Abstract
The position of the Moon in relation to the Earth and the Sun gives rise to several predictable cycles, and natural changes in nighttime light intensity are known to cause alterations to physiological processes and behaviors in many animals. The limited research undertaken to date on the physiological responses of animals to the lunar illumination has exclusively focused on the synodic lunar cycle (full moon to full moon, or moon phase) but the moon's orbit—its distance from the Earth—may also be relevant. Every month, the moon moves from apogee, its most distant point from Earth—and then to perigee, its closest point to Earth. Here, we studied wild barnacle geese (Branta leucopsis) to investigate the influence of multiple interacting lunar cycles on the physiology of diurnally active animals. Our study, which uses biologging technology to continually monitor body temperature and heart rate for an entire annual cycle, asks whether there is evidence for a physiological response to natural cycles in lunar brightness in wild birds, particularly “supermoon” phenomena, where perigee coincides with a full moon. There was a three‐way interaction between lunar phase, lunar distance, and cloud cover as predictors of nighttime mean body temperature, such that body temperature was highest on clear nights when the full moon coincided with perigee moon. Our study is the first to report the physiological responses of wild birds to “supermoon” events; the wild geese responded to the combination of two independent lunar cycles, by significantly increasing their body temperature at night. That wild birds respond to natural fluctuations in nighttime ambient light levels support the documented responses of many species to anthropogenic sources of artificial light, that birds seem unable to override. As most biological systems are arguably organized foremost by light, this suggests that any interactions between lunar cycles and local weather conditions could have significant impacts on the energy budgets of birds.
Keywords: circadian, energy expenditure, lunar cycles, supermoon
1. INTRODUCTION
In many animals, alterations to physiological processes and behaviors often occur at times which coincide with optimal conditions for key life‐history events such as reproduction, migration, or molt (Raible, Takekata, & Essmar‐Raible, 2017). This co‐occurrence can be observed at multiple scales, including annual, diurnal, tidal, and solar cycles (Cheeseman, Fewster, & Walker, 2017). Among these cycles, the position of the Moon in relation to the Earth and the Sun gives rise to several predictable cycles. The lunar synodic cycle (full moon to full moon, or moon phase) has an average length of 29.5 days and causes changes in the geomagnetic field, gravitational pull, and ambient light levels (Lohman & Willows, 1987). These changes are detectable by a wide range of organisms (McDowell, 1969), and the synodic cycle of the moon therefore has a strong influence on the behavior, foraging efficiency, and energy expenditure of organisms (McDowell, 1969; Navarro‐Castilla & Barja, 2014; Smit, Boyles, Brigham, & McKechnie, 2011). The lunar synodic cycle provides cues that are used by numerous marine organisms, for example, to stimulate and synchronize reproduction (Grant et al., 2009), while changes in nighttime ambient light levels have been shown to affect the behavioral strategies of both predator and prey (Penteriani, et al., 2013). Many nocturnally active animals, for example, alter their behavior with the changing light conditions in connection with the lunar synodic cycle, typically a result of either a change in predation risk or a change in prey availability (Lang, Kalko, Romer, Bockholdt, & Dechmann, 2006).
While the influence of the synodic lunar cycle on nocturnal animals is perhaps intuitive, there is emerging evidence that diurnally active animals can also be influenced by lunar cycles. For diurnal species, full moon events will coincide with the sleep phase of the 24‐hr cycle. Many behaviors, including sleep‐wake cycles, calling, activity, and foraging patterns are controlled by the circadian system (Gwinner, 1996). Most circadian rhythms are endogenously controlled and synchronized by the local light‐dark cycle of the environment (Gwinner, 2003; Tarlow, Hau, Anderson, & Wikelski, 2003). For most diurnal species, there is a characteristic increase in body temperature and heart rate at dawn, both of which remain elevated during the active daytime period, and are then significantly lower at night during the rest period (Brown et al., 2002). These likely reflect a state of reduced energy expenditure at night for diurnal animals. In some species of diurnal endotherms, this circadian rhythm in body temperature can include bouts of nighttime torpor, a state of decreased endogenous heat production and lowered body temperature which is used to further conserve energy (Butler & Woakes, 2001; Calder & Booser, 1973; Cheke, 1971). Bright nights may, therefore, have significant implications for the daily energy expenditure and energy budget of diurnal species if their typical circadian rhythms of rest and activity are interrupted.
Increases in nighttime ambient light levels can be caused either by, (a) the synodic lunar cycle, with full moons being brighter, or (b) the moon's orbit, the distance of the moon from the Earth. The limited research undertaken to date on the physiological responses of animals to the lunar illumination has exclusively focused on the synodic lunar cycle but the moon's orbit may also be relevant. The moon's distance from Earth varies throughout its monthly path because the moon's orbit is not perfectly circular (Chapront‐Touze and Chapront, 1983). Every month, this eccentric orbit carries the moon to apogee, its most distant point from Earth—and then to perigee, its closest point approximately two weeks later. During the perigee phase, the moon is ~46,000 km closer to the earth than at apogee (Chapront‐Touze and Chapront, 1983) and it appears significantly larger in the sky (Figure 1). Since this cycle—the lunar distance—operates independently of the synodic lunar cycle, when perigee occurs in conjunction with a new moon approximately three times a year, a “supermoon” is observed (Kyba, Mohar, & Posch, 2017). During a “supermoon” (the perigee syzygy), the light from the moon will be at its brightest—approximately 30% brighter—and has the potential to have a further impact on the circadian physiology of animals. A typical summer full moon, for example, at temperate latitudes provides only about 0.05–0.1 lux, compared a “supermoon” in the tropics directly overhead potentially providing up to 0.36 lux (Kyba et al., 2017). However, this potential influence has never previously been investigated, and has yet to be integrated into the synodic lunar cycle, in order to fully understand how these two cycles interact to influence the physiology of animals. Here, to study the influence of multiple interacting lunar cycles on the physiology of diurnally active animals, we use migratory barnacle geese (Branta leucopsis) as an exemplar species. Barnacle geese are visual‐feeding herbivorous terrestrial grazers, exhibiting a strict diurnal behavioral pattern during the winter months when in Scotland of feeding on salt marsh and farmland during the day, and roosting on water at night (Phillips et al., 2003). Our study, which uses biologging technology to study body temperature and heart rate, asks whether there is evidence for a physiological response to natural cycles in lunar brightness, particularly “supermoon” phenomena, in wild birds.
2. MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1. Birds
Eight wild adult barnacle geese (mean body mass, 2.1 ± 0.6 kg) were caught at Ny‐Ålesund research station on the island of Spitsbergen in the Svalbard archipelago (788,550 N, 118,560 E) during the flightless period of their annual wing molt in July 1999 (see Portugal, Thorpe, Green, Myatt, & Butler, 2009; Portugal, Butler, Green, & Cassey, 2011 for molt definitions and further details). All captured geese were color‐ringed to aid recapture in 2000. The population that breeds at Ny‐Ålesund winter on the Solway Firth (south‐west Scotland, 54.981253, −3.489971), and migrate along the Norwegian coast during their annual autumn migration (Butler, Woakes, & Bishop, 1998). During the winter period, from October to April, the Svalbard population of barnacle geese occupy the smallest known area for a wild goose population in the world (Phillips et al., 2003), predominantly inhabiting the Wildfowl and Wetlands Trust (WWT) reserve at Caerlaverock. The birds roost approximately half a kilometer offshore within the Solway estuary on salt marsh, and are thus safe from land predators such as red foxes (Vulpes vulpes). Thus, although the precise locations of the eight geese were not known, we can be confident that they were feeding and roosting within a known area of ~100 km2. The birds then depart Scotland late April, and travel North to Svalbard over a period of 5–6 weeks. During the summer in Svalbard, the geese are exposed to 24‐hr daylight, with light levels never dropping below civil twilight.
2.2. Body temperature and heart rate measurements
Abdominal body temperature (T ab) and heart rate (f H) were measured continuously throughout the annual cycle by custom‐made implantable heart rate data loggers (Butler & Woakes, 2001), successfully used with this species on previous occasions (Butler et al., 1998; Portugal, Thorpe, et al., 2009; Portugal, White, Green, & Butler, 2019; Ward, Bishop, Woakes, & Butler, 2002). The loggers (5 cm × 2.5 cm × 0.7 cm) were programmed to record every 5 s, and ran from early August 1999 to mid‐May 2000 (Figure 2). Recordings of T ab and f H for a minimum of 9 months were obtained from all six birds that were recaptured in the summer of 2000; the remaining two birds were resighted but recapture was not possible. The six geese were failed breeders in 1999 prior to capture; although eggs were laid, they were not successfully hatched.
Methods for logger implantation and removal are described in detail elsewhere (Bevan, Woakes, Butler, & Croxall, 1995; Guillemette et al., 2016). Before use, the loggers were encased in paraffin wax and encapsulated in silicon rubber to provide waterproofing and bio‐compatibility. The temperature sensor of the encapsulated data logger was calibrated by immersing the device in water baths of known temperature. The mass of the internally implanted device (21 g) was comfortably below the 5% of total body mass recommended when deploying internally implanted biologging devices on birds (Portugal & White, 2018; White et al., 2013). Mean daily heart rate of all geese was used to determine daily activity levels (see Portugal, Green, White, Guillemette, & Butler, 2012), in order to identify the autumn migratory flights taking place in September, from Svalbard to Scotland. Nighttime mean T ab and f H were calculated on an individual basis as the mean between 23:00 and 03:00. Daytime mean T ab and f H were calculated as means between 09:00 and 15:00.
2.3. Meteorological and astronomical data
Minimum and maximum temperatures (°C), precipitation (mm), and cloud cover (%) were acquired as daily means for the winter study period (October 6, 1999 until 20 April 2000) from the British Meteorological Office, obtained from a weather station based at Eskdalemuir (55.267751, −3.175316), a 20 km (beeline) distance from the WWT Caerlaverock reserve. Cloud cover data were cross‐referenced with local newspaper archives, and if a discrepancy of more than 10% in cloud cover was observed between the local archives and the British meteorological office, a mean was taken (N = 7). If the discrepancy was <10%, the percentage cloud cover value from the British meteorological office was used. Lunar data (synodic phase and distance from Earth) were calculated using the lunar package in R (Lazaridis, 2014; R Core Team, 2017). We calculated lunar distance as a continuum between, and including, perigee and apogee, scaled to fall between 0 [nearest distance over the observation period] and 1 [furthest difference during the observation period]), and synodic phase (calculated as the absolute value of cosine of the lunar phase angle, in radians, minus π, such that a value of zero corresponds to the full moon and a value of π corresponds to a new moon). Daylength was calculated using the geosphere package in R (Forsythe, Rykiel, Stahl, Wu, & Schoolfield, 1995).
2.4. Analysis
Preliminary analyses revealed among‐bird variation in all measures of both T ab and f H, as well as substantial within‐bird temporal autocorrelation in both T ab and f H. Data were therefore analyzed using a linear mixed model framework to account for the among‐individual differences, and to model the temporal autocorrelation explicitly. Linear mixed models were implemented in the nlme package of R (Pinheiro & Bates, 2000; R Core Team, 2017), with variances associated with the random effects estimated by Maximum Likelihood. We modeled both nighttime and daytime T ab and f H as dependent variables, for a total of four models. All models included lunar distance, synodic phase, cloud cover (%), and the two‐ and three‐way interactions between these as fixed effects, to determine whether these (as determinants of lunar illumination) had a significant effect on T ab or f H. All models also included minimum temperature (°C) and precipitation (mm) as fixed effects, because of the possible influence of these on T ab and f H (Bryant & Westerterp, 1983; Moreno, 1989; White, Blackburn, Martin, & Butler, 2007; White, Grémillet, Green, Martin, & Butler, 2011). Date was included as a continuous fixed effect in all models to account for potential trends in T ab or f H through time, and all models also included a random intercept for individual ID, and an order 1 temporal autocorrelation structure for each bird. Fixed effects and interactions were considered significant at p < 0.05. Variance inflation factors for the interaction terms in these models were always high (greater than 10 in all cases), indicating that multicollinearity among predictors results in inflated estimates of standard errors and conservative tests of significance (O'Brien, 2007). Conclusions regarding significant interactions are therefore robust, but caution is warranted when interpreting the nonsignificant interactions. So nonsignificant interactions (all data is available in the supplementary material).
3. RESULTS
Barnacle geese are exposed to constant daylight during the summer breeding season at Svalbard, exhibit a period of seasonal hyperthermia prior to their autumn migration, and the migration itself is characterized by high heart rates (Butler & Woakes, 2001; Figure 2). Although the geese experienced perigee events during their summer season at Svalbard (Figure 2a), the analysis was restricted to the winter period when the birds were at Caerlaverock (Scotland, UK; Section 2) to exclude the possible effects of long days, migration, and seasonal hypothermia on responses to the lunar cycle. During the 1999–2000 winter season (October 6, 1999 until 20 April 2000), when the barnacle geese were at Caerlaverock, they experienced seven perigee events (Figure 2a). These perigee events varied in distance by up to 10,000 km, with the minimum perigee distance between the Moon and Earth occurring during November, December, and January, and further perigee events occurring during October, February, March, and April. Full moons can occur at any point along the Moon's elliptical path, but are referred to as a “supermoon” when the new moon is an exact match (i.e., the same day) in time with perigee. This occurred during all three closer approaches in November, December, and January. The perigees in October and February would be defined as a “near match” (2–3 days either side; Figure 2a; Kyba et al., 2017).
The geese exhibited a pronounced circadian rhythm in heart rate (f H) and abdominal body temperature (T ab), with both increasing at dawn and decreasing at dusk during the winter months (Figure 3). There was no obvious rhythm in either physiological parameter during the constant daylight of Svalbard (Figure 3), but Lombe‐Scargle periodograms (Ruf, 1999) revealed rhythms with a periodicity of ~24 hr in two of five birds for f H, and four of five birds for T ab.
There was a three‐way interaction between lunar phase, lunar distance, and cloud cover as predictors of nighttime mean T ab (Table 1). The result of this was that the negative effect of distance on nighttime mean T ab was strongest during full moons and clear nights, when the full moon coincided with perigee moon (Figure 4a–f), leading to elevations in nighttime mean T ab (Figure 3b). There was a negative relationship between nighttime mean T ab and precipitation, and nighttime mean T ab declined significantly throughout the study period (Table 1). There was no effect of cloud cover, lunar distance, or lunar phase on daytime mean T ab, which exhibited a negative relationship with precipitation and declined throughout the study period (Table 2).
Table 1.
Value | SE | t‐value | p‐value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 66.6 | 5.29 | 12.6 | <0.0001 |
Cloud | −0.0043 | 0.0010 | −4.15 | <0.0001 |
Distance | −1.16 | 0.20 | −5.96 | <0.0001 |
Phase | −0.44 | 0.07 | −5.97 | <0.0001 |
Temperature | −0.0013 | 0.0028 | −0.46 | 0.65 |
Precipitation | −0.0026 | 0.0011 | −2.27 | 0.02 |
Date | −0.0026 | 0.0005 | −5.46 | <0.0001 |
Cloud × Distance | 0.0041 | 0.0023 | 1.73 | 0.08 |
Cloud × Phase | 0.031 | 0.001 | 3.52 | 0.0004 |
Distance × Phase | 0.62 | 0.11 | 5.76 | <0.0001 |
Cloud × Distance × Phase | −0.0028 | 0.0012 | −2.26 | 0.02 |
Autocorrelation parameter = 0.68. Variance estimates for the random effects: intercept: 2.25, Residual: 0.19. Degrees of freedom for all parameters = 1,247. Significant effects are highlighted in bold.
Table 2.
Value | SE | t‐value | p‐value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 79.8 | 4.81 | 16.6 | <0.0001 |
Cloud | −0.00050 | 0.00052 | −0.97 | 0.33 |
Distance | 0.046 | 0.11 | 0.41 | 0.69 |
Phase | −0.005 | 0.044 | −0.11 | 0.91 |
Temperature | −0.0016 | 0.0014 | −1.10 | 0.27 |
Precipitation | −0.0013 | 0.0006 | −2.21 | 0.03 |
Date | −0.0038 | 0.0004 | −8.66 | <0.0001 |
Cloud × Distance | 0.0006 | 0.0012 | 0.56 | 0.57 |
Cloud × Phase | 0.0052 | 0.00043 | 1.20 | 0.23 |
Distance × Phase | 0.018 | 0.06 | 0.30 | 0.76 |
Cloud × Distance × Phase | −0.00057 | 0.00062 | −0.92 | 0.36 |
Autocorrelation parameter = 0.81. Variance estimates for the random effects: intercept: 2.33, Residual: 0.08. Degrees of freedom for all parameters = 1,247. Significant effects are highlighted in bold.
Although f H appeared to vary with the lunar phase of the synodic cycle and the distance of the Moon from Earth in a similar pattern to T ab (Figure 2c), there were no significant two‐ or three‐way interactions between cloud cover, lunar distance, or lunar phase on nighttime mean f H (Table 3). There was, however, a significant main effect of distance, such that f H decreased with lunar distance irrespective of cloud cover or lunar phase, although the relationship was weak (Figure 4g–l). There was also a significant positive relationship between nighttime mean f H and mean ambient temperature (Table 3). There was no effect of cloud cover, lunar distance, or lunar phase on daytime mean f H, which also exhibited a positive relationship with precipitation (Table 4).
Table 3.
Value | SE | t‐value | p‐value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 140.6 | 212.2 | 0.66 | 0.51 |
Cloud | 0.021 | 0.037 | 0.57 | 0.57 |
Distance | −17.7 | 7.2 | −2.44 | 0.01 |
Phase | −4.66 | 2.74 | −1.70 | 0.09 |
Temperature | 0.24 | 0.10 | 2.34 | 0.02 |
Precipitation | −0.07 | 0.04 | −1.71 | 0.09 |
Date | −0.004 | 0.019 | −0.23 | 0.82 |
Cloud × Distance | −0.06 | 0.08 | −0.72 | 0.47 |
Cloud × Phase | −0.01 | 0.03 | −0.34 | 0.74 |
Distance × Phase | 4.98 | 3.96 | 1.26 | 0.20 |
Cloud × Distance × Phase | 0.038 | 0.044 | 0.87 | 0.39 |
Autocorrelation parameter = 0.68. Variance estimates for the random effects: intercept: 525, Residual: 386. Degrees of freedom for all parameters = 1,234. Significant effects are highlighted in bold.
Table 4.
Value | SE | t‐value | p‐value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 127.7 | 141.7 | 0.90 | 0.37 |
Cloud | −0.049 | 0.039 | −1.24 | 0.21 |
Distance | −5.40 | 6.57 | −0.82 | 0.41 |
Phase | 0.50 | 2.44 | 0.21 | 0.84 |
Temperature | −0.21 | 0.10 | −1.99 | 0.046 |
Precipitation | 0.18 | 0.04 | 4.20 | <0.0001 |
Date | 0.001 | 0.013 | 0.07 | 0.94 |
Cloud × Distance | 0.12 | 0.09 | 1.38 | 0.17 |
Cloud × Phase | 0.018 | 0.033 | 0.55 | 0.58 |
Distance × Phase | 0.04 | 3.63 | 0.01 | 0.99 |
Cloud × Distance × Phase | −0.06 | 0.05 | −1.31 | 0.19 |
Autocorrelation parameter = 0.57. Variance estimates for the random effects: intercept: 525, Residual: 386. Degrees of freedom for all parameters = 1,234. Significant effects are highlighted in bold.
4. DISCUSSION
Our study is the first to report the physiological responses of wild birds to “supermoon” events. The wild geese responded to the combination of two independent lunar cycles, by significantly increasing their body temperature (T ab) during “supermoon” events. Given the role that the daily, lunar, and seasonal cycles of light have played in driving the development of physiological pathways and individual behavior (Gaston, Bennie, Davies, & Hopkins, 2013), it is surprising birds have not evolved a way of ignoring natural celestial events such as “supermoons” and bright light to save increasing T ab and f H. That wild birds respond to natural fluctuations in nighttime ambient light levels support the documented responses of many species to anthropogenic sources of artificial light, that birds seem unable to override (Gaston et al., 2013). As most biological systems are arguably organized foremost by light (Gaston, Visser, & Holker, 2014), this suggests that any interactions between lunar cycles and local weather conditions (e.g., cloud cover) could have significant impacts on the energy budgets of birds.
It is likely that the combination of the moon being full and close to the Earth caused a “masking” effect, whereby the higher ambient light levels acted as a stimulus to cause an immediate overriding of the typical endogenous circadian clocks of the geese (see Kronfeld‐Schor et al., 2012, for a description of masking effects). During a full moon, for example, mRNA levels of cryptochrome (Cry)—one of the key circadian clock genes—are significantly elevated compared to those during the rest of the lunar cycle in numerous species of coral (Anthozoans) and fish (e.g., Siganus guttatus), providing a potential underlying mechanism for behavioral changes observed in response to the lunar cycle (Fukoshiro et al., 2011). The mechanism behind the changes observed in T ab in the geese is likely to be the interruption of normal melatonin rhythms, which causes extensive disruptive effects linking multiple body systems (Goto, Oshima, Tomita, & Ebihara, 1989). Light is a vital component for the regulation of both daily and seasonal processes in birds, and it has been suggested that light during the nighttime may alter the ability of bird's to detect changes in daylength the following day (Longcore & Rich, 2004; Titulaer, Spoelstra, Lange, & Visser, 2012). Being receptive to changes in daylength—photosensitivity—is the primary stimulus (zeitgeber) for many key annual events such as migratory departure dates and the onset of molt (Gwinner, 2003).
Melatonin is one of the fundamental hormones involved in the regulation of daily physiological cycles, and is released at night and suppressed by daylight (Gwinner & Hau, 2000). The exogenous application of melatonin directly changes typical circadian rhythms, with Japanese quails (Cortunix japonica), for example, altering the standard circadian rhythm in T ab following melatonin supplementation (Nakahara, Kawano, Shiota, & Murukami, 2003). This suggests that the proximate mechanism behind increases in T ab in the geese during a “supermoon” night is likely due to the suppression of the release of nighttime melatonin (Bentley, 2001; Turek, McMillan, & Menaker, 1976; Ubuka, Bentley, Ukena, Wingfield, & Kazuyoshi Tsutsui, 2005). In experimental settings, the intensity of the nighttime illumination directly determines the degree of the suppression of the melatonin release (Dominoni, Goyman, Helm, & Partecke, 2013). Similarly, Tarlow et al. (2003) demonstrated that full moons significantly inhibit the production of melatonin in Nazca boobies (Sula granti), with diel variations in melatonin concentrations vanishing when the moon was full. The increase in brightness of a “supermoon” is likely sufficient to cause substantial changes in the chronobiology of the geese. Future studies into melatonin release and suppression during “supermoon” and perigee events would be fruitful to elucidate the exact mechanisms that cause the rise in T ab in the geese. Furthermore, ascertaining whether the birds wake up during “supermoon” and/or perigee events as a result of the higher ambient light levels would give further insight into the potential carry‐over effects of higher light levels at night.
Due to the field‐based nature of this study, it is not possible to determine whether the responses in physiology of the geese are a result of a direct effect of the changes in light levels on the circadian physiology, or via other alternative pathways. Our paper presents novel phenomenological findings that show wild geese physiologically respond to “supermoon” events. Owing to the age of the data (collected in 1999–2000), there are technological limitations that may potentially limit the generality of our findings. Were this study to be repeated now, birds would be deployed with GPS and accelerometry loggers (Gleiss, Wilson, & Shepard, 2011; Halsey, Portugal, Smith, Murn, & Wilson, 2009; Portugal et al., 2014; Taylor, Portugal, & Biro, 2017; Voelkl et al., 2015) in conjunction with the heart rate data loggers, to fully understand where the birds precisely where, and what levels of activity and movement they were exhibiting during the “supermoon” events.
Sleep is a fundamental physiological process (Roth et al., 2010; Vorster & Born, 2014), the deprivation of which could have both proximate and ultimate consequences for the geese. Electroencephalography (EEG) studies on birds during “supermoon” events would be productive, to ascertain whether they are waking up, and to determine the extent of sleep disruption. Similarly, establishing whether “supermoon” events disrupt the previously observed correlations between mean, minimum, and maximum daily heart rates (Portugal et al., 2016) would ascertain the functional significance of sleep interruption at the energetics level. The disruption to the typical circadian T ab rhythm in geese occurring in response to natural cycles of ambient light levels may offer insight into how animals are responding to artificial light (De Jong, et al., 2016). Prior studies have demonstrated suppression of rest‐phase torpor in urban birds and mammals (Le Tallex et al., 2015) in response to artificial light, which assumingly must come at a metabolic cost (Guppy & Withers, 1999).
The increased potential risk of being predated upon when the ambient light level is higher means it could be beneficial for animals to wake up during a “supermoon.” Risk of predation during higher light levels can have a significant impact on the behavior of many species. Seabirds such as petrels avoid returning to their breeding colonies on moonlit nights to avoid predation from diurnal predators (Yamamoto et al., 2008), while Galapagos fur seals (Arctocephalus galapagoensis) haul out ashore, thought to be in response to the increased risk of predation from visual predators such as sharks (Horning and Trillmich, 1999). Bright nights as a result of a full moon or perigee should favor visually guided predators, compared to predators relying on other senses, such as olfaction or echolocation (Martin, 2010). Some species, such as willets (Catoptrophorus semipalmatus), switch from tactile foraging on moonless nights to visually guided foraging on moonlit nights (McNeil & Rompre, 1995), suggesting there is some plasticity in foraging approaches in response to ambient light levels for species which use multiple sensory‐ecological approaches to finding food. The barnacle geese wintering at Caerlaverock forage on agricultural land and roost on water (Phillips et al., 2003). Thus, despite being visually guided foragers, it is very unlikely the geese are waking up to forage on moonlit nights, as there is little to no food available to them in the immediate vicinity of their roosts. Furthermore, unlike T ab, f H does not show such an extreme increase during perigee events as to nearly match daytime f H activity levels (Portugal, Green, & Butler, 2007; Portugal et al., 2012, 2019; Figure 2). This small degree of increase in f H compared to flight confirms that it is unlikely that the birds are becoming active or moving extensively since even small regular movements are typically accompanied by increases in heart rate (Butler, Green, Boyd, & Speakman, 2004). Despite the geese being generally safe from predators when roosting on or near the water, it is likely that the increased potential risk of being predated upon is one of the drivers for why the geese may be waking up, instigated by the higher ambient light levels masking their typical circadian physiology. Although for this specific population predation at night from ground predators is rare, for other populations and indeed other geese species, it is possible that there may be an elevated risk of being predated upon during supermoon events, particularly by predators relying more on visually guided hunting techniques.
During “supermoon” events, the increases in T ab are not matched by significant increases in f H, and this may be linked to the lack of sustained movement that the geese are undertaking on these nights. Lunar phase has been shown to significantly impact heterothermy in freckled nightjars (Caprimulgus tristigma), and thermoregulatory patterns were tightly coupled with the level of nocturnal lunar illumination, rather than ambient temperature (Smit et al., 2011). However, the nightjars became increasingly heterothermic during new moon periods of the lunar cycle, thought to be linked to a reduction in foraging opportunities due to lower light levels, rather than a direct physiological response to the lower light levels themselves. For the barnacle geese in the present study, it is feasible that this occasional increases T ab and f H have little impact on the energy budgets of healthy adult barnacle geese in good physiological condition. However, for an individual in poor condition, young birds, or during extreme weather events (e.g., very low temperatures), it is conceivable that the energy used to warm up in response to a “supermoon” event could have energetic implications for an individual.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
None declared.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Conceptualization, S.J.P., P.B.F., and P.J.B.; methodology, S.J.P., C.R.W., P.B.F., J.A.G., and P.J.B; data collection, S.J.P. (cloud data) and P.J.B (all field and logger data); preliminary analysis, P.B.F.; formal analysis, C.R.W; writing, reviewing, and editing, S.J.P. wrote the first draft of the manuscript with input from all authors.
ETHICAL APPROVAL
Work in Svalbard was undertaken with full permissions from the Governor of Svalbard and adhered to the Norwegian Animal Welfare Act. Although the United Kingdom Animal (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 does not apply to Svalbard where this study was conducted, we were meticulous in following its provisions, especially those set out by the Home Office in the Official Guidance on the operation of the Act.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We are grateful to Anthony Woakes for assistance with the logging devices, and to Maarten Loonen for help with catching geese. We thank the following people for useful discussions; Michaela Hau, Theunis Piersma, Graham Martin, Magella Guillemette, Andrew Biewener. Funding for this study was provided by a N.E.R.C. grant to P.J.B, while S.J.P was funded by a B.B.S.R.C. studentship. C.R.W. is funded by the A.R.C.
Portugal SJ, White CR, Frappell PB, Green JA, Butler PJ. Impacts of “supermoon” events on the physiology of a wild bird. Ecol Evol. 2019;9:7974–7984. 10.1002/ece3.5311
Data Availability Statement: All data are available as electronic supplementary material.
DATA AVAILABILITY
All data are available as electronic supplementary material.
REFERENCES
- Bentley, G. E. (2001). Unraveling the enigma: The role of melatonin in seasonal processes in birds. Microscopy Research & Technique, 53, 63–71. 10.1002/jemt.1069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bevan, R. M. , Woakes, A. J. , Butler, P. J. , & Croxall, J. P. (1995). Heart rate and oxygen consumption of exercising gentoo penguins. Physiological Zoology, 68, 855–877. 10.1086/physzool.68.5.30163935 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, S. A. , Zumbrunn, G. , Fleury‐Olela, F. , Preitner, N. , & Schibler, U. (2002). Rhythms of mammalian body temperature can sustain peripheral circadian clocks. Current Biology, 12, 1574–1583. 10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01145-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryant, D. M. , & Westerterp, K. R. (1983). Short‐term variability in energy turnover by breeding house martins Delichon urbica: A study using doubly‐labelled water (D2 18O). Journal of Animal Ecology, 52, 525–543. 10.2307/4570 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Butler, P. J. , Green, J. A. , Boyd, I. L. , & Speakman, J. R. P. J. (2004). Measuring metabolic rate in the field: The pros and cons of the doubly labeled water and heart rate methods. Functional Ecology, 18, 168–183. [Google Scholar]
- Butler, P. J. , & Woakes, A. J. (2001). Seasonal hypothermia in a large migrating bird: Saving energy for fat deposition? Journal of Experimental Biology, 204, 1361–1367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler, P. J. , Woakes, A. J. , & Bishop, C. M. (1998). Behaviour and physiology of Svalbard Barnacle Geese Branta leucopsis during their autumn migration. Journal of Avian Biology, 29, 536–545. 10.2307/3677173 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Calder, W. A. , & Booser, J. (1973). Hypothermia or broad‐tailed hummingbirds during incubation in nature with ecological correlations. Science, 180, 751–753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapront‐Touze, M. , & Chapront, J. (1983). The lunar ephemeris. Astronomical Astrophysics, 124, 50–62. [Google Scholar]
- Cheeseman, J. F. , Fewster, R. M. , & Walker, M. M. (2017). Circadian and circatidal clocks control the mechanism of semilunar foraging behavior. Scientific Reports, 7, 3780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheke, R. A. (1971). Temperature rhythms in African monate sunbirds. Ibis, 113, 500–506. [Google Scholar]
- De Jong, M. , Jeninga, L. , Ouyang, J. Q. , van Oers, K. , Spoelstra, K. , & Visser, M. E. (2016). Dose‐dependent responses of avian daily rhythms to artificial light at night. Physiological Behaviour, 155, 172–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dominoni, D. M. , Goyman, W. , Helm, B. , & Partecke, J. (2013). Urban‐like night illumination reduces melatonin release in European blackbirds: Implications of city life for biological time‐keeping of songbirds. Frontiers in Zoology, 10, 60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forsythe, W. C. , Rykiel, E. J. Jr , Stahl, R. S. , Wu, H. , & Schoolfield, R. (1995). A model comparison for daylength as a function of latitude and day of the year. Ecological Modelling, 80, 87–95. [Google Scholar]
- Fukushiro, M. , Takeuchi, T. , Takeuchi, Y. , Hur, S.‐P. , Sugama, N. , Takemura, A. , … Okano, T. (2011). Lunar phase‐dependent expression of cryptochrome and a photoperiodic mechanism for lunar phase‐recognition in a reef fish, Goldlined Spinefoot. PLoS ONE, 6, e28643 10.1371/journal.pone.0028643 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaston, K. J. , Bennie, J. , Davies, T. W. , & Hopkins, J. (2013). The ecological impacts of night‐time light pollution: A mechanistic appraisal. Biological Reviews, 88, 912–927. 10.1111/brv.12036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaston, K. J. , Visser, M. E. , & Holker, F. (2014). The biological impacts of artificial light at night: The research challenge. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B., 370, 20140133 10.1098/rstb.2014.0133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gleiss, A. C. , Wilson, R. P. , & Shepard, E. L. C. (2011). Making overall dynamic body acceleration work: On the theory of acceleration as a proxy for energy expenditure. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 23–33. 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2010.00057.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Goto, M. , Oshima, I. , Tomita, T. , & Ebihara, S. (1989). Melatonin content of the pineal gland in different mouse strains. Journal of Pineal Research, 7, 195–204. 10.1111/j.1600-079X.1989.tb00667.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant, R. A. , Chadwick, E. A. , & Halliday, T. (2009). The lunar cycle: A cue for amphibian reproductive phenology? Animal Behaviour, 78, 349–357. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Guillemette, M. , Woakes, A. J. , Larochelle, J. , Polymeropoulos, E. , Granbois, J.‐M. , Butler, P. J. , … Portugal, S. J. (2016). Does hyperthermia constrain flight duration in a short–distance migrant? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371, 20150386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guppy, M. , & Withers, P. (1999). Metabolic depression in animals: Physiological perspectives and biochemical generalizations. Biological Reviews, 74, 1–40. 10.1017/S0006323198005258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gwinner, E. (1996). Circadian and circannual programmes in avian migration. Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 49–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gwinner, E. (2003). Circannual rhythms in birds. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 13, 770–778. 10.1016/j.conb.2003.10.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gwinner, E. , & Hau, M. (2000). The pineal gland, circadian rhythms, and photoperiodism In Whittew G. C. (Ed.), Sturkie's avian physiology, 5th ed. (pp. 557–568). New York, NY: Academic. [Google Scholar]
- Halsey, L. G. , Portugal, S. J. , Smith, J. A. , Murn, C. A. , & Wilson, R. P. (2009). Recording raptor behaviour on the wing via accelerometry. Journal of Field Ornithology., 80, 171–177. [Google Scholar]
- Horning, M. , & Trillmich, F. (1999). Lunar cycles in diel prey migrations exert a stronger effect on the diving of juveniles than adult Galapagos fur seals. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 266, 1127–1132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kronfeld‐Schor, N. , Dominoni, D. , de laIglesia, H. , Levy, O. , Herzog, E. D. , Dayan, T. , & Helfrich‐Forster, C. (2012). Chronobiology by moonlight. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 280, 20123088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kyba, C. C. M. , Mohar, A. , & Posch, T. (2017). How bright is moonlight? Astronomical Geophysics, 58, 1–2. 10.1093/astrogeo/atx025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lang, A. B. , Kalko, E. K. V. , Romer, H. , Bockholdt, C. , & Dechmann, D. K. N. (2006). Activity levels of bats and katydids in relation to the lunar cycle. Oecologica, 146, 659–666. 10.1007/s00442-005-0131-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lazaridis, E. (2014). lunar: Lunar phase & distance, seasons and other environmental factors (Version 0.1‐04). http://www.statistics.lazaridis.eu [Google Scholar]
- Le Tallex, T. , Thery, M. , & Perret, M. (2015). Effect of light pollution on seasonal estrus and daily rhythms in a nocturnal primate. Journal of Mammalology, 96, 438–445. [Google Scholar]
- Lohman, K. J. , & Willows, A. O. (1987). Lunar‐modulated geomagnetic orientation by a marine mollusc. Science, 235, 331–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Longcore, T. , & Rich, C. (2004). Ecological light pollution. Frontiers in Ecology and Environment, 2, 191–198. 10.1890/1540-9295(2004)002[0191:ELP]2.0.CO;2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Martin, G. R. (2010). Birds by night. London, UK: A&C Black. [Google Scholar]
- McDowell, R. M. (1969). Lunar rhythms in aquatic animals. Tuatara, 17, 133–134. [Google Scholar]
- McNeil, R. , & Rompre, G. (1995). Day and night feeding territoriality in Willets Catoptrophorus semipalmatus and Whimbrel Numenius phaeopus during the non‐breeding season in the tropics. Ibis, 137, 169–176. 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1995.tb03236.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moreno, J. (1989). Variation in daily energy expenditure in nesting northern wheatears (Oenanthe oenanthe). The Auk, 106, 18–25. 10.2307/4087752 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nakahara, K. , Kawano, T. , Shiota, K. , & Murukami, N. (2003). Effects of microinjection of melatonin into various brain regions of Japanese quail on locomotor activity and body temperature. Neuroscience Letters, 345, 117–120. 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00514-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navarro‐Castilla, A. , & Barja, I. (2014). Does predation risk, through moon phase and predator cues, modulate food intake, antipredatory and physiological responses in wood mice. Behavioural Ecology & Sociobiology, 68, 1505–1512. [Google Scholar]
- O'Brien, R. M. (2007). A cation regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors. Quality & Quantity, 41, 673–690. [Google Scholar]
- Penteriani, V. , Kuparinen, A. , del Mar Delgado, M. , Palomares, F. , López‐Bao, J. V. , Fedriani, J. M. , … Lourenço, R. (2013). Responses of a top and a meso predator and their prey to moon phases. Oecologica, 173, 753–766. 10.1007/s00442-013-2651-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips, R. A. , Cope, D. R. , Rees, E. C. , & O'Connell, M. J. (2003). Site fidelity and range size of wintering Barnacle Geese Branta leucopsis. Bird Study, 50, 161–169. [Google Scholar]
- Pinheiro, J. , & Bates, D. (2000). Mixed‐effects models in S and S‐PLUS. New York, NY: Springer Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- Portugal, S. J. , Butler, P. J. , Green, J. A. , & Cassey, P. (2011). Indications of phenotypic plasticity in moulting birds: Captive geese reveal adaptive changes in mineralisation of their long bones during wing moult. Journal of Ornithology., 152, 1055–1061. 10.1007/s10336-011-0699-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Portugal, S. J. , Green, J. A. , & Butler, P. J. (2007). Annual changes in body mass and resting metabolism in captive barnacle geese (Branta leucopsis): The importance of wing moult. Journal of Experimental Biology, 210, 1391–1397. 10.1242/jeb.004598 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portugal, S. J. , Green, J. A. , Cassey, P. , Frappell, P. B. , & Butler, P. J. (2009). Predicting the rate of oxygen consumption from heart rate in barnacle geese: Effects of captivity and annual changes in body condition. Journal of Experimental Biology, 212, 2941–2948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portugal, S. J. , Green, J. A. , Halsey, L. G. , Arnold, W. , Careau, V. , Dann, P. , … Butler, P. J. (2016). Associations between resting, activity and daily metabolic rate in free‐living endotherms: No universal rule for birds and mammals. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology, 89, 251–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portugal, S. J. , Green, J. A. , White, C. R. , Guillemette, M. , & Butler, P. J. (2012). Wild geese do not increase flight behaviour prior to migration. Biology Letters, 8, 469–472. 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0975 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portugal, S. J. , Hubel, T. Y. , Hailes, S. , Heese, S. , Trobe, D. , Voelkl, B. , … Usherwood, J. R. (2014). Position and flap phasing between ibises in formation flight: Evidence for upwash exploitation and downwash avoidance? Nature, 505, 399–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portugal, S. J. , Thorpe, S. K. S. , Green, J. A. , Myatt, J. P. , & Butler, P. J. (2009). Testing the use/disuse hypothesis: Pectoral and leg muscle changes in captive barnacle geese during wing moult. Journal of Experimental Biology., 212, 2403–2410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portugal, S. J. , & White, C. R. (2018). Miniaturisation of biologgers is not alleviating the 5% rule. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 9, 1662–1666. [Google Scholar]
- Portugal, S. J. , White, C. R. , Green, J. A. , & Butler, P. J. (2019). Flight feather moult drives minimum daily heart rate in wild geese. Biology Letters, 14, 20180650 10.1098/rsbl.2018.0650 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team . (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; https://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
- Raible, F. , Takekata, H. , & Essmar‐Raible, K. (2017). An overview of monthly rhythms and clocks. Frontiers in Neurology, 8, 189 10.3389/fneur.2017.00189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth, T. C. , Rattenborg, N. C. , & Pravosudov, V. V. (2010). The ecological relevance of sleep: The trade‐off between sleep, memory and energy conservation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 945–959. 10.1098/rstb.2009.0209 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruf, T. (1999). The Lomb‐Scargle periodogram in biological rhythm research: Analysis of incomplete and unequally spaced time‐series. Biological Rhythm Research, 30, 178–201. 10.1076/brhm.30.2.178.1422 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Smit, B. , Boyles, J. G. , Brigham, R. M. , & McKechnie, A. E. (2011). Torpor in dark times: Patterns of heterothermy are associated with the lunar cycle in a nocturnal bird. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 26, 241–248. 10.1177/0748730411402632 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tarlow, E. M. , Hau, M. , Anderson, D. J. , & Wikelski, M. (2003). Diel changes in plasma melatonin and corticosterone concentrations in tropical Nazca boobies in relation to moon phase and age. General Comparative Endocrinology, 78, 297–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, L. A. , Portugal, S. J. , & Biro, D. (2017). Homing pigeons (Columba livia) modulate wingbeat characteristics as a function of route familiarity. Journal of Experimental Biology., 220, 2908–2915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Titulaer, M. , Spoelstra, K. , Lange, C. , & Visser, M. E. (2012). Activity patterns during food provisioning are affected by artificial light in free living Great Tits (Parus major). PLoS ONE, 7, e37377 10.1371/journal.pone.0037377 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turek, F. W. , McMillan, J. P. , & Menaker, M. (1976). Melatonin: Effects on the circadian locomotor rhythm of sparrows. Science, 194, 1441–1443. 10.1126/science.1006311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ubuka, T. , Bentley, G. E. , Ukena, K. , Wingfield, J. C. , & Kazuyoshi Tsutsui, K. (2005). Melatonin induces the expression of gonadotropin‐inhibitory hormone in the avian brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102, 3052–3057. 10.1073/pnas.0403840102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voelkl, B. , Portugal, S. J. , Unsold, M. , Usherwood, J. R. , Wilson, A. M. , & Fritz, J. (2015). Matching times of leading and following suggest cooperation through direct reciprocity during V‐formation flight of Northern bald ibis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States, 112, 2115–2120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vorster, A. P. , & Born, J. (2014). Sleep and memory in mammals, birds and invertebrates. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 50, 103–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward, S. , Bishop, C. M. , Woakes, A. J. , & Butler, P. J. (2002). Heart rate and the rate of oxygen consumption of flying and walking barnacle geese (Branta leucopsis) and bar‐headed geese (Anser indicus). Journal of Experimental Biology, 205, 3347–3356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White, C. R. , Blackburn, T. M. , Martin, G. R. , & Butler, P. J. (2007). Basal metabolic rate of birds is associated with habitat temperature and precipitation, not primary productivity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274, 287–293. 10.1098/rspb.2006.3727 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White, C. R. , Cassey, P. , Schimpf, N. , Green, J. A. , Halsey, L. G. , & Portugal, S. J. (2013). Implantation reduces the negative effects of bio‐logging devices on birds. Journal of Experimental Biology, 216, 537–542. 10.1242/jeb.076554 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White, C. R. , Grémillet, D. , Green, J. A. , Martin, G. R. , & Butler, P. J. (2011). Metabolic rate throughout the annual cycle reveals the demands of an Arctic existence in Great Cormorants. Ecology, 92, 475–486. 10.1890/09-1951.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto, T. , Takahashni, A. , Yoda, K. , Katsumata, N. , Watanabe, S. , Sato, K. , & Trathan, P. N. (2008). The lunar cycle affects at‐sea behaviour in a pelagic seabird, the streaked shearwater, Calonectris leucomelas . Animal Behaviour, 76, 1647–1652. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data are available as electronic supplementary material.