Significance
Serotonin transporter polymorphisms within the primate-specific promoter repeat region are associated with altered emotion regulation, vulnerability to psychiatric disorders, and differences in antidepressant responsiveness. However, the underlying neurobiological mechanisms are poorly understood. Using positron emission tomography imaging, a behavioral anxiety test, and post-mortem expression measurements, we show that marmoset monkeys carrying a high anxiety-related serotonin transporter variant have reduced serotonin 2A receptor (5HT2A) binding and RNA expression in right posterior insula and differential sensitivity to anxiogenic effects of acute 5HT2A antagonism, the latter predicted by 5HT2A binding in adjacent right anterior insula and insula proisocortex. Our findings highlight the value of marmosets in studying gene–brain–behavior relationships and may contribute to genetic and imaging screens development in personalized medicine for treating anxiety.
Keywords: serotonin transporter polymorphism, insula cortex, serotonin 2A receptor, anxiety
Abstract
Genetic variation in the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) is associated with vulnerability to affective disorders and pharmacotherapy efficacy. We recently identified sequence polymorphisms in the common marmoset SLC6A4 repeat region (AC/C/G and CT/T/C) associated with individual differences in anxiety-like trait, gene expression, and response to antidepressants. The mechanisms underlying the effects of these polymorphisms are unknown, but a key mediator of serotonin action is the serotonin 2A receptor (5HT2A). Thus, we correlated 5HT2A binding potential (BP) and RNA gene expression in 16 SLC6A4 genotyped marmosets with responsivity to 5HT2A antagonism during the human intruder test of anxiety. Voxel-based analysis and RNA measurements showed a reduction in 5HT2A BP and gene expression specifically in the right posterior insula of individuals homozygous for the anxiety-related variant AC/C/G. These same marmosets displayed an anxiogenic, dose-dependent response to the human intruder after 5HT2A pharmacological antagonism, while CT/T/C individuals showed no effect. A voxel-based correlation analysis, independent of SLC6A4 genotype, revealed that 5HT2A BP in the adjacent right anterior insula and insula proisocortex was negatively correlated with trait anxiety scores. Moreover, 5HT2A BP in both regions was a good predictor of the size and direction of the acute emotional response to the human intruder threat after 5HT2A antagonism. Our findings suggest that genetic variation in the SLC6A4 repeat region may contribute to the trait anxious phenotype via neurochemical changes in brain areas implicated in interoceptive and emotional processing, with a critical role for the right insula 5HT2A in the regulation of affective responses to threat.
Genetic variation in the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) has been associated with early life stress reactivity, vulnerability to affective disorders, and altered social cognition (1). Specifically, a variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) located upstream of the promoter region have been identified in human and non-human primates, with short alleles being linked to reduced gene expression and emotionally vulnerable phenotypes (2, 3). Neuroimaging studies have provided strong evidence supporting these gene-behavioral associations and suggest neurodevelopmental mechanisms underlying the SLC6A4 genetic variation. Individuals carrying the short alleles show hyperactivity to threat-related stimuli (e.g., fearful faces or Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in humans, and a human intruder in macaques) or during negative self-reflection in a network of structures that consistently include the amygdala, insula, and dorsomedial prefrontal/anterior cingulate cortex (4–11). Altered connectivity has also been reported between structures in this network and related areas, including medial, lateral, and orbital prefrontal cortex in short allele carriers (8, 12–14), suggesting impaired regulation of the emotional response. In particular, the right anterior insula has been implicated in altered emotion regulation in short allele carriers (9, 15, 16). This key region integrates the interoceptive information from the posterior insula with cognitive processing from prefrontal areas, and it is hypothesized to form the subjective feeling of emotion and self-awareness, specifically with respect to negative affect (17, 18). In addition to these functional changes, reduced gray matter volume in areas involved in emotional processing, including the amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampus, have also been described in short allele carriers (12, 19). Finally, although serotonin transporter binding studies have not provided consistent results (19–22), reduced serotonin 1A receptor binding in cortical areas has been reported in short allele carriers, both in humans and macaques (23, 24).
Polymorphisms in SLC6A4 have also been associated with treatment efficacy of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), with short allele carriers showing a slower remission rate and more severe side effects (25). While SSRIs are the most widely used drugs for the treatment of anxiety and depression, 1/3 of the patients show a poor response (26), and it has been proposed that the anxiety experienced by some patients at the early stages of treatment may contribute to this reduced treatment efficacy (27). In some SSRI-resistant patients, a therapy combining SSRIs with serotonin 2 receptor (5HT2) antagonists has been shown to improve the efficacy of the antidepressant treatment (27–29). However, the mechanisms by which 5HT2 regulates the emotional response are still unknown and vary according to the specific brain areas and the type of behaviors measured (28). Reports studying brain serotonin receptor alterations have provided conflicting results with respect to depression (30), and little is known about anxiety disorders. However, the density of medial prefrontal cortex serotonin 2A receptor (5HT2A) does correlate negatively with right amygdala reactivity to threat stimuli (fearful faces) (31), but only when 5HT1A binding is relatively low (32). In addition, a positron emission tomography (PET) imaging study in monozygotic twins suggests that ∼40 to 50% of interindividual variability in cortical 5HT2A density is genetically driven (33). However, whether the SLC6A4 polymorphisms contribute to alterations in cortical 5HT2A density, and subsequently to the anxious phenotype, has not yet been explored.
Studies in animals can provide a detailed mechanistic understanding of the brain–behavior interactions related to this polymorphism, but other than humans, the SLC6A4 repeat region has only been found in primates, including apes and monkeys, but not in prosimians or rodents (2). Moreover, a VNTR within the SLC6A4 repeat region has only been reported in humans and Old World monkeys, but not in New World monkeys such as marmosets (34). However, we recently identified sequence variation within the common marmoset SLC6A4 repeat region that revealed a difference in SLC6A4 RNA expression patterns and sensitivity to threat stimuli similar to that of the human and macaque SLC6A4 VNTR. Thus, marmosets carrying the low-expressing haplotype (AC/C/G) show an enhanced anxiety-like behavioral repertoire toward a human intruder threat compared with marmosets carrying the high-expressing variant (CT/T/C) (35). Moreover, these 2 haplotypes are associated with, respectively, opposing anxiogenic and anxiolytic effects of acute administration of the SSRI citalopram during the human intruder threat, effects that were specifically associated with the average distance maintained from, and reflecting avoidance of, the human intruder, without changing the overall behavioral repertoire. Such genotype-dependent differential sensitivity to the acute effects of an SSRI may underlie, at least in part, differential sensitivity to pharmacotherapy in people with anxiety and depression (35).
The marmoset’s small size, short gestation period, and accelerated development compared with Old World monkeys, alongside its sophisticated social and emotional behavior (36, 37) and primate brain with expanded associated neocortex compared with rodents, make it an ideal species for laboratory studies of gene–brain–behavior interactions during development and adulthood (38). Moreover, the use of marmosets that are bred “in house” affords considerable control/restriction over the environmental influences during development, thereby helping to expose the influence of genetic variation. Thus, taking advantage of this newly discovered polymorphism in marmosets, the relationship between cortical 5-HT2A density and the anxious phenotype in the 2 SLC6A4 homozygous haplotypes was determined. We employed a unique combination of PET imaging of the 5HT2A-specific radioligand [18F]altanserin (39) and psychopharmacological challenge with a 5-HT2A antagonist in vivo, followed by measurements of post-mortem 5-HT2A RNA expression in those brain regions showing differential altanserin binding. Based on the neurobiological changes reported in short allele carriers mentioned above, and the relationship between cortical 5-HT2A density and amygdala reactivity, we hypothesized that marmosets homozygous for the AC/C/G anxiety-related haplotype may show reduced 5-HT2A binding in brain areas implicated in emotional processing compared with the low anxious CT/T/C homozygous. To define the relationship between genotype, brain 5-HT2A receptors, and sensitivity to the behavioral effects of a 5HT2A pharmacological challenge, we measured each marmoset’s anxiety response to an unknown human (uncertain threat) after an acute dose of a specific 5-HT2A antagonist (M100907) and investigated whether the response was related to 5-HT2A binding potential (BP).
Results
Selective Reductions in 5HT2A BP and Post-mortem RNA Levels in the Right Posterior Insula of Marmosets Homozygous for the High Anxiety-Related SLC6A4 Haplotype AC/C/G.
PET imaging of the 5HT2A-specific radioligand [18F]altanserin (39) in a cohort of 16 marmosets, balanced by SLC6A4 genotype and sex, revealed that whole-brain 5HT2A BP did not differ significantly between genotypes (global signal AC/C/G vs. CT/T/C; 95% CI: −0.06 to 0.15; 2-tailed t test, P = 0.36). However, a voxel-based analysis identified one specific cluster within the right posterior insula (Fig. 1A and Table 1) that showed a 27.3% (95% CI [21, 34]) reduction of 5HT2A BP in those animals carrying the anxiety-related AC/C/G haplotype compared with those carrying the CT/T/C haplotype (one-way ANOVA: F(1,15) = 78.72, P = 4.02E-07; Fig. 1B). Consequently, a real-time PCR assay was performed on the right posterior insula post mortem, and this revealed paralleled findings. The expression of 5HT2A RNA not only correlated positively with 5HT2A BP (Pearson correlation: r = 0.722, P = 0.005; SI Appendix, Fig. S1A) but also showed a similar reduction in the animals homozygous for the anxiety-related AC/C/G haplotype (20.1%, 95% CI [10.4, 29.9]; one-way ANOVA: F(1,12) = 20.96, P = 7.9E-04; Fig. 1C). To confirm the laterality of this effect, the right posterior insula cluster identified with PET was reflected about the midline to extract homologous values of 5-HT2A BP from the left hemisphere. While the left posterior insula similarly showed a significant reduction in 5HT2A BP in the AC/C/G group (one-way ANOVA: F(1,15) = 4.69, P = 0.048; SI Appendix, Fig. S2G), it did not survive corrections for multiple comparisons. Likewise, RNA expression in the left posterior insula, which correlated positively with 5HT2A BP (Pearson correlation: r = 0.633, P = 0.020; SI Appendix, Fig. S1D), only showed a trend toward a reduction in the AC/C/G group (one-way ANOVA: F(1,12) = 4.26, P = 0.063; SI Appendix, Fig. S3G).
Table 1.
Cluster extent, voxels | Cluster P | Peak level, t | Peak voxel left/right, mm* | Peak voxel ant/post, mm* | Peak voxel dors/vent, mm* | Structure name* | ||
Contrast analysis between 5-HT2A BP of marmosets homozygous for the SLC6A4 genotypes, correction for multiple comparisons: P < 0.05 (FWE) | ||||||||
Cluster 1 | 377 | 0.014† | 7.14 | −7.1 | 8.7 | 10.2 | Posterior insula (DI, GI) | |
Correlational analysis between anxiety scores in the HI test (PC1) and 5-HT2A BP, correction for multiple comparisons: P < 0.05 (FWE) | ||||||||
Cluster 1 | 88 | 0.001‡ | 4.01 | −7.1 | 12.2 | 7.9 | Anterior insula (Gu) | |
Cluster 2 | 57 | 0.001‡ | 3.73 | −8.9 | 8.1 | 9.1 | Insula proisocortex (IPro) |
ant, anterior; DI, dysgranular insular cortex; dors, dorsal; GI, granular insular cortex; Gu, gustatory cortex; IPro, insular proisocortex; pos, posterior; vent, ventral.
From ref. 70.
Family-wise error (FWE)-corrected.
Uncorrected.
SLC6A4 Genotype-Dependent Anxiogenic Response to Threat after Acute 5HT2A Antagonism in Marmosets Homozygous for the Anxiety-Related Haplotype AC/C/G.
A previous study from our group has shown that the SLC6A4 polymorphisms in marmosets are associated with anxiety-like trait, as measured by the human intruder test (HI test) (35). The HI test measures an array of behavioral responses directed toward an unfamiliar person staring at the marmoset for 2 min while the animal is in its home cage (Fig. 2A). The uncertain nature of this stimulus induces a pattern of anxiety-like behavior. A principal component analysis (PCA) of the array of behavioral variables, including vocalizations, locomotion, head bobbing, and distance from the intruder, revealed 2 principal components, PC1 (anxiety) and PC2 (coping strategy), which together explained the variability observed within the behavioral repertoire. Specifically, the AC/C/G homozygous marmosets presented increased trait anxiety (high PC1 scores) characterized by larger average distance from the human intruder, reduced locomotion and jumps to the front, and high numbers of head bobbing and Egg and Tse-like vigilant calls. In addition, they showed a passive coping strategy (low PC2 scores) driven mainly by low numbers of Tsik and Tsik-Egg aggression-related calls. Remarkably, the CT/T/C group showed the opposite pattern (35). Comparable to this large cohort (n = 52) from which the PCA was derived, the cohort in our current study (n = 16) showed a similar differential pattern for PC1 and PC2. However, as expected for polymorphisms presenting high frequency and low penetrance, such as those in the SLC6A4 repeat region, the effects of genetic variation on phenotype in a small sample (n = 16 vs. n = 52) became weaker due to increased variation and reduced power, with only PC2 reaching statistical significance (a priori hypothesis: for PC1, AC/C/G > CT/T/C and for PC2, AC/C/G < CT/T/C; 1-tailed t test: PC1, P = 0.07 and PC2, P = 0.03; SI Appendix, Fig. S4 and Table S1).
Given that the present study revealed specific reductions in 5-HT2A in the right posterior insula of the anxiety-related AC/C/G group, we hypothesized that the anxiety-like response of the animals homozygous for this haplotype would be more sensitive to 5-HT2A antagonism. To test this hypothesis, we administered a 5-HT2A antagonist (M100907) peripherally and measured the responsivity of marmosets to a human intruder using a repeated-measure block design (Materials and Methods).
Previous studies have shown that acute administration of anxiolytics (40) has a marked impact on average distance from the human threat (avoidance response), and we recently identified a SLC6A4 genotype-dependent response to acute administration of the SSRI citalopram on this measure too, without altering the overall anxiety-like trait (i.e., PC1, PC2) (35). Thus, we focused the subsequent analysis of the acute effects of M100907 on average distance (Materials and Methods), although all other behavioral measures and PCA scores calculated for this pharmacological study are summarized in SI Appendix, Table S2. As predicted, after acute 5-HT2A pharmacological antagonism with the higher of the 2 doses administered, marmosets homozygous for the anxiety-related haplotype AC/C/G displayed an enhanced anxiogenic response to the human intruder, with a significant increase in average distance from the human intruder compared with vehicle (planned comparisons: vehicle vs. M100907: 0.3-mg/kg dose; paired 2-tailed t test: P = 0.038; Fig. 2B). There was no effect of either dose in the CT/T/C group and no effects on any other measure (SI Appendix, Table S2).
Together, these results suggest a functional role for the observed genotype differences in 5HT2A BP and RNA expression in the posterior insula in the regulation of anxiety-like behavior that may underlie their differences in trait anxiety.
5-HT2A BP and RNA Levels in the Right Anterior Insula and Insula Proisocortex Predict Trait Anxiety.
To establish whether 5HT2A BP in the posterior insula or anywhere else in the brain was related to trait anxiety per se, we performed a voxel-based analysis correlating 5-HT2A BP with trait anxiety (PC1) and coping strategy (PC2) scores of the present cohort of marmosets, independent of SLC6A4 genotype.
At the same level of significance used previously, corresponding to a maximum P value of 0.001 for cluster-generating threshold as recommended by Eklund et al. (41), there were no behavioral correlations. However, as an exploratory approach, we relaxed the stringency of the correction for multiple comparisons with a cluster-generating threshold of P < 0.005 as a less stringent, but still widely used, value to conduct an exploratory analysis, and a smaller extent threshold (k) of 50 voxels, which still excluded clusters too small to be of interest (details are provided in Materials and Methods). This analysis revealed 2 specific clusters for PC1 (Fig. 3 and Table 1): a cluster located in the right anterior insula (Fig. 3 A–C) and another located in the right insula proisocortex (Fig. 3 D–F). Both areas showed a negative relationship, with low 5-HT2A BP corresponding to high anxiety scores (Pearson correlation: right anterior insula: r = −0.753, P = 0.001; right insula proisocortex: r = −0.746, P = 0.001; Fig. 3 B and E, respectively). Direct measurement of 5-HT2A RNA levels within post-mortem tissue of the right anterior insula region assessed using real-time PCR mirrored this negative relationship (Pearson correlation: r = −0.656, P = 0.015; Fig. 3C). There was no such relationship between 5-HT2A RNA expression and anxiety scores in the right insula proisocortex (Pearson correlation: r = −0.173, P = 0.571; Fig. 3F). While the right anterior insula 5-HT2A RNA expression levels correlated positively with 5-HT2A BP (Pearson correlation: r = 0.661, P = 0.014; SI Appendix, Fig. S1B), this was not the case for the right insula proisocortex (Pearson correlation: r = −0.466, P = 0.108; SI Appendix, Fig. S1C). We used the same approach as described above to confirm the laterality of this effect, with no significant relationship between anxiety and either 5-HT2A BP or RNA expression within these 2 regions in the left side (SI Appendix, Figs. S2 and S3). No significant findings were detected for PC2 at either threshold.
To determine whether these variables were also predictors of the trait anxiety scores, we performed linear regression analyses with the significant correlating variables. We included 5-HT2A BP and RNA expression in right anterior insula and 5-HT2A BP in right insula proisocortex as predictors and anxiety scores (PC1) as the dependent variable. All 3 variables were good predictors of anxiety scores (Table 2).
Table 2.
B | SE B | β | |
R2 = 0.57, n = 16 | |||
Constant | 2.7 | 0.55 | |
Right anterior insula 5HT2A BP | −1.73 | 0.40 | −0.75** |
R2 = 0.56, n = 16 | |||
Constant | 4.28 | 0.93 | |
Right insula proisocortex 5HT2A BP | −2.63 | 0.63 | −0.75** |
R2 = 0.43, n = 13 | |||
Constant | 1.81 | 0.52 | |
Right anterior insula 5HT2A RNA | −1.50 | 0.52 | −0.66* |
**P < 0.001; *P < 0.05; B, regression coefficient.
5-HT2A BP in the Right Anterior Insula or Insula Proisocortex Predicts the Behavioral Response to Threat after 5-HT2A Antagonism in the HI Test.
Since both PET and RNA expression provided strong evidence that 5-HT2A in the right anterior insula and insula proisocortex contributed to overall trait anxiety scores, we next determined whether 5-HT2A in these regions also contributed to a marmoset’s responsivity to the human intruder after acute 5-HT2A antagonism with M100907, as described above. To test this, we analyzed the relationship between 5-HT2A BP and RNA levels and the pharmacologically induced anxiety response after 5-HT2A antagonism. We calculated the drug-induced effect on average distance from the human threat as the percentage of change of the average distance variable following vehicle (Materials and Methods). This measure correlated negatively with 5-HT2A BP in both right anterior insula (Pearson correlation: r = −0.660, P = 0.014) and insula proisocortex (Pearson correlation: r = −0.645, P = 0.017) (Fig. 4). In contrast, right anterior insula RNA expression did not correlate significantly with the response to threat. A regression analysis on the significant correlating measures revealed that 5-HT2A BP in both right anterior insula and insula proisocortex were good predictors of the increased anxiety response after 5-HT2A antagonism (Table 3). BP of 5-HT2A in both insula regions also highly correlated with each other (Pearson correlation: r = 0.857, P = 0.0002); thus, they were not significantly different as predictors of the threat response.
Table 3.
B | SE B | β | |
R2 = 0.44, n = 16 | |||
Constant | 74.37 | 23.65 | |
Right anterior insula 5HT2A BP | −50.01 | 17.15 | −0.66* |
R2 = 0.42, n = 16 | |||
Constant | 123.22 | 41.91 | |
Right insula proisocortex 5HT2A BP | −77.50 | 27.66 | −0.64* |
*P < 0.05; B, regression coefficient.
Discussion
The neurobiological mechanisms that contribute to the development of emotionally vulnerable phenotypes with increased risk for psychiatric disorders are still poorly understood. One of the most studied genetic variants within the psychiatric field is the SLC6A4 repeat region polymorphisms, in which low-expressing variants have been associated with vulnerability to psychiatric disorders and low treatment efficacy. Here, we show that marmosets homozygous for the low-expressing, high anxiety-related SLC6A4 variant AC/C/G show significantly reduced 5HT2A BP and RNA expression specifically in the right posterior insula, and display an enhanced anxiety-like response to a human threat after acute systemic 5HT2A pharmacological antagonism. In addition, we reveal that 5HT2A BP and RNA levels in the adjacent right anterior insula and 5HT2A BP in the right insula proisocortex have a negative relationship with trait anxiety regardless of genotype, with lower 5HT2A levels corresponding to higher anxiety scores. Moreover, 5HT2A BP in the right anterior insula or the right insula proisocortex were both good predictors of the size and direction of the emotional response to threat after 5HT2A acute pharmacological antagonism.
This study not only investigates the contribution of the SLC6A4 polymorphisms to 5HT2A levels and its relationship with trait anxiety but also, critically, directly compares in vivo BP and post-mortem RNA expression in the same cohort of monkeys and demonstrates correspondence across measures. Our findings of reduced 5HT2A BP and associated RNA expression in the right posterior insula of marmosets carrying the low-expressing, emotionally vulnerable SLC6A4 variant extend previous studies, one in humans (24) and another in macaques (23), reporting reduced 5HT1A BP in individuals homozygous for the short allele in cortical areas, including the insula. It is also consistent with a study showing reduced 5HT2A cortical receptor binding in SLC6A4 knockout mice (42). Besides the relationship between the SLC6A4 variant and 5-HT2A levels in the posterior insula, we revealed a negative relationship independent of genotype, between cortical 5HT2A and overall anxiety scores in the adjacent right anterior insula and the more posterior insula proisocortex. While the negative relationship between BP and anxiety scores was mirrored by RNA levels in the right anterior insula, this was not the case for the insula proisocortex. Given that BP and RNA expression correlated positively in right anterior insula but not in insula proisocortex, a likely reason for this noncorrespondence may be due to the presence of presynaptic 5HT2A specifically within this region. Presynaptic 5HT2A would contribute to BP but not necessarily to RNA levels, since the RNA coding for presynaptic receptors would normally reside in cell bodies in a distal part of the brain, such as, for example, the 5HT2A-expressing thalamocortical inputs to the insula (43). Thus, depending on the overall balance between pre- and postsynaptic 5HT2A, there will be more or less correspondence between BP and RNA in different brain areas. Nonetheless, the negative relationship found between 5HT2A BP within these 2 insula regions and anxiety scores is supported by our results from the pharmacological study in which the magnitude and direction of the behavioral response to the human intruder threat after 5HT2A antagonism was directly associated with 5HT2A BP within both regions (Fig. 4). A similar negative correlation between 5HT2A and, in this case, amygdala reactivity to fearful faces has been found in medial prefrontal cortex (31), suggesting a role for prefrontal cortex 5HT2A in the regulation of amygdala reactivity during anxiety (44). In contrast, a study in healthy volunteers using questionnaire measures of neuroticism showed a positive relationship with 5HT2A BP in frontolimbic areas, including the left but not the right insula (45). Thus, while these studies support the role of cortical 5HT2A in the regulation of emotion, different brain regions selectively involved in the regulation of distinct aspects of emotional behavior may show specific 5HT2A functional patterns (46).
In the case of patients with depression, imaging and post-mortem studies show increased cortical 5HT2A levels (30). Consistent with this, imaging studies have detected a down-regulation of 5-HT2A receptor in the brain of such patients in response to SSRI treatment (47), and the therapeutic benefit gained by some patients when treated with a combination of both an SSRI and 5HT2A antagonist seems to be in line with these observations (48). However, little is known about 5HT2A levels in patients with anxiety disorders. While 5HT2A antagonists have been used for the treatment of anxiety disorders (46), 5HT2A agonists (serotonin hallucinogens) have provided improvement of anxiety and depressive symptoms in patients suffering from life-threatening diseases (49). Thus, the role of cortical 5HT2A in the regulation of anxiety may depend not only on the specific brain area and type of behavior but also on the emotional context.
In the present study, the insula was the brain region that consistently showed alterations in 5-HT2A BP and RNA levels associated with the SLC6A4 polymorphisms, trait anxiety scores, and anxiety-related response after 5-HT2A antagonism. The insula has been implicated in interoceptive processing and emotional decision making (50, 51), subjective feelings (52), emotion, and self-awareness (53). In primates, the insula cortex presents a complex structural and functional architecture, transitioning from granular neocortex in the posterior-dorsal insula to agranular neocortex in the anterior-ventral insula, with an intermediate zone of dysgranularity (54). While the posterior insula receives sensory information from the body, it is proposed that the anterior region integrates this somatic representation with cognitive information from prefrontal areas to process subjective feelings and self-awareness (17, 18). Thus, together, the posterior insula cortex and anterior insula cortex play an integrated role in emotion regulation. Consistent with this is the increased anterior insula activity during emotional processing in patients with anxiety disorders (55). In addition, in depressed patients, higher neuroticism was related to increased activity within the right anterior insula cortex to incongruent compared with congruent emotional stimuli (56). Moreover, anterior insula hyperreactivity was associated with overinterpretation of interoceptive states as threatening or negative by patients with anxiety and depression, respectively (57). Perhaps not surprisingly then, imaging studies have also shown neurophysiological changes in the insula cortex associated with SLC6A4 polymorphism. In humans, enhanced right anterior insula activation in short allele carriers has been reported in response to an unpredictable laboratory stressor (15), fear conditioning (16), and negative self-reflection (9). Moreover, in patients with social anxiety disorder (SAD), short allele carriers showed greater anterior insula activation to fearful faces (10). In macaques, increased anterior insula activity in short allele carriers has also been shown using [18F]fluoro-2-deoxy-d-glucose (FDG) PET scanning immediately after the HI test (6). The same group has also identified a positive relationship between freezing behavior in the HI test and anterior insula activity (11), which was highly heritable (58).
Here, we report a significant reduction of 5HT2A levels specifically in the posterior insula and an anxiogenic response to threat after 5HT2A antagonism, in marmosets homozygous for the high anxiety-related SLC6A4 variant. However, it was the 5HT2A binding in the anterior insula, where no significant differences in 5HT2A levels could be detected between the SLC6A4 genotypes, that predicted the anxiety response to threat. Thus, the SLC6A4 polymorphism may be contributing to this differential anxiety response to threat after 5HT2A antagonism through altered 5HT2A-mediated functions in posterior insula (e.g., altered processing of interoceptive information). This altered information is subsequently transferred to the anterior insula, as predicted by the model of information flow in this area (18), where it is integrated with cognitive information from prefrontal cortex under 5HT2A-mediated regulation, ultimately contributing to the emotional response. Although the present study clearly reveals the importance of 5HT2A function within the insula in relation to anxiety and the SLC6A4 polymorphism, the extent to which this genetic association between insula activity and SLC6A4 polymorphism is determined by 5HT2A density requires further investigation.
The SLC6A4 haplotype dependency of the heightened sensitivity to acute 5HT2A antagonism was anticipated, since high anxious marmosets presented with reduced right insula 5HT2A BP and RNA levels, suggesting that further functional reduction of these receptors would have a greater behavioral impact on this haplotype. We have previously shown that these same high anxious marmosets displayed an anxiogenic response to the human intruder after an acute dose of the SSRI citalopram (35). Consistent with this, studies increasing serotonin output with acute SSRIs have shown a differential effect on right insula activation depending on the SLC6A4 polymorphism. Smith et al. (59) found enhanced reductions in right anterior insula activity of long allele homozygotes compared with subjects homozygous for the short allele, during FDG PET after intravenous (i.v.) infusion of citalopram. In addition, oral citalopram increased amygdala and posterior insula activity in long-allele homozygotes but not short-allele ones during perception of fearful faces (60). Together, these findings demonstrate genetically driven differential insula activation in response to drugs that target the 5-HT system. Their therapeutic relevance is highlighted by recent work identifying right anterior insula metabolism as the best predictor of escitalopram treatment success (61). Based on our findings, we propose that insula 5HT2A levels may contribute to the mechanism by which SLC6A4 polymorphisms modulate insula activity and subsequent anxiety responses.
In conclusion, the present study implicates 5HT2A receptors in the right posterior insula as the neurochemical mechanism by which genetic variation in the SLC6A4 gene contributes to the anxious, vulnerable phenotype. We reveal a differential sensitivity to acute 5HT2A antagonists depending on SLC6A4 genotype, with individuals that carry the high anxiety-related variant and present reduced right posterior insula 5HT2A BP and RNA levels, displaying an anxiogenic response to threat. Moreover, we show that 5HT2A BP in the right anterior insula and insula proisocortex were good predictors of the anxious response. Altogether, these findings highlight the specificity of the neurobiological changes associated with SLC6A4 polymorphisms that are highly relevant to our understanding of the development and treatment of mood and anxiety disorders. Dissecting the neurobiological mechanisms underlying genetic variation that link selective brain areas and receptors to specific emotional behaviors will increase our understanding of individual differences not only in anxiety and mood disorder symptoms but also in treatment efficacy, bringing us a step closer to the development of more effective personalized therapies.
Materials and Methods
Animals and Housing.
Sixteen naive common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus, (26 ± 2 mo, 413 ± 17 g) balanced for sex and genotype were used in this study. All animals had MRI and [18F]altanserin PET scans, HI test, and snake testing [procedures described previously by Shiba et al. (62)] before entering a pharmacological study, which consisted of repeated HI test with acute intramuscular (i.m.) doses of citalopram [behavioral data reported elsewhere (35)] and, after 2 mo, with the 5HT2A antagonist M100907 (present study). Marmosets were bred onsite at the Innes Marmoset Colony (Behavioral and Clinical Neuroscience Institute) and housed in pairs. Temperature (24 °C) and humidity (55%) conditions were controlled, and a dawn/dusk-like 12-h period was maintained. They were provided with a balanced diet and water ad libitum. This research has been regulated under the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 Amendment Regulations 2012 following ethical review by the University of Cambridge Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body.
Genotyping.
Marmoset were genotyped for the SLC6A4 polymorphisms using methods previously described (35). Briefly, hair samples were taken from the animal’s back. Samples were processed using a QIAamp DNA Micro Kit (Qiagen Ltd.). The primers used flanked the SLC6A4 polymorphic repeat promoter region: RPRF (CAGACAACCGTGTTCATCTG) and RPRR (GATTCTAGTGCCACCTAGAC). HotStarTaq Plus DNA Polymerase (Qiagen Ltd.) was used in a BioRad C1000 thermal cycler (conditions: activation for 15 min at 94 °C; 44 cycles of 30 s at 94 °C, 30 s at 55 °C, and 1 min at 72 °C; and termination after 5 min at 72 °C). The PCR product was visualized in an agarose gel, purified using a QIAquick Gel Extraction Kit (Qiagen Ltd.), and sent for sequencing (Genservice Ltd.) using the primers SeqF1 (AGCAGCACCTAACCCTCCTA) and SeqF2 (TCCCCACTAGGCATTGCTAC).
HI Test.
We have previously characterized the HI test of anxiety in marmosets using a large population (n = 52) from our colony (35). Briefly, marmosets were isolated in the upper right-hand quadrant of their home cage (separated phase). After 8 min, an unfamiliar person entered the room (intruder phase). The intruder stood in front of the cage and stared at the marmoset’s eyes for 2 min. Marmoset performance was recorded with a high-definition video camera and a shotgun condenser microphone. Several measures were scored offline by an experimenter. A PCA was performed on all variables with a large cohort of marmosets, including the ones used in this study. PC1 and PC2 explained over 63% of the variance. According to variable loadings, PC1 corresponded to “anxiety” and PC2 corresponded to “coping strategy” in the emotional response to threat. The scores for each individual included in this study were derived from this population PCA and used for the subsequent imaging correlational analysis (discussed below).
PET Scan Protocol.
Animals were imaged for 3 h using a microPET Focus-220 scanner (Concorde Microsystems). Anesthesia and body temperature were maintained. In addition, oxygen saturation, heart rate, and respiratory rate were measured and maintained within physiological limits throughout. Before injection, singles-mode transmission data were acquired for 515 s using a rotating 68Ge point source (∼20 MBq). An attenuation correction sinogram was produced from this scan and a blank scan of the same duration using the reconstruction and segmentation software on the Focus-220 scanner. The [18F]altanserin (3-[2-[4-(4-[18F]fluorobenzoyl)piperidin-1-yl]ethyl]-2-sulfanylidene-1H-quinazolin-4-one) (0.5 ± 0.04 nmol/kg) was injected i.v. over 10 s, followed by a 5-s heparinized saline flush. For all scans, the injected amount of altanserin was ∼0.5 nmol/kg. Dynamic data were acquired in list mode for a 350- to 650-keV energy window and a 6-ns timing window. Data were subsequently histogrammed into sinograms for the following time frames: 12 × 5 s, 6 × 10 s, 3 × 20 s, 4 × 30 s, 5 × 60 s, 10 × 120 s, and 30 × 5 min. Corrections were applied for random, dead time, normalization, attenuation, and decay. Fourier rebinning (63) was used to compress the 4-dimensional sinograms to 3 dimensions before reconstruction with a 2-dimensional filtered back-projection with a Hann window cutoff at the Nyquist frequency. The image voxel size was 0.95 × 0.95 × 0.80 mm, with an array size of 128 × 128 × 95. The reconstructed images were converted to kilobecquerels per milliliter using global and slice factors determined from imaging a uniform phantom filled with a [18F]fluoride solution.
PET Data Analysis.
Following affine and nonlinear registration, BP maps were smoothed using an adaptive Gaussian kernel to exclude voxels outside the brain mask (the full width at half maximum was 1 mm isotropic). To assess the effect of SLC6A4 genotype, voxel-based comparisons were made with SPM12 using a general linear model. Factors were included for genotype, sex, and weight. Two-tailed t tests were used to assess the main effect of genotype. A cluster-based correction for multiple comparisons was used to control the family-wise error at P < 0.05. The cluster-generating threshold was P < 0.001 (41). To investigate correlations with behavior, separate analyses were conducted with further covariates for PC1 and PC2 from the HI test. P < 0.005 was used as a less stringent, but still widely used, value to conduct an exploratory analysis (the most prevalent in the AFNI [Analysis of Functional NeuroImages] software, for example, from the National Institute of Mental Health, https://afni.nimh.nih.gov). An extent threshold of 50 voxels was chosen to avoid clusters too small to be of interest. We did not calculate a random-field theory threshold for this cluster size (as we did for the P < 0.001 threshold) to control the family-wise error directly, as Eklund et al. (41) point out that the assumptions of the calculations used do not hold for more liberal thresholds. Clusters found were followed up by RNA expression measurements within these areas.
Pharmacological Manipulation on HI Test.
Due to health-related issues, only 13 of the 16 marmosets (6 AC/C/G: female [F] = 3, male [M] = 3; 7 CT/T/C: F = 4, M = 3) were available for inclusion in this next stage of the study. Animals were injected i.m. with the selective 5-HT2A antagonist M100907 (Sigma–Aldrich) (low dose [0.1 mg/kg] or high dose [0.3 mg/kg]) or vehicle (0.01 M phosphate-buffered saline-HCl) 25 min before the human intruder entered. M100907 doses were selected based on their reported effects on macaques (64) and squirrel monkeys (65). Repeated HI test procedures were the same as described above. To avoid habituation to the human intruder across sessions, we used different realistic rubber human masks each time (Greyland Film spol. s.r.o.). The experimental design was a Latin square randomized by sex, genotype, and masks. Treatment order was the same for all individuals (vehicle, low dose, high dose, vehicle) with 2 wk between each session. To calculate the PCA scores for each treatment, each variable was standardized using the mean and SD of the control condition (average of the 2 injections with vehicle) of the experimental subpopulation used in this study (n = 13). These standardized values were then used in the PCA function derived from the population. Average distance from the human intruder was calculated by dividing the testing quadrant into regions and scoring the time spent in each location, as previously described (35). Percentage of change from vehicle in average distance was calculated as follows: [(average distance after low or high dose of M100907/average distance after vehicle) × 100] − 100. Planned comparisons were performed using paired 2-tailed t tests.
Real-Time PCR.
At the end of the study, animals were premedicated with ketamine hydrochloride before being euthanized with pentobarbital sodium (Dolethal; 1 mL of a 200-mg/mL solution; Merial Animal Health). Brain were dissected, frozen using liquid nitrogen, and kept at −80 °C until use. The brains were then sliced in a cryostat at −20 °C to 200-μm-thick sections. Tissue samples for each target region were excised using punches of 1.5- and 2-mm radio length (SI Appendix, Fig. S5). Total RNA was extracted with the RNeasy Plus Universal Mini Kit (Qiagen Ltd.) in accordance with the manufacturer’s protocol. Samples were quantified using a NanoDrop spectrometer (Thermo Fisher Scientific) with an average concentration of 9.40 ± 0.24 ng/μL. RNA integrity, analyzed using an Agilent RNA 6000 Pico Kit with an Agilent 2100 Bioanalyzer instrument, had an average RNA integrity number (RIN) of 8.60 ± 0.04. Samples were stored at −80 °C until use. Complementary DNA (cDNA) synthesis and real-time qPCR were performed using a Brilliant II SYBR Green QRT-PCR Master Mix Kit, 1-Step (Agilent Technologies), adding 2 μL of sample into each primer combination reaction. Primers for the 5HT2A gene were designed using Primer-BLAST, a software tool from the National Center for Biotechnology Information (66). The predicted 5HT2A messenger RNA sequence used to design the primers was obtained from the Ensembl database (67). Four marmoset-specific reference genes were selected: ACTB (β-actin), TBP (TATA-box binding protein), GAPDH (glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase) (68), and SDHA (succinate dehydrogenase complex, subunit A) (68, 69). All primer combinations were designed to span exon-exon boundaries (primer sequences are shown in SI Appendix, Table S3). All reactions were performed in duplicate for each individual and control (conditions: cDNA synthesis for 30 min at 50 °C, activation step for 10 min at 95 °C, 40 2-step cycle of denaturation 30 s at 95 °C, and combined annealing/extension for 1 min at 60 °C, with final melting curves to check the specificity of the product) using the Bio-Rad CFX96 Touch Real-Time PCR Detection System. Results were obtained using BioRad CFXManager software. Using specific amplification efficiencies for each primer set previously calculated and an interrun calibrator, we ran a gene study where the relative expression levels of the gene of interest (5HT2A) were normalized to the 4 reference genes (ACTB, TBP, GAPDH, and SDHA) and calculated using the ΔΔCq mathematical method: R = [(Etarget)DCqtarget(Mean control – Mean sample)]/[(ERef index)DCqRef index(Mean control – Mean sample)], where the ratio (R) is calculated based on the amplification efficiencies (E) of the gene of interest (target) and multiple references genes (Ref index) using the difference between the quantification cycle (Cq) for each sample and control.
Statistical Analysis.
Analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics (version 24; IBM Corp.). Data are presented as mean ± SEM, and P ≤ 0.05 was considered statistically significant. After confirming normal distribution and homogeneity of variance, one-way ANOVA and Pearson correlations were performed.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by Medical Research Council (MRC) Programme Grant MR/M023990/1 (to A.C.R.). S.J.S. was supported by the Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, supported jointly by the Wellcome Trust and MRC. P.J.R. and the development of the PET ligand were supported by MRC Grant G0900903 (to F.I.A. and A.C.R.).
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1902087116/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.Canli T., Lesch K.-P., Long story short: The serotonin transporter in emotion regulation and social cognition. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1103–1109 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lesch K. P., et al. , Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274, 1527–1531 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bennett A. J., et al. , Early experience and serotonin transporter gene variation interact to influence primate CNS function. Mol. Psychiatry 7, 118–122 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hariri A. R., et al. , Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science 297, 400–403 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Canli T., et al. , Neural correlates of epigenesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 16033–16038 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kalin N. H., et al. , The serotonin transporter genotype is associated with intermediate brain phenotypes that depend on the context of eliciting stressor. Mol. Psychiatry 13, 1021–1027 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Klumpers F., et al. , Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex mediates the impact of serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region genotype on anticipatory threat reactions. Biol. Psychiatry 78, 582–589 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Klucken T., et al. , The association between the 5-HTTLPR and neural correlates of fear conditioning and connectivity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 700–707 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ma Y., et al. , 5-HTTLPR polymorphism modulates neural mechanisms of negative self-reflection. Cereb. Cortex 24, 2421–2429 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Klumpp H., et al. , Serotonin transporter gene alters insula activity to threat in social anxiety disorder. Neuroreport 25, 926–931 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Shackman A. J., et al. , Neural mechanisms underlying heterogeneity in the presentation of anxious temperament. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 6145–6150 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pezawas L., et al. , 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: A genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 828–834 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pacheco J., et al. , Frontal-limbic white matter pathway associations with the serotonin transporter gene promoter region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism. J. Neurosci. 29, 6229–6233 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Madsen M. K., et al. , Threat-related amygdala functional connectivity is associated with 5-HTTLPR genotype and neuroticism. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 140–149 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Drabant E. M., et al. , Neural mechanisms underlying 5-HTTLPR-related sensitivity to acute stress. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 397–405 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hermann A., et al. , Functional gene polymorphisms in the serotonin system and traumatic life events modulate the neural basis of fear acquisition and extinction. PLoS One 7, e44352 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Singer T., et al. , Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Craig A. D., The sentient self. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 563–577 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Jedema H. P., et al. , Cognitive impact of genetic variation of the serotonin transporter in primates is associated with differences in brain morphology rather than serotonin neurotransmission. Mol. Psychiatry 15, 512–522, 446 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Christian B. T., et al. , Serotonin transporter binding and genotype in the nonhuman primate brain using [C-11]DASB PET. Neuroimage 47, 1230–1236 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Parsey R. V., et al. , Effect of a triallelic functional polymorphism of the serotonin-transporter-linked promoter region on expression of serotonin transporter in the human brain. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 48–51 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Murthy N. V., et al. , Serotonin transporter polymorphisms (SLC6A4 insertion/deletion and rs25531) do not affect the availability of 5-HTT to [11C] DASB binding in the living human brain. Neuroimage 52, 50–54 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Christian B. T., et al. , Serotonin transporter genotype affects serotonin 5-HT1A binding in primates. J. Neurosci. 33, 2512–2516 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.David S. P., et al. , A functional genetic variation of the serotonin (5-HT) transporter affects 5-HT1A receptor binding in humans. J. Neurosci. 25, 2586–2590 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Keers R., et al. , Interaction between serotonin transporter gene variants and life events predicts response to antidepressants in the GENDEP project. Pharmacogenomics J. 11, 138–145 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Penn E., Tracy D. K., The drugs don’t work? Antidepressants and the current and future pharmacological management of depression. Ther. Adv. Psychopharmacol. 2, 179–188 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Harmer C. J., Cowen P. J., ‘It’s the way that you look at it’–A cognitive neuropsychological account of SSRI action in depression. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368, 20120407 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Quesseveur G., Nguyen H. T., Gardier A. M., Guiard B. P., 5-HT2 ligands in the treatment of anxiety and depression. Expert Opin. Investig. Drugs 21, 1701–1725 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Arnone D., Horder J., Cowen P. J., Harmer C. J., Early effects of mirtazapine on emotional processing. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 203, 685–691 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Savitz J. B., Drevets W. C., Neuroreceptor imaging in depression. Neurobiol. Dis. 52, 49–65 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Fisher P. M., et al. , Medial prefrontal cortex 5-HT(2A) density is correlated with amygdala reactivity, response habituation, and functional coupling. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2499–2507 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Fisher P. M., et al. , Medial prefrontal cortex serotonin 1A and 2A receptor binding interacts to predict threat-related amygdala reactivity. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 1, 2 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Pinborg L. H., et al. , The 5-HT2A receptor binding pattern in the human brain is strongly genetically determined. Neuroimage 40, 1175–1180 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Pascale E., et al. , Monomorphic region of the serotonin transporter promoter gene in New World monkeys. Am. J. Primatol. 74, 1028–1034 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Santangelo A. M., et al. , Novel primate model of serotonin transporter genetic polymorphisms associated with gene expression, anxiety and sensitivity to antidepressants. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 2366–2376 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Oikonomidis L., et al. , A dimensional approach to modeling symptoms of neuropsychiatric disorders in the marmoset monkey. Dev. Neurobiol. 77, 328–353 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Miller C. T., et al. , Marmosets: A neuroscientific model of human social behavior. Neuron 90, 219–233 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Izpisua Belmonte J. C., et al. , Brains, genes, and primates. Neuron 86, 617–631 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Riss P. J., et al. , Validation and quantification of [18F]altanserin binding in the rat brain using blood input and reference tissue modeling. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 31, 2334–2342 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Carey G. J., Costall B., Domeney A. M., Jones D. N., Naylor R. J., Behavioural effects of anxiogenic agents in the common marmoset. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 42, 143–153 (1992). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Eklund A., Nichols T. E., Knutsson H., Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 7900–7905 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rioux A., et al. , Adaptive changes of serotonin 5-HT2A receptors in mice lacking the serotonin transporter. Neurosci. Lett. 262, 113–116 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Barre A., et al. , Presynaptic serotonin 2A receptors modulate thalamocortical plasticity and associative learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, E1382–E1391 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Aznar S., Klein A. B., Regulating prefrontal cortex activation: An emerging role for the 5-HT2A serotonin receptor in the modulation of emotion-based actions? Mol. Neurobiol. 48, 841–853 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Frokjaer V. G., et al. , Frontolimbic serotonin 2A receptor binding in healthy subjects is associated with personality risk factors for affective disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 569–576 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Moulédous L., Roullet P., Guiard B. P., “Brain circuits regulated by the 5-HT2A receptor: Behavioural consequences on anxiety and fear memory” in 5-HT2A Receptors in the Central Nervous System (Springer International Publishing, Cham, 2018), pp. 231–258. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Meyer J. H., et al. , The effect of paroxetine on 5-HT(2A) receptors in depression: An [(18)F]setoperone PET imaging study. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 78–85 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Marek G. J., Carpenter L. L., McDougle C. J., Price L. H., Synergistic action of 5-HT2A antagonists and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in neuropsychiatric disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 402–412 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Reiche S., et al. , Serotonergic hallucinogens in the treatment of anxiety and depression in patients suffering from a life-threatening disease: A systematic review. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 81, 1–10 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Craig A. D., How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Dunn B. D., et al. , Can you feel the beat? Interoceptive awareness is an interactive function of anxiety- and depression-specific symptom dimensions. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 1133–1138 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Critchley H. D., Wiens S., Rotshtein P., Öhman A., Dolan R. J., Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 189–195 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Craig A. D., How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Bauernfeind A. L., et al. , A volumetric comparison of the insular cortex and its subregions in primates. J. Hum. Evol. 64, 263–279 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Klumpp H., Post D., Angstadt M., Fitzgerald D. A., Phan K. L., Anterior cingulate cortex and insula response during indirect and direct processing of emotional faces in generalized social anxiety disorder. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 3, 7 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Fournier J. C., et al. , Neuroticism and individual differences in neural function in unmedicated major depression: Findings from the EMBARC study. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2, 138–148 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Paulus M. P., Stein M. B., Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 451–463 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Fox A. S., et al. , Intergenerational neural mediators of early-life anxious temperament. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 9118–9122 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Smith G. S., et al. , Effects of serotonin transporter promoter polymorphisms on serotonin function. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 2226–2234 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Ma Y., et al. , Allelic variation in 5-HTTLPR and the effects of citalopram on the emotional neural network. Br. J. Psychiatry 206, 385–392 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.McGrath C. L., et al. , Toward a neuroimaging treatment selection biomarker for major depressive disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 821–829 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Shiba Y., et al. , Individual differences in behavioral and cardiovascular reactivity to emotive stimuli and their relationship to cognitive flexibility in a primate model of trait anxiety. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 137 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Defrise M., et al. , Exact and approximate rebinning algorithms for 3-D PET data. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 16, 145–158 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Murnane K. S., Andersen M. L., Rice K. C., Howell L. L., Selective serotonin 2A receptor antagonism attenuates the effects of amphetamine on arousal and dopamine overflow in non-human primates. J. Sleep Res. 22, 581–588 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Fantegrossi W. E., et al. , Role of dopamine transporters in the behavioral effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) in nonhuman primates. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 205, 337–347 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Ye J., et al. , Primer-BLAST: A tool to design target-specific primers for polymerase chain reaction. BMC Bioinformatics 13, 134 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Flicek P., et al. , Ensembl 2014. Nucleic Acids Res. 42, D749–D755 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Shimamoto Y., et al. , Selection of suitable reference genes for mRNA quantification studies using common marmoset tissues. Mol. Biol. Rep. 40, 6747–6755 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Fujii Y., et al. , Immune-related gene expression profile in laboratory common marmosets assessed by an accurate quantitative real-time PCR using selected reference genes. PLoS One 8, e56296 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Paxinos G, Watson C, Petrides M, Rosa M, Tokuno H, The Marmoset Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates (Academic Press, Ed. 1, 2012). [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.