Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2019 Jul 16;9:10313. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-46482-4

Genotypic trade-off between appetitive and aversive capacities in honeybees

Pierre Junca 1, Lionel Garnery 1, Jean-Christophe Sandoz 1,
PMCID: PMC6635639  PMID: 31311964

Abstract

Honey bees can learn both appetitive and aversive associations, using two olfactory conditioning protocols. Appetitive conditioning of the proboscis extension response (PER) involves associating an odor, the conditioned stimulus (CS) with a sucrose solution, the unconditioned stimulus (US). Conversely, aversive conditioning of the sting extension response (SER) involves associating the odor CS with an electric or thermal shock US. Here, we investigated the relationship between bees’ appetitive and aversive learning capacities at the individual level and the influence of bees’ genotype. As learning performance was shown to depend on an individuals’ sensitivity to the US, we systematically measured four different traits in each individual bee: sensitivity to sucrose, PER learning performance with a sucrose US, sensitivity to temperature, SER learning with a temperature US. First, we confirmed for both conditioning types that learning performance correlates with US responsiveness. Second, we found a trade-off between appetitive and aversive learning performances: bees that were better appetitive learners (and had a lower sucrose US threshold) learned less efficiently in the aversive conditioning (and had a higher temperature US threshold). Because the honey bee queen typically mates with 15–20 males, the workers from a honey bee hive belong to as many different patrilines, allowing for the search of the genetic determinism of cognitive abilities. Using microsatellite analysis, we show that a genetic determinism underlies the trade-off between appetitive and aversive capacities, with appetitively vs aversively biased patrilines. The honey bee hive thus appears as a genetically structured cognitive community.

Subject terms: Haplotypes, Classical conditioning

Introduction

Where to find food and how to avoid danger? These are two simple but critical questions animals need to answer for surviving in a wild environment. Individual experience plays a major role in solving these questions, since animals can learn to associate initially neutral environmental stimuli (odors, sounds, colors, etc.) with their upcoming consequences, both beneficial (appetitive) and noxious (aversive). Therefore, an important part of an individual’s potential fitness resides in its genetically-determined appetitive and aversive learning abilities. This is particularly true for solitary species, in which individuals must be skilled in both types of tasks since they must provide alone for all of their needs. The emergence of sociality, multiple times in the course of evolution, has fundamentally changed this rule, because in a social group different abilities may be distributed among different members, giving rise to behavioral specialization1. Such inter-individual differences are thought to be beneficial for a social group’s ecological success. In meerkats, for instance, particular individuals in the group are dedicated to the surveillance of the surroundings while others take care of the youth and still others forage for the group2,3. In noisy miners, different birds specialize in either defense against predators or in provisioning4. Such behavioral specialization is even more conspicuous within social insect colonies, where division of labor among non-reproductive individuals is a hallmark of social lifestyle57. At the proximal level, division of labor is commonly explained through self-organization based on individual behavioral rules that rely on inter-individual differences in responses to environmental stimuli7,8. The fixed-threshold model, in particular, assumes that specialization in a social group arises spontaneously from differences among individuals in their response threshold to stimuli associated with specific tasks911. Generally, individuals with the lowest threshold will engage in the corresponding task, provoking a reduction in the intensity of the task-associated stimulus. Division of labor may thus appear through simple inter-individual differences in the response threshold to different signals.

Response thresholds do not only influence the propensity of individuals to perform a specific task, but they also control associative learning performances, as shown for both aversive and appetitive modalities in the honeybee Apis mellifera. In the appetitive conditioning of the Proboscis Extension Response (PER)12,13, in which bees have to associate an odor with a sucrose reward, learning performances are strongly under the influence of individual response thresholds to sucrose14,15. Thus, bees that are more sensitive (i.e. show a higher responsiveness) to sucrose display higher learning performances when associating an odor with sucrose. Likewise, in the aversive conditioning of the Sting Extension Response (SER), in which bees have to associate an odor with an electric shock or heat punishment16,17, learning performances are directly correlated with an individual’s responsiveness to the aversive reinforcer (electric shock18; heat16). The self-organization theoretical account presented above predicts that within a social group, different individuals should display different response thresholds to appetitive and aversive stimuli, as they are related to different tasks, respectively food-associated tasks and defense-oriented tasks. Interestingly, at the population level, a trade-off has been observed between a hives’ foraging activity and its defensive ability19. Hives with a high foraging activity displayed low defense responses and vice versa. As this trade-off is thought to rely on a genetic background, one could expect to find a similar trade-off in individuals’ aversive and appetitive abilities. While some individuals would be biased towards appetitive abilities (and would be comparably less skilled for aversive tasks) other individuals would be biased towards aversive abilities. This attractive hypothesis has seldom been tested directly and no demonstration of its validity exists yet.

In honeybees, numerous studies have led to the idea that bees’ sensitivity to sucrose was the main determinant of task allocation14,2022. Evidence showing that sucrose responsiveness correlates with responsiveness to a number of other sensory stimuli initially supported this idea (e.g. tactile23; light24). However, the stimuli tested in these studies were mostly connected to foraging-related tasks. More recently, Roussel et al.18 compared bees’ responsiveness to sucrose with responsiveness to a stimulus unrelated to foraging, but rather belonging to the aversive hedonic modality: an electric shock. This study reported that sucrose responsiveness and electric shock responsiveness are not correlated, suggesting the existence of other determinants to bees’ behavior18. This study concluded that appetitive and aversive sensitivities belong to two independent behavioral modules, associated respectively to foraging-related and defense-related tasks. The lack of correlation observed by Roussel et al. could be taken for an invalidation of the hypothesis of a trade-off between appetitive and aversive abilities proposed above. However, these experiments were carried out on individuals of unknown age, which may have added a confounding variable in the analysis. Indeed, the sucrose response threshold varies with the bees’ age21,25,26 as does their sensitivity to aversive stimuli (electric shock27,28). Therefore, controlling the bees’ age may be critical for unraveling potential appetitive vs aversive trade-offs among individuals.

A major question that arises from threshold models of self-organization and the data presented above concerns the genetic substrate underlying such differences in sensory thresholds among individuals. The monogynous and polyandrous reproductive system of honeybees provides a good opportunity for studying this question. In a honeybee colony, the diploid queen mates on average with 15–20 haploid males29. Therefore, the workers, her daughters, belong to as many different patrilines with different genetic backgrounds within the hive. Workers’ patriline origin has an impact on task allocation as observed on brood care, foraging and defensive behavior30. In addition, it is known to have an impact on sensory responsiveness and learning performances. In the aversive modality, we showed previously that bees from different patrilines have different thermal response thresholds and show accordingly different aversive learning performances with this reinforcement16. In the appetitive modality, differences in learning performances among patrilines are suspected31, especially because sucrose response thresholds vary among them32. So far, the study of genotypic determinism on responsiveness and learning has been studied independently within the appetitive or within the aversive modality. Therefore, a possible trade-off in aversive vs appetitive learning abilities among different patrilines is utterly unknown.

In the present study, we asked how sensitivity and learning capacity in appetitive and aversive modalities are distributed among individuals composing a honeybee colony, in particular with regards to their patriline of origin. Performing the experiments on age-controlled individuals, we found a clear trade-off between aversive and appetitive abilities at the individual level. This aversive vs appetitive trade-off appeared also when taking into account the bees’ patrilines. These results suggest that within a eusocial insect colony workers compose an equilibrium of cognitively-specialized individuals, giving rise to a complex but highly-adaptable cognitive community.

Results

To assess how appetitive and aversive sensitivities and learning performances are related, series of four experiments were carried out on age-controlled (two weeks old) honey bee workers. Half of the bees went through an appetitive evaluation day followed by an aversive one, and the other half underwent the reversed schedule. The appetitive evaluation day comprised a sucrose responsiveness procedure followed by a PER conditioning procedure. Analogously, the aversive evaluation day comprised a heat responsiveness procedure followed by a thermal SER conditioning procedure. In the responsiveness procedures, bees received appetitive (sucrose) or aversive (temperature) stimuli of increasing intensity alternated with control stimulations (water and tactile respectively). In the conditioning procedures, bees were subjected to a differential conditioning protocol in which they had to differentiate between a reinforced odor (CS+) and a non-reinforced odor (CS−). For appetitive learning, the CS+ was associated with a sucrose reward and for aversive learning, the CS+ was associated with a temperature punishment. Bees received 8 CS +and 8 CS− trials in a pseudorandomized order with 10 min inter-trial intervals. For appetitive procedures, the bees’ PER were measured, while for aversive procedures, the SER were measured.

Responsiveness to appetitive and aversive stimulations

In the heat responsiveness experiment (Fig. 1A), bees’ SER increased significantly with increasing temperature (from 17% to 96%, ANOVA for repeated measurements: F5,1125 = 148.7, p < 0.001). In contrast, responses to alternated tactile stimulus applications remained stable throughout the experiment (between 12% and 13%, F5,1125 = 2.07, NS). Accordingly, responses to heat stimuli increased more quickly than control stimulations throughout the procedure (stimulus x trial interaction, F5,1125 = 106.5, p < 0.001). In the sucrose responsiveness experiment (Fig. 1B), bees’ PER increased significantly with increasing sucrose concentration (from 13% to 95%, F6,1350 = 180.1, p < 0.001. A response increase was also noticed in the control water stimulations (from 13% to 38%, F6,1350 = 24.7, p < 0.001) but on a smaller scale. This increase in water responses can be attributed to a non-associative sensitization effect which probably built up in the course of the procedure33. In any case, sucrose responses increased more quickly that control stimulations throughout the experiment (stimulus x trial interaction, F6,1350 = 59.5, p < 0.001).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Responsiveness and learning protocols for appetitive and aversive hedonic modalities performed on the same individuals (n = 226). (A) Heat responsiveness. Red circles, %SER to a series of increasing temperatures; white circles, %SER of the same bees to the presentation of a tactile (unheated) stimulus (control). (B) Sucrose responsiveness. Blue circles, %PER to a series of sucrose solutions of increasing concentration; white circles, %PER of the same bees to the presentation of water (control). (C) Differential aversive conditioning of the SER. Red circles, %SER to the CS+ (reinforced odor) along the 8 trials; white circles, %SER to the CS− (non-reinforced odor). (D) Differential appetitive conditioning of the PER. Blue circles, %PER to the CS+ along the 8 trials; white circles, %PER to the CS−. (ANOVA for repeated measurements, AB; stimulus x concentration or CD: stimulus x trial interaction, ***p < 0.001).

Appetitive and aversive conditioning performances

Bees learned both appetitive and aversive tasks effectively. In the aversive learning protocol (Fig. 1C), bees’ SER to the reinforced (CS+) odorant increased significantly (from 15% to 46%, F7,1575 = 20.8, p < 0.001), while their responses to the non-reinforced odorant (CS−) decreased (F7, 1575 = 5.87, p < 0.001). Consequently, bees’ responses to the CS+and CS− developed differently (stimulus x trial interaction: F7,1575 = 25.8, p < 0.001). In the appetitive learning protocol (Fig. 1D), bees’ PER to the CS+ increased along trials (from 8% to 61%, F7,1575 = 98.7, p < 0.001) while responses to the CS− decreased (F7,1575 = 7.80, p < 0.001). Overall, bees managed to differentiate between the two conditioned stimuli (stimulus x trial interaction: F7,1575 = 91.1, p < 0.001). Data obtained in responsiveness and learning experiments for aversive and appetitive modalities were consistent with previous studies performed separately on these two modalities16,34.

Appetitive and aversive relationships at the individual level

To study the relationships between responsiveness and learning performances within each hedonic modality or between the two modalities, we calculated individual scores16,18. Responsiveness scores consisted in the sum of responses to sucrose stimuli or to heat stimuli in each procedure. Similarly, learning scores were calculated as the sum of PER or SER responses to the CS+ along trials for appetitive and aversive learning protocols respectively. Previous work showed clearly that individual learning performance and responsiveness to the reinforcing stimulus are strongly correlated both in the aversive modality (electric shock18; heat16) and in the appetitive modality (sucrose14,34,35). But are these relationships noticeable when experiments are performed on the same individuals? In full agreement with previous work, we found strong and significant correlations between heat responsiveness and aversive learning performance (Fig. 2A; Spearman correlation, ρ = 0.94, p < 0.01) and between sucrose responsiveness and appetitive learning performance (Fig. 2B; ρ = 0.96, p < 0.001).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Relationship between responsiveness and learning performances within appetitive and aversive modalities. (A) Correlation between heat responsiveness scores and aversive learning scores in the same bees. Individuals were grouped according to their heat responsiveness scores. (B) Correlation between sucrose responsiveness scores and appetitive learning performance scores in the same bees. Individuals were grouped according to their sucrose responsiveness score. Numbers of individuals are indicated in boxes above each category (Spearman correlations, **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001, ntot = 226, mean ± SEM).

Measuring appetitive and aversive scores in the same individuals provided us with the opportunity to compare responsiveness and learning performances between hedonic modalities. To do that, individual bees can be grouped either according to aversive scores or to appetitive scores. Figure 3 presents both possibilities (aversive grouping: Fig. 3A–C; appetitive grouping: Fig. 3B–D). We found a clear negative correlation between appetitive and aversive responsiveness scores, which was present both when grouping individuals according to heat responsiveness scores (Fig. 3A: ρ = −0.94; p < 0.01) or to sucrose responsiveness scores (Fig. 3B: ρ = −0.86; p < 0.05). When comparing appetitive and aversive learning scores, we observed a significant negative correlation when grouping individuals according to appetitive learning scores (Fig. 3D: ρ = −0.77; p < 0.05) but the relation was not significant when grouping bees according to aversive learning scores (Fig. 3C: ρ = −0.47; p = 0.21).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Relationship between appetitive and aversive performances at the individual level. (A,B) Relationship between heat responsiveness and sucrose responsiveness scores. Bees were grouped according to either heat responsiveness scores (A) or sucrose responsiveness scores (B). (C,D) Relationship between appetitive and aversive learning scores. Bees were grouped according to either aversive learning scores (C) or appetitive learning score (D). Numbers of individuals are indicated in boxes above each category (Spearman correlations, *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01, NS: not significant; ntot = 226, mean ± SEM).

The grouping of individual responses according to the each type of score, as performed above, produced in some cases uneven data distributions (from 6 to 89 bees per group) which could make correlation analyses tricky. To avoid this problem, a Factor Analysis (FA), which does not require grouping data according to one or the other modality, was performed (Fig. 4A). The four variables (heat responsiveness, sucrose responsiveness, appetitive learning and aversive learning scores) were best explained by 2 main factors (73.1% of overall variance). Factor 1 (45.6% overall variance) clearly segregated the hedonic modalities, with appetitive responsiveness and learning scores corresponding to positive values on Factor 1 and aversive scores corresponding to negative values. The coordinates of individual bees on this axis correlated positively with appetitive variables (responsiveness: ρ = 0.63; p < 0.001; learning: ρ = 0.60; p < 0.001) and negatively with aversive variables (responsiveness: ρ = −0.37; p < 0.001; learning: ρ = −0.30; p < 0.001). Accordingly, the bees that had the lowest loading on Factor 1 (<10th percentile) showed high aversive scores and weak appetitive scores (Fig. 4B). Conversely, bees that had the highest loading on Factor 1 (>90th percentile) showed high appetitive scores and weak aversive scores.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Factor analysis on appetitive and aversive performances. (A) factor analysis on the 4 response scores (Sucrose: sucrose responsiveness; CS+Ap: appetitive learning score; Heat: heat responsiveness; CS+Av: aversive learning score) measured in 226 individuals. Two main factors with eigenvalues higher than 1 are extracted. Factor 1 (45.6% variance) shows a clear opposite relationship between appetitive and aversive variables. Factor 2 (27.5% variance) is related to differences in average response magnitude among individuals. (B) Response scores (mean ± SEM) of the first and last 10% of the distribution of individuals on Factor 1 or Factor 2.

Factor 2 (27.5% variance) was positively correlated with both aversive and appetitive modalities (aversive responsiveness: ρ = 0.49; p < 0.001; aversive learning: ρ = 0.55; p < 0.001; appetitive responsiveness: ρ = 0.24; p < 0.001; appetitive learning: ρ = 0.27; p < 0.001), and represented the general response magnitude of bees over all scores. Thus, bees below the 10th percentile on Factor 2 showed generally low scores while bees above the 90th percentile displayed high scores. This analysis shows that bees’ behavior could be defined primarily as a hedonic bias (Factor 1) and secondarily as a general response magnitude (Factor 2). These data thus demonstrate the opposite relationship existing at the individual level between appetitive and aversive performances. We next evaluated whether these relationships rely on a genotypic determinism.

Appetitive and aversive learning at the patriline level

To evaluate whether responsiveness and learning performance relationships are influenced by the bees’ genotype, we used a microsatellite analysis and determined each worker’s patriline. From the initial 226 individuals from 2 colonies, we obtained 25 patrilines containing between 3 and 28 individuals. For assessing patriline performance scores accurately, we only used data from the 11 patrilines which contained more than 8 individual bees. The bees’ responsiveness and learning scores in both modalities were pooled according to each worker’s patriline (Fig. 5). Within each modality, we found that patrilines that were highly responsive to thermal stimuli also presented high aversive learning performances and vice versa (Fig. 5A; ρ = 0.84; p < 0.01). Similarly, patrilines with a high sucrose responsiveness score presented a high appetitive learning score and vice versa (Fig. 5B; ρ = 0.65; p < 0.05). This confirms at the genotype level, the relationships observed above between responsiveness and learning within each modality.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Patriline influence on the relationship between aversive and appetitive performances. Individual scores are grouped according to each worker’s patriline. (A) Correlation between heat responsiveness and aversive learning scores among patrilines. (B) Correlation between sucrose responsiveness and appetitive learning performance scores among patrilines. (C) Relationship between heat responsiveness and sucrose responsiveness scores at the patriline level. (D) Relationship between appetitive and aversive learning performance scores at the patriline level. (Spearman correlations, p = 0.067, *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01, NS: not significant; n = 11 patrilines, 9 df, mean ± SEM).

When correlations were performed across aversive and appetitive modalities, we noticed a difference with observations at the individual level (Fig. 3C,D). Thus, the negative relationship between heat responsiveness and sucrose responsiveness scores was only near significant (Fig. 5C; ρ = −0.60, p = 0.067). Moreover, aversive and appetitive learning showed a rather scattered relationship and the correlation coefficient was not significant (Fig. 5D; ρ = −0.15; NS).

We reasoned that such apparent lack of consistency between data at the individual and at the patriline level (Figs 3C,D vs 5C,D) may be explained by some patrilines behaving very differently from the rest. To understand this phenomenon, we subjected the patriline data to a factor analysis (FA) and to a cluster analysis (Fig. 6). These analyses confirmed our hypothesis and indicated the existence of two subgroups. First, the factor analysis extracted two main factors (Fig. 6A, 90.1% of overall variance), which were the same factors that appeared at the individual level (Fig. 4A). Factor 1 (65.7% variance) represented the hedonic bias, patrilines exhibiting high performances in aversive procedures and weak performances in appetitive procedures being located on the left of this axis and vice versa for patrilines located on the right (compare with Fig. 6B). As above, Factor 2 (24.4% variance) represented general response magnitude.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Multivariate analyses of appetitive and aversive performances at the patriline level. (A) Factor analysis presenting each patriline according to 2 main factors. Factor 1 is a hedonic bias factor, equivalent to Factor 1 in Fig. 4A. Patrilines on the left show high response scores in aversive procedures, while patrilines on the right display stronger appetitive performances. Two patrilines contribute significantly to Factor 2 and exhibit weak scores in both appetitive and aversive procedures. (B) Hierarchical clustering dendrogram (Ward’s method) showing for each patriline its average performance score (mean ± SEM): sucrose responsiveness (light blue), appetitive learning (CS+ Ap, dark blue), heat responsiveness (light red), aversive learning (CS+ Av, dark red). Numbers of individuals are indicated in boxes above each patriline.

Two patrilines with generally low scores (lines 10 and 11) contributed 66.7% to this Factor, and were segregated from the other patrilines (lines 1 to 9). These two patrilines were also clearly segregated by the cluster analysis (Fig. 6B). As they did not follow the general response pattern, we further evaluated the relationship between aversive and appetitive scores without the contribution of patrilines 10 and 11. This data selection did not modify the positive relationships between responsiveness and learning (Fig. 7A,B; aversive: ρ = 0.78; p < 0.05; appetitive: ρ = 0.68; p < 0.05). However, it allowed demonstrating at the patriline level the negative correlation existing between appetitive and aversive modalities. Thus, heat responsiveness was negatively correlated to sucrose responsiveness (Fig. 7C; ρ = −0.77; p < 0.05) and aversive learning performance was negatively correlated to that in appetitive learning (Fig. 7D; ρ = −0.67; p < 0.05). These negative correlations between hedonic modalities support the idea of some genetic specialization of patrilines in either appetitive or aversive abilities.

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Patriline influence on the relationship between aversive and appetitive performances, without low-score patrilines. Individual scores are grouped according to each worker’s patriline. (A) Correlation between heat responsiveness and aversive learning performance scores among patrilines. (B) Correlation between sucrose responsiveness and appetitive learning performance scores among patrilines. (C) Correlation between heat responsiveness and sucrose responsiveness scores. (D) Correlation between appetitive and aversive learning performance scores. (Spearman correlations, *p < 0.05; n = 9 patrilines, 7 df, mean ± SEM).

Discussion

In this study, we assessed responsiveness and learning performance in both appetitive and aversive hedonic modalities on the same, age-controlled, individuals. At the individual level, we confirmed within each modality that responsiveness to a given reinforcement (sucrose or heat) determines learning performance with this reinforcement (PER conditioning or SER conditioning). Moreover, we found a trade-off between appetitive and aversive modalities, so that performances within one modality were negatively correlated with those in the other. Using microsatellite analysis, we confirmed both within-modality and between-modality relationships on a patriline level, thus demonstrating a genetic influence on the appetitive/aversive trade-off. Our data also show that a low proportion of the patrilines displays generally low scores in both hedonic modalities and do not follow the general trade-off.

A hedonic trade-off within the hive

Our results, both at the individual and at the patriline level demonstrate the existence of a trade-off between bees’ appetitive and aversive abilities. This result suggests that the honeybee colony is a cognitive community composed of specialized individuals displaying an appetitive or an aversive bias. The idea of possible interactions between appetitive and aversive skills in worker bees has been suggested early on because opposite tasks like foraging and colony defense are both undertaken by older bees36. Thus, according to the response threshold model, there should be differences among older bees in response thresholds to stimuli associated with each type of task. Our results obtained on 2 weeks old individuals, an age at which foragers may already engage in foraging or guarding37,38, provide explicit support for this idea. One needs however to remain cautious when predicting how this trade-off will affect a real hive. First, we took a snapshot at 2 weeks of age and do not know how well this trade-off is maintained throughout the bees’ lifetime. In a real colony, workers of all ages interact, which may makes the situation difficult to predict. Second, how this trade-off may translate into actual task allocation within the hive is not trivial. Previous studies already demonstrated that nectar foragers and guards differ in their responsiveness to both sucrose39 and electric shock (18, equivalent to our heat stimulus). One could thus expect our bees with different hedonic biases to engage in different tasks, for instance foraging and guarding. However, direct predictions are difficult because task allocation is under the control of many environmental variables, including colony size, time of year, climatic conditions and food availability5. In addition, the observed relationship between sensory responsiveness and performed task does not appear to be as simple as stated by response threshold models. For instance, contrary to the prediction of these models, nectar foragers were found to be less sensitive to sucrose than guards39, while guards are less sensitive to the electric shock than foragers18. Indeed, nectar foragers and guards are particularly selective with regards to the stimulus intensities to which they should respond in nature, instead of being more sensitive. Such high selectivity may be adaptive for honeybees, when taking costs and benefits for the colony into account: nectar foragers would optimize this ratio by compensating the high flying costs by gathering only nectar at the highest sugar concentrations, while guards would optimize this ratio by triggering costly defensive responses only to the strongest aggressions18,22. Given this apparent inconsistency between the predictions of threshold based models and task allocation in honeybees, it will be especially important to relate in future work the hedonic bias we have shown here with the actual propensity of workers to engage in foraging or guarding tasks.

Inter-individual differences in appetitive and aversive sensitivities

Our data show genetically-determined inter-individual differences in the bees’ sensitivity to sucrose and thermal stimuli, which translate into differences in learning performances in both modalities. These discrepancies in sensory sensitivity between individuals may be based on neuroanatomical and/or neurophysiological differences and could involve multiple levels of the respective sensory pathways, from receptors at the periphery until neural circuits in the brain. Inter-individual differences in sucrose sensitivity, for instance, could happen because of different numbers and/or affinities of sucrose (AmGr1) receptors within gustatory neurons; different numbers of gustatory neurons present on the bees’ antennae; different numbers of synaptic contacts of gustatory neurons with second-order neurons; different intensities of local inhibition in gustatory circuits; or any combination of these processes4042. For temperature detection, although much less is known at the moment, different sensitivities could also be due to different types/qualities of TRP channels at the periphery (possibly involving HsTRPA)43,44 or to different neuron/circuit organizations at more central levels. Physiologically, inter-individual differences in appetitive or aversive sensitivities may also arise due to different neuromodulator levels. Biogenic amines, for instance, could be involved, as they have an orchestral function in the modulation of insect behavior45, most prominently in social insects46. The biogenic amines octopamine and dopamine play an instructive role in appetitive and aversive learning in bees, by representing respectively the appetitive and the aversive US in the brain17,47,48. This instructive role is however limited to the associative learning event, but biogenic amines are thought to have wider-ranging roles, including the modulation of bees’ responsiveness to sensory stimuli4952. It has been observed for instance that octopamine, tyramine and dopamine can modulate sucrose responsiveness49,51. While injections of octopamine or tyramine increase bees’ sucrose responsiveness, injections of dopamine or a dopamine receptor agonist decrease it. The effect of biogenic amines on sting responsiveness to thermal stimuli, as used here, has not been tested yet. However, recent data using pharmacological injections of amine receptor antagonists suggested that both serotonin and dopamine can reduce bees’ responsiveness to an electric shock, while octopamine has no effect50. In our case, different individuals may display discrepancies in biogenic amine levels which would translate into differences in their sensitivity to sucrose and to heat stimuli. It would thus be especially interesting in future work to evaluate whether our bees with lower sucrose responsiveness show lower octopamine/tyramine levels, and bees with lower heat responsiveness show higher serotonin levels, as predicted by the studies above49,50.

The appetitive/aversive sensory trade-off

The most important finding of our study is that the sensitivities of bees toward appetitive and aversive stimuli are under the influence of a genotypic trade-off. Bees with a high sensitivity to sucrose tended to show a low sensitivity to thermal stimuli, and vice versa. How does such a trade-off come about? In theory, the hedonic trade-off could follow a monogenic determinism, if the responsible gene displayed a high allelic polymorphism and had pleiotropic effects on both appetitive and aversive sensitivities. In this case, different patrilines would carry different alleles, giving rise to a continuous distribution of hedonic biases, from aversively-biased to appetitively-biased individuals, as observed here. For instance, a gene that would act positively on both tyramine (or octopamine) and serotonin levels could act on the hedonic bias. Increasing the levels of both amines would give rise to appetitively-biased bees (by increasing sucrose responsiveness and decreasing thermal responsiveness), while decreasing the levels of both amines would favor aversively-biased bees49,50. It is however much more likely that the hedonic trade-off is under polygenic influence, as many quantitative traits actually depend on intricate networks of interacting genes53,54. The genes responsible for the hedonic trade-off we have described may be related to previous QTL (Quantitative Trait Loci) identified in the honeybee genome and involved in variations of foraging (pln1-4,5557) or defensive behaviors (sting1-3,57,58). Interestingly, genes associated with biogenic amine signaling have been identified within these QTL regions57. Thus, pln2 contains AmTyr1, coding for the honeybee tyramine receptor59 and sting3 contains Am5HT7, coding for one of the honeybee serotonin receptors60. Alternatively or in addition to the hypothesis of different biogenic amine levels mentioned above, bees’ appetitive and aversive responsiveness may depend on different allelic forms of tyramine and serotonin receptors respectively. In any case, for the trade-off to appear, the genes supporting appetitive and aversive responsiveness need to engage in epistatic interactions. Genes supporting a high sucrose sensitivity would negatively affect processes involved in heat sensitivity, and vice versa. Such epistasis could happen at several levels, from direct gene interactions by transcription factors61 or RNA interference processes62,63, or more indirectly, from interactions of the products of these gene with biosynthetic pathways and/or developmental processes. Such epistatic interactions are expected to be highly complex and intensive work will be needed to understand the genotypic trade-off on a functional level. The present study thus paves the way for a long-term exploration of epistatic interactions between aversive and appetitive genetic pathways.

The hedonic trade-off at the evolutionary level

It has been suggested that social insect colonies with a high genetic diversity are more adaptable than low-diversity colonies64,65. Similarly, colonies with a high proportion of specialized individuals are thought to be more efficient that homogeneous colonies30,66,67. The hedonic specialization of patrilines, as demonstrated here, may be an adaptive mechanism for honeybees, allowing them to respond efficiently to the ecological constraints surrounding the colony, both in terms of food availability and of prevalence of potential predators and parasites. At the individual level, the trade-off suggests that a high sensitivity and high learning performances in one hedonic modality come at the cost of a lower sensitivity and lower learning performances in the other. At the colony level, however, ecological success and fitness may be more related to the simultaneous presence of both strongly appetitively-biased and strongly aversively-biased workers. Therefore, the hedonic trade-off may have been selected over evolutionary times. Different ratios of appetitive-biased/aversive-biased workers may be adapted to different environmental conditions, with for instance a better fitness for a higher proportion of appetitively oriented individuals in high-resource sites and a higher proportion of aversively oriented individuals in low-resource sites. However, the long term interest of the species would be to maintain a good balance of both types of individuals for adapting to local conditions. Honeybees are characterized by a monogynous polyandrous mating system, with typically as many as 15–20 males inseminating a queen29. This high polyandry increases the probability of sampling alleles from the whole genetic diversity in the population and maintaining rare alleles that may not be currently adapted but may be beneficial in the future68. A next step for understanding the evolution of the hedonic trade-off and possible adaptations to local conditions would be to measure the hedonic bias in workers from colonies with a common genetic origin but maintained over generations in high- or low-resource sites. We expect to find in these colonies different proportions of appetitively- and aversively-biased individuals. Such adaptations of the hedonic bias may be a basis for the observation that, at the population level, hives with a high foraging activity display low defense responses and vice versa19.

In conclusion, we found a trade-off in honeybees’ sensitivity and learning abilities between appetitive and aversive hedonic modalities, which depends on a genotypic determinism. Such trade-off may be instrumental for efficient task allocation within the colony and for its rapid adaptation to local environmental conditions. On a proximal level, future work will need to focus on the epistatic effects giving rise to this trade-off. On a more distal level, studying how bees adapt this trade-off with local conditions may help understand its possible beneficial effect for bees’ ecological success.

Methods

Animals

Age-controlled bees were used in the experiments to avoid any potential impact of age on bees’ behavioral responses. Thirteen-14 day-old bee workers were obtained from two hives on the CNRS campus Gif-sur-Yvette. Shortly, a comb with capped brood, close to emergence was taken from the hive and all adult bees were gently brushed aside. The comb was then placed in a closed box in an incubator at 34 °C. On the next day, newly emerged bees were painted with a two-color code (Posca, France) and then placed back into the hive. Thirteen days later, the bees were taken from the hive and used in the behavioral experiments. At this age, honey bees usually start to perform tasks outside the hive such as guarding or foraging37.

After chilling on ice, 16 individuals were harnessed in a metal holder as described in Junca et al.16. With this holding procedure, both sting- and proboscis extension could be clearly monitored. Bees were fed with 5 µl of sucrose solution (50% w/w) every morning and evening to keep them in a good condition for the two experimental days and were conserved in a dark and humid box between experiments. One group of 16 bees was tested over two days. Four experimental procedures were carried out on these individuals according to the following schedule: half of the bees were subjected to the measure of sucrose responsiveness followed by appetitive conditioning on the first day and to the measure of heat responsiveness followed by aversive conditioning on the second day. For the other half, the two experimental days were swapped. At the end of the second day, all bees were placed in individual Eppendorf tubes filled with 96% ethanol solution for microsatellite analysis.

Bees’ responsiveness to temperature and sucrose stimuli

Once mounted, bees were placed in a dark and humid box for two hours to avoid any stress. Thermal responsiveness was measured following the procedure of Junca et al.16. Bees received a succession of six stimulations of increasing temperature (from ambient temperature ~25 °C to 75 °C), in steps of 10 °C. Thermal stimulations were provided by means of a pointed copper cylinder (widest diameter: 6 mm; length: 13 mm), mounted onto the end of a minute soldering iron running at low voltage (HQ-Power, PS1503S). Temperature at the end of the cylinder was controlled using a contact thermometer (Voltcraft, Dot-150). Thermal stimulations alternated with tactile controls, provided as above with an identical unheated probe.

Sucrose responsiveness was measured following the protocol described in Scheiner et al.34. Bees were presented sucrose solutions of increasing concentration following an exponential progression (0%; 0.1%; 0.3%; 1%; 3%; 10%; 30% w/w). Sucrose stimulations were alternated with water control. Sucrose and water stimulations were provided with a soaked toothpick to the bees’ two antennae simultaneously, and the PER (extension or not of the proboscis) was noted.

In both heat and sucrose responsiveness experiments each trial lasted 38 s. The bee was placed in the holding setup, and left for 20 s before stimulus application started. The sucrose or thermal stimulation lasted for 1 s, and was applied to both antennae for sucrose responsiveness or to the mouthparts for heat responsiveness. The bee was then left in the setup for 17 s and was removed. For a given bee, all stimulations were performed at 10 min intervals.

Bees’ aversive and appetitive learning performance

On each day, the learning procedure started 1 h after the responsiveness procedure. Learning procedures were identical for appetitive and for aversive conditioning, except for the US used and the behavioral response measured. During appetitive conditioning, the US was a 30% sucrose solution and PER were measured. During aversive conditioning, the US was a 65 °C thermal stimulation to the mouthparts and SER were measured.

Bees were subjected to differential conditioning procedures, in which one odorant (the CS+) was associated with either appetitive or aversive reinforcement (the US), while another odorant was presented without reinforcement (the CS−). Two pairs of odorants were chosen according to Guerrieri et al.69, in such a way that all odorants were well differentiated from each other by bees. For each bee, one odorant pair was used for aversive conditioning while the other was used for appetitive conditioning. To avoid producing a high number of subgroups, within each odorant pair, one odorant was used as CS+ while the other was used as CS−. The two pairs of odors were: (1) 1-nonanol (CS+) and 2-heptanol (CS−); (2) hexanal (CS+) and 2-octanone (CS−) (Sigma Aldrich, Deisenhofen, Germany). Five μl of pure odorant were applied onto a 1 cm² piece of filter paper which was transferred into a 20 ml syringe (Terumo) allowing manual odorant delivery to the antennae. Half of the bees were conditioned with odorant pair 1 for aversive conditioning and odorant pair 2 for appetitive conditioning, and vice versa for the other half.

Each conditioning procedure was composed of 16 trials (8 reinforced and 8 non-reinforced) in a pseudo-random sequence (e.g. ABBABAAB) starting with odorant A or B in a balanced way. The inter-trial interval (ITI) was 10 min. Each conditioning trial lasted 38 s. The bee was placed in the stimulation site in front of the air extractor, and left for 18 s before being exposed to the odorant paired with the US. Each odorant (CS+ or CS−) was delivered manually for 4 s. The thermal stimulus started 3 s after odorant onset and finished with the odorant (1 s temperature stimulation). The bee was then left in the setup for 14 s and was then removed.

Determination of patriline origin

Patriline determination was carried out by genotyping microsatellite areas conserved in the bees’ genome. Microsatellites are non-coding DNA fragments, made of repeated pairs (duo) or triplets (or more) of nucleotides. Sizes of microsatellites are conserved in bees’ offspring (patrilines) like alleles. To precisely determine the patriline origin of each bee, 12 loci were amplified70.

DNA was extracted using the 10% Chelex method adapted for squashed bee head tissues71. The head of the bee was cut off and placed in an Eppendorf tube with an iron marble. The tube was then placed into a grinder (Retsch MM301). Once the head crushed, 600 µl of 10% Chelex (BioRad) at 60 °C were added. Composed of micromarble, the Chelex chelates impurities and ions which could interfere with the following PCR. Then, 18 µl of proteinase K were added and after 1 h digestion at 50 °C in a heating block, the tubes were placed 30 min at 90 °C to remove proteinase K. The iron marbles were then removed and the solutions centrifugated for 10 min at 12000 rpm. They were then conserved in a freezer (−20 °C).

Microsatellites amplifications were performed using 3 different multiplexes, which allowed analyzing several loci simultaneously. Multiplex 1 was composed of loci A88, A28, A24, Ap55 and A66. Multiplex 2 was composed of loci A113, A7, Ap43 and Ap81. Multiplex 3 analyzed loci Ap33, A43, A8. A multiplex contains pure water, buffer (Promega), Bovine serum albumin (BSA; Sigma Aldrich) and Taq polymerase which allows replicating the fragments of interest. In a PCR dish, 1 µL of non-diluted DNA and 9 µL of the chosen plex were deposited. The time spent in the thermocycler (Biometra, UNO-thermobloc) was calibrated for each multiplex, depending on the primers used. For genotyping in the sequencer, a mix of Rox350 and Formamide was added to the PCR product. DNA fragments were identified using an ABI 3130 Genetic Analyzer and the Genscan analysis software (version 3.7.1). Allelic sizes were labeled using Genemapper 4.1. Allele nomenclature was standardized using reference samples7274.

The multilocus genotype of the queen was verified, using the Colony 1.2 program75. The program analyzes haplo-diploid systems based on the expression of codominant genetic markers, such as DNA microsatellites. It calculates the probabilities of all possible queen genotypes, based on the observed allele frequencies in the population. Paternal alleles for each worker were then characterized after subtracting the queen’s allele from each worker’s genotype. Workers were considered as belonging to the same patriline when the same alleles were shared over all (12) analyzed loci. Patrilines with more than 8 individuals were used in the analyses. This threshold yielded 11 patrilines in total, nine from colony 1 [patrilines n°1–8 and 11 in Fig. 6] and two from colony 2 [patrilines 9 and 10 in Fig. 6]). Although maternal genetic effects may also play a role in the variability between these groups (because of the two colonies), we stuck to the term patriline throughout for commodity.

Statistical analysis

All recorded data were dichotomous, with a sting or proboscis extension being recorded as 1 and a non-extension as 0. Over all analyses, bees which did not respond during either one of the responsiveness experiments were excluded from the analysis, as they were considered as not appetitively or aversively motivated enough to learn in the following conditioning experiments.

We calculated for each bee its thermal responsiveness score (from 1 to 6) and sucrose responsiveness score (from 1 to 7) by counting the number of times it responded to the thermal or sucrose stimulus of increasing intensities. Higher scores indicate bees that started to respond at lower temperatures or sucrose concentrations, and are thus more sensitive to temperature or sucrose respectively. In the same manner, two learning performance scores were calculated. For the aversive and appetitive learning scores, we counted the number of times bees responded to the reinforced odorant (CS+). A higher score indicated a good learner, which quickly associated the CS+ with reinforcement.

Data from both colonies (colony 1, n = 157; colony 2, n = 69) and for both subgroups which received different arrangements of odorant pairs for aversive/appetitive tasks (appetitive conditioning with 1-nonanol/2-heptanol, n = 108; with 1-hexanol/2-octanone, n = 118 – vice versa for aversive conditioning) were pooled. This was possible because initial statistical analyses indicated that these subgroups had no differential effects on bees’ response threshold and learning scores. While the colony had a general effect on bees’ average score to the four tasks (Repeated-measure ANOVA, F1,222 = 8.23, p < 0.01), there was no interaction between colony and task (F3,666 = 2.16, NS). Thus, while scores were generally higher in one colony than in the other, this did not affect the relative scores of the bees in the four tasks. Concerning the subgroups based on the odorant pairs used for each task, we found no general effect on bees’ scores in the four tasks (RM-ANOVA, F1,222 = 0.90, NS) and no interaction between odorant pair and task (F3,666 = 1.24, NS). Second, when taking into account both the valence (appetitive vs aversive) and the type of task (threshold vs conditioning) for the scores (2 repeated measures), we found no significant valence x task x colony interaction (RM-ANOVA, F1,222 = 0.62, NS) and no valence x task x odorant pair interaction (RM-ANOVA, F1,222 = 0.88, NS).

To analyze thermal and sucrose responsiveness curves or appetitive and aversive conditioning curves, we used repeated measure ANOVAs with stimulus (either thermal (sucrose) vs tactile (water), or CS+ vs CS−) and trial as repeated factors. Monte Carlo studies have shown that it is permissible to use ANOVA on dichotomous data only under controlled conditions, which are met in these experiments76.

A correlative approach was chosen to analyze relationships between responsiveness and learning performances within or across hedonic modalities at the individual and at the patriline levels. For studying correlations at the individual level, bees were grouped by heat responsiveness score and their average learning performance scores were calculated, thus allowing a clear representation of the relationship between the two variables. At the patriline level, bees thermal and sucrose responsiveness scores and aversive and appetitive learning scores were averaged for each patriline. Correlations were assessed by calculating the Spearman correlation coefficient. To further reveal positive or negative relationships among response scores, Factor Analyses (FA) were used. These analyses were complemented with a cluster analysis based on Euclidian distances between patrilines’ behavioral responses in order to highlight putative groupings of patrilines exhibiting similar hedonic biases. All data were analyzed with STATISTICA V5.5 and V10.0 (StatSoft, Tulsa, USA). In the graphs, data are presented as the mean ± SEM.

Author Contributions

A preliminary version of this work appeared in the doctoral thesis of the first author (Junca, P. Bases comportementales et génétiques des apprentissages aversif et appétitif chez l’abeille, Apis mellifera, Université Paris-Sud, 2015). P.J., L.G. and J.C.S. conceived the experiments. P.J. collected the data. P.J., L.G. and J.C.S. analyzed the data and interpreted the results. P.J. and J.C.S. wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Data Availability

All data are available upon request from the corresponding author.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Jeanson R, Weidenmuller A. Interindividual variability in social insects - proximate causes and ultimate consequences. Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 2014;89:671–687. doi: 10.1111/brv.12074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Manser MB. Response of foraging group members to sentinel calls in suricates, Suricata suricatta. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 1999;266:1013–1019. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0737. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Madden J, Drewe J, Pearce G, Clutton-Brock T. The social network structure of a wild meerkat population: 3. Position of individuals within networks. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2011;65:1857–1871. doi: 10.1007/s00265-011-1194-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Arnold KE, Owens IP, Goldizen AW. Division of labour within cooperatively breeding groups. Behaviour. 2005;142:1577–1590. doi: 10.1163/156853905774831927. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Robinson GE. Regulation of division of labor in insect societies. Annu Rev Entomol. 1992;37:637–665. doi: 10.1146/annurev.en.37.010192.003225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Traniello JFA, Rosengaus RB. Ecology, evolution and division of labour in social insects. Animal Behaviour. 1997;53:209–213. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0289. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Duarte A, Weissing FJ, Pen I, Keller L. An Evolutionary Perspective on Self-Organized Division of Labor in Social Insects. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics. 2011;42:91–110. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-102710-145017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Beshers SN, Fewell JH. Models of division of labor in social insects. Annu Rev Entomol. 2001;46:413–440. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bonabeau E, Theraulaz G, Deneubourg J-L. Quantitative study of the fixed threshold model for the regulation of division of labour in insect societies. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 1996;263:1565–1569. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1996.0229. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Page RE, Jr, Mitchell SD. Self-organization and the evolution of division of labor. Apidologie. 1998;29:171–190. doi: 10.1051/apido:19980110. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Jeanson R, Fewell JH, Gorelick R, Bertram SM. Emergence of increased division of labor as a function of group size. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2007;62:289–298. doi: 10.1007/s00265-007-0464-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bitterman ME, Menzel R, Fietz A, Schäfer S. Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1983;97:107–119. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.97.2.107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Giurfa M, Sandoz JC. Invertebrate learning and memory: Fifty years of olfactory conditioning of the proboscis extension response in honeybees. Learn Mem. 2012;19:54–66. doi: 10.1101/lm.024711.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Scheiner R, Page RE, Jr., Erber J. Responsiveness to sucrose affects tactile and olfactory learning in preforaging honey bees of two genetic strains. Behav Brain Res. 2001;120:67–73. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00359-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Behrends A, Scheiner R. Octopamine improves learning in newly emerged bees but not in old foragers. J Exp Biol. 2012;215:1076–1083. doi: 10.1242/jeb.063297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Junca P, Carcaud J, Moulin S, Garnery L, Sandoz JC. Genotypic influence on aversive conditioning in honeybees, using a novel thermal reinforcement procedure. PLoS One. 2014;9:e97333. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Vergoz V, Roussel E, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M. Aversive learning in honeybees revealed by the olfactory conditioning of the sting extension reflex. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e288. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Roussel E, Carcaud J, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M. Reappraising social insect behavior through aversive responsiveness and learning. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e4197. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Giray T, et al. Genetic variation in worker temporal polyethism and colony defensiveness in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Behavioral Ecology. 2000;11:44–55. doi: 10.1093/beheco/11.1.44. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Page RE, Jr., Erber J, Fondrk MK. The effect of genotype on response thresholds to sucrose and foraging behavior of honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1998;182:489–500. doi: 10.1007/s003590050196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Pankiw T, Page RE., Jr. The effect of genotype, age, sex, and caste on response thresholds to sucrose and foraging behavior of honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) J Comp Physiol A. 1999;185:207–213. doi: 10.1007/s003590050379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Page RE, Jr., Scheiner R, Erber J, Amdam GV. 8. The development and evolution of division of labor and foraging specialization in a social insect (Apis mellifera L.) Current topics in developmental biology. 2006;74:253–286. doi: 10.1016/S0070-2153(06)74008-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Scheiner R, Page RE, Erber J. Sucrose responsiveness and behavioral plasticity in honey bees (Apis mellifera) Apidologie. 2004;35:133–142. doi: 10.1051/apido:2004001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Erber J, Hoormann J, Scheiner R. Phototactic behaviour correlates with gustatory responsiveness in honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) Behavioural brain research. 2006;174:174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.07.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Behrends A, Scheiner R, Baker N, Amdam GV. Cognitive aging is linked to social role in honey bees (Apis mellifera) Exp Gerontol. 2007;42:1146–1153. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2007.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Behrends A, Scheiner R. Evidence for associative learning in newly emerged honey bees (Apis mellifera) Anim Cogn. 2009;12:249–255. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0187-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hunt GJ. Flight and fight: a comparative view of the neurophysiology and genetics of honey bee defensive behavior. J Insect Physiol. 2007;53:399–410. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2007.01.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Burrell BD, Smith BH. Age- but not caste-related regulation of abdominal mechanisms underlying the sting reflex of the honey bee. Apis mellifera Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1994;174:581–592. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Estoup A, Solignac M, Cornuet J-M. Precise assessment of the number of patrilines and of genetic relatedness in honeybee colonies. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 1994;258:1–7. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1994.0133. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Page, R. E. & Robinson, G. E. In Advances in insect physiology Vol. 23 117–169 (Elsevier, 1991).
  • 31.Laloi D, Pham-Delegue M-H. Patriline-level variability in olfactory learning in the honey bee. Apidologie. 2010;41:436–442. doi: 10.1051/apido/2009080. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Scheiner R, Arnold G. Effects of patriline on gustatory responsiveness and olfactory learning in honey bees. Apidologie. 2010;41:29–37. doi: 10.1051/apido/2009040. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hammer M, Braun G, Mauelshagen J. Food-induced arousal and nonassociative learning in honeybees: dependence of sensitization on the application site and duration of food stimulation. Behavioral and neural biology. 1994;62:210–223. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(05)80019-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Scheiner R, Barnert M, Erber J. Variation in water and sucrose responsiveness during the foraging season affects proboscis extension learning in honey bees. Apidologie. 2003;34:67–72. doi: 10.1051/apido:2002050. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Scheiner R, Kuritz-Kaiser A, Menzel R, Erber J. Sensory responsiveness and the effects of equal subjective rewards on tactile learning and memory of honeybees. Learn Mem. 2005;12:626–635. doi: 10.1101/lm.98105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Breed MD, Robinson GE, Page REJ. Division of labor during honey bee colony defense. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1990;27:395–401. doi: 10.1007/BF00164065. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Seeley TD. Adaptive significance of the age polyethism schedule in honeybee colonies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1982;11:287–293. doi: 10.1007/BF00299306. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Winston, M. L. The biology of the honey bee. (Harvard University Press, 1987).
  • 39.Pacheco J, Breed MD. Sucrose-response thresholds and the expression of behavioural tasks by middle-aged honeybee workers. Animal Behaviour. 2008;76:1641–1646. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.08.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Jung JW, Park KW, Ahn Y-J, Kwon HW. Functional characterization of sugar receptors in the western honeybee, Apis mellifera. Journal of Asia-Pacific Entomology. 2015;18:19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.aspen.2014.10.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.de Brito Sanchez MG. Taste Perception in Honey Bees. Chemical Senses. 2011;36:675–692. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjr040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Degirmenci, L., Thamm, M. & Scheiner, R. Responses to sugar and sugar receptor gene expression in different social roles of the honeybee (Apis mellifera). J Insect Physiol (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 43.Kohno K, Sokabe T, Tominaga M, Kadowaki T. Honey bee thermal/chemical sensor, AmHsTRPA, reveals neofunctionalization and loss of transient receptor potential channel genes. J Neurosci. 2010;30:12219–12229. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2001-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Junca, P. & Sandoz, J.-C. Heat Perception and Aversive Learning in Honey Bees: Putative Involvement of the Thermal/Chemical Sensor AmHsTRPA. Frontiers in Physiology6 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 45.Libersat F, Pflueger H-J. Monoamines and the Orchestration of Behavior. BioScience. 2004;54:17–25. doi: 10.1641/0006-3568(2004)054[0017:MATOOB]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kamhi JF, Traniello JFA. Biogenic Amines and Collective Organization in a Superorganism: Neuromodulation of Social Behavior in Ants. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2013;82:220–236. doi: 10.1159/000356091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hammer M, Menzel R. Multiple sites of associative odor learning as revealed by local brain microinjections of octopamine in honeybees. Learning & Memory. 1998;5:146–156. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Giurfa M. Associative learning: the instructive function of biogenic amines. Curr Biol. 2006;16:R892–895. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.09.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Scheiner R, Pluckhahn S, Oney B, Blenau W, Erber J. Behavioural pharmacology of octopamine, tyramine and dopamine in honey bees. Behav Brain Res. 2002;136:545–553. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00205-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Tedjakumala SR, Aimable M, Giurfa M. Pharmacological modulation of aversive responsiveness in honey bees. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;7:221. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Scheiner, R. et al. Learning, gustatory responsiveness and tyramine differences across nurse and forager honeybees. Journal of Experimental Biology, jeb. 152496 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 52.Mancini N, Giurfa M, Sandoz J-C, Avarguès-Weber A. Aminergic neuromodulation of associative visual learning in harnessed honey bees. Neurobiology of learning and memory. 2018;155:556–567. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Chesler EJ, et al. Complex trait analysis of gene expression uncovers polygenic and pleiotropic networks that modulate nervous system function. Nature genetics. 2005;37:233. doi: 10.1038/ng1518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Crow JF. On epistasis: why it is unimportant in polygenic directional selection. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2010;365:1241–1244. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hunt GJ, Page RE, Jr., Fondrk MK, Dullum CJ. Major quantitative trait loci affecting honey bee foraging behavior. Genetics. 1995;141:1537–1545. doi: 10.1093/genetics/141.4.1537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Ruppell O, Pankiw T, Page RE., Jr. Pleiotropy, epistasis and new QTL: the genetic architecture of honey bee foraging behavior. J Hered. 2004;95:481–491. doi: 10.1093/jhered/esh072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hunt GJ, et al. Behavioral genomics of honeybee foraging and nest defense. Naturwissenschaften. 2007;94:247–267. doi: 10.1007/s00114-006-0183-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hunt GJ, Guzmán-Novoa E, Fondrk MK, Page RE. Quantitative trait loci for honey bee stinging behavior and body size. Genetics. 1998;148:1203–1213. doi: 10.1093/genetics/148.3.1203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Blenau W, Balfanz S, Baumann A. Amtyr1: characterization of a gene from honeybee (Apis mellifera) brain encoding a functional tyramine receptor. Journal of neurochemistry. 2000;74:900–908. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2000.0740900.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Schlenstedt J, Balfanz S, Baumann A, Blenau W. Am5-HT7: molecular and pharmacological characterization of the first serotonin receptor of the honeybee (Apis mellifera) Journal of neurochemistry. 2006;98:1985–1998. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.04012.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Gerke J, Lorenz K, Cohen B. Genetic interactions between transcription factors cause natural variation in yeast. Science. 2009;323:498–501. doi: 10.1126/science.1166426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hannon GJ. RNA interference. nature. 2002;418:244. doi: 10.1038/418244a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Aravin AA, et al. Dissection of a natural RNA silencing process in the Drosophila melanogaster germ line. Molecular and cellular biology. 2004;24:6742–6750. doi: 10.1128/MCB.24.15.6742-6750.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Tarpy DR. Genetic diversity within honeybee colonies prevents severe infections and promotes colony growth. Proc Biol Sci. 2003;270:99–103. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Page RE, Robinson GE, Fondrk MK, Nasr ME. Effects of worker genotypic diversity on honey bee colony development and behavior (Apis mellifera L.) Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1995;36:387–396. doi: 10.1007/BF00177334. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Trumbo ST, Robinson GE. Learning and task interference by corpse‐removal specialists in honey bee colonies. Ethology. 1997;103:966–975. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1997.tb00138.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Langridge EA, Sendova-Franks AB, Franks NR. How experienced individuals contribute to an improvement in collective performance in ants. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2008;62:447–456. doi: 10.1007/s00265-007-0472-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Fuchs S, Moritz RFA. Evolution of extreme polyandry in the honeybee Apis mellifera L. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1999;45:269–275. doi: 10.1007/s002650050561. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Guerrieri F, Schubert M, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M. Perceptual and neural olfactory similarity in honeybees. PLoS Biology. 2005;3:e60. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Garnery L, Solignac M, Celebrano G, Cornuet J-M. A simple test using restricted PCR-amplified mitochondrial DNA to study the genetic structure ofApis mellifera L. Experientia. 1993;49:1016–1021. doi: 10.1007/BF02125651. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Estoup A, Solignac M, Cornuet J, Goudet J, Scholl A. Genetic differentiation of continental and island populations of Bombus terrestris (Hymenoptera: Apidae) in Europe. Molecular Ecology. 1996;5:19–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.1996.tb00288.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Franck P, Garnery L, Solignac M, Cornuet JM. The origin of west European subspecies of honeybees (Apis mellifera): new insights from microsatellite and mitochondrial data. Evolution. 1998;52:1119–1134. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1998.tb01839.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Estoup A, Garnery L, Solignac M, Cornuet J-M. Microsatellite variation in honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) populations: hierarchical genetic structure and test of the infinite allele and stepwise mutation models. Genetics. 1995;140:679–695. doi: 10.1093/genetics/140.2.679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Garnery L, et al. Genetic diversity of the west European honey bee (Apis mellifera mellifera and A. m. iberica) II. Microsatellite loci. Genetics Selection Evolution. 1998;30:S49. doi: 10.1186/1297-9686-30-S1-S49. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Wang J. Sibship reconstruction from genetic data with typing errors. Genetics. 2004;166:1963–1979. doi: 10.1534/genetics.166.4.1963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Lunney GH. Using analysis of variance with a dichotomous dependent variable: an empirical study. Journal of educational measurement. 1970;7:263–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-3984.1970.tb00727.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All data are available upon request from the corresponding author.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES