Abstract
Objectives:
This study looks at fear of falling (FOF) as a predictor of falls in light of mobility limitation and other confounders.
Methods:
Data on community dwelling older Europeans, aged 65 and older, were drawn from two consecutive waves of the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). The analysis regressed fall status in 2013 on reported FOF two to three years earlier, controlling for previous falls.
Results:
FOF predicted subsequent falls when mobility limitation was low to moderate. However, the effect of FOF on fall probability was reversed when mobility limitation was at a high level.
Discussion:
The analysis underscores a complex association between FOF and mobility limitation in relation to late-life falls. People who are worried about falling tend to fall more than those who are not similarly worried. However, also more likely to fall are those who have a high level of mobility limitation but lack FOF. In the case of considerable mobility limitation, FOF may act as a protective buffer. The less worried in this group, however, may be subject to greater falling, and thus require greater attention.
Introduction
Falls in late life are a growing health problem as populations around the world age. Falling constitutes a major cause of morbidity and mortality (Ambrose, Paul, & Hausdorff, 2013), as well as an antecedent of activity restriction and lower quality of life (Boyd & Stevens, 2009). A key factor within the fall domain is fear of falling (FOF) (Choi & Ko, 2015). Whether defined as a phobic reaction to standing or walking (ptophobia), loss of confidence in balance abilities, low fall-related efficacy or simply worry about falling (Legters, 2002), FOF is seen as an important concomitant of falls (Hadjistavropoulos et al., 2007). Some researchers view FOF as the result of previous falling (Bryant, Rintala, Hou, & Protas, 2015), while others identify it as an independent phenomenon (Clemson, Kendig, Mackenzie, & Browning, 2015). Further complicating the field is the fact that FOF and falls are often perceived as inter-related and part of a mutually reinforcing downward spiral (Friedman, Munoz, West, Rubin, & Fried, 2002). Thus, it is challenging to disentangle the respective effects.
FOF is also a somewhat elusive concept (Hadjistavropoulos, Delbaere, & Fitzgerald, 2011; Jung, 2008). Some researchers define it as a loss of confidence in one’s balance abilities (Maki, Holliday, & Topper, 1991), while others describe it as an apprehension that precipitates the avoidance of activities (Tinetti & Powell, 1993). Early inquiry into the phenomenon saw FOF as the psychological reaction to having experienced a fall. It was believed that this “post-fall syndrome” leads to the development of intense fear as well as to walking disorders (Murphy & Isaacs, 1982). More recent works see FOF as a possible concomitant of increasing fragility (Kressig et al., 2001) or as being inter-related with activity avoidance (Bertera & Bertera, 2008).
The mutual occurrence of falls, FOF and other related phenomenon has led some scholars to query what comes first. One study of the temporal relationship between falls and FOF among more than two thousand community dwelling participants, aged 65 to 84, in Salisbury Maryland, found that baseline FOF indeed predicted falls at follow up, twenty months later. However, falls at baseline also predicted FOF at follow-up in this same sample. The investigators concluded that persons who experience either of these phenomena are at risk for developing the other. That is, they discerned a “spiraling risk” of falls, FOF and functional decline (Friedman et al., 2002). In contrast, a prospective cohort study among some 321 older primary-care clients in Hong Kong found that while FOF at baseline predicted both falls and FOF at follow-up, falls at baseline were not predictors of subsequent FOF or falls (Chou & Chi, 2007). Based on these findings, the researchers concluded that a vicious cycle of falls and FOF was not present in that study sample.
Another factor that is important in understanding the FOF/ falls nexus is mobility limitation, but the association among these variables is still unclear (Allison, 2013). Much of the research on this topic views mobility limitation as a predictor of FOF or of activity restriction due to FOF. Thus, for example, Vellas et al. (1997) found balance and gait abnormalities to predict FOF among prior fallers in New Mexico. Austin et al. (2007) similarly revealed that impairments of balance and mobility predicted FOF among community-dwelling older Australian women. However, others have found that mobility limitation was not related to FOF (Trombetti, et al. 2016). As for activity restriction due to FOF, Guthrie et al. (2012) have noted that difficulty in climbing stairs predicted this outcome. Similarly, Martin et al. (2005) showed that difficulty in standing from an armless chair was associated with FOF limiting activity.
Other studies have considered the reverse association, namely that FOF predicts mobility limitation. Thus, for example, Viljanen et al. (2012) found that difficulties in walking 2 km was higher for people with FOF. A study that employed accelerometers among 1,680 British men, aged 71–92 years, revealed that those who were fearful of falling took fewer steps per day than those who were not (Jefferis et al., 2014). Among community dwelling adults in Oregon and California, aged 60–97, those with FOF had slower gait speed and shorter stride length than those who were not fearful of falling (Chamberlin, Fulwider, Sanders, & Medeiros, 2005). A Canadian study found that self-reported anxiety was related to five out of six gait parameters (Hadjistavropoulos et al., 2012).
However, few studies specifically consider mobility limitation in relation to the inter-relationship between FOF and falls. One Australian study did report slower gait speed to predict falls, but it did not predict FOF (Clemson, et al. 2015). The aim of the current analysis, therefore, is to address the FOF/ falls nexus in light of mobility limitation, given the gap in the literature in this domain
FOF is but one potential risk factor among many in the domain of falling in later life. Another relevant factor in predicting falls is the effect of having experienced previous falls. An examination of the records of home care recipients linked to Emergency Department data in Ontario, Canada, revealed, for example, that persons with a greater number of recent falls had a higher risk of a future injurious fall (Poss & Hirdes, 2016). Prior falls as predictors of subsequent falls have also been found in studies in Mexico (Samper-Ternent, Karmarkar, Graham, Reistetter, & Ottenbacher, 2012) and Denmark (Vind, Andersen, Pedersen, Joergensen, & Schwarz, 2011). However, as noted earlier, a study in Hong Kong (Chou & Chi, 2007) did not find a similar association.
Other risk factors of note are age and gender. Advanced age is associated with greater propensity for falling (Ambrose et al., 2013; Clemson et al., 2015; Rossat et al., 2010), especially among women (Chang & Do, 2015). This may be due to an increase in the risk of sarcopenia as one ages and its association with falls (Scott et al., 2014). Female gender is related also to FOF, as has been documented in several settings (Filiatrault, Desrosiers, & Trottier, 2009; LeBouthillier, Thibodeau, & Asmundson, 2013; Zijlstra et al., 2007). However, the association of age with FOF is less certain. One study found no such association between age and activity-related FOF (Kressig et al., 2001).
Additional demographic confounders that need to be taken into account when considering the relationship of FOF to falls are marital status, socioeconomic status and race or ethnicity. A study in Finland reported a risk for falls among the unmarried (Koski, Luukinen, Laippala, & Kivela, 1998). Similarly, a Korean study found living alone to be associated with a high risk of falls (Choi et al., 2014). It should be noted, however, that a study of older adults in Texas did not find an association between marital status and falls (Alamgir, Wong, Hu, Marshall, & Yu, 2015). On the other hand, the study by Filiatrault and others (2009) in the Province of Quebec, that was cited earlier, found support from a spouse to be a protective factor against FOF.
Income and education were not found to predict falls in the previously mentioned Texas study. However, a study in Korea did find lower educational level to be associated with FOF (Kim & So, 2013). Race (Friedman et al., 2002; Nicklett & Taylor, 2014) and/or ethnicity (Kalula, Ferreira, Swingler, & Badri, 2016) are also relevant factors in fall risk analyses.
Frailty is another factor that may be related to falls. A study of data from the first wave of the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) found a higher degree of frailty in Southern European countries than in Northern European countries (Santos-Eggimann, Cuenoud, Spagnoli, & Junod, 2009). The data from that study suggest, moreover, that education was a key contributing factor behind these differential regional frailty rates. A frailty index was recently validated using SHARE data (Romero-Ortuno et al., 2010). It is based on measures that objectively reflect physical capacity, such as grip strength. Inclusion of this index, in the present study allows us to separate the effects of mobility limitation on falls from those of frailty.
Several other health factors are related to falling as well, and they need to be controlled. These include measures of mental health, particularly cognition (Ambrose et al., 2013) and depression (Clemson et al., 2015). Cognition has also been found to relate to FOF (Choi & Ko, 2015), but the same was not found for depression (Chou & Chi, 2008). The markers of physical health that have been found to be associated with FOF, falls, or both, include body mass index (BMI) (Koski et al., 1998), vision and hearing impairment (Ambrose et al., 2013; Kamil et al., 2016; Viljanen et al., 2013), number of medications (Doi et al., 2012; Kelly et al., 2003), extent of frailty (Bryant et al., 2015; Kojima et al., 2015; Samper-Ternent et al., 2012) and morbidity (Choi et al., 2014; Friedman et al., 2002; Guthrie et al., 2012; Jamison, Neuberger, & Miller, 2003).
Based upon this review of the literature, the present study seeks to examine the association between FOF and falls in a large sample of older European adults, taking into account the many other key risk factors. Our analysis considers two hypotheses:
FOF is an independent risk factor for falling, beyond the effects of sociodemographic background, health/frailty and having experienced a prior fall.
Mobility limitation moderates the effect of FOF on falling.
Methods
The analysis in the present study is based upon data that were collected in the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) (Börsch-Supan et al., 2013). SHARE is a biennial panel survey of persons aged 50 and older and their spouse of any age. It commenced in 2004 and now includes some 27 European countries (eight of which joined just recently in 2016) and Israel. Each country sample is a probability sample. Sixteen countries participated in Wave 4 (2011) and 15 countries in Wave 5 (2013). Thirteen of these countries participated in both Waves 4 and 5 and they constitute the sample in the present study: Austria, Belgium, Czech Republic, Denmark, Estonia, France, Germany, Italy, the Netherlands, Slovenia, Spain, Sweden and Switzerland. The fourth wave served as the baseline in the analysis. Combining data from Waves 4 and 5 facilitated the examination of baseline measures of falling, FOF and several confounders and their relationship to falling and mobility limitation two years later.
The inquiry focused exclusively on community-dwelling Europeans aged 65 and older who participated in both the fourth (2011) and fifth waves of the survey (2013). The initial analytical sample numbered 22,533 persons (see Table 1 for the distribution by country). As may be seen, respondents from Germany and Denmark were somewhat under-represented and those from Estonia, over-represented. Consequently, the country variable was controlled in the analyses. The sample in the first two of the multivariate analyses numbered 19,023 persons. This reflected a drop of 15.6 percent from the initial sample due to missing data on one or more of the variables (mostly on the frailty measure). We subsequently ran two additional multivariate analyses without the frailty measure included. This lowered the missing data rate to 8.3 percent and brought the sample up to 20,654 respondents.
Table 1:
Study variables: Univariate description (Wave 5)
VARIABLES | N | % Mean (SD) | Range |
---|---|---|---|
Sociodemographic background | |||
Age | 22,533 | 74.37 (6.94) | 65 – 104 |
Female | 22,533 | 56.50 | |
Married/Partnered | 22,265 | 64.14 | |
Education | 22,117 | ||
Primary | 46.91 | ||
Secondary education | 29.60 | ||
Post-secondary education | 23.50 | ||
Country | 22,533 | ||
Austria | 2,264 | 10.05 | |
Belgium | 2,026 | 8.99 | |
Czech Republic | 2,358 | 10.46 | |
Denmark | 991 | 4.40 | |
Estonia | 3,338 | 14.81 | |
France | 2,170 | 9.63 | |
Germany | 693 | 3.08 | |
Italy | 1,689 | 7.50 | |
Netherlands | 1,314 | 5.83 | |
Slovenia | 1,078 | 4.78 | |
Spain | 1,897 | 8.42 | |
Sweden | 1,119 | 4.97 | |
Switzerland | 1,596 | 7.08 | |
Mental health | |||
Cognition score | 21,998 | −0.21 (0.79) | −2.89 – 2.79 |
Depressive symptoms | 21,840 | 2.56 (2.28) | 0 – 12 |
Physical health | |||
BMI | 21,844 | ||
BMI- underweight | 1.33 | ||
BMI- Normal | 34.74 | ||
BMI- overweight | 42.35 | ||
BMI- obese | 21.58 | ||
Poor eyesight | 22,494 | 5.50 | |
Poor hearing | 22,518 | 5.74 | |
Medications | 22,507 | 2.15 (1.66) | 0 – 11 |
Frailty (pre-frail or frail) | 20,118 | 15.30 | |
Morbidity1 | 22,498 | ||
Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, senility | 2.46 | ||
Cataracts | 12.03 | ||
Diabetes | 15.53 | ||
Heart attack | 15.67 | ||
Hip or femoral fracture | 2.31 | ||
Osteoarthritis/other rheumatism | 21.91 | ||
Parkinson | 1.27 | ||
Rheumatoid arthritis | 11.70 | ||
Mobility limitation | 22,529 | 2.13 (2.63) | 0 – 10 |
Falls | |||
Fell in W4 | 22,533 | 5.50 | |
Fell in W5 | 22,533 | 10.75 | |
FOF (W4) | 22,533 | 12.26 |
Conditions that existed before the fall-reporting period.
Study variables
The dependent variable in the current study was a dichotomous measure that reflected whether or not the respondent had fallen in the six months that preceded the Wave 5 interview (2013). It was obtained from the following probe: For the past six months at least, have you been bothered by any of the health conditions on this card? 1) Falling down, 2) Fear of falling down, 3) Dizziness, faints or blackouts, and 4) Fatigue. A corresponding measure from the Wave 4 interview (2011) was used to control for having fallen previously. In that question, phrased exactly as the one delineated above, “falling down” was included among 12 symptoms that were listed on the show card: 1) Pain in your back, knees, hips or any other joint, 2) Heart trouble or angina, chest pain during exercise, 3) Breathlessness, difficulty breathing, 4) Persistent cough, 5) Swollen legs, 6) Sleeping problems, 7) Falling down, 8) Fear of falling down, 9) Dizziness, faints or blackouts, 10) Stomach or intestine problems, including constipation, air, diarrhea, 11) Incontinence or involuntary loss of urine, and 12) Fatigue. The key independent variable—fear of falling—was obtained from the same list of Wave 4 symptoms just described. It, too, was employed in the analysis as a dichotomous baseline measure.
The domain of mobility limitation was measured from a list of ten difficulties (0–10), such as getting up from a chair after sitting for long periods, climbing one flight of stairs without resting, and stooping, kneeling, or crouching. We employed the sum score on this variable from Wave 5 to reflect the most up to date self-assessment of mobility status. The higher the score on this measure, the more limited was one’s mobility.
The sociodemographic background variables that were controlled as possible confounders in the present analysis were age at Wave 5, gender (female=1; male=0), marital status (married or partnered=1; single, widowed, divorced or separated=0) and education. Three levels of education were considered, based on the coding of the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED-97) that is utilized by SHARE: (1) primary schooling or less partial (ISCED-97 = 0–2); (2) secondary schooling (ISCED-97 = 3) and post-secondary schooling (ISCED-97 = 4–6). In addition, country was taken into account by a series of dummy variables using effect coding, such that each country’s respective score was its deviation from the mean of all the countries in relation to the same variable.
Health variables were also controlled in the analysis. Two measures of mental health were considered: cognition and depressive symptoms. The cognition variable was a summary score of three separate indicators: memory (immediate and delayed tests of verbal learning and information retention), numeracy (based on a serial sevens test), and fluency, measured by asking respondent to name distinct animals in a one-minute period. The score was standardized such that the mean centered at zero and deviations from the mean were either positive (higher than the mean) or negative (lower than the mean). Depressive symptoms (0–12) were measured by the Euro-D depression scale, a tool that was developed to enable cross-country comparisons in Europe (Prince et al., 1999).
Physical health at baseline was taken into account via several variables. Body mass index (BMI) was measured at four levels: underweight, normal, overweight and obese. Eyesight and hearing were ranked by respondents on measurement scales that ranged from poor to excellent. Two dichotomous variables were created for the purpose of the present analysis, to distinguish between those with poor eyesight and/or hearing (using a hearing aid as usual) and the rest of the respondents. The number of medications was calculated as a count of the health problems (e.g. diabetes, joint inflammation) for which drugs were taken at least once a week. Frailty was calculated using five dimensions (weight loss, exhaustion, grip strength, walking speed and physical activity) according to Fried’s definition (Fried et al., 2001) and its corresponding operationalization for SHARE (Romero-Ortuno, Walsh, Lawlor, & Kenny, 2010). Based on these dimensions, three categories were created (non-frail=0; pre-frail=1; and frail=2). Finally, morbidity was controlled by consideration of eight illnesses: Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, senility; cataracts; diabetes; heart attack; hip or femoral fracture; osteoarthritis/other rheumatism; Parkinson; and rheumatoid arthritis. Respondents were asked the following probe in order to retrieve this information: Has a doctor ever told you that you had/Do you currently have any of the conditions on this card?.
Analyses
The statistical analysis began with univariate descriptions of the study variables and examination of the bivariate associations of the independent and control variables with the fall outcome in Wave 5. These associations were considered using unadjusted logistic regressions. Next, the Wave 5 fall outcome was regressed on all the study variables by means of adjusted logistic regressions. As noted above, we ran the regressions with and without the frailty measure. In the second and fourth models of the regressions, we entered a term for the interaction of baseline FOF and mobility limitation at follow-up.
Results
Table 1 presents the distributions of each of the study variables at follow-up (Wave 5), plus falls and FOF at baseline (Wave 4). It shows that the respondents ranged in age from 65–104. A small majority were women and almost two thirds were married or partnered. A bit less than half had primary schooling only. The mean of the measure of cognitive health was about zero, by design, and it ranged from the mean point by almost 3 points in each direction. Respondents reported some two and a half depressive symptoms, on average. About a third of the sample had normal BMI while only about one percent were underweight. Some six percent reported problematic vision and/or hearing. Medications taken at least weekly ranged from 0–11. Fifteen percent of the sample were pre-frail or frail, and the percentage of those with physical illness ranged from one percent (Parkinson) to 22 percent (osteoarthritis). Mobility limitation at follow-up ranged from 0–10 (mean=2.1). About six percent reported having fallen at baseline and some 11 percent, at follow-up. One eighth of the sample reported fear of falling (FOF) at baseline.
Table 2 shows the unadjusted bivariate associations between the study variables and the dichotomous outcome variable—falling at follow-up. As may be seen, all the variables were significantly related. Fallers were more likely to be older and female, and less likely to be married and with secondary education or higher. Four countries (Austria, Slovenia, Sweden and Switzerland) were more likely to have had lower than average fall rates while five other countries (Belgium, Czech Republic, Estonia, France and Spain) were more likely to have had higher than average rates of falls. Having fallen was more likely among the more depressed and less likely among those with better cognition. Compared to those with normal BMI, those who were overweight had a lesser likelihood of falls, and both the underweight and the obese had a greater likelihood. Falling at follow-up was also more likely among those with poor eyesight, poor hearing, more medications taken weekly, frailty, and any of the eight illnesses considered. It was also somewhat more likely among those with mobility limitation at follow-up. Finally, falling at follow-up was more likely among those with FOF at baseline, and much more likely among those who had fallen at baseline.
Table 2:
Association of study variables with falling in the past 6 months: Unadjusted odds ratios
VARIABLES | OR |
---|---|
Sociodemographic background | |
Age | 1.074*** |
Female | 1.946*** |
Married/Partnered | .513*** |
Secondary educationA | 0.623*** |
Post-secondary educationA | 0.603*** |
CountryB | |
Austria | 0.839* |
Belgium | 1.195* |
Czech Republic | 1.612*** |
Denmark | 0.880 |
Estonia | 1.338*** |
France | 1.446*** |
Germany | 0.856 |
Italy | 0.900 |
Netherlands | 0.899 |
Slovenia | 0.602*** |
Spain | 1.706*** |
Sweden | 0.751** |
Switzerland | 0.680*** |
Mental health | |
Cognition score. | 0.550*** |
Depressive symptoms | 1.291*** |
Physical health | |
BMI- underweightA | 1.838*** |
BMI- overweightA | 0.844** |
BMI- obeseA | 1.164** |
Poor eyesight | 3.260*** |
Poor hearing | 2.556*** |
Medications | 1.402*** |
Frailty (pre-frail or frail)A | 3.308*** |
Morbidity | |
Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, senility | 3.041*** |
Cataracts | 2.045*** |
Diabetes | 1.637*** |
Heart attack | 1.912*** |
Hip or femoral fracture | 3.518*** |
Osteoarthritis/other rheumatism | 2.095*** |
Parkinson | 3.679*** |
Rheumatoid arthritis | 2.184*** |
Mobility limitation | 1.339*** |
Falls | |
Fell in W4A | 5.293*** |
FOF (W4)A | 3.722*** |
Reference categories: Education - Primary; BMI - Normal; Frailty - Non-frail; Falls - Did not fall/ Was not afraid of falling.
Effect coding (deviation from the country mean)
p<0.05,
p<0.01,
p<0.001
Model 1 in the first set of multivariate logistic regression analysis (Table 3) reveals that about half of the confounding variables retained their prior bivariate associations with the outcome variable, albeit to lesser degrees, when all study variables were considered simultaneously. The education variable, however, reversed its prior direction of association. Model 1 also underscores that having fallen at baseline retained its significant bivariate effect on falling at follow-up. As for the independent variables of interest, mobility limitation at follow-up was related to a somewhat greater likelihood of having fallen at follow-up. FOF at baseline was not related.
Table 3:
Predictors of Falling in the past 6 months: Logistic regression
SET 1 | SET 2 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
(With frailty variable) | (Without frailty variable) | |||
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | |
N | 19,023 | 19,023 | 20,654 | 20,654 |
VARIABLES | OR | OR | OR | OR |
Sociodemographic background | ||||
Age | 1.024*** | 1.023*** | 1.023*** | 1.022*** |
Female | 1.290*** | 1.274*** | 1.249*** | 1.235*** |
Married/Partnered | 0.765*** | 0.764*** | 0.789*** | 0.788*** |
Secondary educationA | 1.158* | 1.159* | 1.094 | 1.095 |
Post-secondary educationA | 1.283** | 1.293** | 1.259** | 1.268** |
Mental health | ||||
Cognition score. | 0.862** | 0.862** | 0.881** | 0.882** |
Depressive symptoms | 1.096*** | 1.095*** | 1.097*** | 1.096*** |
Physical health | ||||
BMI- underweightA | 0.907 | 0.888 | 0.937 | 0.924 |
BMI- overweightA | 0.898 | 0.899 | 0.874* | 0.874* |
BMI- obeseA | 0.862 | 0.862* | 0.870* | 0.870* |
Poor eyesight | 1.432*** | 1.434*** | 1.401*** | 1.407*** |
Poor hearing | 1.131 | 1.126 | 1.084 | 1.082 |
Medications | 1.094*** | 1.092*** | 1.091*** | 1.089*** |
FrailtyA | 1.213* | 1.210* | ||
Morbidity | ||||
AD, dementia, senility | 1.002 | 0.997 | 0.864 | 0.853 |
Cataracts | 1.159* | 1.158* | 1.168* | 1.167* |
Diabetes | 1.026 | 1.027 | 1.031 | 1.031 |
Heart attack | 1.055 | 1.056 | 1.044 | 1.044 |
Hip or femoral fracture | 1.845*** | 1.847*** | 1.732*** | 1.756*** |
Osteoarthritis/other rheumatism | 1.202** | 1.195** | 1.169** | 1.162* |
Parkinson | 1.622* | 1.607* | 1.751*** | 1.734*** |
Rheumatoid arthritis | 1.151 | 1.155 | 1.137 | 1.143 |
Mobility limitation | 1.159*** | 1.184*** | 1.171*** | 1.195*** |
Falls | ||||
Fell in W4A | 2.400*** | 2.399*** | 2.280*** | 2.294*** |
FOF (W4)A | 1.149 | 1.664*** | 1.172* | 1.711*** |
FOF (W4) × activity reduction (W5) | 0.918*** | 0.921*** | ||
Pseudo R-squared | 0.144 | 0.145 | 0.150 | 0.151 |
Note: Adjusted for county (results available on request).
Reference categories: Education - Primary; BMI - Normal; Frailty - Non-frail; Falls - Did not fall/ Was not afraid of falling.
p<0.05,
p<0.01,
p<0.001
Model 2 shows the results of the regression after entry of the interaction term. As may be seen, the effect of the confounders remained about the same. However, the interaction of baseline FOF and mobility limitation at follow-up was negative and significant. Moreover, with the entry of the interaction term, the association between mobility limitation and falls at follow-up increased in strength very slightly, while the effect of baseline FOF on falls at follow-up was strengthened considerably, and became statistically significant. Model 3 in the second set of analyses presents the same analysis as in Model 1, but without the frailty measure that was subject to a fair amount of missing data. As may be seen, the results remained about the same. More importantly, as may be seen in Model 4, the entry of the interaction term into the larger sample (i.e. including those who had been missing data on frailty) further increased the effect of baseline FOF on falls at follow-up.
The effect of the interaction in Model 4 may be seen more clearly in Figure 1, which shows the relationship between mobility limitation and the adjusted probability of falling at follow-up, by FOF at baseline. It can be seen, first, that up through a high degree of mobility limitation, those with no FOF at baseline were less likely to have fallen at follow-up than those with FOF. This trend is reversed, however, at the highest level of mobility limitation. That is, those with FOF at baseline and the highest extent of mobility limitation at follow-up were more likely to have fallen at the time of follow-up than those with the same degree of mobility limitation but no prior FOF. Reversing the coding of the mobility limitations variable and re-running the regression analyses underscored, moreover, that the differences at the two extremes were both significant.
Figure 1:
The association of activity reduction and the adjusted probability of falling by FOF
Discussion
The present study sought to clarify whether fear of falling (FOF) is indeed a risk factor for subsequent falling among older adults, independent of the effects of other factors. This is a difficult association to confirm because FOF is integrally related to other variables, particularly prior falls. Moreover, it is complicated to determine what comes first, falls or fear of falls, because both of these phenomena are part of the same downward spiral of functionality that increases one’s vulnerability to falling in late life.
The first hypothesis in our study posited that FOF is an independent risk factor for falling, beyond the effects of sociodemographic background, health/frailty and having had experienced a prior fall. The multivariate analyses confirmed that, indeed, baseline FOF was predictive of reported having fallen two years later, beyond the respective effects of the other study variables. In the first and third regression models that examined this hypothesis, the effect of FOF was significant, albeit weak. The amount of explained variance in this model was also modest. The strength of FOF as a predictor increased considerably in the final model, however, which is discussed next.
The second hypothesis maintained that mobility limitation moderates the effect of FOF on falling. This hypothesis was confirmed as well. In the final model, both the mobility limitation variable and the FOF measure were significant. Moreover, as mentioned in the previous paragraph, the strength of FOF as a predictor increased meaningfully with the entry of the interaction term between these two variables. The strength of mobility limitation as a predictor in the respective models, on the other hand, remained about the same with and without the interaction.
The graph of the respective effects shed light on the dynamics that are behind the association between FOF and mobility limitation in relation to overall fall probability. The graph showed that FOF predicted subsequent falls when mobility limitation was low to moderate. It was only when mobility limitation was at a high level that the effect of FOF on fall probability became reversed. That is, when mobility limitation was high, it was lack of FOF that was a stronger predictor of subsequent falls. The graph thus underscores that there is a complex association between FOF and mobility limitation in relation to late-life falls.
Our study, which sought to disentangle the effect of FOF through a number of ways, had several advantages. First, the analysis was based upon two consecutive waves of data from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe, a major research infrastructure for the analysis of important late life phenomena. The two waves of data took place two years apart, allowing us to consider the effect of FOF at one point in time on the actual reporting of having fell a relatively close period afterwards. In addition, the study design allowed us to control for prior falls, a well-known risk factor for subsequent falling. By controlling for prior falls we were able to examine more accurately the unique effect of FOF.
A third advantage of the present research was its inclusion of the variable of mobility limitation as a moderator. Both falls and FOF are related to mobility limitation, which in turn may increase vulnerability for future falls. Hence, we included in the analysis a count of limitations of ten key physical mobility functions in order to capture and to account for this domain. Furthermore, we interacted FOF at baseline with current mobility limitation in order to better understand the dynamics between the two phenomena. Yet another asset of the current analysis is that it also controlled for baseline frailty in part of the analyses.
Finally, our utilization of a very large sample, which is uniquely provided by SHARE, allowed us to control for the possible effects of a wide range of confounders. The large sample gave sufficient statistical power to take these many variables into account. Moreover, the data on such a large sample of community-dwelling older adults from a wide range of countries offered an unprecedented opportunity to examine the association between FOF and subsequent falls among older adults living at home.
A few limitations of the present analysis should also be noted. First, falls were reported for a given period on a single item. Some research in the domain of late life falling addresses the nature and extent of falls in a more specific and nuanced manner. This was not the case in the SHARE survey, which utilizes a comprehensive questionnaire addressing a wide range of topics. The amount of time that can be devoted to each topic in SHARE is limited. This is indeed a shortcoming, but is offset by the fact that the SHARE database provides a great number of variables for analysis and a very large number of respondents from nationally representative random samples.
Second, the rate of reported falls in this study is a bit low, about 12 percent at follow-up and even fewer at baseline. One review has maintained that between 30–40% of patients over 65 fall yearly (Ambrose et al., 2013). A study of community dwelling older adults in the United States, a population that is closer to that of the present study, found that falls were reported by 24 percent (Bertera & Bertera, 2008). One explanation for the lower rates reported in SHARE is that the period for which information was solicited referred to the previous six months. Indeed, one American study that related to falls in the previous three months estimated that about ten percent of people aged 65 and over reported falling at least once (Boyd & Stevens, 2009). Thus, the lower reported rate of falls in the SHARE data could well stem from the shorter reference period that was used, presumably, to provide greater accuracy to the responses.
A word needs to be said, also, about the increase in the percentage of reported fallers at follow-up. This may be partly due to the fact that falling down was one of only four symptoms queried in the Wave 5 questionnaire compared to 12 symptoms at baseline, in Wave 4. The greater number of symptoms in the baseline probe may have lead to the overlooking of various items, yielding a lower rate of falls. However, it must also be taken into account that all respondents were two years older at follow-up, and age is known to be related to a higher degree of falling.
A final limitation concerns the period of measurement that was considered. Since falls were reported in each wave of SHARE for the previous six months, falls that may have occurred in the interim may not have been reported. Thus, it could be that the real number of fallers is under-reported and that some of them are included among the non-fallers at follow-up. This concern requires that the results of the present study be considered cautiously. This limitation notwithstanding, the fact that the SHARE data allows longitudinal analysis is a strong advantage, as it minimizes concerns of reverse causality that are frequent in cross-sectional analysis.
The findings from our study have several important implications. First, we underscored that FOF is an independent risk factor for falling. Stated differently, we find support for the probability that people who are worried about falling tend to fall more often than those who are not similarly worried. There seems to be, therefore, a self-fulfilling prophecy at work. This observation stresses the importance of addressing the self-confidence of older people as a preventive factor in the attempt to reduce falling among older populations. Self-confidence is a malleable feature that can certainly be strengthened through appropriate interventions.
Second, our finding that those who have a high level of mobility limitation but lack FOF are more likely to fall highlights another group at risk, one that has, perhaps, received less attention in the literature. In the case of considerable mobility limitation, it may well be that FOF acts as a protective buffer, limiting the most vulnerable from taking actions that may precipitate falls. The less worried in this group, however, may not be as careful and, as a result, may be subject to greater falling. This topic requires more research and greater professional attention.
In conclusion, we have found that the association between FOF and mobility limitation in relation to late-life falls is a complex dynamic. We believe that with proper understanding of these dynamics, the downward spiral of FOF and falling might be altered to some degree. For this purpose and given the potential benefits that can ensue, continued inquiry into this domain is indeed warranted.
References
- Alamgir H, Wong NJ, Hu YH, Marshall A, & Yu SC (2015). Epidemiology of Falls in Older Adults in Texas. Southern Medical Journal, 108(2), 119–124. doi: 10.14423/smj.0000000000000237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allison LK, Painter JA, Emory A, Whitehurst P, & Raby A (2013). Participation Restriction, Not Fear of Falling, Predicts Actual Balance and Mobility Abilities in Rural Community-Dwelling Older Adults. Journal of Geriatric Physical Therapy, 36(1), 13–23. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0b013e3182493d20 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ambrose AF, Paul G, & Hausdorff JM (2013). Risk factors for falls among older adults: A review of the literature. Maturitas, 75(1), 51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2013.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Austin N, Devine A, Dick I, Prince R, & Bruce D (2007). Fear of falling in older women: A longitudinal study of incidence, persistence, and predictors. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 55(10), 1598–1603. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01317.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertera EM, & Bertera RL (2008). Fear of falling and activity avoidance in a national sample of older adults in the United States. Health & Social Work, 33(1), 54–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Börsch-Supan A, Brandt M, Hunkler C, Kneip T, Korbmacher J, Malter F, . . . Team, Share Cent Coordination. (2013). Data Resource Profile: The Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(4), 992–1001. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyd R, & Stevens JA (2009). Falls and fear of falling: burden, beliefs and behaviours. Age and Ageing, 38(4), 423–428. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afp053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryant MS, Rintala DH, Hou JG, & Protas EJ (2015). Relationship of Falls and Fear of Falling to Activity Limitations and Physical Inactivity in Parkinson’s Disease. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 23(2), 187–193. doi: 10.1123/japa.2013-0244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chamberlin ME, Fulwider BD, Sanders SL, & Medeiros JM (2005). Does fear of falling influence spatial and temporal gait parameters in elderly persons beyond changes associated with normal aging? Journals of Gerontology Series a-Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 60(9), 1163–1167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang VC, & Do MT (2015). Risk Factors for Falls Among Seniors: Implications of Gender. American Journal of Epidemiology, 181(7), 521–531. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu268 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi EJ, Kim SA, Kim NR, Rhee JA, Yun YW, & Shin MH (2014). Risk Factors for Falls in Older Korean Adults: The 2011 Community Health Survey. Journal of Korean Medical Science, 29(11), 1482–1487. doi: 10.3346/jkms.2014.29.11.1482 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi K, & Ko Y (2015). Characteristics Associated With Fear of Falling and Activity Restriction in South Korean Older Adults. Journal of Aging and Health, 27(6), 1066–1083. doi: 10.1177/0898264315573519 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chou KL, & Chi I (2007). The temporal relationship between falls and fear-of-falling among Chinese older primary-care patients in Hong Kong. Ageing & Society, 27, 181–193. doi: [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chou KL, & Chi I (2008). Reciprocal relationship between fear of falling and depression in elderly Chinese primary care patients. Aging & Mental Health, 12(5), 587–594. doi: 10.1080/13607860802343068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clemson L, Kendig H, Mackenzie L, & Browning C (2015). Predictors of Injurious Falls and Fear of Falling Differ: An 11-Year Longitudinal Study of Incident Events in Older People. Journal of Aging and Health, 27(2), 239–256. doi: 10.1177/0898264314546716 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doi T, Ono R, Ono K, Yamaguchi R, Makiura D, & Hirata S (2012). The Association between Fear of Falling and Physical Activity in Older Women. Journal of Physical Therapy Science, 24(9), 859–862. [Google Scholar]
- Filiatrault J, Desrosiers J, & Trottier L (2009). An Exploratory Study of Individual and Environmental Correlates of Fear of Falling Among Community-Dwelling Seniors. Journal of Aging and Health, 21(6), 881–894. doi: 10.1177/0898264309340694 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, Newman AB, Hirsch C, Gottdiener J, . . . Cardiovascular Hlth Study, Collabor. (2001). Frailty in older adults: Evidence for a phenotype. Journals of Gerontology Series a-Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 56(3), M146–M156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman SM, Munoz B, West SK, Rubin GS, & Fried LP (2002). Falls and fear of falling: Which comes first? A longitudinal prediction model suggests strategies for primary and secondary prevention. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 50(8), 1329–1335. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50352.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guthrie DM, Fletcher PC, Berg K, Williams E, Boumans N, & Hirdes JP (2012). The Role of Medications in Predicting Activity Restriction Due to a Fear of Falling. Journal of Aging and Health, 24(2), 269–286. doi: 10.1177/0898264311422598 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadjistavropoulos T, et al. (2012). “The Relationship of Fear of Falling and Balance Confidence With Balance and Dual Tasking Performance.” Psychology and Aging 27(1): 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadjistavropoulos T, Delbaere K, & Fitzgerald TD (2011). Reconceptualizing the Role of Fear of Falling and Balance Confidence in Fall Risk. Journal of Aging and Health, 23(1), 3–23. doi: 10.1177/0898264310378039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadjistavropoulos T, Martin RR, Sharpe D, Lints AC, McCreary DR, & Asmundson GJG (2007). A longitudinal investigation of fear of falling, fear of pain, and activity avoidance in community-dwelling older adults. Journal of Aging and Health, 19(6), 965–984. doi: 10.1177/0898264307308611 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jamison M, Neuberger GB, & Miller PA (2003). Correlates of falls and fear of falling among adults with rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis & Rheumatism-Arthritis Care & Research, 49(5), 673–680. doi: 10.1002/art.11383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jefferis BJ, Iliffe S, Kendrick D, Kerse N, Trost S, Lennon LT, . . . Whincup PH (2014). How are falls and fear of falling associated with objectively measured physical activity in a cohort of community-dwelling older men? Bmc Geriatrics, 14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-14-114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jung D (2008). Fear of Falling in Older Adults: Comprehensive Review. Asian Nursing Research, 2(4), 214–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalula SZ, Ferreira M, Swingler GH, & Badri M (2016). Risk factors for falls in older adults in a South African Urban Community. Bmc Geriatrics, 16. doi: 10.1186/s12877-016-0212-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamil RJ, Betz J, Powers BB, Pratt S, Kritchevsky S, Ayonayon HN, . . . Hlth ABC study. (2016). Association of Hearing Impairment With Incident Frailty and Falls in Older Adults. Journal of Aging and Health, 28(4), 644–660. doi: 10.1177/0898264315608730 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly KD, Pickett W, Yiannakoulias N, Rowe BH, Schopflocher DP, Svenson L, & Voaklander DC (2003). Medication use and falls in community-dwelling older persons. Age and Ageing, 32(5), 503–509. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afg081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim S, & So WY (2013). Prevalence and correlates of fear of falling in Korean community-dwelling elderly subjects. Experimental Gerontology, 48(11), 1323–1328. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2013.08.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kojima G, Kendrick D, Skelton DA, Morris RW, Gawler S, & Iliffe S (2015). Frailty predicts short-term incidence of future falls among British community-dwelling older people: a prospective cohort study nested within a randomised controlled trial. Bmc Geriatrics, 15. doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0152-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koski K, Luukinen H, Laippala P, & Kivela SL (1998). Risk factors for major injurious falls among the home-dwelling elderly by functional abilities - A prospective population-based study. Gerontology, 44(4), 232–238. doi: 10.1159/000022017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kressig RW, Wolf SL, Sattin RW, O’Grady M, Greenspan A, Curns A, & Kutner M (2001). Associations of demographic, functional, and behavioral characteristics with activity-related fear of falling among older adults transitioning to frailty. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 49(11), 1456–1462. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.4911237.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeBouthillier DM, Thibodeau MA, & Asmundson GJG (2013). Severity of Fall-Based Injuries, Fear of Falling, and Activity Restriction: Sex Differences in a Population-Based Sample of Older Canadian Adults. Journal of Aging and Health, 25(8), 1378–1387. doi: 10.1177/0898264313507317 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legters K (2002). Fear of falling. Physical Therapy, 82(3), 264–272. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maki BE, Holliday PJ, & Topper AK (1991). Fear of falling and postural performance in the elderly. Journals of Gerontology, 46(4), M123–M131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin FC, Hart D, Spector T, Doyle DV, & Harari D (2005). Fear of falling limiting activity in young-old women is associated with reduced functional mobility rather than psychological factors. Age and Ageing, 34(3), 281–287. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afi074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy J, & Isaacs B (1982). The post-fall syndrome: A study of 36 elderly patients. Gerontology, 28(4), 265–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicklett EJ, & Taylor RJ (2014). Racial/Ethnic Predictors of Falls Among Older Adults: The Health and Retirement Study. Journal of Aging and Health, 26(6), 1060–1075. doi: 10.1177/0898264314541698 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poss JW, & Hirdes JP (2016). Very Frequent Fallers and Future Fall Injury: Continuous Risk Among Community-Dwelling Home Care Recipients. Journal of Aging and Health, 28(4), 587–599. doi: 10.1177/0898264315599941 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prince MJ, Reischies F, Beekman ATF, Fuhrer R, Jonker C, Kivela SL, . . . Copeland JRM (1999). Development of the EURO-D scale - a European Union initiative to compare symptoms of depression in 14 European centres. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 330–338. doi: 10.1192/bjp.174.4.330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romero-Ortuno R, Walsh CD, Lawlor BA, & Kenny RA (2010). A Frailty Instrument for primary care: findings from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). Bmc Geriatrics, 10. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-10-57 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossat A, Fantino B, Nitenberg C, Annweiler C, Poujol L, Herrmann FR, & Beauchet O (2010). Risk factors for falling in community-dwelling older adults: Which of them are associated with the recurrence of falls? Journal of Nutrition Health & Aging, 14(9), 787–791. doi: 10.1007/s12603-010-0089-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samper-Ternent R, Karmarkar A, Graham J, Reistetter T, & Ottenbacher K (2012). Frailty as a Predictor of Falls in Older Mexican Americans. Journal of Aging and Health, 24(4), 641–653. doi: 10.1177/0898264311428490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santos-Eggimann B, Cuenoud P, Spagnoli J, & Junod J (2009). Prevalence of Frailty in Middle-Aged and Older Community-Dwelling Europeans Living in 10 Countries. Journals of Gerontology Series a-Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 64(6), 675–681. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glp012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott D, Hayes A, Sanders KM, Aitken D, Ebeling PR, & Jones G (2014). Operational definitions of sarcopenia and their associations with 5-year changes in falls risk in community-dwelling middle-aged and older adults. Osteoporosis International, 25(1), 187–193. doi: 10.1007/s00198-013-2431-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tinetti ME, & Powell L (1993). Fear of falling and low self-efficacy: A cause of dependence in elderly persons. Journals of Gerontology, 48, 35–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trombetti A, Reid KF, Hars M, Herrmann FR, Pasha E, Phillips EM, & Fielding RA (2016). Age-associated declines in muscle mass, strength, power, and physical performance: impact on fear of falling and quality of life. Osteoporosis International, 27(2), 463–471. doi: 10.1007/s00198-015-3236-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viljanen A, Kulmala J, Rantakokko M, Koskenvuo M, Kaprio J, & Rantanen T (2012). Fear of Falling and Coexisting Sensory Difficulties As Predictors of Mobility Decline in Older Women. Journals of Gerontology Series a-Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 67(11), 1230–1237. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viljanen A, Kulmala J, Rantakokko M, Koskenvuo M, Kaprio J, & Rantanen T (2013). Accumulation of Sensory Difficulties Predicts Fear of Falling in Older Women. Journal of Aging and Health, 25(5), 776–791. doi: 10.1177/0898264313494412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vellas BJ, Wayne SJ, Romero LJ, Baumgartner RN, & Garry PJ (1997). Fear of falling and restriction of mobility in elderly fallers. Age and Ageing, 26(3), 189–193. doi: 10.1093/ageing/26.3.189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vind AB, Andersen HE, Pedersen KD, Joergensen T, & Schwarz P (2011). Who will fall again? Predictors of further falls in one year following an injurious fall. European Geriatric Medicine, 2(3), 145–149. doi: 10.1016/j.eurger.2011.05.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zijlstra GAR, van Haastregt JCM, van Eijk JTM, van Rossum E, Stalenhoef PA, & Kempen Gijm (2007). Prevalence and correlates of fear of falling, and associated avoidance of activity in the general population of community-living older people. Age and Ageing, 36(3), 304–309. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afm021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]