Abstract
Background
Cognitive capabilities in childhood and in late life are inversely associated with mortality rates. However, it is unclear if adult cognition, at a time still relatively free from comorbidity, is associated with subsequent mortality, and whether this explains the associations of early life factors with adult mortality.
Methods
We used data from the MRC National Survey of Health and Development, a birth cohort study prospectively assessing 5362 participants born in 1946. The present analysis includes participants followed up from age 43 and undergoing cognitive assessment (verbal memory and search speed). Mortality outcomes were notified through linkage with a national register. Cox regression was used to estimate mortality hazards in relation to cognitive performance at age 43, adjusting for early life factors, socioeconomic position and health status.
Results
Data were available on 3192 individuals. Univariable analyses indicated that adult verbal memory and search speed, parental factors, childhood cognition and educational attainment were associated with mortality. However, multivariable models showed that the mortality associations with earlier life factors were explained by adult cognitive capability. A standard deviation increase in verbal memory and search speed scores was associated with lower mortality rates [hazard ratio (HR) = 0.86, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.77-0.97, P = 0.02; HR = 0.88, 95% CI 0.78-1.00, P = 0.05, respectively), after adjustment for adult health.
Conclusions
Cognitive capability in early midlife was inversely associated with mortality rates over 25 years and accounted for the associations of family background, childhood cognitive ability and educational attainment with mortality. These findings, in a nationally representative cohort with long-term follow-up, suggest that building cognitive reserve may improve later life health and survival chances.
Keywords: Birth cohort, life course epidemiology, cognition, verbal memory, search speed, mortality
Introduction
Cognitive capabilities underpin health and well-being, and inverse relationships with mortality have been consistently described.1–4 Observed associations between cognition and mortality may have multiple explanations, perhaps varying in importance at different stages of growth, stability and decline. 5 Most of the literature on cognitive capability and mortality has examined the relationships when cognition is measured in childhood and early adulthood6–8 or in late life;9,10–15 few studies have measures of cognitive capabilities in child and adult life and a long mortality follow-up.
A systematic review and meta-analysis of 16 prospective studies showed each standard deviation advantage in child or adolescent cognitive ability was associated with a hazard ratio (HR) of 0.76 [95% confidence interval (CI) 0.74-0.77] for all-cause mortality.6 Whether this association is due to cognitive ability acting as a mediator of early adversity, a marker of ‘physiological integrity’ or a determinant of subsequent educational attainment, adult socioeconomic position and other life chances is an ongoing and policy-relevant debate;3,16–18 these possibilities are not mutually exclusive. In the systematic review, adjusting for SEP in childhood did not affect the HR, though accounting for adult SEP and education attenuated the estimates of effect size. These observations are consistent with findings from the Medical Research Council (MRC) National Survey of Health and Development (NSHD) demonstrating that childhood cognition explained some, but not all, of the association between childhood SEP and adult mortality in adulthood up to age 60 years, whereas education and adult SEP strongly attenuated the association.19
The cognition-mortality associations seen in older populations9,12–14 may also be driven by lifetime socioeconomic factors and/or by a range of health conditions (clinical or subclinical) including obesity, diabetes, vascular disease and dementia.20–22 Here, the development of common chronic pathophysiological processes (e.g. atherosclerosis, inflammation) may have an impact on cognition as well as mortality.
Are the associations found in youth or later life also evident in early midlife? Do the factors associated with mortality identified in childhood exert their influence through adult cognition, and at an age still relatively free from comorbidity? The relative impact of child and adult cognition on mortality might have implications for interventions aimed at reducing health inequalities.23 Similarly, whether any associations are linear (for the whole population) or only pertain to a subset of cognitive ability, is relevant from a policy perspective. Finally, associations with mortality in connection with specific cognitive domains or tasks (e.g. episodic memory or reaction speed) or specific causes of death, may suggest different underlying biological mechanisms. For example, neuroimaging studies lend some support to the hypothesis that search speed is a fundamental mental function representing brain integrity, and as such might be more closely related to mortality than other cognitive tasks.24,25 Cohort studies with prospective life course data, such as the NSHD, offer an opportunity to investigate how different measures of cognitive capability in early midlife are related to subsequent mortality, and whether any associations are influenced by lifetime socioeconomic circumstances, early cognitive ability or health status.6,19 This study set out to address the following questions.
Are verbal memory and search speed in early midlife (at age 43) associated with all-cause mortality over the subsequent 25 years?
To what extent are the associations of family background and childhood cognitive ability with mortality explained by cognition in early midlife?
To what extent are any relationships between cognitive capability in early midlife and subsequent mortality explained by adult SEP or health status?
To what extent are the relationships between cognitive capability in early midlife and subsequent all-cause mortality also seen for cardiovascular mortality and cancer mortality?
Methods
The NSHD is a birth cohort of 5362 men and women, a socially stratified sample from a maternity survey of all births recorded during 1 week in March 1946 in England, Scotland and Wales, and followed up over 20 times since.26,27 Study participants were flagged for death notification on the National Health Service Central Register in 1971 at age 25.
In 1989, when participants were 43 years old, 361 (7%) of the original sample had died, 540 (10%) had permanently refused, 607 (11%) were living abroad and 90 (2%) were permanently untraced. Of the remaining 3749, 3262 (87%) were seen by a research nurse at home;26 the majority of these (3247) were flagged for death notification. Previous analyses have examined the representativeness of the cohort over time.28 Ethical approval was obtained from the Multicentre Research Ethics Committee, and written informed consent was obtained from the study member for each component of the data collection.
Outcome
We included deaths from any cause notified from March 1989 (43 years) until March 2014 (68 years). The primary cause of death was coded using either ICD9 or ICD10 disease classifications, and mortality from cardiovascular diseases (ICD9 codes 401-454 and ICD10 codes I10-I89) and cancers (ICD9 codes 140-239 and ICD10 codes C00-C97) were distinguished from other causes.
Verbal memory and visual search speed at age 43
All cognitive assessments were carried out by research nurses according to a standardized protocol. Verbal memory was assessed using a 15-item word learning task, where each word was presented for 2 s. The score represents the total number of words correctly recalled over three identical trials (maximum 45). The visual search speed task required participants to cross out the letters P and W, randomly embedded within a grid of other letters, in 1 min. The score represents the total number of letters searched (maximum 600), minus the number of targets missed. Of the 3247 participants, 3192 (98%) had at least one of these measures available; this is the overall sample for analysis.
Covariates
Covariates were selected on the basis of factors previously demonstrated to influence mortality rates in adulthood.19,28–30 These include childhood socioeconomic conditions, cognitive development based on childhood cognitive tests, educational attainment and adult health and lifestyle factors.
Childhood socioeconomic conditions were assessed by paternal and maternal education (primary, more than primary) and father’s occupation when participants were aged 4 (or at 11 or 15 years if missing at age 4, n = 29). Father’s occupational class was defined according to the Registrar General’s classification (classes I and II, professional and managerial; class III, skilled non-manual and manual; and IV and V, semi-skilled or unskilled manual occupations). Childhood cognition was measured at age 8, using tests of reading comprehension, pronunciation, vocabulary and non-verbal reasoning, designed especially for the study by the National Foundation for Educational Research in England and Wales.31 In keeping with previous analyses, scores in each of these domains were summed (after rescaling for equal weighting of each test) and standardized to the sample.32 Educational attainment by age 26 was classified as: no qualification or below ordinary secondary qualifications (vocational); ordinary secondary qualifications (‘O’ levels and their training equivalents); advanced secondary qualifications (‘A’ levels and their equivalents, and higher qualifications of degree or equivalent).
Participants’ occupational class in adulthood was defined according to the Registrar-General’s classification in the same way as for father’s occupational class. Indicators of health status were obtained by the research nurse at the home visit at age 43.33 These included: two measurements of systolic blood pressure using a random zero sphygmomanometer (the second measure was used in this analysis); body mass index (BMI) calculated from height and weight measured by standard protocols; self-reported doctor-diagnosed cancer, stroke and diabetes; and the World Health Organization Rose angina scale.34
Statistical analyses
The distribution and completeness of all variables was examined. Proportional hazards for mortality were assessed in a series of Kaplan-Meier plots and Cox regression models, where outcomes were death between ages 43 and 68 (Figure 1). Follow-up was until date of death, date of emigration, or March 2014 (if still alive and continuing to participate). Verbal memory and search speed were both standardized to have a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1 in order to compare the effect sizes. Each analysis used cases where complete data were available for all covariates.
Correlations between the child and adult cognitive scores were examined. The relationships between both of the adult cognitive scores and subsequent mortality were first investigated in Cox regression models using the maximum available samples, and Kaplan-Meier survival curves were plotted with the scores divided by tertiles. The first series of Cox regression models tested the sex-adjusted associations between each adult cognitive score and each early life variable (parental variables, childhood cognition, education) with mortality, and then mutually adjusted in a multivariable model, using participants with complete data. Interactions between sex and cognition at age 43 were tested. A second series of multivariable models included both adult cognitive scores and the health status variables and then additionally adult occupational class. Comparison of model fit, assessed through tests of maximum likelihood, was used to identify the final model. The modelling procedures were repeated for cause-specific mortality (cardiovascular disease and cancer), using a competing risks model where deaths from other causes were censored at date of death.
Post-estimation procedures included Schoenfeld residuals for checking assumptions of proportionality. In order to assess linearity of any associations across the distribution of cognition, restricted cubic splines were fitted (five knots), plotting log-hazard ratio for mortality against z-score of cognition, and linearity visually assessed. Stata version 12.1 was used for all statistical procedures.
Results
Characteristics of participants
Between ages 43 and 68, 315 deaths occurred during 80 014 person-years of observation (3.9 deaths (95% CI 3.6-4.3 deaths) per 1000 person-years). Median age at death was 60 years (interquartile range 54 to 64 years). Table 1 describes the characteristics of the sample, along with the degree of missing data. At age 43, mean number of words recalled was 25/45 (SD 6.4) and mean number of letters searched was 342/600 (SD 76). The correlations between childhood cognition and verbal memory and search speed were ρ = 0.46 and ρ = 0.09, respectively. Half of the sample was male (n = 1599, 50%), and 1062 (33%) had educational qualifications at ‘Advanced’ level or beyond. Average BMI was 25.5 kg/m2 and 30% were current smokers. By age 43, few participants had been diagnosed with diabetes, stroke or cancer and less than 4% had a positive Rose angina score.
Table 1.
N or mean | % or SD | Missing | % | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Verbal memory (raw score mean, SD) | 24.7 | 6.4 | 147 | 4.6 |
Search speed (raw score mean, SD) | 342 | 76 | 49 | 1.5 |
Men (N, %) | 1599 | 50 | 0 | |
Father’s occupational class at child age 4 | 246 | 7.7 | ||
I + II (N, %) | 680 | 21 | ||
III NM + M (N, %) | 1468 | 46 | ||
IV + V (N, %) | 798 | 25 | ||
Father’s education by age 6, > primary (N, %) | 1230 | 39 | 368 | 11.5 |
Mother’s education by age 6, > primary (N, %) | 1088 | 34 | 344 | 10.8 |
General cognition age 8 (mean, SD) | 0.0 | 1 | 366 | 11.5 |
Educational attainment by age 26 (N, %) | 171 | 5.3 | ||
< Ordinary level (N, %) | 1111 | 35 | ||
Ordinary level (N, %) | 848 | 27 | ||
Advanced level (N, %) | 1062 | 33 | ||
Adult factors at age 43 | ||||
BMI (kg/m2) (mean, SD) | 25.5 | 4.2 | 20 | 0.6 |
Systolic BP (mmHg) (mean, SD) | 123 | 16 | 53 | 1.7 |
Diabetes (N, %) | 32 | 1 | 4 | 0.1 |
Smoking status | 4 | 0.3 | ||
Current (N, %) | 957 | 30 | ||
Ex (N, %) | 1312 | 41 | ||
Never (N, %) | 919 | 29 | ||
Positive WHO Rose angina score (N, %) | 110 | 3.5 | 4 | 0.1 |
Stroke (N, %) | 11 | 0.3 | 0 | 0.0 |
Cancer (N, %) | 58 | 2 | 13 | 0.4 |
Occupational class (N, %) | 15 | 0.5 | ||
I + II (N, %) | 1382 | 43 | ||
III NM + M (N, %) | 1309 | 41 | ||
IV + V (N, %) | 486 | 15 | 15 | 0.5 |
Cognitive capability in early midlife and subsequent mortality
Correlation between verbal memory and search speed at age 43 was low (ρ = 0.16). In the maximum samples (n = 3192), performance on verbal memory and search speed (z-scores) were both associated with mortality rate (per 1 SD increase in performance, sex-adjusted verbal memory HR 0.79, 95% CI 0.70-0.89, P < 0.01; sex-adjusted search speed HR 0.83, 95% CI 0.74-0.93, P < 0.01). Kaplan-Meier survival curves describe the association of cognitive performance with mortality from age 43, by thirds of cognitive score (Figure 2). For both z-scores in verbal memory and search speed (Figure 2), mortality rates increased with decreasing test score. This linearity in the relationship was also seen when plotting restricted cubic splines of the logHR against z-score for each cognitive performance (Supplementary figures, available as Supplementary data at IJE online).
Does cognitive capability in early midlife explain the associations between early life factors and mortality?
Each adult cognitive score and each early life factor was related to mortality in the sex-adjusted models (Table 2, left column). There was no evidence of interactions between sex and the cognitive scores at age 43. In the multivariable model (Table 2, right column), the hazard ratios for the two cognitive scores were only slightly attenuated, whereas the hazard ratios for the early life variables were substantially reduced.
Table 2.
Cognition | Sex-adjusted models | Multivariable model | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HR | 95% CI | P | HR | 95% CI | P | |||
Verbal memory age 43 (per SD) | 0.73 | 0.64 | 0.83 | <0.01 | 0.76 | 0.65 | 0.89 | <0.01 |
Search speed age 43 (per SD) | 0.80 | 0.70 | 0.91 | 0.01 | 0.86 | 0.75 | 0.98 | 0.02 |
Sex (F cf. M) | 0.76 | 0.59 | 0.98 | 0.03 | 0.86 | 0.66 | 1.13 | 0.3 |
Early life factors | ||||||||
Father’s occupational class | <0.01 | 0.8 | ||||||
I + II | [ref] | [ref] | ||||||
III NM + M | 1.34 | 1.06 | 1.76 | 1.19 | 0.82 | 1.74 | ||
IV + V | 1.66 | 1.27 | 2.18 | 1.09 | 0.70 | 1.71 | ||
Father’s education (primary cf. > primary) | 0.71 | 0.54 | 0.92 | 0.01 | 0.83 | 0.59 | 1.15 | 0.3 |
Mother’s education (primary cf. > primary) | 0.74 | 0.61 | 0.91 | <0.01 | 1.10 | 0.79 | 1.51 | 0.4 |
General cognition age 8 (per SD) | 0.81 | 0.69 | 0.94 | <0.01 | 1.02 | 0.86 | 1.21 | 0.8 |
Educational attainment age 26 | <0.01 | 0.7 | ||||||
<Ordinary level | [ref] | [ref] | ||||||
Ordinary level | 0.84 | 0.61 | 1.14 | 0.95 | 0.68 | 1.34 | ||
Advanced level | 0.62 | 0.45 | 0.84 | 0.93 | 0.63 | 1.37 |
Verbal memory and search speed standardized to sample undertaking testing at age 43. Univariable models are estimated in the same sample as the multivariable model (n = 2337). All models are adjusted by sex. Sex-verbal memory and sex-search speed at age 43 interactions were tested in the sex-only adjusted models; there was no evidence of any interactions and they were not considered further. The multivariable model also includes the earlier life factors.
Schoenfeld residuals test for proportional hazards for multivariable model P = 0.2
M, male; F, female; cf., compared with.
Are relationships between cognitive capability in early midlife and subsequent mortality explained by adult SEP or health status?
Associations between cognitive capability at age 43 and mortality remained robust when included in a model with all other covariates independently associated with hazard for death [BMI, systolic blood pressure (BP), diabetes and smoking status] (fully adjusted verbal memory HR 0.86, 95% CI 0.77-0.97, P = 0.02; search speed HR 0.88, 95% CI 0.78-1.00, P = 0.05) (Table 3, Health Status model). In this model, other health factors (stroke, cancer, WHO angina score) were not associated with mortality. Occupational class at age 43 was not associated with mortality after adjusting for the adult cognitive scores, BMI, systolic BP, diabetes and smoking status (Table 3, SEP model); these latter variables were included in a final model (Table 3, Final model).
Table 3.
Cognition | Health status model | SEP model | Final model | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HR | 95% CI | P | HR | 95% CI | P | HR | 95% CI | P | ||||
Verbal memory age 43 (per SD) | 0.86 | 0.76 | 0.97 | 0.02 | 0.89 | 0.79 | 1.02 | 0.09 | 0.86 | 0.76 | 0.97 | 0.01 |
Search speed age 43 (per SD) | 0.88 | 0.78 | 1.00 | 0.05 | 0.89 | 0.79 | 1.01 | 0.07 | 0.88 | 0.78 | 1.00 | 0.05 |
Sex (F cf. M) | 0.86 | 0.67 | 1.10 | 0.2 | 0.81 | 0.63 | 1.04 | 0.1 | 0.87 | 0.68 | 1.11 | 0.3 |
Health | ||||||||||||
BMI (per 1 kg/m2) | 1.03 | 1.00 | 1.05 | 0.06 | 1.03 | 1.00 | 1.05 | 0.07 | 1.03 | 1.00 | 1.06 | 0.05 |
Systolic BP (per mmHg) | 1.01 | 1.01 | 1.02 | <0.01 | 1.01 | 1.01 | 1.02 | <0.01 | 1.02 | 1.01 | 1.02 | <0.01 |
Diabetes (yes cf. no) | 3.17 | 1.66 | 6.06 | <0.01 | 3.30 | 1.73 | 6.27 | <0.01 | 3.25 | 1.71 | 6.17 | <0.01 |
Smoking status at age 43 | <0.01 | <0.01 | ||||||||||
Current | [ref] | [ref] | [ref] | |||||||||
Ex | 0.53 | 0.41 | 0.69 | 0.55 | 0.42 | 0.71 | 0.53 | 0.40 | 0.69 | <0.01 | ||
Never | 0.40 | 0.29 | 0.56 | 0.41 | 0.29 | 0.57 | 0.40 | 0.28 | 0.55 | <0.01 | ||
WHO Rose angina score (positive cf. negative) | 1.39 | 0.84 | 2.28 | 0.2 | ||||||||
Stroke (yes cf. no) | 1.72 | 0.42 | 6.95 | 0.5 | ||||||||
Cancer (yes cf. no) | 1.09 | 0.48 | 2.46 | 0.8 | ||||||||
Occupational class age 43 | 0.06 | |||||||||||
I + II | [ref] | |||||||||||
III NM + M | 1.22 | 0.92 | 1.61 | |||||||||
IV + V | 1.41 | 0.99 | 2.02 |
Verbal memory and search speed standardized to sample undertaking testing at age 43. Schonfeld residuals test of proportional hazards for final model P = 0.2.
M, male; F, female; cf., compared with.
Are the relationships between cognitive capability in early midlife and subsequent all-cause mortality also seen for cardiovascular and cancer mortality?
In competing risks models, verbal memory was more strongly associated with cardiovascular death rate (HR 0.79, 95% CI 0.63-0.99, P = 0.04, adjusted for search speed, sex, BMI, systolic blood pressure, diabetes and smoking; search speed itself was not associated with cardiovascular mortality) (Table 4). Conversely, search speed was more strongly associated with cancer mortality (HR 0.77, 95% CI 0.65-0.91, P < 0.01, with the same adjustments reported above, and no association with verbal memory itself and cancer mortality).
Table 4.
Verbal memory Sex-adjusted model | Search speed Sex-adjusted model | Mutually and sex-adjusted | Mutually and sex-, BMI-, systolic BP-, diabetes-, smoking-adjusted | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HR | 95% CI | P | HR | 95% CI | P | HR | 95% CI | P | HR | 95% CI | P | |||||
Cardiovascular deaths (n = 89) | ||||||||||||||||
Verbal memory age 43 (per SD) | 0.68 | 0.55 | 0.84 | <0.01 | 0.68 | 0.54 | 0.84 | <0.01 | 0.79 | 0.63 | 0.99 | 0.04 | ||||
Search speed age 43 (per SD) | 0.78 | 0.63 | 0.97 | 0.03 | 0.83 | 0.67 | 1.04 | 0.1 | 0.95 | 0.76 | 1.19 | 0.7 | ||||
Cancer deaths (n = 158) | ||||||||||||||||
Verbal memory age 43 (per SD) | 0.92 | 0.78 | 1.09 | 0.3 | 0.94 | 0.80 | 1.12 | 0.5 | 0.98 | 0.82 | 1.16 | 0.8 | ||||
Search speed age 43 (per SD) | 0.75 | 0.64 | 0.88 | <0.01 | 0.76 | 0.64 | 0.91 | <0.01 | 0.77 | 0.65 | 0.91 | <0.01 |
Verbal memory and search speed standardized to sample undertaking testing at age 43.
Discussion
These analyses demonstrated an association between cognitive capability in early midlife and all-cause mortality over 25 years of follow-up, with independent effects observed for both verbal memory and search speed. Mortality associations previously shown in the NSHD with factors earlier in life, namely family background, childhood cognition and educational attainment,16,19 appeared to act through midlife cognitive capability at age 43. Associations remained evident after adjusting for health status and smoking, and were linear across the whole distribution of cognition, in contrast to findings on midlife physical capability and mortality in NSHD.30
The major strengths of these analyses are the cognitive measures from early midlife and the long, prospective follow-up of a nationally-representative population sample. They uniquely allow premature mortality from early midlife to be understood in relation to earlier measures of cognition and educational attainment. Examining a birth cohort where all participants are of the same age also allows investigation of factors unconfounded by age. Nonetheless, several limitations should be highlighted. First, in common with other cohort studies, these associations may be underpinned by residual confounding by other factors. There has been some inevitable loss to follow-up, yet the sample remains broadly representative of the census population.35 We adjusted for childhood cognition at age 8 as a measure of ability near the beginning of education; however, growth in cognitive capability throughout schooling and professional development may not be fully captured here. By and large, there were few missing data, but estimates may have been biased by complete case analysis. However, comparison of the estimates using a maximum sample size (sex-only adjusted model) showed little difference compared with those in Tables 2 and 3. Finally, the observed associations may be subject to secular trends and therefore specific to the generational era.23
Our findings are consistent with and extend other studies reporting mortality associations with cognition, by having prospective measures of cognitive capabilities in childhood and in two domains in early midlife and following up the sample for a longer duration. In the Whitehall II study, 5572 individuals were followed up from an average age in the fifth decade over 8 years, also demonstrating linear mortality associations over several domains.36 Our results are also comparable to analyses in the Caerphilly Prospective Study, despite the greater age of that cohort and differences in analytical approach (n = 1870 men age 55 to 69 at baseline for 15 years).17 The Lothian Birth Cohort had measures of cognition at ages 11 and 79; here, cognition in childhood appeared to have less influence than cognition assessed in older age on subsequent survival, and the change in cognition over the life course was also informative,37
The degree to which observed associations with mortality are specific to a cognitive domain remains unclear. The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study (n = 11 444, mean age 57 years, 6.3 years’ follow-up), showed that tests of speed (digit substitution) and recall (delayed word recall) were independently associated with mortality, even in this cohort with relatively little subclinical vascular disease.38 Participants in the Twenty-07 study (n = 898, mean age 56, 14 years’ follow-up) showed that general intelligence (IQ) was not associated with mortality once the estimate was adjusted for reaction time (itself robustly associated with mortality).24 This finding was broadly consistent with analyses in the Health and Lifestyle Survey (n = 6424, age range 18 to 97, followed for 21 years), but too few events occurred after measuring midlife cognition to be conclusive.15 However, both these contrast with a finding that verbal memory was more important when the Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 was assessed at age 79.37 Taken together with the analyses presented here, these differing observations may reflect underlying differences in the cohorts, the specificity of a given test for a particular cognitive domain, the age at time of testing and the degree of adjustment with other cognitive and health variables.
The underlying mechanisms that link cognition in early midlife to mortality remain largely unknown, though childhood cognition may have an impact on biological factors putatively associated with accelerated ageing.39 Prospective measures of SEP and health factors do not fully account for these observations, at least to the extent such variables were measured in NSHD. Both verbal memory (a largely cortical function) and search speed (with a large subcortical component) are evidently important, and we did not demonstrate primary importance for one measured domain over the other. It is unclear why these might differentially be associated with cardiovascular or cancer mortality, though we would emphasize the need for caution when interpreting these classifications. Other comparable studies have not always reported cause-specific mortality in relation to cognitive domains, and results are not necessarily consistent in magnitude or direction.14,15,40 Health literacy may play a part, but may itself be a marker of general cognitive capability.41,42 Future analysis should consider trajectories of cognitive decline from age 43 onwards, particularly if longitudinal change is even more strongly associated with widening socioeconomic inequality or change in health status.
The extent to which adult cognition is modifiable in the population has been under-explored from a policy perspective. 43 Nonetheless, population-based interventions to enhance cognitive capability at all stages of the life course may be beneficial at the individual and societal levels.44,45
In conclusion, in a large British cohort study, cognitive capability in early midlife was associated with mortality into the seventh decade of life, independently of physical health and smoking status, and accounted for the previous effects of childhood cognition, educational attainment and lifetime socioeconomic position. Overall, these findings raise interesting possibilities around building cognitive reserve into midlife, with implications for social, educational and public health policy.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary data are available at IJE online.
Key Messages.
Previous studies have shown that cognitive capabilities in childhood and in late life are inversely associated with mortality rates; studies with cognitive measures in child and adult life and long mortality follow-up are rare.
Higher cognitive capability in early midlife, as measured by verbal memory and search speed, was associated with lower mortality rates over 25 years of follow-up.
The associations were linear across the whole range of these cognitive domains and were not explained by adjustment for socioeconomic position or health status.
Cognitive capability in early midlife accounted for the inverse associations of family background, childhood cognitive ability and educational attainment with mortality rates.
These findings suggest that social, educational and public health policies that build cognitive reserve could have important implications for later life health and survival.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank all NSHD study members and the NSHD scientific and data collection teams. Data used in this publication are available upon request to the MRC National Survey of Health and Development Data Sharing Committee. Further details can be found at http://www.nshd.mrc.ac.uk/data. doi: 10.5522/NSHD/Q101; doi: 10.5522/NSHD/Q102.
Funding
The NSHD is supported by core and grant funding (programme codes: MC_UU_12019/1, MC_UU_12019/3, MC_UU_12019/4) from the UK Medical Research Council.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
- 1.Fried LP, Kronmal RA, Newman AB, et al. Risk factors for 5-year mortality in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study. JAMA. 1998;279:585–92. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.8.585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Deary IJ, Whiteman MC, Starr JM, Whalley LJ, Fox HC. The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: following up the Scottish mental surveys of 1932 and 1947. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;86:130–47. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Gale CR, Martyn CN, Cooper C. Cognitive impairment and mortality in a cohort of elderly people. BMJ. 1996;312:608–11. doi: 10.1136/bmj.312.7031.608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Tilvis RS, Kahonen-Vare MH, Jolkkonen J, Valvanne J, Pitkala KH, Strandberg TE. Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59:268–74. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.3.m268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ben-Shlomo Y, Kuh D. A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. Int J Epidemiol. 2002;31:285–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Calvin CM, Deary IJ, Fenton C, et al. Intelligence in youth and all-cause-mortality: systematic review with meta-analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40:626–44. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sjolund S, Hemmingsson T, Gustafsson JE, Allebeck P. IQ and alcohol-related morbidity and mortality among Swedish men and women: the importance of socioeconomic position. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2015;69:858–64. doi: 10.1136/jech-2014-204761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Batty GD, Wennerstad KM, Davey Smith G, et al. IQ in early adulthood and mortality by middle age: cohort study of 1 million Swedish men. Epidemiology. 2009;20:100–09. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31818ba076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Dewey ME, Saz P. Dementia, cognitive impairment and mortality in persons aged 65 and over living in the community: a systematic review of the literature. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;16:751–61. doi: 10.1002/gps.397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Guehne U, Angermeyer MC, Riedel-Heller S. Is mortality increased in mildly cognitively impaired individuals? A systematic literature review. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;21:403–10. doi: 10.1159/000092846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kane RL, Shamliyan T, Talley K, Pacala J. The association between geriatric syndromes and survival. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60:896–904. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.03942.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Contador I, Bermejo-Pareja F, Mitchell AJ, et al. Cause of death in mild cognitive impairment: a prospective study (NEDICES) Eur J Neurol. 2014;21:253–59. doi: 10.1111/ene.12278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Clarke PJ, Blount V, Colantonio A. Cognitive impairment predicts fatal incident stroke: findings from a national sample of older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:1490–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03494.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.O’Donnell M, Teo K, Gao P, et al. Cognitive impairment and risk of cardiovascular events and mortality. Eur Heart J. 2012;33:1777–86. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehs053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Shipley BA, Der G, Taylor MD, Deary IJ. Cognition and mortality from the major causes of death: the Health and Lifestyle Survey. J Psychosom Res. 2008;65:143–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kuh D, Richards M, Hardy R, Butterworth S, Wadsworth ME. Childhood cognitive ability and deaths up until middle age: a post-war birth cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2004;33:408–13. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Gallacher J, Bayer A, Dunstan F, Yarnell J, Elwood P, Ben-Shlomo Y. Can we understand why cognitive function predicts mortality? Results from the Caerphilly Prospective Study (CaPS) Intelligence. 2009;37:535–44. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Batty GD, Der G, Macintyre S, Deary IJ. Does IQ explain socioeconomic inequalities in health? Evidence from a population based cohort study in the west of Scotland. BMJ. 2006;332:580–84. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38723.660637.AE. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kuh D, Shah I, Richards M, Mishra G, Wadsworth M, Hardy R. Do childhood cognitive ability or smoking behaviour explain the influence of lifetime socioeconomic conditions on premature adult mortality in a British post war birth cohort? Soc Sci Med. 2009;68:1565–73. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Arden R, Gottfredson LS, Miller G. Does a fitness factor contribute to the association between intelligence and health outcomes? Evidence from medical abnormality counts among 3654 US Veterans. Intelligence. 2009;37:581–91. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Batty GD, Gale CR, Mortensen LH, Langenberg C, Shipley MJ, Deary IJ. Pre-morbid intelligence, the metabolic syndrome and mortality: the Vietnam Experience Study. Diabetologia. 2008;51:436–43. doi: 10.1007/s00125-007-0908-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Belsky DW, Caspi A, Goldman-Mellor S, et al. Is obesity associated with a decline in intelligence quotient during the first half of the life course? Am J Epidemiol. 2013;178:1461–68. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwt135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Richards M, Power C, Sacker A. Paths to literacy and numeracy problems: evidence from two British birth cohorts. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2009;63:239–44. doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.064923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Deary IJ, Der G. Reaction time explains IQ’s association with death. Psychol Sci. 2005;16:64–69. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00781.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Penke L, Maniega SM, Bastin ME, et al. Brain-wide white matter tract integrity is associated with information processing speed and general intelligence. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17:955. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wadsworth M, Kuh D, Richards M, Hardy R. Cohort Profile: The 1946 National Birth Cohort (MRC National Survey of Health and Development) Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35:49–54. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyi201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kuh D, Pierce M, Adams J, et al. Cohort Profile: Updating the cohort profile for the MRC National Survey of Health and Development: a new clinic-based data collection for ageing research. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40:e1–9. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wadsworth ME, Mann SL, Rodgers B, Kuh DJ, Hilder WS, Yusuf EJ. Loss and representativeness in a 43 year follow up of a national birth cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1992;46:300–04. doi: 10.1136/jech.46.3.300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Strand BH, Kuh D, Shah I, Guralnik J, Hardy R. Childhood, adolescent and early adult body mass index in relation to adult mortality: results from the British 1946 birth cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2012;66:225–32. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.110155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cooper R, Strand BH, Hardy R, Patel KV, Kuh D. Physical capability in mid-life and survival over 13 years of follow-up: British birth cohort study. BMJ. 2014;348:g2219. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g2219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Douglas JWB. The Home and the School: A Study of Ability and Attainment in the Primary Schools. London: MacGibbon and Kee; 1964. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Richards M, Sacker A. Lifetime antecedents of cognitive reserve. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2003;25:614–24. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.614.14581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hardy R, Wadsworth ME, Langenberg C, Kuh D. Birthweight, childhood growth, and blood pressure at 43 years in a British birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2004;33:121–29. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rose GA. The diagnosis of ischaemic heart pain and intermittent claudication in field surveys. Bull World Health Organ. 1962;27:645–58. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Wadsworth ME, Butterworth SL, Hardy RJ, et al. The life course prospective design: an example of benefits and problems associated with study longevity. Soc Sci Med. 2003;57:2193–205. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(03)00083-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Sabia S, Gueguen A, Marmot MG, Shipley MJ, Ankri J, Singh-Manoux A. Does cognition predict mortality in midlife? Results from the Whitehall II cohort study. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31:688–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Murray C, Pattie A, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Does cognitive ability predict mortality in the ninth decade? The Lothian Birth Cohort 1921. Intelligence. 2012;40:490–98. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Pavlik VN, de Moraes SA, Szklo M, Knopman DS, Mosley TH, Jr, Hyman DJ. Relation between cognitive function and mortality in middle-aged adults: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157:327–34. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Schaefer JD, Caspi A, Belsky DW, et al. Early-life intelligence predicts midlife biological age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015 May 26; doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv035. pii: gbv035. [Epub ahead of print.] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Benito-Leon J, Romero JP, Louis ED, Bermejo-Pareja F. Faster cognitive decline in elders without dementia and decreased risk of cancer mortality: NEDICES Study. Neurology. 2014;82:1441–48. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bostock S, Steptoe A. Association between low functional health literacy and mortality in older adults: longitudinal cohort study. BMJ. 2012;344:e1602. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e1602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Reeve CL, Basalik D. Is health literacy an example of construct proliferation? A conceptual and empirical evaluation of its redundancy with general cognitive ability. Intelligence. 2014;44:93–102. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Government Office for Science. Foresight Mental Capital and Wellbeing Project. Final project report, executive summary. London: GO-Science; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Richards M, Deary IJ. A life course approach to cognitive capability. In: Kuh D, Cooper R, Hardy R, Richards M, Ben-Shlomo Y, editors. A Life Course Approach to Healthy Ageing. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Richards M, Deary IJ. A life course approach to cognitive reserve: a model for cognitive aging and development? Ann Neurol. 2005;58:617–22. doi: 10.1002/ana.20637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.