contrasting early visual cortical activation states causally involved in visual imagery and short-term memory. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30:1393–1400. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06911.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chun MM, Potter MC. A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1995;21:109–127. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.21.1.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Courtney SM, Ungerleider LG, Keil K, Haxby JV. Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory. Nature. 1997;386:608–611. doi: 10.1038/386608a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cowan N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav Brain Sci. 2000;24:87–114. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x01003922. discussion 114–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. D'Esposito M, Postle BR, Rypma B. Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp Brain Res. 2000;133:3–11. doi: 10.1007/s002210000395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. De Weerd P, Reithler J, van de Ven V, Jacobs C, Been M, Sack AT. Post-training TMS of striate cortex disrupts consolidation early in visual skill learning. J Neurosci. 2012 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3712-11.2011. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dill M, Fahle M. The role of visual field position in pattern-discrimination learning. Proc Biol Sci. 1997;264:1031–1036. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Eng HY, Chen D, Jiang Y. Visual working memory for simple and complex visual stimuli. Psychon Bull Rev. 2005;12:1127–1133. doi: 10.3758/bf03206454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Enns JT, Di Lollo V. What's new in visual masking? Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:345–352. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01520-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fuster JM. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT; 1995. Memory in the cerebral cortex: an empirical approach to neural networks in the human and nonhuman primate. [Google Scholar]
  17. Fuster JM, Jervey JP. Neuronal firing in the inferotemporal cortex of the monkey in a visual memory task. J Neurosci. 1982;2:361–375. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-03-00361.1982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Giesbrecht B, Di Lollo V. Beyond the attentional blink: visual masking by object substitution. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1998;24:1454–1466. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.24.5.1454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Goldman-Rakic PS. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron. 1995;14:477–485. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Harris JA, Clifford CW, Miniussi C. The functional effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation: signal suppression or neural noise generation? J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20:734–740. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Harrison SA, Tong F. Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature. 2009;458:632–635. doi: 10.1038/nature07832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Honey GD, Fu CH, Kim J, Brammer MJ, Croudace TJ, Suckling J, Pich EM, Williams SC, Bullmore ET. Effects of verbal working memory load on corticocortical connectivity modeled by path analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Neuroimage. 2002;17:573–582. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jolicoeur P, Dell'Acqua R. The demonstration of short-term consolidation. Cogn Psychol. 1998;36:138–202. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jonides J, Lewis RL, Nee DE, Lustig CA, Berman MG, Moore KS. The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:193–224. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kammer T. Masking visual stimuli by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Psychol Res. 2007;71:659–666. doi: 10.1007/s00426-006-0063-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kammer T, Puls K, Erb M, Grodd W. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in the visual system. II. Characterization of induced phosphenes and scotomas. Exp Brain Res. 2005a;160:129–140. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1992-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kammer T, Puls K, Strasburger H, Hill NJ, Wichmann FA. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in the visual system. I. The psychophysics of visual suppression. Exp Brain Res. 2005b;160:118–128. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1991-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Karni A, Sagi D. The time course of learning a visual skill. Nature. 1993;365:250–252. doi: 10.1038/365250a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kastner S, Demmer I, Ziemann U. Transient visual field defects induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation over human occipital pole. Exp Brain Res. 1998;118:19–26. doi: 10.1007/s002210050251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lalonde J, Chaudhuri A. Task-dependent transfer of perceptual to memory representations during delayed spatial frequency discrimination. Vision Res. 2002;42:1759–1769. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00111-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lamme VA, Roelfsema PR. The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:571–579. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01657-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lewis CM, Baldassarre A, Committeri G, Romani GL, Corbetta M. Learning sculpts the spontaneous activity of the resting human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:17558–17563. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902455106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Linden DE, Bittner RA, Muckli L, Waltz JA, Kriegeskorte N, Goebel R, Singer W, Munk MH. Cortical capacity constraints for visual working memory: dissociation of fMRI load effects in a fronto-parietal network. Neuroimage. 2003;20:1518–1530. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Luck SJ, Vogel EK. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature. 1997;390:279–281. doi: 10.1038/36846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Luria R, Sessa P, Gotler A, Jolicoeur P, Dell'Acqua R. Visual short-term memory capacity for simple and complex objects. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22:496–512. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. MacMillan N, Creelman C. Detection theory: a user's guide. 2nd edition. New Jersey: Psychology; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  37. Magnussen S, Greenlee MW. The psychophysics of perceptual memory. Psychol Res. 1999;62:81–92. doi: 10.1007/s004260050043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Magnussen S, Greenlee MW, Thomas JP. Parallel processing in visual short-term memory. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1996;22:202–212. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.22.1.202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Meyer BU, Diehl R, Steinmetz H, Britton TC, Benecke R. Magnetic stimuli applied over motor and visual cortex: influence of coil position and field polarity on motor responses, phosphenes, and eye movements. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. 1991;43:121–134. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Miller EK, Erickson CA, Desimone R. Neural mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. J Neurosci. 1996;16:5154–5167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-16-05154.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Miyashita Y, Chang HS. Neuronal correlate of pictorial short-term memory in the primate temporal cortex. Nature. 1988;331:68–70. doi: 10.1038/331068a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Munk MH, Linden DE, Muckli L, Lanfermann H, Zanella FE, Singer W, Goebel R. Distributed cortical systems in visual short-term memory revealed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:866–876. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.8.866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Pascual-Leone A, Walsh V. Fast backprojections from the motion to the primary visual area necessary for visual awareness. Science. 2001;292:510–512. doi: 10.1126/science.1057099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pascual-Leone A, Walsh V, Rothwell J. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience: virtual lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2000;10:232–237. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00081-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pasternak T, Greenlee MW. Working memory in primate sensory systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:97–107. doi: 10.1038/nrn1603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Peters JC, Goebel R, Roelfsema PR. Remembered but unused: the accessory items in working memory that do not guide attention. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:1081–1091. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Romei V, Brodbeck V, Michel C, Amedi A, Pascual-Leone A, Thut G. Spontaneous fluctuations in posterior alpha-band EEG activity reflect variability in excitability of human visual areas. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:2010–2018. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A. Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin Neurophysiol. 2009;120:2008–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sack AT, van der Mark S, Schuhmann T, Schwarzbach J, Goebel R. Symbolic action priming relies on intact neural transmission along the retino-geniculo-striate pathway. Neuroimage. 2009a;44:284–293. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sack AT, Cohen Kadosh R, Schuhmann T, Moerel M, Walsh V, Goebel R. Optimizing functional accuracy of TMS in cognitive studies: a comparison of methods. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009b;21:207–221. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Schoups AA, Vogels R, Orban GA. Human perceptual learning in identifying the oblique orientation: retinotopy, orientation specificity and monocularity. J Physiol. 1995;483:797–810. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Shapiro KL, Raymond JE, Arnell KM. Attention to visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid serial visual presentation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1994;20:357–371. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.20.2.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Silvanto J, Cattaneo Z. Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals the content of visual short-term memory in the visual cortex. Neuroimage. 2010;50:1683–1689. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Silvanto J, Cowey A, Lavie N, Walsh V. Striate cortex (V1) activity gates awareness of motion. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:143–144. doi: 10.1038/nn1379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Silvanto J, Muggleton NG, Cowey A, Walsh V. Neural adaptation reveals state-dependent effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:1874–1881. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05440.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Snodgrass JG, Corwin J. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1988;117:34–50. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.117.1.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Stanislaw H, Todorov N. Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 1999;31:137–149. doi: 10.3758/bf03207704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Supèr H, Spekreijse H, Lamme VA. A neural correlate of working memory in the monkey primary visual cortex. Science. 2001;293:120–124. doi: 10.1126/science.1060496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Tambini A, Ketz N, Davachi L. Enhanced brain correlations during rest are related to memory for recent experiences. Neuron. 2010;65:280–290. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Thielscher A, Reichenbach A, Uğurbil K, Uludağ K. The cortical site of visual suppression by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:328–338. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Todd JJ, Marois R. Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature. 2004;428:751–754. doi: 10.1038/nature02466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Vogel EK, Woodman GF, Luck SJ. The time course of consolidation in visual working memory. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2006;32:1436–1451. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.6.1436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Wassermann EM. Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5–7, 1996. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1998;108:1–16. doi: 10.1016/s0168-5597(97)00096-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Woodward TS, Cairo TA, Ruff CC, Takane Y, Hunter MA, Ngan ET. Functional connectivity reveals load-dependent neural systems underlying encoding and maintenance in verbal working memory. Neuroscience. 2006;139:317–325. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.05.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES