Abstract
Discrimination between sensory stimuli associated with safety and threat is crucial for behavioral decisions. Discriminative conditioning paradigms with two acoustic conditioned stimuli (one paired with shock [CS+], the other unpaired with shock [CS−]) have been widely used as an experimental model for fear learning. However, no attention has been paid to the effect of the CS− on safety in the paradigms, because the CS− served as a neutral cue or elevated the freezing level due to fear generalization although less effectively than the CS+. By using a noise and a tone as two acoustic CSs in a discriminative auditory fear conditioning (AFC) paradigm, here we demonstrate that mice learn safety for the CS− while showing fear for the CS+ with opposing emotional behaviors. We found that after learning mice exhibited a significant suppression of context-dependent freezing during the CS−, but not during the CS+, indicating learned safety without fear generalization for the CS−. In contrast, the mice showed an enhanced level of freezing during the CS+ even in a novel spatial context, indicating cued fear for the CS+. Moreover, the CS+ also induced rapid defensive behaviors, whereas the CS− disinhibited normal exploratory behaviors. On the other hand, mice showed no significant suppression of contextual fear during the CS− in a paradigm with a pair of tone CSs at different frequencies, although they clearly discriminated the two tones. These results suggest our AFC paradigm with the noise and tone CSs as a useful experimental model for cue-dependent discriminative learning of safety and threat.
Learning to discriminate sensory stimuli associated with safety from those associated with threat is crucial for correct behavioral decisions. Impaired discrimination of the associated emotional valence between these stimuli can lead to fear generalization (Dunsmoor and Paz 2015), a symptom of anxiety disorders observed in a wide variety of neuropsychiatric disorders, such as post-traumatic stress disorder (Etkin and Wager 2007; Jovanovic et al. 2012), schizophrenia (Holt et al. 2012; Braga et al. 2013; Kiran and Chaudhury 2016), and autism spectrum disorders (van Steensel et al. 2011; White et al. 2014). Therefore, experimental models of discriminative safety versus fear learning are beneficial for the research on neural mechanisms of emotional discrimination and generalization, as well as on related disorders (Kheirbek et al. 2012).
As classical experimental models of safety learning, two types of fear conditioning paradigms have been used in rodent studies: contextual fear conditioning with a single acoustic conditioned stimulus (CS) that is associated with a period of time in the absence of aversive unconditioned stimuli (US) (Rogan et al. 2005; Pollak et al. 2008, 2010), and cued fear conditioning with two CSs: one paired with an aversive US (CS+) and the other explicitly unpaired with the US (CS−) (Hammond 1967; Christianson et al. 2012; Sangha et al. 2013; Foilb et al. 2016). A critical point of the difference between these two paradigms is how to evaluate learned safety. In the single-cue paradigm, learned safety is evaluated by a suppressive effect of the CS on contextual fear that is acquired by the aversive US in the conditioning context (Rogan et al. 2005; Pollak et al. 2008, 2010). On the other hand, in the dual-cue paradigm, conditioned safety is evaluated by an attenuating effect of the CS− on CS+-evoked fear when presenting both the CSs simultaneously in the testing session, a test referred to as a summation test (Hammond 1967; Rescorla 1969; Christianson et al. 2012). Importantly, to prevent animal subjects from misunderstanding the superposed CSs as a novel cue, two cues with different sensory modalities, such as sound and light, need to be assigned to the CSs in the dual-cue paradigm (Hammond 1967; Christianson et al. 2012; Sangha et al., 2013; Foilb et al. 2016). Although a retardation test is another way to evaluate learned safety that assesses a low effectiveness of the CS on fear acquisition after the same CS being acquired as a safety cue (Rescorla 1969), safety conditioning and the subsequent reconditioning for fear need to be performed independently (Rescorla 1969; Rogan et al. 2005; Pollak et al. 2008; Christianson et al. 2012; Sangha et al. 2013; Foilb et al. 2016).
A large number of studies have taken advantage of discriminative auditory fear conditioning (AFC) paradigms with two sound CSs for investigating fear learning and memory (Antunes and Moita 2010; Letzkus et al. 2011; Aizenberg and Geffen 2013; Courtin et al. 2014; Ghosh and Chattarji 2014; Botta et al. 2015; Abs et al. 2018). However, although the effect of the CS+ on conditioned fear has been assessed by comparing fear intensities between the CS+ and the CS−, based on an increase in freezing responses above the baseline level, no attention has been paid to the effect of the CS− on safety learning. This is probably because the freezing level was either unaffected by the CS− (Ghosh and Chattarji 2014) suggestive of the CS− being a neutral cue that lacks an emotional impact, or elevated by the CS− (albeit to a lesser extent than the CS+; Antunes and Moita 2010; Letzkus et al. 2011; Aizenberg and Geffen 2013; Botta et al. 2015; Abs et al. 2018), presumably due to fear generalization to the acoustic cues (Laxmi et al. 2003; Ghosh and Chattarji 2014). Thus, a paradigm in which animals learn both safety and fear in a cue-dependent manner with opposing behavioral expressions remains to be established. By using a discriminative AFC paradigm with multiple conditioning sessions and distinct acoustic features of CSs, here we demonstrate that C57BL/6J (B6) mice learn to discriminate between the CS− and the CS+, showing suppression of context-dependent freezing and disinhibited exploratory behaviors to the CS−, but cue-evoked freezing and rapid defensive behaviors to the CS+.
Results
Acquisition of contextual and cued fear
We first tested contextual and cued fear acquisition in a discriminative AFC paradigm with B6 mice. We performed conditioning for three consecutive days and retrieval testing for the next 2 d, in one session per day (Fig. 1A). Each conditioning session during the first 3 d had four trials in which a white noise CS+ was paired with an electric footshock US, and four trials in which a 12 kHz tone CS− was explicitly unpaired with the US; trials were executed in a pseudorandom order (Fig. 1A). The testing session on day 4 and day 5 had six trials in which the CS+ and the CS− were alternately presented in the absence of the US; with the session on day 4 performed in the same context of the conditioning chamber, and the session on day 5 performed in a novel spatial context (Fig. 1A). To avoid a masking effect on contextual fear acquisition, we omitted sessions for habituation to the chamber before conditioning. To assess fear strength in mice, we quantified body movements as the motion indices that are determined by pixels changed between successive frames in a recorded video, and measured time spent in freezing behavior defined as the motion indices of 0–2 lasting more than or equal to 1 sec (see Materials and Methods). In the first session of conditioning on day 1, mice displayed exploratory behavior and little freezing during the time prior to the CS (PreCS) (Fig. 1B, Day 1, 2.4 ± 0.9%, n = 12), indicating no intrinsic fear to the context of the chamber before learning. However, the freezing level during the PreCS was increased in most of the mice tested (10 of 12 mice) in the second session on day 2 (Fig. 1B). In the third conditioning session on day 3, 9 mice exhibited an increase in the freezing levels from those on day 2, and 4 mice showed extremely high levels (>60%) of freezing (Fig. 1B). On average, we found significantly higher level of freezing during the PreCS on day 2, and the high level was maintained on day 3 (Fig. 1B; P = 0.00739 (day 1 vs. day 2), P = 0.0105 (day 1 vs. day 3), P = 0.826 (day 2 vs. day 3), Bonferroni's test; 32.0 ± 7.9% for day 2, 38.9 ± 10.2% for day 3, n = 12). These results indicate that the mice acquired contextual fear during conditioning.
To assess contextual fear memory, we examined the level of freezing at the beginning of the testing session on day 4 at which the mice were only subjected to the spatial context but not yet to the CSs on the day. The result showed a significantly higher level of freezing compared to the initial level on day 1, but in the following testing session on day 5 when the spatial context of the chamber was changed, the freezing level dropped to the level before learning (Fig. 1C; P = 0.00368 (day 1 vs. day 4), P = 0.00587 (day 4 vs. day 5), P = 0.249 (day 1 vs. day 5), Bonferroni's test; 1.4 ± 1.0% for day 1 1st PreCS, 25.1 ± 6.2% for day 4 1st PreCS, 3.3 ± 1.6% for day 5 1st PreCS, n = 12). The decrease of freezing level on day 5 is unlikely due to an attenuation of fear memories because the CS+ induced a marked increase of freezing level in the same session (Fig. 1D; P = 7.38 × 10−6, 7.8 ± 2.5% for the PreCS+, 47.4 ± 4.2% for the CS+, n = 12), indicating the retention of cued fear memory to the CS+ on day 5. Taken together, the above results suggest that the mice acquire two features of conditioned fear, contextual fear and cued fear, during conditioning and retain the memory.
Learned safety for the CS−
Interestingly, we also found that the freezing level was significantly lower during the tone CS− compared to that during the PreCS− in the testing session on day 4 (Fig. 2A; P = 0.00294, 35.1 ± 7.3% for the PreCS−, 10.1 ± 4.2% for the CS−, n = 12). In contrast, no difference was found for the noise CS+ in this session (Fig. 2B; P = 0.107, 26.8 ± 6.2% for the PreCS+, 39.9 ± 5.0% for the CS+, n = 12), although there was a marked difference on day 5 (Fig. 1D). To further assess cue-dependent effects, we analyzed changes in freezing levels from the PreCS period to the CS period in a trial-by-trial manner (see Materials and Methods) to subtract a component of contextual fear included in each trial. We found that 11 out of 12 mice exhibited negative changes from the PreCS− to the CS− in a trial on day 4 (Fig. 2C, CS− on day 4). In contrast, all of the mice tested displayed positive changes from the PreCS+ to the CS+ in a trial on day 5 (Fig. 2C, CS+ on day 5). The averaged data showed a significantly negative change for the CS− as opposed to a positive change for the CS+ on both day 4 and day 5 (Fig. 2C; P = 0.000564, −22.1 ± 5.1% for the CS−, P = 0.0457, 14.9 ± 8.1% for the CS+ (day 4); P = 0.0468, −6.8 ± 3.7% for the CS−, P = 1.39 × 10−6, 38.4 ± 4.4% for the CS+ (day 5), n = 12). These results strongly suggest that contextual fear is suppressed by the CS− while cued fear is retrieved by the CS+.
In line with this observation, normal exploratory behaviors that are primarily represented as the motion indices ranging from 3 to 1000 appeared more frequently during the CS− compared to the PreCS−, while decreasing the time with the motion index 0, that stands for no movement (see above for the definition of freezing), in the testing session on day 4 (Fig. 3A, C; P = 0.000646, 36.0 ± 5.9% for the PreCS−, 15.2 ± 4.2% for the CS− (Motion index 0); P = 0.000693, 55.7 ± 4.5% for the PreCS−, 75.3 ± 3.4% for the CS− (Motion index 3–1000); P = 0.905, 1.6 ± 1.0% for the PreCS−, 1.7 ± 1.0% for the CS− (Motion index > 2000), n = 12). This result strongly suggests that the CS− serves not as a neutral cue that has no influence on behaviors but as a safety cue that allows exploratory behaviors of the mice. In contrast, we observed that the mice sometimes rushed to the corner or jumped in the chamber during the CS+ in the session on day 4. These defensive behaviors (Gerlai 1998; Laxmi et al. 2003) could be reflected in the motion indices larger than 2000 during the CS+ (Fig. 3B, D; P = 0.00169, 1.0 ± 0.5% for the PreCS+, 5.0 ± 1.0% for the CS+ (Motion index > 2000), n = 12). Such behaviors were observed in several short time epochs only during the CS+ (Fig. 3B). Concomitantly, the occurrence of normal body movements with motion indices 3–1000 was decreased during the CS+ (Fig. 3D; P = 0.00412, 64.9 ± 4.7% for the PreCS+, 47.6 ± 2.9% for the CS+ (Motion index 3–1000), n = 12), while no significant difference was found in the motion indices of 0 between the PreCS+ and the CS+ in the session on day 4 (Fig. 3D; P = 0.122, 29.0 ± 5.7% for the PreCS+, 39.6 ± 3.7% for the CS+ (Motion index 0), n = 12). Thus, these behavioral differences during the CSs suggest emotional difference in the behavioral expressions elicited by the CS− and CS+.
To find whether the suppression of freezing observed during the tone CS− was acquired in conditioning sessions, we analyzed the difference of the freezing levels between the PreCS− and the CS− across three conditioning sessions (day 1–3). Interestingly, we observed that the freezing levels during the PreCS− and during the CS− showed similar increase on day 2, and there was, therefore, no significant difference in the freezing level between the PreCS− and the CS−, as seen in day 1 (Fig. 2D; P = 0.189, 6.8 ± 2.2% for the PreCS−, 3.6 ± 1.5% for the CS− (day 1); P = 0.580, 35.2 ± 8.7% for the PreCS−, 30.6 ± 5.2% for the CS− (day 2), n = 12). However, the difference of the freezing levels between the PreCS− and the CS− became significant on day 3, due to a decrease of the freezing level during the CS− relative to that on day 2 (Fig. 2D; P = 0.0157, 36.8 ± 8.6% for the PreCS−, 17.5 ± 5.4% for the CS− (day 3); P = 0.0415 (day 2 vs. day 3 for the CS−), n = 12). This result demonstrates that the mice acquired the CS− evoked suppression of contextual fear in the third conditioning session.
To test whether the suppression of freezing during the CS− is specific for pure tones, we performed the same conditioning experiment, with the 12 kHz tone and the white noise being oppositely assigned as the CS+ and CS−, respectively, for another group of mice. In accord with the previous results with the tone CS−, we found a significantly lower freezing level during the noise CS− compared to that during the PreCS− on day 4 (Fig. 4A; P = 0.0226, 22.1 ± 4.0% for the PreCS−, 11.1 ± 3.8% for the CS−, n = 12). We also found a significant reduction of contextual fear only for the noise CS−, but was not for the tone CS+ on day 4 (Fig. 4B, day 4; P = 0.0245, −13.1 ± 3.9% for the CS− relative to the PreCS−, P = 0.0959, 17.6 ± 7.0% for the CS+ relative to the PreCS+, n = 12), indicating that cue-evoked suppression of freezing is not limited to the 12 kHz tone. In addition, the 12 kHz tone CS+, but not the noise CS−, elevated freezing levels relative to the PreCS in a novel spatial context on day 5 (Fig. 4B, day 5; P = 0.134, −0.4 ± 1.9% for the CS− relative to the PreCS−, P = 0.000177, 36.1 ± 4.5% for the CS+ relative to the PreCS+, n = 12), indicating that the 12 kHz tone as the CS+ acts to retrieve fear memory in this protocol. Taken together, these results demonstrate that mice can learn the CS− and the CS+ as safety and threat signals, respectively, showing suppression of contextual fear for the CS− and expression of cued fear for the CS+ in our AFC paradigm with a noise and a tone as two acoustic CSs.
No suppressive effect of the CS− on contextual fear in a paradigm with tone/tone pair of the CSs
To test whether the CS− shows a suppressive effect on contextual fear when two tones are used in our discriminative conditioning protocol, we carried out another experiment with 4 and 12 kHz tones assigned as the CS+ and CS−, respectively, for a different group of mice. Although 4 of 12 mice displayed a large decrease (more than 10% differences compared to that during the PreCS−) in freezing levels during the CS− in the testing session on day 4, the remaining mice (8 of 12 mice) did not show such reductions consistently (Fig. 5A). On average, no significantly lower level of freezing was observed during the CS− compared to that during the PreCS− on day 4 (Fig. 5A; P = 0.506, 15.7 ± 4.1% for the PreCS−, 20.5 ± 5.4% for the CS−, n = 12). This result indicates no apparent effect of the CS− on contextual fear in this protocol. However, we found markedly higher level of freezing during the CS+ relative to that during the PreCS+ (Fig. 5B; P = 0.000158, 10.8 ± 4.0% for the PreCS+, 47.5 ± 5.6% for the CS+, n = 12), and a significant difference between the CS− and the CS+ in freezing level changes from the PreCS period to the CS period on day 4 (Fig. 5C; P = 0.000443, 1.8 ± 6.0% for the CS− relative to the PreCS−, 35.2 ± 5.9% for the CS+ relative to the PreCS+, n = 12), indicating that the mice discriminated the two tone CSs. We also observed a positive change in the freezing level relative to that during the PreCS only during the 4 kHz tone CS+ but not during the 12 kHz tone CS− in a novel spatial context on day 5 (Fig. 5D; P = 0.319, 1.3 ± 2.6% for the CS− relative to the PreCS−, P = 1.39 × 10−7, 49.0 ± 4.4% for the CS+ relative to the PreCS+, n = 12), indicating cued fear memory for the CS+. These results suggest that mice fail to express safety behavior during the CS− in our AFC paradigm with a tone/tone pair of CSs, although they can discriminate between the two CSs.
Discussion
In this study, we designed a discriminative AFC paradigm with 3-d conditioning sessions in which a pair of noise and tone pulses was used as CSs, and demonstrated that B6 mice learned contextual fear and suppression of contextual fear in response to the CS−, as well as cue-specific fear to the CS+. Our study provides an experimental model of discriminative learning and memory for safety versus fear that is useful for elucidating the neural mechanisms of emotion regulation and dysregulation.
Two types of safety conditioning paradigms have been classically utilized in rodent studies: a single-cue paradigm and a dual-cues paradigm (Hammond 1967; Rescorla 1969; Rogan et al. 2005; Kong et al. 2014). The CS in the single-cue paradigm exerts an inhibitory effect on contextual fear (Rogan et al. 2005; Pollak et al. 2008, 2010), while the CS− in the dual-cues paradigm have an attenuating effect on cued fear induced by the CS+ (Hammond 1967; Rescorla 1969; Christianson et al. 2012; Sangha et al., 2013; Kong et al. 2014; Foilb et al. 2016). In the present study, we integrated the evaluation of safety learning based on a suppression of contextual fear, classically used in single-cue paradigms (Rogan et al. 2005; Pollak et al. 2008, 2010), into the auditory discriminative paradigm that involves two acoustic cues. This allowed us to assess not only safety for the CS− but also fear for the CS+ in the same manner as the CS's effect on contextual fear.
Many rodent studies with discriminative AFC paradigms used two pure tones with different frequencies (Antunes and Moita 2010; Aizenberg and Geffen 2013; Ghosh and Chattarji 2014), two frequency-modulated sweeps with the same frequency range but opposite sweep directions (Letzkus et al. 2011; Abs et al. 2018), or a pair of a tone and a noise (Courtin et al. 2014; Botta et al. 2015) as the CSs. These examinations, however, resulted in either an increase in the freezing level even during the CS− compared to that during the PreCS or the baseline level (Antunes and Moita 2010; Letzkus et al. 2011; Aizenberg and Geffen 2013; Botta et al. 2015; Abs et al. 2018), or unaltered freezing levels during the CS− (Ghosh and Chattarji 2014) (except for Courtin et al. 2014 which made no statistical comparison between pre- and post-conditioning for the CS−), although there was a significant difference in freezing level between the CS− and CS+ after learning (Antunes and Moita 2010; Letzkus et al. 2011; Aizenberg and Geffen 2013; Courtin et al. 2014; Ghosh and Chattarji 2014; Botta et al. 2015; Abs et al. 2018).
There are several potential factors that affect specification and generalization of fear in discriminative AFC paradigms. First, when the physical properties of the two sound stimuli are similar, fear generalization to the sound stimuli occurs more likely (Aizenberg and Geffen 2013). In our present study, we chose a white noise and a tone delivered at different pulse rates as sound CSs to allow mice to discriminate more easily the two stimuli. As a result, the mice did not show generalized fear to the CS− but displayed a safety response selectively to the CS− after learning. We also found that a pure tone and a white noise as the CS− had a similar effect on conditioned safety in our discriminative conditioning paradigm. On the other hand, we found no learned suppression of contextual fear in response to the CS− in the same paradigm but with a tone/tone pair of the CSs, which is consistent with the previous studies described above. Second, to detect a suppressive effect of the CS− on contextual fear, it is important for mice to be subjected to the CS− in the presence of relatively high contextual fear that is caused by fear generalization. However, preconditioning habituation protocols can attenuate generalized fear to the spatial context that can be otherwise enhanced by the US during conditioning (see below). In our current protocol, we omitted the habituation session to avoid its masking effect on fear generalization. As we expected, mice showed a substantial level of contextual fear in the second and third sessions of conditioning and also in the testing session with the same spatial context (Figs. 1B, 2D). Importantly, all the above previous studies performed retrieval testing only in a cage that differs from the one used for conditioning (i.e., a novel context) (Antunes and Moita 2010; Letzkus et al. 2011; Aizenberg and Geffen 2013; Courtin et al. 2014; Ghosh and Chattarji 2014; Botta et al. 2015; Abs et al. 2018). The context change for testing in those studies may have masked the suppressive effect of the CS− on contextual fear. Actually, we found a decreased freezing level during the PreCS in the testing session with a novel context (day 5) in our conditioning paradigm. We cannot rule out the possibility that this reduction was caused in part by an attenuation of contextual fear memory or extinction learning during the testing session on day 4. Nevertheless, we found cued fear selective to the CS+, but not to the CS−, in a novel spatial context (Figs. 1D, 2C day 5), indicating the retention of fear memory on day 5. Additionally, the total number of trials with US as well as the intensity of the US can affect fear generalization to the context (Laxmi et al. 2003; Baldi et al. 2004) and also to the CS− (Laxmi et al. 2003; Antunes and Moita 2010; Ghosh and Chattarji 2014), and can thus determine the level of fear specificity to the CS+. Indeed, we found a high level of freezing during the CS− which was comparable to that during the PreCS− in the second conditioning session, but suppression of freezing during the CS− appeared in the third session (Fig. 2D, day 2 vs. day 3), although the effect of the CS+ on freezing is likely masked by contextual fear in these sessions. An optimal set of conditioning parameters might therefore be required for the successful detection of contextual fear, its suppression by the CS−, and cued fear to the CS+ in discriminative fear conditioning paradigms.
Besides the suppression of freezing, we also observed increased occurrence of normal exploratory behaviors during the CS− in conditioned mice, in stark contrast to defensive behaviors caused by the CS+ (Fig. 3). This result suggests the potential overlap between safety and reward. In support of this notion, safety learning enhanced CS− evoked responses in the striatum (Rogan et al. 2005) and the basal amygdala (Sangha et al. 2013), the brain regions known to regulate behaviors motivated by reward (Balleine et al. 2007; Ambroggi et al. 2008; Haber and Knutson 2010; Namburi et al. 2015). A recent study further demonstrated the involvement of dopamine signaling in the basolateral amygdala in learned safety (Ng et al. 2018). Further studies will be needed to address whether the CS− acts as a rewarding stimulus in our conditioning paradigm. Nevertheless, our discriminative AFC paradigm serves as an experimental model for discriminatory behaviors in mice that depend on emotional valence of the acoustic cues.
Materials and Methods
Animals
Male B6 (6–10 wk old) mice used for the experiments were purchased from Japan SLC (Shizuoka, Japan) or bred in our facility. All animals were individually housed and maintained for at least 2 d until experiments in a breeding room on a 12 h light–dark cycle with food and water available ad libitum. All experiments were approved by the Committee for Animal Experiments of Kumamoto University, and performed in accordance with the Guidelines for Use of Animals in Experiments of Kumamoto University.
Fear conditioning apparatus
All type of fear conditioning and testing experiments were performed in fear conditioning chambers (MED-AFC-M1, Med-Associates) individually placed in sound attenuating cubicles (NSB-B101) with an exhaust fan on the wall and a room lamp on the ceiling. The conditioning chamber was equipped with a chamber speaker and a metal grid floor connected with a programable shocker (ENV-414S, Med-Associates). A video camera was placed outside of a clear acrylic wall of the chamber. Scrambled footshock unconditioned stimuli (US; 0.6 mA, 2 sec) were generated by the shock generator and delivered through the grids of the floor. FreezeFrame 3 (Actimetrics) software was used for sound stimulus configuration, temporal control of electric shocks, video tracking, and quantification of locomotion and freezing behavior. “Significant Motion Pixel” analysis in the software was chosen to quantify animal movements (Kopec et al. 2007). The frame rate of the video recording was 7.5 frames per second. Before experiments the grid floor with an excreta tray was washed and dried, and the inside walls of conditioning chambers were cleaned with 50 ppm sodium hypochlorite. Additionally, the doors of sound attenuating cubicles and conditioning chambers were opened and exhaust fans were turned on for ventilation until experiments. For testing with a novel context, a gray acrylic triangle roof and a white acrylic floor were used (see below).
Conditioning and testing protocols
All experiments were conducted within the light cycle. A discriminative fear conditioning paradigm consisted of 3-d conditioning (day 1 to 3) and 2-d testing (day 4 and 5) sessions with a conditioning context (context A) on day 1 to 4 and a novel context (context B) on day 5 (Fig. 1A). In a conditioning session, four trials with conditioned stimuli (CS+) paired with the US and four trials with conditioned stimuli (CS−) explicitly unpaired with the US were performed in a pseudorandom order, with the inter-trial interval randomly selected from four intervals: 60, 70, 80, and 90 sec. Each conditioning session lasted for 600 sec, and the next session started 24 ± 2 h later (one session in a day). CS+ was a 10 sec white noise (125 msec pulse delivered at 4 Hz), and CS− was a 10 sec 12 kHz tone (250 msec pulse delivered at 2 Hz). In the second experiments, the tone and noise pulse trains were assigned as the CS+ and CS−, respectively. A 4 kHz and a 12 kHz tone pulse trains (both of which were delivered as 250 msec pulses at 2 Hz) were assigned as the CS+ and CS−, respectively, in the third experiments. The CS was presented at 80 dB sound pressure level. The US was delivered during the last 2 sec of the CS+, so that they coterminated. Mice were not habituated to the conditioning chamber and sound stimuli before conditioning to avoid a masking effect on contextual fear acquisition and a potential effect on reversal learning for the CS+, respectively. For testing sessions on day 4 and 5, three pairs of trials with the CS+ and CS− were performed in an alternate order with the same inter-trial interval range as conditioning sessions in the absence of the US. Each testing session lasted for 450 sec. For testing with a novel context (context B) on day 5, the session was carried out in the conditioning chamber whose grid floor was covered with a white acrylic plate and a gray acrylic triangle roof. Soon after each session, mice were returned to their home cage and transferred to the breeding room.
Data analysis
Freezing was defined as the motion index less than or equal to 2 with the bout duration more than or equal to 1 sec. To evaluate behavioral responses to the CS, the percentage of time spent in freezing during CS (CS+/CS−) and during the 10 sec prior to the CS (PreCS+/PreCS−) was calculated for each trial and each session. The final 2 sec of both CS+ and CS− in the 3-d conditioning sessions was excluded from analysis to eliminate the effect of the US. To compare cue-evoked responses among animals, freezing level during the PreCS was subtracted from that during the CS for each trial to eliminate the influence of contextual fear. To eliminate a potential bias for the freezing levels caused by incidental extreme outliers that can appear in a trial of the session, median values were sampled from the data sets of four trials (Day 1–3) or three trials (Day 4 and 5) in each session for the PreCS+, CS+, PreCS−, CS−, or trial-by-trial subtraction of the PreCS from the corresponding CS for each animal tested, and averaged by the number of animals for each session. For the comparison of the PreCS among days (day 1–3, Fig. 1B), medians from eight trials (including four trials with the CS+ and four trials with the CS−) were extracted for each animal, and averaged by the number of animals for each session. For the assessment of contextual fear memory, data were sampled from the PreCS period of the first trial in each session (Fig. 1C). For statistical analysis, the percentages of time spent in freezing were compared using a paired t-test except for multiple comparisons among 3-d sessions with the Bonferroni's multiple comparison test. Significance of negative or positive changes in freezing level from the PreCS to the CS was analyzed using a one-tailed t-test. Data are expressed as means ± standard error of the mean (SEM). Differences were considered statistically significant if P < 0.05.
Competing interest statement
The authors declare that this study was conducted in the absence of any financial or commercial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank the Kumamoto University Institute of Resource Development and Analysis Center for Animal Resources and Development for animal care and management of fear conditioning devices. This work was supported by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C) (16K10218) and partly by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (17H05749) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science.
Footnotes
Article is online at http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/doi/10.1101/lm.049577.119.
References
- Abs E, Poorthuis RB, Apelblat D, Muhammad K, Pardi MB, Enke L, Kushinsky D, Pu DL, Eizinger MF, Conzelmann KK, et al. 2018. Learning-related plasticity in dendrite-targeting layer 1 interneurons. Neuron 100: 684–699. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aizenberg M, Geffen MN. 2013. Bidirectional effects of aversive learning on perceptual acuity are mediated by the sensory cortex. Nat Neurosci 16: 994–996. 10.1038/nn.3443 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ambroggi F, Ishikawa A, Fields HL, Nicola SM. 2008. Basolateral amygdala neurons facilitate reward-seeking behavior by exciting nucleus accumbens neurons. Neuron 59: 648–661. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antunes R, Moita MA. 2010. Discriminative auditory fear learning requires both tuned and nontuned auditory pathways to the amygdala. J Neurosci 30: 9782–9787. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1037-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldi E, Lorenzini CA, Bucherelli C. 2004. Footshock intensity and generalization in contextual and auditory-cued fear conditioning in the rat. Neurobiol Learn Mem 81: 162–166. 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine BW, Delgado MR, Hikosaka O. 2007. The role of the dorsal striatum in reward and decision-making. J Neurosci 27: 8161–8165. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1554-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botta P, Demmou L, Kasugai Y, Markovic M, Xu C, Fadok JP, Lu T, Poe MM, Xu L, Cook JM, et al. 2015. Regulating anxiety with extrasynaptic inhibition. Nat Neurosci 18: 1493–1500. 10.1038/nn.4102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braga RJ, Reynolds GP, Siris SG. 2013. Anxiety comorbidity in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 210: 1–7. 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.07.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christianson JP, Fernando ABP, Kazama AM, Jovanovic T, Ostroff LE, Sangha S. 2012. Inhibition of fear by learned safety signals: a mini-symposium review. J Neurosci 32: 14118–14124. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3340-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Courtin J, Chaudun F, Rozeske RR, Karalis N, Gonzalez-Campo C, Wurtz H, Abdi A, Baufreton J, Bienvenu TCM, Herry C. 2014. Prefrontal parvalbumin interneurons shape neuronal activity to drive fear expression. Nature 505: 92–96. 10.1038/nature12755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, Paz R. 2015. Fear generalization and anxiety: behavioral and neural mechanisms. Biol Psychiatry 78: 336–343. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etkin A, Wager TD. 2007. Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am J Psychiatry 164: 1476–1488. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foilb AR, Flyer-Adams JG, Maier SF, Christianson JP. 2016. Posterior insular cortex is necessary for conditioned inhibition of fear. Neurobiol Learn Mem 134: 317–327. 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerlai R. 1998. Contextual learning and cue association in fear conditioning in mice: a strain comparison and a lesion study. Behav Brain Res 95: 191–203. 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00144-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghosh S, Chattarji S. 2014. Neuronal encoding of the switch from specific to generalized fear. Nat Neurosci 18: 112–120. 10.1038/nn.3888 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haber SN, Knutson B. 2010. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35: 4–26. 10.1038/npp.2009.129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammond LJ. 1967. A traditional demonstration of the properties of Pavlovian inhibition using differential CER. Psychon Sci 9: 65–66. 10.3758/BF03330761 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Holt DJ, Coombs G, Zeidan MA, Goff DC, Milad MR. 2012. Failure of neural responses to safety cues in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69: 893–903. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jovanovic T, Kazama A, Bachevalier J, Davis M. 2012. Impaired safety signal learning may be a biomarker of PTSD. Neuropharmacology 62: 695–704. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.02.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kheirbek MA, Klemenhagen KC, Sahay A, Hen R. 2012. Neurogenesis and generalization: a new approach to stratify and treat anxiety disorders. Nat Neurosci 15: 1613–1620. 10.1038/nn.3262 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiran C, Chaudhury S. 2016. Prevalence of comorbid anxiety disorders in schizophrenia. Ind Psychiatry J 25: 35 10.4103/0972-6748.196045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kong E, Monje FJ, Hirsch J, Pollak DD. 2014. Learning not to fear: neural correlates of learned safety. Neuropsychopharmacology 39: 515–527. 10.1038/npp.2013.191 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopec CD, Kessels HWHG, Bush DEA, Cain CK, LeDoux JE, Malinow R. 2007. A robust automated method to analyze rodent motion during fear conditioning. Neuropharmacology 52: 228–233. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.07.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laxmi TR, Stork O, Pape HC. 2003. Generalisation of conditioned fear and its behavioural expression in mice. Behav Brain Res 145: 89–98. 10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00101-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Letzkus JJ, Wolff SBE, Meyer EMM, Tovote P, Courtin J, Herry C, Lüthi A. 2011. A disinhibitory microcircuit for associative fear learning in the auditory cortex. Nature 480: 331–335. 10.1038/nature10674 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Namburi P, Beyeler A, Yorozu S, Calhoon GG, Halbert SA, Wichmann R, Holden SS, Mertens KL, Anahtar M, Felix-Ortiz AC, et al. 2015. A circuit mechanism for differentiating positive and negative associations. Nature 520: 675–678. 10.1038/nature14366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ng KH, Pollock MW, Urbanczyk PJ, Sangha S. 2018. Altering D1 receptor activity in the basolateral amygdala impairs fear suppression during a safety cue. Neurobiol Learn Mem 147: 26–34. 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.11.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak DD, Monje FJ, Zuckerman L, Denny CA, Drew MR, Kandel ER. 2008. An animal model of a behavioral intervention for depression. Neuron 60: 149–161. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak DD, Monje FJ, Lubec G. 2010. The learned safety paradigm as a mouse model for neuropsychiatric research. Nat Protoc 5: 954–962. 10.1038/nprot.2010.64 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. 1969. Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychol Bull 72: 77–94. 10.1037/h0027760 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rogan MT, Leon KS, Perez DL, Kandel ER. 2005. Distinct neural signatures for safety and danger in the amygdala and striatum of the mouse. Neuron 46: 309–320. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sangha S, Chadick JZ, Janak PH. 2013. Safety encoding in the basal amygdala. J Neurosci 33: 3744–3751. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3302-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Steensel FJA, Bögels SM, Perrin S. 2011. Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 14: 302–317. 10.1007/s10567-011-0097-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White SW, Mazefsky CA, Dichter GS, Chiu PH, Richey JA, Ollendick TH. 2014. Social-cognitive, physiological, and neural mechanisms underlying emotion regulation impairments: understanding anxiety in autism spectrum disorder. Int J Dev Neurosci 39: 22–36. 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.05.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]