Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019 Jan 29;28(9):1193–1202. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-01267-4

Impulsivity and affect reactivity prospectively predict disordered eating attitudes in adolescents: A 6-year longitudinal study

Brittney C Evans 1, Julia W Felton 2, Madeline A Lagacey 3, Stephanie M Manasse 1, Carl W Lejuez 4, Adrienne S Juarascio 1
PMCID: PMC6663641  NIHMSID: NIHMS1519808  PMID: 30693374

Abstract

Introduction.

Eating disorders (EDs) are associated with significant psychological and physical comorbidities, and adolescence is a particularly high-risk time for the development of EDs. Impulsivity (i.e., acting with little conscious judgment or forethought) and affect reactivity (i.e., changes in negative affect in response to a stressor) are hypothesized to contribute to the development of binge/purge ED pathology. The current study is the first to examine the prospective relationships between impulsivity and affect reactivity as predictors of the development of ED-attitudes in adolescents over time.

Methods.

206 adolescents participated in a longitudinal study examining the development of psychopathology. ED-attitudes were assessed via the College Eating Disorders Screen (COEDS) annually for 6 years. Baseline impulsivity and affect reactivity were also assessed. Affect reactivity, impulsivity, and their interaction were examined as baseline predictors of changes in ED-attitudes over time using latent growth modeling.

Results.

Results of latent growth modeling indicated that ED-attitudes increased over time. The interaction between impulsivity and affect reactivity significantly predicted the slope of ED-attitudes, such that the relationship between impulsivity and ED-attitudes was strongest for those with elevated levels of affect reactivity.

Discussion.

Findings suggest that greater levels of affect reactivity and impulsivity are key risk factors for the development of ED-attitudes in adolescents. Subsequent research should examine the relation between affect reactivity and impulsivity in predicting objectively measured ED-behaviors, in addition to ED-attitudes. Further investigation may implicate affect reactivity and impulsivity as important targets for early intervention to prevent onset of ED symptoms in adolescents.

Keywords: Eating disorders, Adolescents, Impulsivity, Affect reactivity, Longitudinal

Introduction

Eating disorders (EDs) are serious disorders associated with significant psychological and physical comorbidities [1] and increased risk for mortality [2]. Adolescence is a particularly high-risk time for the emergence of ED pathology, and anorexia nervosa (AN) and bulimia nervosa (BN) have the highest incidence and point prevalence during adolescence [3]. Furthermore, risk of ED pathology in early adolescence is associated with higher rates of AN and BN by the end of adolescence [4,5], as well as higher rates of mental and physical health concerns extending past adolescence and into adulthood [6]. As such, it is important to identify risk factors of ED onset and to identify which adolescents are at particularly high risk of developing EDs, so that early prevention and treatment can occur. In particular, impulsivity (i.e., acting with little conscious forethought; [7]) and affect reactivity (i.e., change in negative affect in response to a stressor; [8,9]) are two risk factors implicated in the development of binge/purge ED pathology (e.g., bulimia nervosa, binge eating disorder, anorexia nervosa binge-eating/purging type) that merit attention, as both traits can be indicators of ED-risk, and both are valuable potential treatment targets in psychological intervention for EDs. However, greater understanding of how impulsivity and affect reactivity influence the development of binge/purge ED pathology over time, both individually and in combination, is needed.

The Role of Impulsivity in the Development of Eating Disorders

Impulsivity is a risk factor implicated in the development of binge/purge ED symptoms. Impulsivity is a broadly defined, multi-dimensional concept, but generally refers to performing behaviors with little conscious forethought, or acting on the spur of the moment [7]. High levels of impulsivity are thought to increase risk of binge/purge ED pathology, as highly impulsive individuals are more likely to engage in rash behavior (e.g., bingeing, purging; [10]) in response to negative cognitions or affective states, as a means of temporarily distracting from or coping with negative emotions [11]. Indeed, in adults, impulsivity is associated with the presence of ED pathology both cross-sectionally [12-23] and longitudinally [24,25]. Cross-sectional data from mixed samples of adult and adolescent patients with binge eating pathology and bulimia nervosa have also shown higher levels of impulsivity in these individuals compared to peers without EDs [26,27]. Impulsivity in adults with ED pathology is also associated with poorer treatment outcomes [28-32] and predicts treatment dropout [33]. Taken together, impulsivity is a well-documented risk factor for the development of ED pathology and treatment outcome in adults.

In adolescent-only samples, the association between high levels of impulsivity and binge/purge ED pathology has also been documented by several cross-sectional studies [34-38]. In longitudinal studies examining the role of impulsivity and the development of binge/purge ED pathology in adolescents, however, the results are mixed. In six previous trials, self-reported levels of impulsivity failed to significantly predict the subsequent development of ED pathology [10,39-43]. Only three trials have found a significant relationship between levels of impulsivity and the subsequent development of ED pathology. In these studies, higher levels of impulsivity in elementary school-aged children were associated with greater levels of bulimic symptoms in mid-adolescence relative to normative comparison groups [44-46]. Thus, despite research indicating that impulsivity is significantly associated with development of EDs, the studies documenting the relationship of impulsivity and ED pathology over time are few in number and present mixed results.

In summary, while evidence indicates that impulsivity is a key risk factor in the development of EDs, further research is needed to better understand the role that impulsivity plays in the development of ED pathology in adolescents over time. This is particularly important given that the few longitudinal trials described above were conducted in specific samples of youths, warranting further investigation in larger, diverse samples. Furthermore, adolescents have high levels of impulsivity in general [47], making it particularly important to assess other psychological traits that may interact with impulsivity in order to better understand why some adolescents develop EDs and others do not. One such moderating trait may be change in negative affect in response to a stressor (i.e., affect reactivity).

The Role of Affect Reactivity in the Development of Eating Disorders

Emerging research has implicated affect reactivity in the development and maintenance of ED pathology [48-50]. Individuals with high levels of affect reactivity are more prone to experience negative affective states more strongly and frequently [51], and may thus be more susceptible to engaging in maladaptive behavior, such as disordered eating, as a means of coping with high levels of negative affect [11]. Indeed, Goldschmidt and colleagues found that binge/purge episodes were immediately preceded by negative affect in response to a stressor [48]. Relatedly, greater levels of negative affect are concurrently associated with greater urges to engage in emotional eating [52]. As such, affect reactivity may be an important predictor and maintenance factor for ED pathology. This relationship may be particularly strong in adolescents, who generally experience greater levels of emotional reactivity, which is empirically linked with greater levels of impulsivity [47]. However, further research is needed into the role of affect reactivity alone, as well as how it may interact with other psychological traits commonly associated with ED pathology, such as impulsivity.

The Interaction of Affect Reactivity and Impulsivity in the Development of Eating Disorders

Impulsivity and affect reactivity are both important individual risk factors for the development of ED pathology. Adolescents who are highly impulsive (and thus prone to engage in impulsive behaviors to cope with negative affective states; [53]) and high in affect reactivity (and thus frequently experiencing strong, negative affective states; [51]) are more likely to engage in rash behaviors, such as disordered eating, in order to reduce or distract from negative affective states [54]. Thus, affect reactivity may moderate the relationship between impulsivity and ED pathology, such that the relationship between impulsivity and ED pathology may be strongest for youth with higher levels of affect reactivity, as a means of coping with aversive negative affect.

However, no study to date has examined the interaction between affect reactivity and impulsivity in relation to the development of ED pathology. Furthermore, given that the risk of ED development is greatest in adolescence, examining the relationship between key risk factors is particularly important in an adolescent sample. Most importantly, a more nuanced understanding of combinations of psychological traits that confer elevated risk for the development of EDs, such as impulsivity and affect reactivity, will advance our understanding of risk and maintenance factors for EDs, with potential implications for more targeted treatment and prevention efforts for at-risk youths.

Current Study

The current study examined affect reactivity and impulsivity as prospective predictors of ED pathology in adolescents in order to better understand how impulsivity in adolescents relates to the emergence of EDs. This study built on recent findings of our team using the same data set, which demonstrated that affect reactivity is a prospective risk factor in both the onset and maintenance of ED-attitudes in adolescents [55]. The present study elaborates on this finding by specifically examining the interaction between affect reactivity and impulsivity traits. This novel interaction effect has not been previously investigated, in our data set or any others, and has potential to enhance our understanding of how individual differences in levels of impulsivity and affect reactivity may influence risk of developing ED pathology. Better understanding the role of individual traits in ED risk may ultimately allow for development of highly personalized and tailored intervention and treatment approaches.

In the current study, we specifically assessed the development of ED-attitudes (i.e., individually- held attitudes regarding eating behavior and weight). ED-attitudes in early adolescence predict the continuation of ED symptoms in later adolescence and early adulthood [4,5]. Such findings are consistent with the cognitive-behavioral model of EDs [56], which describes overvaluation of and concern about shape and weight (i.e., ED-attitudes) as a core maintenance factor of ED symptoms. However, few longitudinal studies have examined the development of ED-attitudes in adolescents across time. The current study was conducted using a large, longitudinal data set that included an assessment of ED-attitudes over multiple assessment points, in order to capture pre-clinical ED symptoms and assess vulnerability to the subsequent development of EDs.

Specifically, we hypothesized that greater levels of impulsivity would predict the development of higher levels of ED-attitudes in adolescents over time. Additionally, we hypothesized that affect reactivity would moderate the relationship between impulsivity and ED-attitude development, such that higher levels of affect reactivity and impulsivity would predict the development of ED-attitudes over time, compared to adolescents with lower levels of one or both of those traits.

Methods

Participants and Procedures

The present study included adolescents taking part in a longitudinal study examining the development of psychopathology. Adolescents and their families were recruited from a metropolitan area community. Inclusion criteria included proficiency in English and the ability to commit to taking part in yearly assessments. The original sample was comprised of 277 (46% female) adolescents and their parents; however, because key measures related to the present study were not introduced until the third year of participant enrollment, the current study utilized data only from those youths who completed all measures at this assessment point (Wave 3 for the parent study, relabeled T1 for clarity in the remainder of the manuscript). As such, the current sample included 206 adolescents with ages ranging from 11 to 15 (Mage = 13.03, SDage = 0.89) at T1. Retention of participants over the course of the study was good; of the original sample of 206 youth, 193 (Mage = 14.00, SDage = 0.89) participated in Wave 4 (T2), 175 (Mage = 15.02, SDage = 0.95) participated in Wave 5 (T3), 152 (Mage = 16.06, SDage = 0.89) participated in Wave 6 (T4), 130 (Mage = 17.00, SDage = 0.95) participated in Wave 7 (T5), and 123 (Mage = 18.05, SDage = 0.97) participated in Wave 8 (T6). Forty-nine percent of the sample identified as White/Caucasian, 37% as Black/African-American, 4% as Latino/a, and 10% as “Other.” The Institutional Review Board of [blinded for review] approved all protocols used in this study.

Measures

Eating Disorder Attitudes.

The College Eating Disorders Screen (COEDS) [57] is a seven-item self-report measure of adolescent’s attitudes regarding eating behavior and weight. Participants were asked to rate the extent to which they had certain eating disorder thoughts or feelings, e.g., “I believe I am fatter than most people say I am.” The COEDS is considered a measure of vulnerability to the development of EDs and was developed as a clinical screening tool to capture preclinical symptoms of EDs [57,58]. The COEDS is particularly useful for identifying individuals who carry beliefs that put them at heightened risk for development of an ED in the future [57,59]. Indeed, COEDS scores predicted restricting and compensatory behavior one year later in an adolescent sample [58]. The COEDS has good convergent validity with other validated measures of ED pathology [59]. The COEDS is valid and reliable for use in an adolescent population [58]. In the current study, Cronbach's alphas ranged from 0.85 to 0.91 across all years.

Affect reactivity.

Affect reactivity was assessed using an adapted version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule for Children (PANAS-C) [60]. The PANAS-C measure asked participants to rate current levels of negative affect including “mad,” “frustrated,” “upset,” “embarrassed,” and “nervous,” on a 10-point rating scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely). Participants completed the questionnaire to capture levels of negative affect immediately prior to and following the completion of the Behavioral Indicator of Resiliency to Distress (BIRD) task [61], which is a laboratory-based behavioral measure of distress intolerance. During this computerized task, individuals click one of ten boxes designated by a dot. Clicking on the correct box results in the participant being awarded one point. Clicking on the incorrect box produces a loud and unpleasant noise, and no point is earned. The BIRD task is divided into three levels that increase in difficulty. At the beginning of the task, participants are informed that they may quit the last (hardest) level at any point, but that they will lose the opportunity to earn more points once they quit. The time the individual persists in the final level of the task is then used as a measure of behavioral distress intolerance, with lower values denoting higher intolerance. The BIRD has demonstrated validity as an indicator of risk taking, externalizing symptoms, and internalizing symptoms among both clinical and community samples [62].

The total score on the PANAS-C measure at both time points was then computed by summing the responses on each item. The measure has been indicated to be both reliable and valid in adolescent populations [60]. In the current study, Cronbach's alpha was 0.76 for the pre-BIRD administration and 0.80 for the post-BIRD evaluation. Affect reactivity was computed by subtracting the post-task PANAS-C score from the pre-task score, with lower scores reflecting greater negative affect reactivity. The resulting change in affect reactivity scores ranged from −40 to 30.

Eysenck Impulsivity Inventory.

We used the Eysenck Impulsivity Subscale, Version 7 (EI-7 subscale) [63] to measure impulsivity, which is a self-report inventory consisting of 19 items. The subscale assesses the tendency to act without being aware of risk or considering consequences. Questions are answered in a yes/no format Sample items include: “I often buy things on impulse” and “I often do things on the spur of the moment.” The total score was computed by adding the score on each individual item, with higher scores indicating higher levels of impulsivity. The coefficient alpha for the subscale in our study was .77.

Data Analytic Plan

In order to examine trajectories of ED-attitudes over time, as well as predictors of these trajectories, we utilized a latent growth curve (LGC) approach, which is a special case of structural equation modeling. LGC allows for examining latent baseline (or intercept) and growth (or slope) terms that describe trajectories of change or stability over time in the population. By adding predictors of the intercept and growth terms to our model, it is also possible to examine how specific covariates may influence these parameters. Because the trend in change over time is not known a priori, we utilized a model building approach that allowed us to determine the shape of the change in ED-attitudes over time. First, we estimated an unconditional model, which does not include any exogenous predictors. We examined a series of trajectory shapes, starting with an intercept-only model (which reflects no change in ED-attitudes over time) and then adding additional growth factors (including linear, quadratic, and cubic change in ED-attitudes) until the most parsimonious and best-fitting model was determined. Improvements in model fit were indexed using a change χ2 statistic and accompanying degrees of freedom. Once the appropriate growth term was determined, we then examined a model in which the residual (or error) variances were constrained to be the same across measures. If this constraint did not result in a significant decrement of fit, it was retained for all subsequent models. The means and variances of the intercept and growth terms where then examined. Statistically significant estimates of latent means suggest that the baseline value is significantly different than zero (in the case of the intercept) and significantly grows or changes over time (in the case of the growth factor). Statistically significant estimates of the variances suggest individual differences around these estimates. Significant variance estimates would support the inclusion of predictors of these factors in the model.

Next, we estimated a conditional model, in which included exogenous predictors of the latent intercept and growth term. First, we examined the main effects of our focal predictors, impulsivity and affect reactivity, as well as our covariates, sex and baseline negative affect. Finally, we included an interaction term, between impulsivity negative emotional reactivity and added it to our model.

Four fit indices were used to determine how well each of these models fit the data: the χ2 statistic, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) [64], the Tucker Lewis Index (TLI) [65], and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) [66]. Nonsignificant χ2 values indicate good fit; however, this index is sensitive to sample size. CFI and TLI values greater than .90 and RMSEA values less than .08 suggest acceptable fit [67]. Mplus 6.0 [68] was used for all LGC model. Mplus uses full information maximum likelihood (ML) estimation to handle missing data that is missing completely at random (MCAR) or missing at random (MAR). ML uses less biased parameter estimates than other missing data procedures, such as listwise or pairwise deletion. Moreover, ML allows for the full sample of 238 youth to be included in the analyses.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

First, we examined our dependent variables (ED-attitudes) at each time point to evaluate univariate normality. Time 1 COEDS was found to be kurtotic; thus, we transformed the variable at each time point by taking its natural log. The transformed variables were all within acceptable bounds for skew and kurtosis (+/− 3.0) and were retained throughout the following analyses. In order to better understand patterns of missing data, we conducted a Little’s MCAR test [69]. Results support the assumption that data were missing completely at random: χ2 (182) = 141.02, p = .989. We then examined the descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations between our key study variables (see Table 1). Of note, we found that sex was correlated with negative affect reactivity and our measure of ED-attitudes at every wave, indicating that girls report greater affect reactivity and higher levels of ED-attitudes. Baseline impulsivity was correlated with ED attitudes at the first and last time points only. Both negative affect reactivity and baseline negative affect were correlated with ED-attitudes at time 1, but only reactivity was associated at time 3, 4, and 5 as well.

Table 1.

Mean, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations of Key Study Variables

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Sex (male) 1.00
2. Time 1 EIS .05 1.00
3. Time 1 Negative Affect Reactivity .19** −.10 1.00
4. Time 1 Negative Affect −.06 .11 .32** 1.00
5. Time 1 COEDS −.32** .19** −.16* .17** 1.00
6. Time 2 COEDS −.36** .12 −.03 .08 .70** 1.00
7. Time 3 COEDS −.31** .12 −.14 .06 .55** .65** 1.00
8. Time 4 COEDS −.32** .07 −.24** .13 .55** .62** .71** 1.00
9. Time 5 COEDS −.36** .16 −.32** .10 .56** .64** .73** .71** 1.00
10. Time 6 COEDS −.28** .17* −.24** .09 .48** .49** .59** .67** .69** 1.00
Mean 0.56 8.27 −3.25 4.47 3.22 3.32 3.34 3.42 3.43 3.47
(SD) (0.50) (4.13) (7.93) (6.33) (0.64) (0.75) (0.69) (0.75) (0.83) (0.78)

Note. EIS = Eysenck Impulsivity Scale; COEDS = College Eating Disorders Screen.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01.

Previously, Juarascio and colleagues [55] investigated the trajectory of ED-attitudes over time in the current sample of adolescents, as well as the role of internalizing symptoms, affect reactivity, and distress intolerance as prospective predictors of increases in eating disorder (ED)-attitudes during adolescence. To do this, the trajectory of ED-attitudes over time was first modeled by examining a series of growth models to determine the best fitting and most parsimonious model. A linear growth model with homoscedastic residuals was the best fitting model (see [55] for details). The results of this model indicated that ED-attitudes at baseline were greater than zero and that they increased significantly over time in a linear fashion. Moreover, both the variances of the mean and slope were significant, suggesting that there are important individual differences around the level of initial ED-attitudes and their rate of increase; supporting the inclusion of predictors of these parameters. The slope and intercept were not correlated with each other (r = .01, p = .388), indicating that the level of baseline ED-attitudes was not predictive of change in attitudes over time. Of the baseline variables examined (internalizing symptoms, affect reactivity, and distress intolerance), only negative affect reactivity was a significant predictor of the latent slope (std. est. = −.32, p = .003), suggesting that youth with greater levels of negative affect reactivity evidenced steeper increases in ED-attitudes over time.

Conditional Growth Models

In order to examine the role of impulsivity and affect reactivity, we added our main effects (baseline impulsivity and affect reactivity) and covariates (sex and baseline negative affect) to the previously established linear growth model as predictors of the latent intercept and slope. This model continued to fit the data well: χ2(df=37) = 58.86, p = .013, CFI = 0.97, TLI = .97, RMSEA = 0.05 (90%CI = 0.02 – 0.07). Results suggest that sex (std. est. =−.41, p < .001), baseline negative affect (std. est. = .15, p = .042), and baseline impulsivity (std. est. = .20, p = .004) were all significant predictors of the intercept. Specifically, girls and adolescents with higher baseline levels of negative affect and impulsivity evidenced higher levels of ED-attitudes at baseline.

Finally, we added an impulsivity*negative affect reactivity interaction term as a predictor, retaining all main effect and covariates in the model (see Figure 1). This model also fit the data well: χ2(df=41) = 69.64, p = .004, CFI = 0.96, TLI = .96, RMSEA = 0.05 (90%CI = 0.03 – 0.08). The interaction term was a significant predictor of the slope only (std est. = −.60, p = .033). In order to better understand the nature of this interaction, we ran a post-hoc multigroup model examining the relation between impulsivity and ED attitudes (controlling for sex and baseline negative affect), in a sub-group of participants who scored +/−1 SD from the mean on negative affect reactivity. Results suggest that impulsivity was not associated with the slope of ED-attitudes for the low negative affect reactivity group (std. est. = .07, p = .901) but was significantly associated for the high negative affect reactivity group (std. est. = .63, p = .010). These findings suggest that for youth with greater negative affect reactivity, higher levels of impulsivity were more strongly related to increases in ED-attitudes over time, relative to less-affectively reactive youth (see Figure 2).

Figure 1. Final latent growth curve model with standardized (and unstandardized) estimates.

Figure 1.

Note: *p < .05, **p < .01.

Figure 2. Graph of the linear trajectories of ED-attitudes for high and low negative affect reactivity and impulsivity groups.

Figure 2.

Note. Groups were created by dividing the sample at the mean of both the Eysenck Impulsivity Scale and the Negative Affect Reactivity measure. Lines represent trajectories rather than point estimates to better capture growth over time and parallel reported analyses. *p < 05, **p < 01.

Discussion

Consistent with our hypotheses, the results of the current study indicated that greater levels of impulsivity predicted the development of higher levels of ED-attitudes in adolescents over time. Furthermore, affect reactivity interacted with impulsivity to increase risk of ED-attitude development, such that for youth with greater affect reactivity, increases in impulsivity were more strongly related to increases in ED-attitudes over time, relative to less-affectively reactive youth. Juarascio and colleagues previously found in the same sample that affect reactivity alone was significantly associated with both baseline levels of ED-attitudes and increases in ED-attitudes over time [55]. The novel results of the current study build upon the previous findings by indicating that higher levels of impulsivity at baseline were also associated with greater levels of ED-attitudes, and that a stronger relationship exists between high levels of impulsivity and ED-attitudes in youth who are also high in affect reactivity. Thus, high levels of both traits (i.e., impulsivity and affect reactivity) in combination confer the greatest risk of ED-attitudes development in adolescents over time.

While impulsivity has previously been investigated in association with ED symptoms, no study to date has examined the moderating effect of affect reactivity on impulsivity in relation to ED-attitudes. The moderating effect of affect reactivity on impulsivity in relation to ED-attitudes may help to explain the discrepant findings in the existing literature regarding the longitudinal impact of impulsivity on ED-pathology over time [10,39-46], and suggests that impulsivity may be most strongly linked to ED-attitude development when levels of emotional reactivity are also high. Thus, the present study enhances our understanding of combinations of psychological traits that confer higher risk of the emergence of ED pathology.

Furthermore, our findings add to our knowledge of potential risk factors for developing ED-attitudes in particular, which measure vulnerability to subsequent development of ED-behavior [57-59]. The current findings indicate that youth with high trait levels of impulsivity and affect reactivity have greater levels of ED-attitudes, which suggests that this subset of youth may be at heightened risk for development of an ED in the future [57,59]. While preliminary, these findings suggest that early intervention or prevention efforts in youth with high trait levels of impulsivity and affect reactivity may be important to prevent heightened vulnerability to ED development. However, future research with comprehensive measures of clinical ED pathology is needed to expand upon these findings and examine these risk factors in relation to the development of objective ED symptomology.

The current findings that high levels of affect reactivity and impulsivity in combination confer higher risk for developing ED-attitudes are consistent with data that youth with high levels of impulsivity have a greater propensity to engage in rash behavior to cope with negative or unpleasant thoughts, emotions, or situations [70]. Impulsively engaging in ED-behaviors (e.g., vomiting to cope with feeling fat) or ED-attitudes (e.g. mentally comparing one’s body to others who are larger to cope with feeling fat) reinforces reliance on ED pathology as a strategy for coping with negative emotion [48]. Highly impulsive youth who are also highly affectively reactive will experience negative affect more strongly and frequently [71]. Thus, more frequent and intense experiences of negative affect may increase the likelihood of engaging in impulsive behaviors to cope with negative affect [72], therefore strengthening the relationship between rash behaviors and engagement in ED-attitudes. As ED-attitudes are a measure of vulnerability of developing ED-behaviors [58], an increase in ED-attitudes in highly impulsive and affectively reactive youth may subsequently manifest in objective ED-behaviors. While the current study provides initial support for the interaction between impulsivity and affect reactivity in relation to development of ED-attitudes, further investigation is needed to understand the relationship between these traits and the development of ED-behaviors. As such, the present findings provide preliminary evidence to support further research into impulsivity and affective reactivity as ED risk factors, which may result in clear clinical implications for early identification and intervention for youth at potential risk of ED development.

Strengths and Limitations

A key strength of the current study is the use of a large, longitudinal data set comprised of a diverse sample of adolescents from the community including both males and females, which provides a representative sample from which conclusions are more broadly generalizable. Furthermore, this study adds to our knowledge of the development of ED-attitudes in adolescents, an area in which there is a paucity of literature. Existing longitudinal investigations of impulsivity and ED development have other limitations that this study was able to address; for instance, the trials that did find a significant effect of impulsivity on ED pathology were conducted within specific samples of youths (e.g., Black females [44], children with ADHD [46]), which limits the conclusions that can be drawn from the findings. The present study was conducted in a large, diverse, community sample, which enhances generalizability. Furthermore, existing longitudinal research on impulsivity and EDs only assessed disordered eating at 2-3 time points. In contrast, the present longitudinal data set assessed ED-attitudes at 6 time points over the span of adolescence, which provides a more nuanced and thorough look at the changes in ED-attitudes over multiple time points.

One notable limitation of the current study was that we were unable to include a measure of clinical ED-behaviors, which would have allowed us to more comprehensively assess the development of ED symptomatology over time. To build upon this study’s preliminary findings and more fully understand the role of impulsivity and affect reactivity in the development of ED symptomatology, future longitudinal research is needed that includes a comprehensive measure of clinical ED pathology. A second limitation is that the measure used in the current study does not clearly distinguish between binge/purge- and restriction-type ED-attitudes. Therefore, we are unable to answer whether the found associations hold for all eating disorder diagnoses. Given existing data on impulsivity and affect reactivity (e.g., [34-36,38,55]), the current findings are likely more applicable to binge/purge-type ED-attitudes, rather than restrictive-type EDs, such as AN. However, future research with improved diagnostic specificity is warranted. Finally, self-report measures were relied upon to assess impulsivity and affect reactivity. Self-report measures are subjective ratings, and subject to inaccuracy and bias, as individuals may be unwilling or unable to report on certain internal experiences [73,74]. In future research, behavioral measures of impulsivity and affect reactivity should be considered to allow for more objective assessment of these traits.

Conclusions and Future Directions

This study is the first to our knowledge to longitudinally assess the interaction of impulsivity and affect reactivity in predicting the development of ED-attitudes in adolescents. Our results suggest that adolescents with high levels of both impulsivity and affect reactivity are at especially high risk of developing ED-attitudes, which are associated with the development of ED-behaviors in previous research. Further research is needed to assess whether the interaction of impulsivity and affect reactivity also predicts the development of ED-behaviors in adolescents, in order to better understand the link between these factors and whether the increase in ED-attitudes manifests in the development of behavioral ED symptoms. Further investigation may implicate affect reactivity and impulsivity as important targets for early intervention to prevent later onset of ED symptoms in adolescents. Interventions targeted towards improving inhibitory control (e.g., inhibitory control training paradigms such as go/no-go tasks or stop-signal tasks) and tolerating high levels of negative affect (e.g., emotion regulation skills taught in dialectical behavior therapy) could be beneficial in directly intervening on the traits that put adolescents at greater risk of developing EDs. However, additional research is first needed to replicate and build upon the current findings in order to pave the way for more targeted prevention and intervention programs.

Acknowledgments

The current study was funded by a grant from the National Institute of Drug Abuse (R01DA18647) to Dr. Lejuez, and funding from the National Institutes of Mental Health to Dr. Juarascio (K23MH105680) and Dr. Manasse (F31MH108279).

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest Statement

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Ethical Standards Statement

This human study was approved by the appropriate ethics committee and has therefore been performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

References

  • 1.Swanson SA, Crow SJ, Grange DL, Swendsen J, Merikangas KR. Prevalence and Correlates of Eating Disorders in Adolescents: Results From the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011. July 4;68(7):714–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Keshaviah A, Edkins K, Hastings ER, Krishna M, Franko DL, Herzog DB, et al. Re-examining premature mortality in anorexia nervosa: a meta-analysis redux. Compr Psychiatry. 2014;55(8):1773–1784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lewinsohn PM, Striegel-Moore RH, Seeley JR. Epidemiology and natural course of eating disorders in young women from adolescence to young adulthood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(10):1284–1292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Killen JD, Taylor CB, Hayward C, Wilson DM, Haydel KF, Hammer LD, et al. Pursuit of thinness and onset of eating disorder symptoms in a community sample of adolescent girls: A three-year prospective analysis. Int J Eat Disord. 1994;16(3):227–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kotler LA, Cohen P, Davies M, Pine DS, Walsh BT. Longitudinal relationships between childhood, adolescent, and adult eating disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(12):1434–1440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Johnson JG, Cohen P, Kasen S, Brook JS. Eating disorders during adolescence and the risk for physical and mental disorders during early adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(6):545–552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1783–1793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sin NL, Graham-Engeland JE, Ong AD, Almeida DM. Affective reactivity to daily stressors is associated with elevated inflammation. Health Psychol. 2015;34(12):1154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.van Eck M, Nicolson NA, Berkhof J. Effects of stressful daily events on mood states: Relationship to global perceived stress. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;75(6):1572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wonderlich SA, Connolly KM, Stice E. Impulsivity as a risk factor for eating disorder behavior: Assessment implications with adolescents. Int J Eat Disord. 2004. September 1;36(2):172–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Haedt-Matt AA, Keel PK. Revisiting the affect regulation model of binge eating: a meta-analysis of studies using ecological momentary assessment. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(4):660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bardone-Cone AM, Butler RM, Balk MR, Koller KA. Dimensions of impulsivity in relation to eating disorder recovery. Int J Eat Disord. 2016. November;49(11):1027–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Davis C, Patte K, Curtis C, Reid C. Immediate pleasures and future consequences. A neuropsychological study of binge eating and obesity. Appetite. 2010;54(1):208–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Engel SG, Corneliussen SJ, Wonderlich SA, Crosby RD, le Grange D, Crow S, et al. Impulsivity and compulsivity in bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2005. November 1;38(3):244–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hege MA, Stingl KT, Kullmann S, Schag K, Giel KE, Zipfel S, et al. Attentional impulsivity in binge eating disorder modulates response inhibition performance and frontal brain networks. Int J Obes. 2015;39(2):353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kukk K, Akkermann K. Fluctuations in negative emotions predict binge eating both in women and men: An experience sampling study. Eat Disord. 2017. January 1;25(1):65–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lavender JM, Goodman EL, Culbert KM, Wonderlich SA, Crosby RD, Engel SG, et al. Facets of impulsivity and compulsivity in women with anorexia nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2017;25(4):309–313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Manasse SM, Espel HM, Forman EM, Ruocco AC, Juarascio AS, Butryn ML, et al. The independent and interacting effects of hedonic hunger and executive function on binge eating. Appetite. 2015;89:16–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Schag K, Teufel M, Junne F, Preissl H, Hautzinger M, Zipfel S, et al. Impulsivity in binge eating disorder: food cues elicit increased reward responses and disinhibition. PloS One. 2013;8(10):e76542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Svaldi J, Naumann E, Trentowska M, Schmitz F. General and food-specific inhibitory deficits in binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord. 2014;47(5):534–542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Sysko R, Ojserkis R, Schebendach J, Evans SM, Hildebrandt T, Walsh BT. Impulsivity and test meal intake among women with bulimia nervosa. Appetite. 2017;112:1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.von Ranson KM, Wallace LM, Holub A, Hodgins DC. Eating disorders, substance use disorders, and impulsiveness among disordered gamblers in a community sample. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2013. March;21(2):148–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Waxman SE. A systematic review of impulsivity in eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2009. November 1;17(6):408–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Culbert KM, Lavender JM, Crosby RD, Wonderlich SA, Engel SG, Peterson CB, et al. Associations between Negative Affect and Binge/Purge Behaviors in Women with Anorexia Nervosa: Considering the Role of Negative Urgency. Compr Psychiatry. 2016. April;66:104–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fischer S, Peterson CM, McCarthy D. A prospective test of the influence of negative urgency and expectancies on binge eating and purging. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(1):294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Claes L, Vandereycken W, Vertommen H. Impulsivity-related traits in eating disorder patients. Personal Individ Differ. 2005;39(4):739–749. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Racine SE, Burt SA, Keel PK, Sisk CL, Neale MC, Boker S, et al. Examining Associations between Negative Urgency and Key Components of Objective Binge Episodes. Int J Eat Disord. 2015. July;48(5):527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Fichter MM, Quadflieg N, Rief W. Course of multi-impulsive bulimia. Psychol Med. 1994;24(3):591–604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Johnson-Sabine E, Reiss D, Dayson D. Bulimia nervosa: a 5-year follow-up study. Psychol Med. 1992;22(4):951–959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Keel PK, Mitchell JE. Outcome in bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(3):313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Manasse SM, Espel HM, Schumacher LM, Kerrigan SG, Zhang F, Forman EM, et al. Does impulsivity predict outcome in treatment for binge eating disorder? A multimodal investigation. Appetite. 2016. October 1;105:172–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Westen D, Muderrisoglu S. Clinical assessment of pathological personality traits. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(7):1285–1287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Peake KJ, Limbert C, Whitehead L. Gone, but not forgotten: An examination of the factors associated with dropping out from treatment of eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2005;13(5):330–337. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Brooks SJ, Dahlberg LS, Swenne I, Aronsson M, Zarei S, Lundberg L, et al. Obsessive-compulsivity and working memory are associated with differential prefrontal cortex and insula activation in adolescents with a recent diagnosis of an eating disorder. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2014. December 30;224(3):246–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cuzzocrea F, Costa S, Larcan R, Toffle ME. Differences between adolescents exhibiting moderate binging and non-binging eating behaviors. SpringerPlus. 2015. October 13;4:593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lee-Winn AE, Townsend L, Reinblatt SP, Mendelson T. Associations of Neuroticism and Impulsivity with Binge Eating in a Nationally Representative Sample of Adolescents in the United States. Personal Individ Differ. 2016;90:66–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lock J, Garrett A, Beenhakker J, Reiss AL. Aberrant brain activation during a response inhibition task in adolescent eating disorder subtypes. Am J Psychiatry. 2011. January;168(1):55–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Naor-Ziv R, Glicksohn J. Investigating cognitive deficits as risk factors for developing eating disorders during adolescence. Dev Neuropsychol. 2016;41(1–2):107–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Leon GR, Fulkerson JA, Perry CL, Keel PK, Klump KL. Three to four year prospective evaluation of personality and behavioral risk factors for later disordered eating in adolescent girls and boys. J Youth Adolesc. 1999;28(2):181–196. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Stice E A prospective test of the dual-pathway model of bulimic pathology: mediating effects of dieting and negative affect. J Abnorm Psychol. 2001;110(1):124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Stice E, Agras WS. Predicting onset and cessation of bulimic behaviors during adolescence: A longitudinal grouping analysis. Behav Ther. 1998;29(2):257–276. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Stice E, Presnell K, Bearman SK. Relation of early menarche to depression, eating disorders, substance abuse, and comorbid psychopathology among adolescent girls. Dev Psychol. 2001;37(5):608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Tyrka AR, Waldron I, Graber JA, Brooks-Gunn J. Prospective predictors of the onset of anorexic and bulimic syndromes. Int J Eat Disord. 2002;32(3):282–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bodell LP, Joiner TE, Ialongo NS. Longitudinal association between childhood impulsivity and bulimic symptoms in African American adolescent girls. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2012. April;80(2):313–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Pearson CM, Combs JL, Zapolski TC, Smith GT. A longitudinal transactional risk model for early eating disorder onset. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121(3):707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Mikami AY, Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE, Hoza B, Hechtman L, Newcorn JH, et al. Bulimia nervosa symptoms in the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD. Int J Eat Disord. 2010. April 1;43(3):248–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The Adolescent Brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124(1):111–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Goldschmidt AB, Wonderlich SA, Crosby RD, Engel SG, Lavender JM, Peterson CB, et al. Ecological momentary assessment of stressful events and negative affect in bulimia nervosa. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2014;82(1):30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lingswiler VM, Crowther JH, Stephens MAP. Emotional reactivity and eating in binge eating and obesity. J Behav Med. 1987;10(3):287–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Smyth JM, Wonderlich SA, Heron KE, Sliwinski MJ, Crosby RD, Mitchell JE, et al. Daily and momentary mood and stress are associated with binge eating and vomiting in bulimia nervosa patients in the natural environment. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2007;75(4):629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Cohen LH, Gunthert KC, Butler AC, O’Neill SC, Tolpin LH. Daily Affective Reactivity as a Prospective Predictor of Depressive Symptoms. J Pers. 2005;73(6):1687–714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Haedt-Matt AA, Keel PK, Racine SE, Burt SA, Hu JY, Boker S, et al. Do emotional eating urges regulate affect? Concurrent and prospective associations and implications for risk models of binge eating. Int J Eat Disord. 2014. December 1;47(8):874–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Cooper ML, Agocha VB, Sheldon MS. A Motivational Perspective on Risky Behaviors: The Role of Personality and Affect Regulatory Processes. J Pers. 2000. December 1;68(6):1059–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Racine SE, Keel PK, Burt SA, Sisk CL, Neale M, Boker S, et al. Exploring the Relationship between Negative Urgency and Dysregulated Eating: Etiologic Associations and the Role of Negative Affect. J Abnorm Psychol. 2013. May;122(2):433–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Juarascio AS, Felton JW, Borges AM, Manasse SM, Murray HB, Lejuez CW. An investigation of negative affect, reactivity, and distress tolerance as predictors of disordered eating attitudes across adolescence. J Adolesc. 2016. June;49:91–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R, Wilson GT. Eating disorders: A transdiagnostic protocol. 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Nowak JA, Roberson-Nay R, Strong DR, Bucceri J, Lejuez CW. Using item response theory in the development and validation of the College-Oriented Eating Disorders Screen. Eat Behav. 2003;4(4):345–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Pearson CM, Guller L, McPherson L, Lejuez CW, Smith GT. Validation of an Existing Measure of Eating Disorder Risk for Use with Early Adolescents. Eat Behav. 2013. April;14(2):113–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Bucceri JM, Roberson-Nay R, Strong DR, Nowak JA, Lejuez CW. Construct validity and reliability of the College Oriented Eating Disorders Screen (COEDS). Eat Behav. 2005;6(4):393–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Laurent J, Catanzaro SJ, Joiner TE Jr, Rudolph KD, Potter KI, Lambert S, et al. A measure of positive and negative affect for children: scale development and preliminary validation. Psychol Assess. 1999;11(3):326. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Lejuez CW, Daughters SB, Danielson CW, Ruggiero K. The behavioral indicator of resiliency to distress (BIRD). Unpubl Man. 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Danielson CK, Ruggiero KJ, Daughters SB, Lejuez CW. Distress tolerance, risk-taking propensity, and PTSD symptoms in trauma-exposed youth: Pilot study. Behav Ther. 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Eysenck SB, Pearson PR, Easting G, Allsopp JF. Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults. Personal Individ Differ. 1985;6(5):613–619. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990;107(2):238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tucker LR, Lewis C. A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1973;38(1):1–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Steiger JH. Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivar Behav Res. 1990;25(2):173–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Schweizer K Some guidelines concerning the modeling of traits and abilities in test construction. 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus (Version 6) Los Angeles. CA: Muthén Muthén; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Little RJ. A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. J Am Stat Assoc. 1988;83(404):1198–1202. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Romer D Adolescent risk taking, impulsivity, and brain development: Implications for prevention. Dev Psychobiol. 2010. April 1;52(3):263–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Spinrad TL, Cumberland A, Liew J, Reiser M, et al. Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Dev Psychol. 2009;45(4):988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Fischer S, Smith GT, Anderson KG. Clarifying the role of impulsivity in bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2003;33(4):406–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Elgar FJ, Roberts C, Tudor-Smith C, Moore L. Validity of self-reported height and weight and predictors of bias in adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2005. November 1;37(5):371–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Van de Mortel T Faking It: Social Desirability Response Bias in Self-report Research. Aust J Adv Nurs. 2008. August;25(4):40. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES