Abstract
Error monitoring by the posterior medial frontal cortex (pMFC) has been linked to post-error behavioral adaptation effects and cognitive control dynamics in lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC). It remains unknown, however, whether control adjustments following errors produce post-error behavioral adjustments (PEBAs) by inhibiting inappropriate responses or facilitating goal-directed ones. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to investigate the hemodynamic correlates of PEBAs in a stimulus–response compatibility task. Our task was designed to test whether PEBAs are implemented by suppressing motor responses primed by irrelevant stimulus features (face location), redirecting attention to relevant features (face gender), or both or neither of these possibilities. Independent of PEBAs, error-related pMFC activation was followed by post-error recruitment of prefrontal and parietal control regions and, crucially, both (1) suppressed response-related activity in sensorimotor cortex and (2) enhanced target processing in face-sensitive sensory cortex (“fusiform face area”). More importantly, by investigating the covariation between post-error hemodynamic activity and individual differences in PEBAs, we showed that modulation of task-related motor and sensory processing was dependent on whether participants produced generally slower responses (“post-error slowing”; PES) or selectively reduced interference effects (“post-error reduction of interference”; PERI), respectively. Each of these behaviorally dependent effects was mediated by distinct LPFC control mechanisms (PES: inferior frontal junction; PERI: superior frontal sulcus). While establishing relationships between PEBAs and cognitive control, our findings suggest that the neural architecture underlying sequential behavioral adaptation may be determined primarily by how control is executed by the individual when adjustments are needed.
Introduction
The existence of a neural system specialized in error detection and compensation (Gehring et al., 1993) was originally inferred from early behavioral observations that participants performing cognitive tasks often adjust behavior following errors by rapidly “correcting” their responses or responding slower and sometimes more accurately on subsequent trials (Rabbitt, 1966). Over the past two decades, much evidence has accumulated implicating the posterior medial frontal cortex (pMFC) in monitoring performance for erroneous or error-prone actions (Botvinick et al., 2001; Brown and Braver, 2005) and integrating reinforcement history (Holroyd and Coles, 2002; Rushworth et al., 2007) to guide cognitive control mechanisms needed to bias lower-level task-related processing in a goal-directed manner and optimize future action outcomes (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004). Empirical support comes in part from studies that have shown conflict- and error-related pMFC activity to predict trial-by-trial behavioral adaptation effects (Gehring et al., 1993; Debener et al., 2005) and increased recruitment of the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) (Kerns et al., 2004; Cavanagh et al., 2009), a brain region broadly implicated in executing cognitive control (Miller and Cohen, 2001; Tanji and Hoshi, 2008). Complementing these findings, studies have shown sequential behavioral adaptation following conflict to be mediated by controlled processing of task-specific motor (Stürmer et al., 2002) and sensory (Egner and Hirsch, 2005) information. To date, however, evidence linking behavioral adaptation effects following errors to modulation of task-specific processing has remained elusive. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to explore relationships between the neural mechanisms underlying error monitoring in an interference task, trial-by-trial post-error behavioral adjustments (PEBAs) and, crucially, post-error processing of task-relevant and task-irrelevant information.
PEBAs can be expressed as a generally slower, apparently more cautious response mode (“post-error slowing”; PES) or a selective improvement in interference resolution (post-error reduction of interference; PERI) (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004; Carter and van Veen, 2007). While PES is often assumed to gauge a strategic increase in the motor response threshold to avoid future mistakes (Botvinick et al., 2001; Holroyd et al., 2005), recent evidence suggests that it may merely reflect an unspecific orienting response (Notebaert et al., 2009; Núñez Castellar et al., 2010). Similarly, it is not known whether the PERI effect reflects selective suppression of irrelevant representations (Ridderinkhof, 2002), enhancement of relevant representations, or both.
We investigated the hemodynamic correlates of PEBAs during performance of a face-gender discrimination version of the Simon stimulus–response (S–R) compatibility task (Simon, 1969) (Fig. 1). Efficient accurate performance required feature-based attention to faces while overriding “direct” response activation primed by irrelevant stimulus location (Kornblum et al., 1990; Stürmer et al., 2002). We analyzed the covariation of individual differences in PES and PERI with post-error changes in target processing in the individually defined fusiform face area (FFA) (Kanwisher et al., 1997) and response-related sensorimotor cortex (SMC). Thus, we could assess whether cognitive control adjustments following errors facilitate behavioral adaptation by redirecting attention to relevant sensory input, suppressing the influence of irrelevant information on motor output, or both or neither of these possibilities.
Materials and Methods
Participants.
Twenty-one male volunteers (mean age = 27.9 years; range = 23–34 years) participated in this study after giving written informed consent according to institutional guidelines based on the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants were right handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and reported no history of neurological, psychiatric, or major medical disorder. They were reimbursed with 10 euros for their participation, which lasted ∼75 min.
Procedure.
Participants performed two tasks during fMRI: a localizer task and the main face-gender classification version of the Simon task (Fig. 1). The purpose of the localizer task was to identify face-sensitive regions of extrastriate cortex (FFA) in which we could use hemodynamic activity during performance of the Simon task as an index to assess whether target processing varied as a function of trial n − 1 accuracy and/or PEBAs. Stimuli for both tasks were a total of 492 grayscale face portraits (50% male/ 50% female) selected from the Picture Database of Morphed Faces (Jäger et al., 2005). Only the original unmorphed images and 100% morphed faces from the database were used. Localizer task stimuli additionally included 52 face images “scrambled” with Adobe Photoshop (Adobe Systems) to yield distinguishable grid-like patterns, but no human features. All stimuli were rescaled to a size of 185 × 270 pixels. For both tasks, stimuli were presented against a black background onto a back-projection screen, which participants viewed in a mirror mounted to the head coil. This setup simulated a monitor viewing distance of ≈100 cm, resulting in stimuli subtending ≈2.8° × 4.1° of visual angle. Participants responded to stimuli according to task instructions with left- and right-hand index finger button presses on the left and right buttons of a two-key response device, respectively. Task programming, stimulus presentation and behavioral recording were performed with Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems).
During the localizer task, participants observed streams of centrally presented face and scrambled images between interspersed null (fixation) blocks. There were 5 pseudorandomly presented epochs of each event type, each including 15 trials with a fixed intertrial interval of 1000 ms. Images were presented for 200 ms followed by 800 ms of fixation. Stimulus epochs included 13 novel images and two 1-back image repetitions. To ensure task engagement, participants were instructed to respond to image repetitions with a right button press.
Immediately following the localizer scan, participants practiced the S–R mapping of the Simon task in a 48-trial simulation block. In each of the 432 trials of the main experiment, a novel male or female face image was presented (150 ms), either left or right of a permanently present central fixation cross (image center = 3.8° of visual angle). Stimulus onset occurred at five systematically offset time points [range: 0–1600 ms; mean interstimulus interval (ISI) = 5000 ms] in relation to fMRI acquisition to improve temporal resolution of the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response. Participants were instructed to rapidly and accurately classify faces according to gender (S–R mapping was counterbalanced across subjects). Faces were presented ipsilateral to the gender-mapped response side in half of all trials (compatible trials; COMP) and contralateral to the correct response side in the other (incompatible trials; INCO). Trials occurred in 12 blocks, each including 36 face presentations and 4 pseudorandomly interspersed null events (fixation only). Trial compatibility frequency varied in a blockwise manner: congruent blocks included 70% COMP/30% INCO trials, incongruent blocks had 30% COMP/70% INCO trials, and balanced blocks were made up of 50% COMP/50% INCO trials. Each block type occurred 4 times (in pseudorandomized order across subjects), resulting in equiprobable compatibility sequences across the experiment. To obtain an adequate number of errors for analyses of PEBAs, participants received individually determined performance feedback during brief pauses (10 s) between task blocks. Depending on previous block accuracy rates, verbal feedback encouraged the participant to either “speed up” in the following block (if <15% errors were made), “slow down” (if >25% errors were made), or “maintain performance quality.”
Behavioral data analysis.
We investigated PEBAs in reaction time (RT) and error rates after excluding the first trial of each block, as these trials carried no valid information regarding performance sequences. PES was calculated as the difference between mean RTs on correct trials immediately following errors (post-error) versus those following correct responses (correct). PERI was calculated as the difference between S–R compatibility effects (i.e., interference effects; INCO − COMP RT) on correct trials versus those on post-error trials. Previous studies have shown that PEBAs can vary as a function of conflict frequency (Ridderinkhof, 2002) and speed/accuracy instructions (Ullsperger and Szymanowski, 2004; Jentzsch and Leuthold, 2006). Exploratory analyses of the current data did not reveal clear evidence for such variation. However, we did not obtain enough post-error trials for each participant under each of the conflict frequency manipulations and performance feedback conditions to conduct legitimate analyses. Therefore, we focus on experiment-wise variation in PEBAs in the current study.
Image acquisition.
Imaging was conducted on a Siemens Trio MR system using a standard head coil. During both tasks, we acquired 28 axial slices positioned parallel to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure (AC–PC) plane (4 mm thickness; 1 mm gap) covering the whole brain using a single-shot gradient echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (Tr: 2000 ms; Te: 30 ms; flip angle: 90°; 64 × 64 pixel matrix; field of view: 192 mm) sensitive to BOLD contrast. Before the functional runs, 2D anatomical modified driven equilibrium Fourier transform (MDEFT) and EPI-T1 slices in plane with the functional data were collected.
Image analysis.
Images were first corrected for motion artifacts offline with the Siemens motion correction protocol. All further processing was performed with the software package Leipzig Image Processing and Statistical Inference Algorithms (Lohmann et al., 2001) (http://static.cbs.mpg.de/lipsia/). Slice-time acquisition differences were adjusted using a cubic-spline interpolation algorithm. Baseline signal drifts were removed using a 1/100 Hz temporal high-pass filter and spatial smoothing was applied using a Gaussian filter with 5.65 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM). To align the functional data slices with a 3D stereotactic coordinate reference system, a rigid linear registration with six degrees of freedom (three rotational and three translational) was performed. The rotational and translational parameters were acquired on the basis of the MDEFT and EPI-T1 slices to achieve an optimal match between these slices and an individual high-resolution 3D reference dataset [MDEFT volume with 160 slices and 1 mm slice thickness standardized to Talairach stereotactic space (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988)] that was acquired for each subject during a previous scanning session. The rotational and translational parameters were subsequently transformed by linear scaling to a standard size. The resulting parameters were then used to transform the functional slices using trilinear interpolation so that the functional slices were aligned with the coordinate system; generating output data with a spatial resolution of 3 × 3 × 3 mm (27 mm3).
Statistical analysis of both fMRI time series was based on a least-squares estimation using a general linear regression with prewhitening (Worsley et al., 2002) and performed separately for each participant on voxel-by-voxel basis. For the localizer task data, face, scrambled, and null epochs were each modeled with a boxcar function convolved with a Gaussian hemodynamic response function (HRF). A voxelwise one-sample t test (random-effects model) across single-subject face versus scrambled contrast images (i.e., estimates of raw score differences between the specified conditions) was used to identify the group average bilateral FFA (face > scrambled) and, for control purposes, scrambled-sensitive regions of extrastriate cortex (scrambled > face). These regions of interest (ROIs) were defined as voxels with Z-standardized t values >3.09 (p < 0.001) belonging to clusters that survived correction for multiple comparisons using a double-thresholding procedure (described in detail below) at an α level of 0.001. To provide a stronger test of post-error modulation of target processing during the Simon task and/or the variation thereof as a function of PEBAs, we also localized the FFA in each participant. Single-subject FFA ROIs were identified by searching each participant's face > scrambled Z-transformed contrast image for clusters (>3 voxels; 81 mm3) with Z values >2.33 (p < 0.01; uncorrected) within a 15 mm radius of the peak group average FFA coordinates.
We constructed the design matrix of the Simon task data to estimate the stimulus-locked, trial n − 1 accuracy-dependent hemodynamic response. Explanatory variables were trials with correct responses preceded by correct trials (correct), incorrect responses preceded by correct trials (error), and correct responses preceded by errors (post-error), respectively convolved with a gamma HRF and its first derivative. Onsets of other events (consecutive errors, omitted responses, and feedback between blocks) were included in the model as regressors of no interest. We additionally modeled each block type with a box-car function (convolved with a Gaussian HRF) to account for potential tonic adaptations. Two linear contrasts were calculated for each participant: error versus correct and post-error versus correct. We conducted two random-effects analyses of the resulting contrast images (one sample t tests) to establish brain regions generally involved in (1) error monitoring and (2) post-error processing in our task (i.e., independent of variability in PEBAs).
Behaviorally informed fMRI analyses.
Our motivating hypothesis stated that if PEBAs indeed reflect cognitive control mechanisms (Botvinick et al., 2001; Holroyd and Coles, 2002; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004; Ullsperger and von Cramon, 2004; Holroyd et al., 2005), then they should not only be associated with engagement of brain areas thought to execute control (e.g., LPFC) as previously demonstrated (Garavan et al., 2002; Kerns et al., 2004; Marco-Pallarés et al., 2008; West and Travers, 2008; Cavanagh et al., 2009), but also goal-directed modulation of activity in regions specialized in lower-level task-related processing. Further, we assumed that such modulation would vary according to how participants adjusted performance on those trials: generally (as indexed by PES) or specifically (as indexed by PERI). We addressed these hypotheses by incorporating individual degrees of PES and PERI (Fig. 2C, compare PES and PERI) as parametric covariates in second-level analyses of the individual post-error > correct contrast images. Thus, we could identify brain regions in which the post-error > correct BOLD signal correlated with individual differences in PEBAs. To explore the hypothesis that PEBAs are triggered by error-related pMFC activity (Botvinick et al., 2001; Holroyd and Coles, 2002; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004; Ullsperger and von Cramon, 2004; Yeung et al., 2004; Holroyd et al., 2005), we also conducted PES- and PERI-informed group analyses of the individual error > correct contrast maps. The PES covariate was calculated for each participant as the Z-standardized individual difference between mean post-error and post-correct RTs in relation to the individual mean RT on all correct trials [(post_error_RT − post_correct_RT)/mean_RT]. The PERI covariate was calculated as the Z-standardized individual difference between the mean Simon interference effect (INCO − COMP RT) on the average correct trial and the mean Simon effect on post-error trials in relation to the individual mean Simon effect on all correct trials [(post_correct_SIMONEFFECT − post_error_SIMONEFFECT)/mean_SIMONEFFECT].
One participant's standardized PERI score was >2.5 SDs from the group mean (z = 2.8) and could thus be considered an outlier. To control for undue influence of this participant's data, we conducted all analyses that investigated PERI both under retention and exclusion of this participant's data. Following outlier removal, no significant effects evident in the entire sample became insignificant. Therefore, unless noted otherwise, we report results based on the data of all 21 participants.
To minimize the likelihood of false-positive results in the Simon task data, an initial voxelwise probability threshold was set to Z > 2.58 (p < 0.005; uncorrected). The voxelwise thresholded data were then corrected for multiple comparisons by using cluster-size and mean cluster-value thresholds at significance level of p < 0.05 as determined by Monte Carlo simulations (1000 iterations), unless noted otherwise.
ROI analyses.
To further specify the hemodynamic response in selected a priori and empirically defined ROIs, we sampled the preprocessed stimulus-locked BOLD signal time course for each participant on trials of interest from a 27 voxel cube (729 mm3) centered on group average- and subject-specific (in the FFA) activation maxima, unless noted otherwise. In each region, percentage BOLD signal change was computed for each condition relative to the mean signal intensity across all time steps over the course of the experiment. We determined the maximum signal amplitude occurring in the time window between 3 and 7 s following stimulation for each participant and submitted the resulting values to appropriate region- and condition-specific statistical analyses.
Results
Behavioral results
Overall Simon task performance was characterized by typical interference effects (Kornblum et al., 1990; Simon, 1990; Lu and Proctor, 1995; Proctor and Vu, 2006). Mean RTs for correct responses (excluding correct post-error trials) were slower on INCO trials (532 ms) than on COMP trials (496 ms; t(20) = 10.8; p < 0.0001). Likewise, mean INCO trial error rates (20.4%) were elevated relative to COMP trial error rates (12.2%; t(20) = 6.6; p < 0.0001).
The interference effects in error rates (INCO − COMP accuracy), together with the observation that error trial RTs (498 ms) were faster than correct responses (513 ms; t(20) = 2.6; p < 0.05), suggested that errors were primarily due to insufficient suppression of location-driven “direct” response activation. However, the relatively high COMP trial error rates, in conjunction with the finding that RTs on these trials (525 ms) were slower than erroneous responses on INCO trials (480 ms; t(20) = 4.2; p < 0.0001), implied that a portion of errors may have been due to difficulties in deciphering face gender. Regardless of the factors contributing to an error on a given trial, our interest was in how participants might adjust behavior on immediately subsequent trials.
Mean post-error RTs (524 ms) were generally slower than the average correct response in successful performance sequences (513 ms; t(20) = 2.2; p < 0.05) (Fig. 2A), constituting PES. Signifying PERI, mean interference effects (INCO − COMP RT) were selectively reduced on post-error trials relative to those following correct responses (24 ms vs 36 ms; t(20) = 2.5; p < 0.05) (Fig. 2B). The only known study to investigate relationships between PES and PERI found that participants who tended to show PES also tended to have larger post-error interference effects (i.e., less PERI) (Carp and Compton, 2009). Here, however, consistent with the proposal that PES and PERI may reflect distinct forms of PEBAs (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004), we observed no relationship between the two measures across participants (r = −0.142; p = 0.54). In other words, participants who showed a high degree of PES did not necessarily show a high or low degree of PERI or vice versa (Fig. 2C).
Although error rates did not change on post-error trials [mean error rate on post-error trials (17.6%) versus error rate following correct trials (16.3%); t(20) = 1.3; p > 0.1], we were interested in whether either form of PEBA might be associated with improved accuracy on post-error trials (Rabbitt, 1966; Laming, 1968; Botvinick et al., 2001; Marco-Pallarés et al., 2008). To this end, we calculated the correlation between (1) the individual difference in error rates following correct trials versus those following errors and (2) the respective individual PES and PERI values. Results indicated that PES was not associated with any change in accuracy on post-error trials (r = 0.1; p > 0.5), but participants who showed greater PERI also showed a tendency to repeat their errors (r = −0.34; p > 0.09). However, this trend diminished under exclusion of the participant whose PERI value could have been considered an outlier (r = −0.28; p = 0.23) (Fig. 2C, participant 7; see also Materials and Methods). Thus, it is unlikely that the post-error RT adjustments investigated here reflected a shift in performance favoring accuracy over speed or vice versa.
fMRI results: error monitoring
An initial analysis of the fMRI data established an expected pattern of error > correct pMFC activation (peak Talairach coordinates: x = 1, y = 17, z = 42; 11,313 mm3; Zmean = 3.43) in our task. Also consistent with previous fMRI investigations of error processing [for review, see Hester et al. (2004), Klein et al. (2007), and Taylor et al. (2007)], further error-related activation was evident in the bilateral anterior insula/frontal operculum (aI/fO) and the right LPFC, specifically in the inferior frontal junction (IFJ) (Fig. 3). A summary of activations, including those evident in the reverse correct > error contrast, can be found in Table 1.
Table 1.
Anatomical area | Hemisphere | x | y | z | Zmax |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Error > correct | |||||
pMFC | L/R | 1 | 17 | 42 | 5.34 |
IFJ | R | 37 | 2 | 39 | 3.48 |
aI/fO | L | −35 | 20 | 6 | 5.21 |
aI/fO | R | 37 | 23 | 3 | 4.55 |
Correct > error | |||||
Paracentral lobule | L | −20 | −37 | 60 | −3.14 |
Precentral gyrus/central sulcus | R | 43 | 28 | 57 | −3.49 |
Precentral gyrus/central sulcus | L | −44 | −19 | 54 | −2.88 |
Precuneus | R | 16 | −73 | 51 | −3.93 |
Superior frontal gyrus | L | −20 | 41 | 42 | −4.11 |
Posterior cingulate gyrus | R | 10 | −25 | 39 | −3.54 |
Posterior cingulate gyrus | L | −5 | −37 | 39 | −3.73 |
Superior occipital gyrus/cuneus** | R | 22 | −79 | 27 | −3.78 |
Dorsal striatum* | L | −18 | 2 | 24 | −3.99 |
Dorsal striatum* | R | 13 | −1 | 24 | −4.02 |
Thalamus (anterior) | L/R | −5 | −12 | 18 | −3.83 |
Posterior cingulate gyrus/precuneus | L | −11 | −52 | 18 | −5.07 |
Putamen* | L | −23 | −7 | 12 | −4.10 |
Superior frontal gyrus | L | −23 | 62 | 9 | −4.01 |
Superior temporal gyrus | R | 55 | −13 | 9 | −3.73 |
Superior temporal gyrus | L | −40 | −40 | 9 | −4.43 |
Caudate nucleus* | R | 10 | 14 | 6 | −5.28 |
Middle occipital gyrus** | R | 43 | −73 | 6 | −4.11 |
Middle occipital gyrus** | R | 34 | −88 | 6 | −4.15 |
Superior temporal gyrus | L | −53 | −10 | 3 | −3.42 |
Perigenual cingulate | R | 13 | 38 | 6 | −3.51 |
Perigenual cingulate/gyrus rectus | L | −8 | 41 | −6 | −4.62 |
Ventral striatum* | L | −18 | 2 | −6 | −4.81 |
Ventral striatum* | R | 16 | 0 | −6 | −4.84 |
Posterior fusiform gyrus** | R | 34 | −70 | −6 | −3.84 |
Hippocampus* | L | −23 | −19 | −9 | −4.58 |
Posterior fusiform gyrus** | L | −29 | −79 | −9 | −4.31 |
Hippocampus* | R | 25 | −16 | −12 | −4.21 |
Cerebellum** | L/R | −2 | −70 | −15 | −4.47 |
Talairach coordinates correspond to activation peaks of independent clusters (Zmean ≥ 2.58) individually composed of ≥23 voxels (623 mm3; p < 0.05; corrected) and separated by a minimum 15 mm radius. Asterisk indicate clusters within greater activations that exceeded 20,000 mm3 (741 voxels):
*continuous subcortical cluster,
**continuous posterior cluster. R, Right; L, left.
A number of studies have found relationships between error-related pMFC activity and both PES (Gehring et al., 1993; Kerns et al., 2004; Debener et al., 2005; Klein et al., 2007; Cavanagh et al., 2009) and PERI (de Bruijn et al., 2004; Ridderinkhof et al., 2002), but findings have been inconsistent (Gehring and Fencsik, 2001; Carp and Compton, 2009; Núñez Castellar et al., 2010), most notably in patients with pMFC damage (Fellows and Farah, 2005). To explore whether error-related activity in this region was associated with post-error behavior in our task, we conducted group analyses of the individual error > correct activation patterns in the empirically defined error-sensitive pMFC (Fig. 3, Table 1) informed with the individual PES and PERI covariates (Materials and Methods). Results revealed a relationship between error-related hemodynamic activity in the rostral cingulate zone (RCZ) and PES and tentative evidence that the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) may mediate PERI (supplemental Fig. S1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).
Post-error control adjustments
Currently influential views of performance monitoring and reinforcement learning propose that the functional significance of error-related pMFC activity is to guide cognitive control mechanisms needed to reinstate goal-directed behavior (Botvinick et al., 2001; Holroyd and Coles, 2002; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004; Ullsperger and von Cramon, 2004; Yeung et al., 2004; Brown and Braver, 2005; Holroyd et al., 2005). In line with these hypotheses and previous investigations of PEBAs (Garavan et al., 2002; Kerns et al., 2004; Hester et al., 2007; Marco-Pallarés et al., 2008; West and Travers, 2008; Cavanagh et al., 2009), an analysis of post-error > correct BOLD activity revealed elevated activity in regions commonly associated with cognitive control and theoretically conducive to restoration of successful performance in our task. Specifically, increased activation was evident in the left anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC) (x = −35, y = 53, z = 15; 675 mm3, Zmean = 2.90), implicated for example in voluntary reallocation of attention (Pollmann, 2004), and the right inferior parietal lobule (IPL) (x = 37, y = −55, z = 45; 1215 mm3, Zmean = 2.91) (Fig. 4), recently shown to be involved in early visuomotor integration in the Simon task (Stürmer et al., 2007). Although these findings suggest increased cognitive control following errors, demonstration of control adjustments also requires that neural activity in brain regions specialized in processing information crucial to meeting task-specific demands is biased in a goal-directed manner (Miller and Cohen, 2001).
Given the demands of our task, we expected that control adjustments following errors would serve to either (1) suppress inappropriate response activation primed by irrelevant spatial stimulus features (Praamstra et al., 1999; Stürmer et al., 2002; Stürmer and Leuthold, 2003) and/or (2) enhance sensory processing of relevant face features. Indicative of increased control over response-related processing following errors, the reverse correct > post-error contrast revealed reduced post-error activity in medial motor structures extending from the supplementary motor area (SMA; local peak: x = −8, y = −4, z = 54; Zmax = −4.61) to the paracentral lobule (x = −5, y = −34, z = 57; 5931 mm3; Zmean = −3.10) and, most notably, bilateral regions of SMC (left, x = −50, y = −19, z = 45; 1782 mm3; Zmean = −2.93; right, x = 43, y = −16, z = 48; 3672 mm3; Zmean = −3.09) (Fig. 4). Although it is unclear whether suppressed post-error SMA activity reflected control over eye movements, index finger responses, or another more complex condition–action association attributed to this region (cf. Nachev et al., 2008), the lateral SMC clusters, which corresponded to the motor hand areas, are suggestive of inhibitory control (Aron, 2007) over potentially inappropriate manual responses (i.e., increased motor response threshold). All activations evident in the post-error versus correct analyses are summarized in Table 2.
Table 2.
Anatomical area | Hemisphere | x | y | z | Zmax |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Post-error > correct | |||||
IPL | L | 37 | −55 | 45 | 3.43 |
aPFC | L | −35 | 53 | 15 | 3.41 |
Correct > post-error | |||||
Paracentral lobule* | L/R | −5 | −34 | 57 | −4.62 |
SMA* | L/R | −8 | −4 | 54 | −4.61 |
Postcentral gyrus/central sulcus | R | 31 | −31 | 54 | −4.03 |
Precentral gyrus/central sulcus | R | 43 | −16 | 48 | −4.75 |
Precentral gyrus/central sulcus | L | −50 | −19 | 45 | −3.54 |
Mid-insula | R | 34 | −4 | 21 | −4.47 |
Parieto-occipital sulcus | R | 19 | −67 | 21 | −3.51 |
Postcentral gyrus | R | 61 | −21 | 18 | −4.37 |
Middle occipital gyrus | L | −35 | −88 | 18 | −3.99 |
Putamen (posterior) | L | −29 | −13 | 9 | −3.27 |
Superior temporal gyrus | L | −47 | −28 | 9 | −4.17 |
Middle occipital gyrus | L | −41 | −73 | 6 | −3.93 |
Putamen (anterior) | L | −32 | 5 | 3 | −3.36 |
Mid-insula | R | 40 | −4 | 3 | −3.46 |
Perigenual cingulate | L/R | −11 | 29 | −3 | −3.61 |
Middle occipital gyrus | L | −53 | −64 | −6 | −3.19 |
Posterior fusiform gyrus | L | −20 | −82 | −6 | −4.13 |
Hippocampus | L | −29 | −16 | −9 | −3.58 |
Talairach coordinates correspond to activation peaks of independent clusters (Zmean ≥ 2.58) individually composed of ≥20 voxels (540 mm3; p < 0.05; corrected) and separated by a minimum 15 mm radius.
*Clusters within greater activations that exceeded 5000 mm3 (185 voxels). R, Right; L, left.
To test whether post-error control adjustments operated to bias sensory processing in favor of task-relevant information, we submitted the BOLD signal change data recorded in the individually defined FFA ROIs (Materials and Methods) (Fig. 5A) to 2 (trial n − 1 accuracy) × 2 (trial n compatibility) repeated-measures ANOVAs. Thus, we could control for the possibility that the hypothesized post-error versus correct differences in target-feature processing were qualified by variation in activation attributable to cognitive control dynamics elicited by conflict processing (Botvinick et al., 2001; Egner and Hirsch, 2005). As predicted, FFA activation was elevated on post-error trials in both the left (F(1,20) = 5.9; p = 0.03) and right (F(1,20) = 7.0; p = 0.02) hemispheres (Fig. 5B). Although neither ROI showed sensitivity to S–R compatibility (both F(1,20) < 1.8; both p > 0.2) and no interactions were present (both F(1,20) < 1.1; both p > 0.3), it is important to note that FFA activation was elevated already on error versus correct trials, but only in the right hemisphere (t(20) = 2.8; p = 0.01) and not in the left (t(20) = 0.7; p > 0.5). One possible explanation for increased FFA activity on error trials that is consistent with recent electrophysiological data (Cohen et al., 2009) is that online control adjustments redirect attention to relevant information during or immediately following response execution, but do not produce sensory representations strong enough cancel the ongoing incorrect response.
Additional analyses of FFA activity excluded the likelihood that increased post-error activation was driven by bottom-up effects attributable to a neural preference for faces presented in the contralateral visual field (Hemond et al., 2007) (supplemental Fig. S2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Furthermore, indicating that post-error modulation of sensory processing was not likely a generic effect in visual cortex, control analyses in empirically defined regions of the ventral visual stream (Materials and Methods) and location-sensitive early visual cortex revealed no activation differences on post-error versus correct trials (supplemental Fig. S2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Interestingly, however, although we did not find clear evidence of distractor-feature inhibition in sensory cortex (Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000; Polk et al., 2008; Weissman et al., 2009) following errors, we uncovered significant post-error reductions in lateralization of BOLD responses driven by task-irrelevant spatial information in both hemispheres of location-sensitive early visual cortex (supplemental Fig. S2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).
Individual differences in PEBAs explain post-error motor suppression and sensory amplification
We predicted that post-error control adjustments would not only be associated with modulation of lower-level task-related motor and/or sensory processing, but that such modulation would depend on how participants adjusted performance following errors. To address this hypothesis, we incorporated individual degrees of PES and PERI as parametric covariates in group analyses of the individual post-error > correct activation patterns (Materials and Methods). Only one region displayed post-error activity positively related to the degree of PES: the right IFJ, partially corresponding to the same area activated on error > correct trials (x = 29, y = 2, z = 27; 513 mm3; Zmean = −3.12) (Fig. 6A; see also Fig. 3). In contrast, negative correlations between PES and post-error activity were evident in the same region of the left SMC identified by the overall correct > post-error analysis (x = −41, y = −16, z = 51; 567 mm3; Zmean = −3.13) and somewhat more superior and posterior bilateral SMC regions (left: x = −26, y = −34, z = 57; 1215 mm3; Zmean = −2.92; right: x = 19, y = −25, z = 60; 621 mm3; Zmean = −2.93) (Fig. 6A; see also Fig. 4). Thus, generally suppressed post-error SMC activity (Fig. 4) could be explained, at least partially, by greater PES. Equally important, negative correlations were also evident in ventral occipitotemporal cortex with significant voxels located within the bilateral group mean FFA (local peak left: x = −41, y = −43, z = −12; Zmax = −3.33; right: x = 37, y = −49, z = −12; Zmax = −2.94) (Fig. 6A). Although the negative relationship between PES and post-error activity was not clearly reflected in the BOLD percentage signal change data recorded in the individually defined FFA ROIs, correlations were negative in both the left (r = −0.41; p = 0.06) and right (r = −0.34; p = 0.13) hemispheres. These particular findings seem to suggest that the more participants slowed down on post-error trials, the less they attended to faces. It is important to note, however, that a negative correlation between greater PES and post-error FFA activity can also indicate a positive correlation between that and less PES (i.e., maintaining/improving response speed following errors). Indeed, inspection of FFA activation based on a median split of post-error “slowers” and post-error “speeders” showed that FFA activity was notably increased on post-error trials in participants who tended to generally maintain/improve RTs following errors, but only negligibly modulated in those who tended to slow down. In contrast, consistent with the notion that PES may reflect an increase in the motor response threshold (Botvinick et al., 2001; Holroyd et al., 2005), post-error SMC activity was reduced in post-error “slowers,” but not modulated in “speeders” (Fig. 6B). A complete list of PES-dependent post-error > correct activation patterns is presented in Table 3.
Table 3.
Anatomical area | Hemisphere | x | y | z | Zmax |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Inferior frontal sulcus/IFJ | R | 29 | 2 | 27 | 3.83 |
Precentral gyrus/central sulcus | R | 19 | −25 | 60 | −3.55 |
Postcentral gyrus/central sulcus/precentral gyrus | L | −26 | −34 | 57 | −3.48 |
Precentral gyrus/central sulcus | L | −41 | −16 | 51 | −3.69 |
Precentral gyrus | L | −59 | −13 | 30 | −3.65 |
Posterior insula | L | −35 | −19 | 21 | −3.32 |
Mid-insula | R | 31 | −7 | 15 | −4.01 |
Superior temporal gyrus** | L | −41 | −37 | 15 | −3.30 |
Superior temporal sulcus | R | 34 | −49 | 12 | −3.48 |
Calcarine sulcus | L/R | 4 | −85 | 12 | −3.71 |
Thalamus (dorsal) | R | 4 | −22 | 9 | −4.02 |
Mid-insula | L | −38 | 5 | −3 | −4.51 |
Collateral sulcus/fusiform gyrus | L | −29 | −49 | −9 | −4.00 |
Inferior temporal/fusiform gyrus | L | −50 | −37 | −12 | −3.63 |
FFA* | L | −41 | −43 | −12 | −3.33 |
Collateral sulcus/fusiform gyrus | R | 19 | −43 | −12 | −3.90 |
FFA* | L | 37 | −49 | −12 | −2.94 |
Cerebellum** | L/R | −5 | −67 | −15 | −3.61 |
Cerebellum** | L | −23 | −64 | −15 | −3.59 |
Cerebellum** | R | 22 | −67 | −15 | −3.32 |
Midbrain/amygdala | L | −11 | −22 | −15 | −4.20 |
Amygdala | R | 22 | −7 | −18 | −3.58 |
Talairach coordinates correspond to activation peaks of independent clusters (Zmean ≥ 2.58) individually comprised of ≥18 voxels (486 mm3; p < 0.05; corrected) and separated by a minimum 15 mm radius.
*Local maximum within the group average FFA volume.
**Clusters within greater activations that exceeded 3000 mm3 (111 voxels). R, Right; L, left.
An analog analysis of the individual post-error > correct activation patterns informed with individual differences in the ability to reduce interference following errors (PERI) revealed positive correlations in the left LPFC, specifically in the superior frontal sulcus (SFS; x = −14, y = 35, z = 42; 702 mm3; Zmean = 2.94), the left superior colliculus (SC; x = −8, y = 31, z = 0; 567 mm3; Zmean = 2.94), and, most importantly, a region of the left fusiform gyrus (x = −35, y = −43, z = −6; 1053 mm3; Zmean = 2.90) that partially overlapped the group average FFA volume (local peak: x = −41, y = −49, z = −10; Zmax = 2.78) (Fig. 7A). Positive correlations were also evident in the group average right FFA, but only at a less conservative significance threshold (x = 34, y = −46, z = −12; 216 mm3; Zmean = 2.54; p < 0.01; uncorrected). Supporting the findings in the FFA, positive relationships between greater degrees of PERI and the post-error BOLD signal change data were also reflected in both the individually defined left (r = 0.44; p = 0.04) and right (r = 0.65; p = 0.001) FFA (Fig. 7B). No regions showing negative correlations with individual differences in PERI reached significance.
Discussion
We used fMRI to investigate the hypothesis that PEBAs index adjustments in cognitive control (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004). Replicating previous investigations of PEBAs (Garavan et al., 2002; Kerns et al., 2004; Marco-Pallarés et al., 2008; Cavanagh et al., 2009), we found a characteristic pattern of error-related activation including the pMFC and post-error increases in activity in prefrontal and parietal control regions. Building on these studies, we showed that post-error control adjustments can bias lower-level task-related processing in a goal-directed manner. Specifically, in a task that required selective attention to gender features of faces while overriding response activation primed by task-irrelevant face location, we found both (1) suppression of potentially inappropriate motor responses (as reflected in decreased SMC activation) and (2) amplification of task-relevant sensory processing (as reflected in increased FFA activity). More importantly, we found that post-error modulation of motor and sensory processing was dependent on how participants adjusted performance on those trials. While participants who tended to show general response slowing following errors (PES) explained suppression of response-related SMC activity, those who tended to show a selective improvement in interference resolution (PERI) and/or maintain/improve response speed on post-error trials explained enhancement of target processing in the FFA. Together, these findings significantly advance our understanding of the neural mechanisms by which performance is adapted following errors.
Post-error slowing
PES is frequently thought to index a strategic control mechanism that prevents future errors by suppressing motor response activation (i.e., increasing the response threshold) (Botvinick et al., 2001; Holroyd et al., 2005). The negative correlation between PES and SMC activity revealed here delivers novel support for this hypothesis. However, although PES occasionally coincides with improved accuracy following errors (Rabbitt, 1966; Laming, 1968; Marco-Pallarés et al., 2008), we found no such relationship and several studies have even reported decreased post-error accuracy (Rabbitt and Rodgers, 1977; Laming, 1979; Hajcak and Simons, 2008). Therefore, alternative interpretations may need to be considered.
It has been suggested that PES may reflect persistence of a breakdown in processing that contributed to the error (Gehring and Fencsik, 2001) or error-evoked arousal that interferes with task preparation (Carp and Compton, 2009). A recent proposal explains PES as the consequence of an unspecific orienting response elicited by unexpected infrequent events (Notebaert et al., 2009) that “turns attention away from the task” and “delays processing of the following stimulus” (Núñez Castellar et al., 2010). Generally in line with these views, participants who showed greater PES in our task showed no noteworthy modulation of target processing in the FFA on post-error trials. Further, PES was associated with post-error activation of the same region of the right LPFC that was already activated on error trials: the IFJ, a region implicated in transient task-set updating (Brass et al., 2005; Derrfuss et al., 2005). Given the proximity of the IFJ to inferior frontal regions implicated in response inhibition (Aron et al., 2004) and premotor regions shown to mediate control over “direct” response activation in the Simon task (Praamstra et al., 1999; Egner et al., 2007), it is tempting to speculate a causal relationship between PES-dependent IFJ activation and suppression of SMC activity in our task. However, although the IFJ is often recruited in response inhibition tasks, its function in such contexts may be more related to an orienting response triggered by infrequent “No-Go” trials than to motor control per se (Chikazoe et al., 2009). Overall, these results suggest that PES may not have reflected deliberate response cautiousness directed at avoiding errors in our task, but rather the consequence of an orienting response that interferes with task preparation and stimulus processing, resulting in the need to suppress potentially incorrect responses activated by irrelevant spatial stimulus features when attention fails to bias sensory processing in favor of relevant features.
Post-error reduction of interference
PERI has been proposed to reflect a “selective suppression of response activation”—a specific inhibitory control mechanism (Ridderinkhof, 2002). Based on this hypothesis and previous findings that behavioral adaptation in the Simon task is achieved by biasing response channels (Stürmer et al., 2002) rather than sensory pathways (Stürmer and Leuthold, 2003), one might expect that PERI would be expressed as motor control in our task. Instead, we found improved interference resolution following errors to be associated with enhancement of task-relevant sensory processing in the FFA. Although enhanced target processing may have been achieved by rapid reallocation of attentional resources following stimulus presentation on the post-error trials, it seems more plausible, given the brief duration of stimulus presentation (150 ms), that PERI-dependent FFA modulation was driven primarily by prestimulus biasing of sensory pathways. This view implies that participants who showed greater PERI maintained relevant representations during the post-error ISI. One possibility is that the region of the left LPFC found to mediate PERI (SFS) held relevant representations online following errors, thereby enabling efficient target selection upon stimulus presentation. Supporting this interpretation, a recent transcranial magnetic stimulation study using a Simon task showed that between-trial behavioral adaptation was impaired by interruption of left LPFC processing in anticipation of the upcoming stimulus (Stürmer et al., 2007). Nonetheless, given that spatial attention to irrelevant spatial stimulus features was more or less imperative in our task, some degree of inhibitory control may have been required to reduce interference following errors. We speculate that PERI-dependent activation of the SC, a region known to exert control over eye movements, may have reflected covert spatial attention (Müller et al., 2005)—that is, withholding saccades toward the source of stimulation, thereby facilitating response selection by minimizing the weight attention assigns to irrelevant spatial features.
PES and PERI: dual control mechanisms?
The neural architecture(s) supporting sequential behavioral adaptation to conflicts in information processing [of which errors are thought to be a special case (Yeung et al., 2004)] have been suggested to be determined by task-specific demands on lower-level processing (Egner, 2007, 2008; Egner et al., 2007). However, the dissociations between PES and PERI revealed here seem to speak to an alternative hypothesis—namely, that cognitive control may be primarily determined by when, and consequently how, it is executed by the individual when adjustments are needed: reactively or proactively (De Pisapia and Braver, 2006; Braver et al., 2007). According to this view, cognitive control varies depending on whether it is “reactive” in nature, directed at resolving interference ex post facto in a transient manner, or “proactive,” focused on preventing interference ex ante facto in a preparatory fashion. Importantly, whereas the distinction of Braver et al. (2007) between dual control mechanisms is based largely on observations on a short- (e.g., event-related) versus long- (e.g., block-related) term time scale, our findings are more consistent with the conceptualization of Ridderinkhof et al. (2010) of reactive within-trial “online action control” and proactive between-trial “anticipatory regulation of online action control.” As noted above, PES seems to reflect an orienting response that impairs task preparation, necessitating a reactive “late correction” control mechanism following stimulus presentation on post-error trials—that is, after irrelevant information has begun to interfere with response selection. In contrast, PERI seems to index a proactive control mechanism characterized by preparatory task-set maintenance that enables “early selection” of relevant information, thereby reducing the influence of irrelevant information on response selection.
Critically, given the poor temporal resolution of the BOLD response, future research is needed to validate the interpretation that PES and PERI may differentially index “late correction” and “early selection” control mechanisms. Further, it should be emphasized that this interpretation is based solely on our observations of between-subjects variation in PEBAs in our particular task. Within subjects, it is conceivable that some errors will elicit an orienting response that allows the individual to be responsive to any potentially relevant stimulus, but others might trigger a proactive control mode that selectively primes relevant representations. An intriguing possibility is that different types of conflict differentially evoke reactive and proactive control adjustments. In any event, the finding that sequential behavioral adaptation in the Simon task can occur not only by biasing response channels (Stürmer et al., 2002; Egner et al., 2007), but also by biasing sensory pathways, supports the notion that the effects of cognitive control on task-related processing may not be determined primarily by task-specific demands (Egner et al., 2007; Egner, 2008), but can vary dramatically depending on how it is executed (Braver et al., 2007).
An important question is how adjustments such as those investigated here might be initiated. We found tentative evidence that distinct error-sensitive pMFC regions were differentially related to PES (RCZ) and PERI (pre-SMA). Although these relationships were not clear-cut, it is interesting to note that the precise constellation of our observations is consistent with meta-analytic data (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004), theoretical considerations (Rushworth, 2008; Hikosaka and Isoda, 2010), and recent empirical findings (Orr and Weissman, 2009) that suggest that the role of the RCZ in cognitive control may be more global in nature, while that of the pre-SMA may be more task specific.
Conclusions
In sum, the current study confirms relationships between PEBAs and cognitive control—that is, goal-directed modulation of lower-level task-related processing. Our data suggest that PES reflects an orienting response that necessitates a reactive, online control mechanism to suppress the influence of irrelevant information on motor output, while PERI seems to index a proactive control mechanism that selectively primes processing of relevant sensory input in a preparatory manner. To validate this interpretation, we are currently collecting electroencephalographic data with the same task. Further avenues of investigation might include determining whether the results obtained here generalize to other tasks and what subject and task variables might promote general versus specific adaptation effects.
Footnotes
The assistance of J. Lepsien, G. Lohmann, K. Müller, J. Neumann, and S. Zysset in data analysis is greatly acknowledged. We also thank H. Schmidt-Duderstedt for assistance in preparing the figures.
References
- Aron AR. The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist. 2007;13:214–228. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev. 2001;108:624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brass M, Derrfuss J, Forstmann B, von Cramon DY. The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:314–316. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braver TS, Gray JR, Burgess GC. Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In: Conway ARA, Jarrold C, Kane MJ, Miyake A, Towse JN, editors. Variation in working memory. Oxford: Oxford UP; 2007. pp. 76–106. [Google Scholar]
- Brown JW, Braver TS. Learned predictions of error likelihood in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science. 2005;307:1118–1121. doi: 10.1126/science.1105783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carp J, Compton RJ. Alpha power is influenced by performance errors. Psychophysiology. 2009;46:336–343. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00773.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter CS, van Veen V. Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: an update of theory and data. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7:367–379. doi: 10.3758/cabn.7.4.367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanagh JF, Cohen MX, Allen JJ. Prelude to and resolution of an error: EEG phase synchrony reveals cognitive control dynamics during action monitoring. J Neurosci. 2009;29:98–105. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4137-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chikazoe J, Jimura K, Asari T, Yamashita K, Morimoto H, Hirose S, Miyashita Y, Konishi S. Functional dissociation in right inferior frontal cortex during performance of go/no-go task. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:146–252. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MX, van Gaal S, Ridderinkhof KR, Lamme VAF. Unconscious errors enhance prefrontal-occipital oscillatory synchrony. Front Hum Neurosci. 2009;3:54. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.054.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Debener S, Ullsperger M, Siegel M, Fiehler K, von Cramon DY, Engel AK. Trial-by-trial coupling of concurrent electroencephalogram and functional magnetic resonance imaging identifies the dynamics of performance monitoring. J Neurosci. 2005;25:11730–11737. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3286-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bruijn ERA, Hulstijn W, Verkes RJ, Ruigt GSF, Sabbe BGC. Drug-induced stimulation and suppression of action monitoring in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2004;177:151–160. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1915-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Pisapia N, Braver TS. A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions. Neurocomputing. 2006;69:1322–1326. [Google Scholar]
- Derrfuss J, Brass M, Neumann J, von Cramon DY. Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:22–34. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egner T. Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7:380–390. doi: 10.3758/cabn.7.4.380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egner T. Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:374–380. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egner T, Hirsch J. Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1784–1790. doi: 10.1038/nn1594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egner T, Delano M, Hirsch J. Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain. Neuroimage. 2007;35:940–948. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fellows LK, Farah MJ. Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control? Brain. 2005;128:788–796. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garavan H, Ross TJ, Murphy K, Roche RA, Stein EA. Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: inhibition, error detection, and correction. Neuroimage. 2002;17:1820–1829. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gehring WJ, Fencsik DE. Functions of the medial prefrontal cortex in the processing of conflict and errors. J Neurosci. 2001;21:9430–9437. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-23-09430.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gehring WJ, Goss B, Coles MGH, Meyer DE, Donchin E. A neural system for error-detection and compensation. Psychol Sci. 1993;4:385–390. [Google Scholar]
- Hajcak G, Simons RF. Oops, I did it again: an ERP investigation of double-errors and action monitoring. Brain Cogn. 2008;68:15–21. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.02.118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hemond CC, Kanwisher NG, Op de Beeck HP. A preference for contralateral stimuli in human object- and face-selective cortex. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e574. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hester R, Fassbender C, Garavan H. Individual differences in error processing: a review and reanalysis of three event-related fMRI studies using the GO/NOGO task. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:986–994. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hester R, Barre N, Mattingley JB, Foxe JJ, Garavan H. Avoiding another mistake: error and posterror neural activity associated with adaptive posterror behavior change. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7:317–326. doi: 10.3758/cabn.7.4.317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hikosaka O, Isoda M. Switching from automatic to controlled behavior: cortico-basal ganglia mechanisms. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:154–161. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd CB, Coles MG. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev. 2002;109:679–709. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd CB, Yeung N, Coles MG, Cohen JD. A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2005;134:163–191. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jäger T, Seiler KH, Mecklinger A. Picture database of morphed faces (MoFa): technical report. 2005. Psydoc Online. Retrieved June 26, 2010 from http://scidok.sulb.uni-saarland.de/volltexte/2005/453/
- Jentzsch I, Leuthold H. Control over speeded actions: a common processing locus for micro- and macro-tradeoff effects? Q J Exp Psychol (Colchester) 2006;59:1329–1337. doi: 10.1080/17470210600674394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci. 1997;17:4302–4311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kastner S, Ungerleider L. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:315–341. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, 3rd, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303:1023–1026. doi: 10.1126/science.1089910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein TA, Endrass T, Kathmann N, Neumann J, von Cramon DY, Ullsperger M. Neural correlates of error awareness. Neuroimage. 2007;34:1774–1781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kornblum S, Hasbroucq T, Osman A. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility-a model and taxonomy. Psychol Rev. 1990;97:253–270. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.2.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laming D. Choice reaction performance following an error. Acta Psychol. 1979;43:199–224. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(79)90032-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laming DR. Information theory of choice-reaction times. London: Academic; 1968. [Google Scholar]
- Lohmann G, Müller K, Bosch V, Mentzel H, Hessler S, Chen L, Zysset S, von Cramon DY. LIPSIA-a new software system for the evaluation of functional magnetic resonance images of the human brain. Comput Med Imaging Graph. 2001;25:449–457. doi: 10.1016/s0895-6111(01)00008-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu CH, Proctor RW. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance—a review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychon Bull Rev. 1995;2:174–207. doi: 10.3758/BF03210959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marco-Pallarés J, Camara E, Münte TF, Rodríguez-Fornells A. Neural mechanisms underlying adaptive actions after slips. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20:1595–1610. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller JR, Philiastides MG, Newsome WT. Microstimulation of the superior colliculus focuses attention without moving the eyes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:524–529. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0408311101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nachev P, Kennard C, Husain M. Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:856–869. doi: 10.1038/nrn2478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Notebaert W, Houtman F, Opstal FV, Gevers W, Fias W, Verguts T. Post-error slowing: an orienting account. Cognition. 2009;111:275–279. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Núñez Castellar E, Kühn S, Fias W, Notebaert W. Outcome expectancy and not accuracy determines posterror slowing: ERP support. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2010;10:270–278. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.2.270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orr JM, Weissman DH. Anterior cingulate cortex makes 2 contributions to minimizing distraction. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:703–711. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polk TA, Drake RM, Jonides JJ, Smith MR, Smith EE. Attention enhances the neural processing of relevant features and suppresses the processing of irrelevant features in humans: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the Stroop task. J Neurosci. 2008;28:13786–13792. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1026-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollmann S. Anterior prefrontal cortex contributions to attention control. Exp Psychol. 2004;51:270–278. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.51.4.270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Praamstra P, Kleine BU, Schnitzler A. Magnetic stimulation of the dorsal premotor cortex modulates the Simon effect. Neuroreport. 1999;10:3671–3674. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199911260-00038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Proctor RW, Vu K-PL. Stimulus-response compatibility: data, theory, and application. Boca Raton, FL: CRC; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Rabbitt P, Rodgers B. What does a man do after he makes an error—an analysis of response programming. Q J Exp Psychol. 1977;29:727–743. [Google Scholar]
- Rabbitt PM. Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. J Exp Psychol. 1966;71:264–272. doi: 10.1037/h0022853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof KR. Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychol Res. 2002;66:312–323. doi: 10.1007/s00426-002-0104-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof KR, de Vlugt Y, Bramlage A, Spaan M, Elton M, Snel J, Band GPH. Alcohol consumption impairs detection of performance errors in mediofrontal cortex. Science. 2002;298:2209–2211. doi: 10.1126/science.1076929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2004;306:443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof KR, Forstmann BU, Wylie SA, Burle B, van den Wildenberg WPM. Neurocognitive mechanisms of action control: resisting the call of the sirens. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Neurosci. 2010 doi: 10.1002/wcs.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushworth MF. Intention, choice, and the medial frontal cortex. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:181–207. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushworth MF, Buckley MJ, Behrens TE, Walton ME, Bannerman DM. Functional organization of the medial frontal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17:220–227. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon JR. Reactions toward the source of stimulation. J Exp Psychol. 1969;81:174–176. doi: 10.1037/h0027448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon JR. The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In: Proctor RW, Reeve TG, editors. Stimulus-response compatibility. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1990. pp. 31–86. [Google Scholar]
- Stürmer B, Leuthold H. Control over response priming in visuomotor processing: a lateralized event-related potential study. Exp Brain Res. 2003;153:35–44. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1579-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stürmer B, Leuthold H, Soetens E, Schröter H, Sommer W. Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2002;28:1345–1363. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.6.1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stürmer B, Redlich M, Irlbacher K, Brandt S. Executive control over response priming and conflict: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Exp Brain Res. 2007;183:329–339. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1053-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: Thieme; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Tanji J, Hoshi E. Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in executive behavioral control. Physiol Rev. 2008;88:37–57. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00014.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SF, Stern ER, Gehring WJ. Neural systems for error monitoring: recent findings and theoretical perspectives. Neuroscientist. 2007;13:160–172. doi: 10.1177/1073858406298184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullsperger M, Szymanowski F. ERP correlates of error relevance. In: Ullsperger M, Falkenstein M, editors. Errors, conflicts, and the brain. Current opinions on performance monitoring. Leipzig: MPI for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences; 2004. pp. 171–177. [Google Scholar]
- Ullsperger M, von Cramon DY. Neuroimaging of performance monitoring: error detection and beyond. Cortex. 2004;40:593–604. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70155-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissman DH, Warner LM, Woldorff MG. Momentary reductions of attention permit greater processing of irrelevant stimuli. Neuroimage. 2009;48:609–615. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West R, Travers S. Tracking the temporal dynamics of updating cognitive control: an examination of error processing. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:1112–1124. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Worsley KJ, Liao CH, Aston J, Petre V, Duncan GH, Morales F, Evans AC. A general statistical analysis for fMRI data. Neuroimage. 2002;15:1–15. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeung N, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev. 2004;111:931–959. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.111.4.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]