Abstract
We examined the role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) in memory retrieval monitoring. Event-related potentials were recorded while patients with VMPFC lesions and matched controls viewed faces of personal acquaintances, and of famous and nonfamous people, and indicated whether they had personally encountered these individuals. Patients were more likely than controls to make both false positive and false negative errors. Both groups showed a large posterior negative wave peaking at ∼170 ms after face onset (N170). In controls, the N170 was larger for both types of familiar faces, regardless of whether overt recognition occurred. Specifically, personal acquaintances that were erroneously judged as unfamiliar evoked the same electrophysiological response as those who were explicitly recognized. Patients' N170 was not modulated by familiarity suggesting VMPFC lesions disrupt early posterior memory-based preconscious cortical distinctions. Following the N170, there was a significant group difference over frontopolar scalp regions where patients were showing a smaller positive modulation at 230–260 ms for all stimulus types. In patients this modulation correlated highly with reaction times of correct responses, suggesting this early frontal modulation is related to the ability to make rapid correct decisions about memory content. Group differences over anterior sites were also noted at 350 ms after stimulus, reflecting a large sustained negativity of patients' waveforms, equal across conditions. The findings are consistent with a hypothesis of frontally mediated dual-monitoring system. An early automatic (preconscious) component is followed by a late elaborate process. We hypothesize that when both components are damaged, confabulation may occur.
Introduction
The reconstructive nature of memory makes it vulnerable to errors and distortions (Schacter et al., 1998). Structures within the prefrontal cortex (PFC) are crucial for reducing or avoiding false memories through strategic processes such as retrieval cue generation and output monitoring (Fletcher and Henson, 2001; Moscovitch and Winocur, 2002; Rugg et al., 2002). The ventromedial PFC (VMPFC), which is densely connected through limbothalamic pathways with the amygdala and hippocampus, is the prefrontal subregion most involved in mnemonic functions while dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) together with posterior parietal heteromodal cortex appear to form an executive/attention functional axis (Mesulam, 2000). Data from monkeys (Mishkin et al., 1984) and humans (DeLuca and Diamond, 1995) suggest that primary memory impairments following VMPFC lesions result from damage encroaching on the basal forebrain. Lesions restricted to the VMPFC itself affect memory monitoring (Moscovitch and Winocur, 2002; Gilboa, 2004), which are complex evaluative processes used to avoid memory errors (Koriat et al., 2000), or memory filtering (Schnider, 2003), which is the suppression of currently irrelevant activated memory traces. The VMPFC's precise role in monitoring or filtering has yet to be determined.
The prefrontal cortex mediates at least two types of memory monitoring and filtering. One is a conscious, deliberate, rational process akin to problem solving in other domains [dubbed by Burgess and Shallice (1996) the “editor”], which crucially depends on the DLPFC. Conversely, the VMPFC is hypothesized to mediate a phenomenological “feeling of rightness” (FOR): the ability to appreciate rapidly or intuitively the appropriateness and accuracy of a response with relation to current personal goals (Gilboa and Moscovitch, 2002; Moscovitch and Winocur, 2002). FOR precedes elaborate cognitive verification of memory veracity and context appropriateness, and is impenetrable to rational influences, suggesting it is formed outside of conscious awareness and may be affectively laden. Gilboa (2004) proposed that FOR partially depends on a correspondence between memory cues and long-term memory representations, which should emerge very early in processing to serve as a basis for FOR. These ideas of FOR in the memory domain are closely associated with more general functions of the VMPFC in emotion-based decision making (Bechara et al., 2000a) or processing of reinforcement contingencies based on the individual's current goals (Schnider et al., 2002; Rolls, 2004).
Event-related potentials (ERPs) have the temporal resolution required to detect the neural signature of rapid preconscious processes such as FOR. Electrophysiological studies of amnesia have reported primarily late recollection-related posterior ERP modulations on anterograde (recent) memory tests following hippocampal/diencephalic lesions (Mecklinger et al., 1998; Olichney et al., 2000; Taylor and Olichney, 2007). FOR, however, is most obvious in autobiographical (remote) memory (Brewer, 1986), and is disrupted by VMPFC lesions. We examined the dual-monitoring system hypothesis by measuring ERPs in patients with focal VMPFC lesions and matched healthy controls during speeded decisions about remote memory representations for faces of famous, personally familiar, and unfamiliar people. FOR disruption following VMPFC lesions, should be reflected by very early anterior electrophysiological group differences which would predict memory accuracy and decision speed.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Eight patients with lesions to the VMPFC following rupture of an ACoA aneurysm were recruited from Baycrest Hospital Psychology Department and the Rotman Research Institute's patient registry. These were all the patients with a diagnosis of ACoA aneurysm rupture who were treated at the Baycrest Psychology Department at the time, or whose name appeared on the Rotman's registry. Eight healthy controls matched for age, gender, and education were recruited from the Rotman Research Institute's healthy volunteer pool. CTs and/or MRIs were obtained for seven of the ACoA patients. Seven of the eight patients had neurosurgical intervention for clipping the aneurysm. We identified the specific frontal regions that were damaged in each patient (Fig. 1) by superimposing their individual scans on a brain template based on previously published guidelines (Stuss et al., 2002). Lesions were drawn by the primary author, and for verification and replication, they were independently drawn by D.T.S. and Dr. Michael Alexander from Harvard Medical School (Boston, MA). Where there were differences, the latter's delineation of the lesion was used.
Patients and controls were matched for age, years of education, sex, handedness, and estimated IQ based on performance on the Shipley Institute of Living Scale (Table 1). Patients ranged with regard to the time since injury from 4 months to more than eight years after surgery with the average being ∼2 years (Table 1).
Table 1.
ACoA patients (n = 8) | Healthy controls (n = 8) | |
---|---|---|
Gender (M/F) | 5/3 | 4/4 |
Handedness (R/L) | 6/2 | 7/1 |
Age (years) | 51.12 (11.44) | 54.5 (12.13) |
(38–68) | (33–65) | |
Education (years) | 15.75 (2.76) | 16.5 (2.72) |
(11–18) | (12–20) | |
Estimated IQ (Shipley) | 103.25 (10.00) | 106.37 (9.86) |
(90–119) | (91–117) | |
Months since loss | 21.37 (27.44) | |
(4–86) |
Numbers in parentheses to the right of the mean are SDs; numbers in parentheses below the mean and SD are ranges.
Patients had varying degrees of executive and anterograde memory impairments as determined by neuropsychological testing (Table 2), but all were at least moderately impaired on one domain of memory (delayed verbal or delayed visual memory) and/or had executive dysfunction (Fig. 2). Retrograde memory, as measured by the Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI) (Kopelman et al., 1990), was within normal range for both episodic (event) memory and personal semantic facts (Table 2). In contrast, for anterograde (recent) autobiographical memory there was more variation across patients, with some of them showing impairments on both parts of the AMI.
Table 2.
Mean (SD) | Range | |
---|---|---|
Executive/attentional | ||
WCST categories | 3.71 (1.80) | 1.00–6.00 |
Number of errors (Z) | −0.51 (0.96) | −1.75 to 0.87 |
% Persev. errors (Z) | −0.59 (1.32) | −2.7 to 1.05 |
Fluency | ||
Phonemic (Z) | −0.97 (1.59) | −2.93 to 1.22 |
Semantic (Z) | −0.84 (1.45) | −2.76 to 0.94 |
SDMT | ||
Oral (Z) | −0.29 (0.83) | −1.65 to 0.68 |
Written (Z) | 0.09 (1.20) | −1.89 to 1.37 |
TMT | ||
Part A (Z) | −0.87 (1.31) | −2.33 to 0.85 |
Part B (Z) | −1.38 (1.58) | −2.88 to 0.62 |
Anterograde memory | ||
WMS III | ||
LM1 (SS) | 7.25 (2.31) | 4.00–11.00 |
LM2 (SS) | 7.13 (2.70) | 4.00–13.00 |
VPA1 (SS) | 5.75 (3.69) | 1.00–12.00 |
VPA2 (SS) | 6.50 (3.34) | 3.00–13.00 |
Recognition (SS) | 8.25 (3.11) | 5.00–14.00 |
RCFT | ||
Immediate (Z) | −1.40 (0.87) | −2.75 to −0.60 |
Delayed (Z) | −1.70 (0.79) | −2.75 to −0.92 |
RAVLT | ||
Trial 1 (Z) | −1.35 (1.28) | −2.68 to 0.37 |
Delayed (Z) | −1.70 (0.92) | −3.41 to 0.56 |
Delayed recognition (hits-FA) (Z) | −1.33 (1.20) | −3.20 to 1.54 |
Remote memory | ||
AMI-EM | ||
Childhooda | 7.00 (1.58) | 5.00–9.00 |
Early adulthooda | 7.40 (1.82) | 5.00–9.00 |
Recent (anterograde)b | 5.00 (3.16) | 0.00–8.00 |
AMI-Sem. | ||
Childhoodc | 19.40 (1.34) | 18.00–21.00 |
Early adulthoodd | 19.30 (1.03) | 18.00–20.50 |
Recent (anterograde)e | 16.30 (5.68) | 8.50–21.00 |
The normative data for the tests were taken from the original manuals or from the sources referenced below. AMI, The Autobiographical Memory Interview (Kopelman et al., 1990); AMI-EM, event memory; AMI-Sem., semantic memory; RAVLT, Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test; RCFT, Rey's Complex Figure Test; SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities Test; TMT, Trails Making Test; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; Persev., perseverations; WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale III; LM1, logical memory immediate; LM2, logical memory delayed; VPA1, verbal paired associates immediate; VPA2, verbal paired associates delayed; SS, scaled score (cutoff for abnormal, ≤ 4); Z, Z score (cutoff for abnormal, ≤ −2).
aCutoff for abnormal, ≤ 3;
bcutoff for abnormal, ≤ 5;
ccutoff for abnormal, ≤ 11;
dcutoff for abnormal, ≤ 14;
ecutoff for abnormal, ≤ 17.
One patient (FS) who was 7 years after his ACoA aneurysm rupture was still confabulating at the time of testing. Another patient (TG) was not actively confabulating, however he spontaneously described himself as having false memories which he did not trust and which had led him to be very cautious about his memory and consult his friends and relatives regularly about events he thought he remembered. In two other patients (FR and MN) a history of confabulations could be verified through interview with family members, clinicians, or the patients themselves. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Baycrest Hospital.
Experimental design and materials
The experimental stimuli were comprised of three kinds of faces: (1) personally familiar faces, (2) famous faces, and (3) unfamiliar faces.
Personally familiar faces.
These were faces cropped out of pictures from family albums, covering the lifetime of the participant. Only pictures in which the participant appeared were used so as to ensure that all of the persons represented in the picture were familiar. Still familiarity of the faces varied, as some of the faces were of close relatives whereas others could be of old school teachers, classmates, work colleagues, etc. Moreover, faces of relatives could be taken from different periods of the participant's life (e.g., a childhood picture of a sibling). The same person's face could serve as a stimulus up to twice, provided that pictures from very different time periods were used (i.e., separated by at least 20 years). Because of the difficulty of obtaining stimuli for the personally familiar faces, the number of stimuli to which participants were exposed varied somewhat among participants (average: 133.56, SD = 16.82, range: 108–156). There was no consistent group difference in the overall number of photos (t(14) = −1.41; p > 0.05) between controls (M = 129.37; SD = 12.69) and patients (M = 137.75; SD = 11.01). The vast majority of photographs were scanned at 300 dpi; when this was not possible due to a photo's low quality; it was scanned at the highest quality possible. Photos that were originally taken in color were turned into grayscale using commercially available software (Adobe Photoshop).
Famous faces.
There were 115 faces of famous personalities, who were selected out of a larger set of 250 faces, based on a pilot study with 20 middle-aged individuals. The faces were scanned out of journal magazines or downloaded from the internet. We attempted to use photographs of famous people in naturalistic poses so as to match the faces from the other conditions. In all conditions faces could be in full frontal view, half views, profiles, etc., and faces could express emotion, reflect a speech act, etc. Additionally, we purposely selected a portion of the pictures of famous faces which were of low quality (blurry, dark, etc.) so as to mimic the faces from family members and acquaintance, which were sometimes taken out of very old or low quality pictures. During the pilot study, participants were asked to rate each of the faces for familiarity, as well as provide some identifying detail if they could as to the identity of the person (name, reason for fame, etc.). Only faces that at least 90% of the pilot participants had identified as famous were selected, although faces varied with regard to the average level of familiarity ratings they received.
Unfamiliar faces.
Participants also viewed 120 faces of unfamiliar people, who were selected out of the personal stimuli of other participants. There were faces of people of different chronological ages and from different decades so as to match the distribution of personally familiar and famous faces.
Task and procedures
Participants were told that they would view faces of people from their personal pasts, faces of famous personalities, and faces of unfamiliar people. They were told that they should respond with “yes” (left mouse button; pointing finger) only for people they had personally encountered and with “no” (right mouse button; middle finger) for all others, regardless of whether they know who they are or not. Each trial was preceded by a fixation with the question “Have you personally met this individual before?” and mapping of the response buttons at the bottom, which appeared for 1000 ms. The question and mapping of the buttons remained on screen the whole time. Following fixation, the faces appeared for 600 ms and after the face disappeared, participants had a time window of up to 2000 ms to respond, following which the next trial automatically began (Fig. 3). Participants were encouraged to respond as quickly as possible but not at the expense of accuracy.
Following the application of the electrode cap, the experiment was run within a single session lasting ∼10–12 min. A pause was included midway through the session (after ∼140 stimuli, depending on the number of personally familiar faces). During the break the experimenter entered the chamber to speak to the participants, to ensure they were not too fatigued and to encourage them. Participants in fact reported enjoying the experiment, and in particular they reported enjoying seeing faces of persons they had not thought about in a long time.
ERP recordings and analysis
Continuous EEG was recorded with Neuroscan software using a Synamps neural amplifier (Compumedics) and 64-channel tin electrode caps (Electro-Cap International), with electrodes placed according to the 10–20 system (Jasper, 1958). EEG recordings were made at a sample rate of 500 Hz, using a Cz reference. The continuous EEG recordings were filtered on line between 0.05 and 100 Hz. During the recording, electrodes placed at the outer canthi and the superior and inferior orbit monitored vertical and horizontal eye movements.
The continuous EEG files were segmented into 700 ms epochs including a 100 ms prestimulus window and the time of stimulus visual presentation. These epochs were then rereferenced to a common-average reference, and baseline corrected over the prestimulus window. Trials contaminated by excessive peak-to-peak deflection (exceeded ±150 μV) at the channels not adjacent to the eyes were automatically rejected. The remaining epochs were then sorted and averaged so that for each participant, each type of face and accuracy of response had their own average [i.e., hits and misses for personally familiar faces; correct rejection (CR) and false alarms (FA) for famous faces, and CR and FA for unfamiliar faces]. Across healthy participants, the number of included trials for hits, CRs famous and CRs nonfamous varied between 68 and 122 for each category. FAs were not included as there were very few such events. For the patients, the number of included trials for hits, CRs famous and CRs nonfamous varied between 36 and 116 for each category.
For each participant, a set of ocular movements was obtained before and after the experiment (Picton et al., 2000). From this set, averaged eye movements were calculated for both lateral and vertical eye movements as well as for eyeblinks. A principal component analysis of these averaged recordings provided a set of components that best explained the eye movements. The scalp projections of these components were then subtracted from the experimental ERPs to minimize ocular contamination such as blinks, saccades, and lateral eye movements for each individual average. ERPs were then digitally low-pass filtered to attenuate frequencies of >20 Hz. ERP amplitudes were measured relative to the mean amplitude over the prestimulus interval.
The effects of VMPFC lesions on face processing and memory decisions were examined on the P1, N170, and P2 waves. The P1 mean amplitude was quantified over the 90–110 ms interval at occipital (O1 and O2) and cerebellar (CB1 and CB2) electrodes in line with previous research on face perception (Caharel et al., 2002, 2006; Jemel et al., 2003; Itier and Taylor, 2004; Jemel et al., 2005; Anaki et al., 2007). Similarly, the effect of lesion and familiarity on the mean amplitude of the face sensitive N170 (Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Caharel et al., 2002, 2006, 2007; Rossion and Gauthier, 2002; Jemel et al., 2003, 2005; Itier and Taylor, 2004; Anaki et al., 2007; Marzi and Viggiano, 2007) was quantified over the 150–190 ms interval at inferior parietal (P7 and P8) and cerebellar (CB1 and CB2) electrodes. The P2 mean amplitude wave was measured for the 280–320 ms interval at occipital (O1 and O2) and cerebellar (CB1 and CB2) electrodes. The effects of VMPFC lesions on ERPs recorded during the face recognition task were also examined for the 230–260 ms interval at frontopolar electrodes (FP1, FPz, FP2). This interval was chosen to ease comparison with a prior ERP study which examined performance of healthy adults on a task known to be sensitive to confabulation (Schnider et al., 2002) as well as with prior research on face recognition showing early memory-related activity for faces peaking at ∼250 ms at frontocentral and frontopolar scalp region (Guillaume and Tiberghien, 2005). Moreover, intracerebral recordings in humans with intractable epilepsy have revealed early visual evoked responses to faces in prefrontal cortex (Halgren et al., 1994; Klopp et al., 2000). Hence, we seek to determine whether VMPFC lesions would attenuate early responses to faces over the frontal scalp region. Another modulation of interest was also measured, namely, the sustained potential over the frontocentral and frontopolar scalp regions. The sustained potential was quantified over the 450–550 ms interval at frontopolar sites (FP1, FPz, FP2), again to facilitate comparison with the study by Schnider et al. (2002) and previous face-processing studies. The mean amplitude measurements were analyzed using repeated-measure ANOVAs with group as the between-subject factor and stimulus type as determined by response accuracy (i.e., hit, miss, correct rejections of famous faces and correct rejections of nonfamous faces) as the within-subjects factor.
Results
Behavioral data
Because the VMPFC is thought to be involved in memory processes, it was hypothesized that patients would show a reduced ability at distinguishing faces of persons from their own past and other faces. As Figure 4A illustrates, hit rate was lower than correct rejections with patients performing worse than controls. This impression was confirmed by ANOVA which showed a significant within-subject main effect of accuracy (F(2,28) = 15.01, p < 0.01), with no accuracy-by-group interaction (F(2,28) = 1.44, p > 0.05). Post hoc contrasts indicated that hit rate was significantly lower than correct rejections of famous (F(1,14) = 22.8, p < 0.01) and nonfamous (F(1,14) = 13.36, p < 0.01) faces. Correct rejection rates of famous and nonfamous faces did not differ significantly from each other (F(1,14) = 1.26, p > 0.05), although both figures show a ceiling effect and this result needs to be considered cautiously. There was also a significant between-subject group main effect (F(1,14) = 14.21, p < 0.01), reflecting the overall lower accuracy of patients compared with controls on this task.
We also computed d′ scores for patients and controls, separately for famous faces false alarms and unfamiliar faces false alarms in relation to family faces hits. Patients' average d′ scores were generally lower with famous faces serving as baseline (patients: d′ = 1.97, range: 1.32–3.39; controls: d′ = 4.03, range: 3.01–4.65; t(14) = 4.89, p < 0.01), with no difference in bias between groups (patients: normalized C = 0.29; controls: normalized C = 0.16; t(14) = −1.39, p > 0.05). With respect to the d′ scores when unfamiliar faces serve as baseline, results were similar. Patients' average d′ scores were generally lower for both famous faces (patients: d′ = 2.11, range: 0.71–4.54; controls: d′ = 3.31, range: 2.34–4.65; t(14) = 2.17; p < 0.05), with no difference in bias for patients (normalized C = 0.25) compared with controls (normalized C = 0.13; t(14) = −0.95; p > 0.05).
Figure 4B shows that both patients and controls could reject famous faces more quickly than familiar faces, and reject unfamiliar ones more quickly than they could endorse family faces. This impression was supported by ANOVA which indicated a main effect of familiarity on reaction time (RT) (F(2,28) = 3.92, p < 0.05), with no RT-by-group interaction (F(2,28) = 1.08, p > 0.05). Contrasts indicated that participants were faster for correct rejections of famous faces than correct rejections of unfamiliar faces (F(1,14) = 6.53, p < 0.05) and RTs to hits for family faces were longer than to correct rejections of unfamiliar faces (F(1,14) = 5.25, p < 0.05). Importantly, there were no significant between-group differences in RT (F(1,14) = 0.30, p > 0.05) which could confound a comparison of the ERPs.
ERP results
Our neuropsychological model predicts three primary ERP patterns: (1) very early frontally distributed differences between patients and controls that would reflect FOR processes. (2) These should rely on a preceding signal reflecting a concordance between a memory cue (picture of a face) and a long-term memory representation that should occur over posterior cortices where the cue is being processed and supposedly LTM representations are stored. (3) Finally, a later frontally distributed component is expected to reflect conscious monitoring of retrieval products. Next we describe the primary ERP waveforms over posterior sites, followed by the waveforms over anterior sites.
Posterior sites
Figure 5 shows the group mean ERPs elicited by personally familiar, unfamiliar, and famous faces in both patients with a VMPFC lesion and age-matched controls. In both groups, faces elicited a clear P1, N1 (N170), and P2 waves at inferior occipital–temporal–parietal sites. The P1 mean amplitude (90–110 ms) showed no main effect of hemisphere (F(1,14) = 0.09, p > 0.05), stimulus type (F(3,42) = 0.55, p > 0.05), or group (F(1,14) = 0.29, p > 0.05), and none of the interactions was significant (Fig. 5A,B).
Numerous ERP studies have described a face-sensitive right lateralized N1 component peaking at ∼170 ms after stimulus onset (Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Caharel et al., 2002, 2006, 2007; Rossion and Gauthier, 2002; Jemel et al., 2003, 2005; Itier and Taylor, 2004; Anaki et al., 2007; Marzi and Viggiano, 2007). A clear N170 component was recorded over the inferior parietal and cerebellar sites on both right (i.e., CB2 and P8) and left (i.e., CB1, P7) hemispheres. The average N170 over right-sided electrodes was larger than that measured over left-sided electrodes (F(1,14) = 6.22; p < 0.05) with no significant group differences (F(1,14) = 1.24; p > 0.05) and a side-by-group interaction only approached significance (F(1,14) = 3.24; p = 0.09).
Examination of the effect of response type (hit, miss, famous, and nonfamous correct rejections) on the N170 for each side separately revealed no significant between-group effects (right: F(1,14) = 2.23; p > 0.05; left: F(1,14) = 0.46; p > 0.05) or overall condition effect (right: F(3,42) = 1.32; p > 0.05; left: F(3,42) = 0.68; p > 0.05). There was, however, a significant group × condition interaction over right-sided electrodes (F(3,42) = 3.91, p < 0.05). Planned contrasts showed that this was the result of the unfamiliar faces correct rejections (CR2) amplitude being significantly lower than correct rejections of famous faces, hits of personal acquaintances, and misses of personal acquaintances (F(1,14) = 5.18; p < 0.05) (Fig. 5, arrow).
Thus, the normal controls' waveforms suggest a distinction between familiar (whether personally familiar or famous personalities) and nonfamiliar faces. The normal controls' waveforms for misses are particularly interesting, as they suggest familiarity with faces previously encountered that is not reflected by performance. It is also interesting to note that the patients do not show a distinction between familiar and nonfamiliar faces. These results are compatible with the second pattern predicted by our model which suggests a very early posterior signal which could code the correspondence between memory cues and long-term memory representations.
Significant group differences were revealed for the P2 wave over posterior sites bilaterally. Patients showed a larger P2 wave than controls averaged over left (O1, CB1) and right (O2, CB2) occipital areas (F(1,14) = 37.33, p < 0.05) with no laterality or interaction effects. There was also an overall response type effect (F(3,42) = 5.11; p < 0.05). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction revealed that the P2 associated with unfamiliar faces was larger than the P2 associated with hits and misses of acquaintances' faces, whereas famous faces fell somewhere in the middle. There were no significant interactions between response type and either group or side.
Anterior sites
The waveforms over anterior sites were markedly different between patients and controls (Fig. 6). As predicted by our model there were significant group differences very early on, immediately following the posterior familiarity-sensitive N170, which may reflect the automatic FOR, and also at a later time window which may be associated with more controlled processes.
Patients showed a distinct N1, P1, and N2, which mirrored the pattern recorded over posterior sites but inverted in polarity and showing later latencies whereas controls showed the N1 and P1 which was followed by a sustained positivity. For the N1 measured over the frontopolar scalp region (averaged over FP1, FPz, FP2; 140–160 ms after onset) there was no main effect of response type (F(3,43) = 2.12, p > 0.05), group (F(1,14) = 0.238, p > 0.05), or group-by-response interaction (F(3,42) = 1.72; p > 0.05).
There was, however, a very early significant difference between patients with VMPFC lesions and controls associated with the positive modulation at frontopolar sites. This positive modulation peaked between 230 and 260 ms. Healthy controls showed greater positivity than patients (F(1,14) = 5.80; p < 0.05) for this component, with no significant within-subject response-type effect (F(3,42) = 2.02; p > 0.05) or group-by-condition interactions (F(3,42) = 1.04; p > 0.05).
To assess the probability that this waveform difference obtained over anterior electrodes in fact was generated by anterior cortical areas, we took the following approach for source analyses using the grand average data from the controls only. Patients' data were omitted from source analyses because breaches to the skull as a result from neurosurgery preclude source analysis of the ERPs measured at the surface. We first used the spatiotemporal multiple source analysis technique (implemented in the BESA 5.2 software). We modeled the visual sensory evoked responses (P1 and N170) using regional sources that were located in extrastriate and inferior temporal cortices which have been described in playing a major role in face perception. These were fixed to be symmetrical in each hemisphere. We then added two regional sources and forced them to be symmetrical. We fit these two sources to the group average data at the 230–260 ms time interval (where a significant difference was detected between patients and controls over anterior electrodes). We kept the locations of the posterior sources, and optimized the location of the two regional sources, which yielded a location near the VMPFC (∼1.5–2 cm from the area of maximum overlap of the patients' lesions). This model accounted for 97.2% of the variance (Fig. 7A).
We also used a different modeling approach. Local autoregressive average (i.e., LAURA) is a weighted minimum norm distributed source modeling implemented in BESA 5.2 software. This distributed approach does not require an a priori decision regarding the number of sources or their location. Applying the LAURA modeling approach for the 245 ms time point revealed bilateral frontal ventromedial sources that overlapped with and were even more ventromedial than the sources obtained with the spatiotemporal multiple source analysis technique (Fig. 7B) and was ∼1 cm away from the area of maximum overlap of the patients' lesions (Fig. 7C). Thus, both an overdetermined (dipolar) and an underdetermined (distributed) source models provided almost the same solution and both models located the generators of the early anterior ERP modulation to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, where the patients' lesions had the most overlap. Together, these inverse solutions provide good evidence that the positive anterior modulation seen in controls but missing in patients may receive substantial contributions from the VMPFC in addition to possible other regions. The source waveforms (Fig. 7D) show the time course of source activity over posterior and anterior regions. As expected, very early sensory processes are best reflected by occipitotemporal sources followed by later activity (beginning at ∼200 ms) which is best reflected in frontal areas.
Based on these models one could argue that the reduced amplitude in patients is related to the lesions sustained in that area. The correspondence between lesions in patients and ERP source in controls, provide evidence from different neuroscience approaches as to the role of the VMPFC in memory retrieval. Although multiple models may account for the data, this model accounts well to the data in controls, and converges with the lesion data.
Following the positive modulation, controls showed a brief negativity followed by a sustained positivity whereas patients showed a clear negative wave that followed in latency the P2 wave recorded at occipital sites, which led to a significant sustained late between-group difference over frontopolar sites, beginning at ∼350 ms (F(1,14) = 11.90, p < 0.05). There were no significant within subject response type effect or response-by-group interaction effect, F < 1 in both cases.
Correlations between performance and electrophysiological data
To explore the behavioral significance of the waveform differences between patients and controls, we examined the relationships between performance and electrophysiological data using reaction times for both groups and accuracy for the patients only (accuracy data could not be used in controls for famous and unfamiliar faces because of ceiling effects). Correlations were computed for the waveform components where significant group or condition effects were observed, separately for controls and patients for hits, correct rejections of famous personalities, and correct rejections of unfamiliar persons.
There were no significant correlations between response times and N170 amplitudes (Table 3). Significant negative correlations were present between early frontal amplitudes and hits and correct rejections of both types in the patients, but not in controls (Table 3, Fig. 8). Thus, patients with faster reaction times presented with a very early frontally distributed more positive deflection, closer to that exemplified by controls. This is consistent with the idea that this early frontal component is related to the ability to make rapid and accurate decisions about retrieved memory representations. Interestingly, the three patients who consistently showed the most negative waveforms for this early anterior ERP component were FS who still actively confabulated, TG who has erroneous memories which he does not relay and MN with a verified history of confabulation.
Table 3.
Posterior right N170 |
Anterior early (230–260) |
Anterior late (450–550) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls | Patients | Controls | Patients | Controls | Patients | |
Family (hits) | 0.39 | 0.12 | 0.03 | −0.79* | −0.49 | 0.44 |
Famous (CR) | 0.06 | 0.26 | 0.25 | −0.68* | −0.44 | 0.14 |
Unfamiliar (CR) | 0.51 | −0.03 | −0.38 | −0.72* | 0.22 | 0.01 |
*p < 0.05.
There was a significant negative correlation between hit rate and N170 amplitudes for patients only (Table 4), which means larger N170 in patients predicted better performance in recognition of family members. There were no significant correlations between the early anterior component and accuracy. There were however significant correlations between late anterior component and accurately rejecting famous and nonfamous faces (Table 4, Fig. 9). Thus, patients who were more accurate had more positive anterior late component, more similar to the controls' waveform. This is compatible with the idea that this late component is associated with the ability to detect and minimize erroneous output during memory retrieval.
Table 4.
Posterior right N170 |
Anterior early (230–260) |
Anterior late (450–550) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls | Patients | Controls | Patients | Controls | Patients | |
Family (hits) | 0.44 | −0.62* | −0.30 | 0.46 | −0.42 | 0.49 |
Famous (CR) | −0.13 | −0.08 | 0.76* | |||
Unfamiliar (CR) | 0.48 | −0.25 | 0.92** |
*p < 0.05;
**p < 0.01.
Discussion
The VMPFC appears to serve an important function during memory retrieval, and more specifically in monitoring of retrieval output, but its precise role is yet to be determined. Theories of VMPFC function have suggested it acts to integrate cognitive processes with emotional somatic signals to bias decision making at a preconscious level (Bechara et al., 1997, 2000a,b). Alternatively, its role has been interpreted in terms of learning and reversal of stimulus-reinforcement associations to bias behavior according to individual goals and drives (Rolls et al., 1994; Rolls, 1996, 2004). Either way, the VMPFC is well situated anatomically to integrate limbic information and information from neocortical heteromodal regions, to allow for such biasing (Barbas, 1995). The VMPFC may serve similar functions in the memory domain, predominantly in autobiographical memories where such processes are pertinent (Conway and Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Fotopoulou et al., 2007).
VMPFC patients in the present study lacked a positive deflection at 230–260 ms after stimulus onsets that receive input from VMPFC as suggested by source modelings in controls. Furthermore, patients who showed larger positive deflections were faster on their accurate memory decisions, consistent with the idea that this early electrophysiological signature is related to rapid selection of memory-based responses. In the same way, patients with history of confabulation had the most negative waveforms and took longer to make correct responses. Schnider et al. (2002) have proposed an intriguing model in which the VMPFC plays a role in rapid filtering of memories according to their relevance to current reality. They report a positive frontally distributed ERP deflection also peaking at 240 ms during a memory condition known to be sensitive to confabulation (Schnider et al., 2002; Schnider, 2003). Although their ERP study only included healthy controls, confabulating patients always fail this condition, which is considered a sign of temporal context confusion (TCC), i.e., a failure of a rapid filtering mechanism for currently irrelevant memory traces. According to their model, this filtering mechanism acts by momentarily inhibiting cortical synchronization at a preconscious stage, before explicit recognition occurs. When filtering fails, people might act according to memory representations that are no longer relevant and behavioral confabulations may arise.
The data from the present study are consistent with our dual-monitoring hypothesis, which suggests an early postecphoric, very early feeling of rightness (FOR) mechanism providing an immediate general sense of the correctness of memory decisions (Gilboa and Moscovitch, 2002; Moscovitch and Winocur, 2002; Gilboa, 2004; Gilboa et al., 2006). The neurocognitive mechanism underlying the early memory-related ERP waveforms observed in the present study and in that of Schnider et al. (2002) might be analogous. In the present study, however, the task has no temporal component, which suggests that the filtering mechanism might serve a more general function than supporting the representation of current reality and in fact helps make any fine-grained distinctions within memory [see Gilboa et al. (2006) for more evidence and discussion of this point]. Furthermore, as discussed below, we present evidence that the early frontal modulation follows an earlier (preconscious) familiarity signal when remote memories are retrieved, although it precedes explicit recognition as suggested by Schnider et al. (2002). Importantly, we show that patients with VMPFC lesions lack that early frontal modulation, as predicted by both models. Interestingly some nonconfabulating patients with VMPFC lesions also fail on Schnider's task (Gilboa et al., 2006) and it may be that this is related to individual differences in this early electrophysiological signature.
Very few electrophysiological studies of memory with neurological patients exist, and even fewer involve patients with focal lesions. In focal MTL anterograde amnesia, modulations of the posterior late positivity component (LPC/P600) old–new effect have been described (Olichney et al., 2000). Similar results were obtained in amnesics with diffuse damage following anoxia (Mecklinger et al., 1998), although these patients showed no LPC old–new or even inverted effects and also had alterations in earlier non-mnemonic ERP components (P300, N400). In contrast, a study of patients with focal dorsolateral PFC lesions using a continuous recognition paradigm (Swick and Knight, 1999) showed no difference in ERP waveforms compared with controls, despite behavioral difference which were manifested primarily as increase in false alarms.
Unlike these previous studies we examined retrograde semantic memory which is poorly correlated with anterograde episodic amnesia. Furthermore, our patients are not amnesic; they suffer from some anterograde memory impairments, and did not perform as well on the experimental retrograde task, but do not meet criteria for amnesia. They do however show deficits on aspects of “working with memory” such as susceptibility for interference, intrusions, and false alarms. These distinctions might account for the differences in the patterns of ERP results between our study and previous studies of amnesic patients, in particular the very early memory related differences over the posterior N170 and the positive anterior modulation at 230–260 ms. In contrast, the later differences observed in the present study might be interpreted either as reflecting later monitoring processes, as suggested by the correlation with accuracy, or as a failure in recollective processes as suggested by previous ERP studies (Taylor and Olichney, 2007). The late monitoring component of our model operates by bringing other relevant information to bear on the recovered memory, such as perceptual characteristics and compatibility with other knowledge which could influence the decision to accept or reject the recovered memory. This process may rely heavily on recollection of additional information for verification, and so the interpretations of the late ERP difference might converge.
Another interesting feature of the present study was a very early group-by-condition interaction associated with the well studied N170 component of face processing (Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Caharel et al., 2002, 2006, 2007; Rossion and Gauthier, 2002; Jemel et al., 2003, 2005; Itier and Taylor, 2004; Anaki et al., 2007; Marzi and Viggiano, 2007). Our healthy controls showed a familiarity effect regardless of whether the face was personally familiar or a famous face, and whether it was explicitly recognized. This suggests that at a preconscious level information about faces of acquaintances that were not consciously recognized was available, similar to the effects seen in covert recognition of faces using psychophysiological measures such as skin conductance (Tranel and Damasio, 1985; Calder and Young, 2005). There was no such differential modulation of the N170 in the patients. We have proposed (Gilboa, 2004; Gilboa et al., 2006) that the basis of FOR may be a correspondence between a retrieval cue and representations in LTM, with stronger representations (as in self-related processing, for example) generating more robust FOR. The familiarity modulation of the posterior N170 which appeared ∼70 ms before the early anterior difference between the groups may have formed the basis of early monitoring in controls which was diminished or undifferentiated in patients.
It should be noted that the sensitivity of N170 to face identity is a contentious issue. Many studies show no modulation of N170 in response to familiarity (Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Eimer, 2000; Schweinberger et al., 2002; Anaki et al., 2007), while others show the same effect observed in the present study (Caharel et al., 2002, 2006; Schweinberger et al., 2002; Jemel et al., 2003; Marzi and Viggiano, 2007). The effect appears to be particularly observable under conditions of degraded stimuli and when familiarity is important for task performance. Both features characterize our study which included many low quality stimuli and which required participants to make fine distinctions between different kinds of familiarity (personal vs public), which may emphasize the top-down contribution of LTM in disambiguating face identity.
The absence of modulation of the posterior N170 in patients with anterior lesions is interesting, although not surprising. Early ERP work has shown differences in ERP waveform between patients with DLPFC lesions and controls during auditory perceptual tasks (Woods and Knight, 1986), suggesting that top-down modulations may occur very early and preconsciously. Frontal top-down control over visual perceptual processes have been demonstrated, particularly when ambiguity had to be resolved regarding object identity (Bar et al., 2006). The missing N170 familiarity signature in patients may contribute to the lack of early anterior positive modulation and consequently for reliance on later more elaborate processing of the stimuli to reach a decision.
To conclude, we believe the present data are consistent with a model in which the compatibility of a memory cue with LTM representations is picked up by posterior cortices (indexed by the N170) and serves as a basis for the formation of an early VMPFC-mediated FOR. Deficits in FOR following VMPFC lesions are akin to deficits in extinction and reward association reversal in non-mnemonic domains in that they too reflect context-insensitive responses and are impenetrable to rationality or explicit knowledge. Interestingly, VMPFC lesions appear to disrupt not only FOR but also the posterior familiarity index possibly through top-down control. In healthy adults, the posterior familiarity index is also related to later anterior processes, whereas patients with VMPFC lesions do not show such an association and appear to recruit different elaborate processes to achieve accurate performance.
Footnotes
This research was conducted with support from the Canadian Foundation for Innovation/Ontario Innovation Trust Grant 1226 and the Heart and Stroke Foundation Centre for Stroke Recovery. A.G. is supported by a European Union FP06 Marie Curie International Reintegration Grant (046544).
References
- Anaki D, Zion-Golumbic E, Bentin S. Electrophysiological neural mechanisms for detection, configural analysis and recognition of faces. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1407–1416. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bar M, Kassam KS, Ghuman AS, Boshyan J, Schmid AM, Dale AM, Hämäläinen MS, Marinkovic K, Schacter DL, Rosen BR, Halgren E. Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:449–454. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0507062103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H. Anatomic basis of cognitive-emotional interactions in the primate prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1995;19:499–510. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(94)00053-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy [see comments] Science. 1997;275:1293–1295. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000a;10:295–307. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000b;123:2189–2202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bentin S, Deouell LY. Structural encoding and identification in face processing: ERP evidence for separate mechanisms. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2000;17:35–54. doi: 10.1080/026432900380472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brewer WF. What is autobiographical memory? In: Rubin DC, editor. Autobiographical memory. New York: Cambridge UP; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Burgess PW, Shallice T. Confabulation and the control of recollection. Memory. 1996;4:359–411. doi: 10.1080/096582196388906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caharel S, Poiroux S, Bernard C, Thibaut F, Lalonde R, Rebai M. ERPs associated with familiarity and degree of familiarity during face recognition. Int J Neurosci. 2002;112:1499–1512. doi: 10.1080/00207450290158368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caharel S, Fiori N, Bernard C, Lalonde R, Rebaï M. The effects of inversion and eye displacements of familiar and unknown faces on early and late-stage ERPs. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;62:141–151. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caharel S, Bernard C, Thibaut F, Haouzir S, Di Maggio-Clozel C, Allio G, Fouldrin G, Petit M, Lalonde R, Rebaï M. The effects of familiarity and emotional expression on face processing examined by ERPs in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2007;95:186–196. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calder AJ, Young AW. Understanding the recognition of facial identity and facial expression. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:641–651. doi: 10.1038/nrn1724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW. The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol Rev. 2000;107:261–288. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeLuca J, Diamond BJ. Aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery: a review of neuroanatomical and neuropsychological sequelae. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1995;17:100–121. doi: 10.1080/13803399508406586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eimer M. Effects of face inversion on the structural encoding and recognition of faces. Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2000;10:145–158. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00038-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher PC, Henson RN. Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain. 2001;124:849–881. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.5.849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fotopoulou A, Conway MA, Solms M. Confabulation: motivated reality monitoring. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2180–2190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilboa A. Autobiographical and episodic memory—one and the same? Evidence from prefrontal activation in neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1336–1349. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilboa A, Moscovitch M. The cognitive neuroscience of confabulation: a review and a model. In: Baddeley AD, Kopelman MD, Wilson BA, editors. Handbook of memory disorders. Ed 2. London: Wiley; 2002. pp. 315–342. [Google Scholar]
- Gilboa A, Alain C, Stuss DT, Melo B, Miller S, Moscovitch M. Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations: a strategic retrieval account. Brain. 2006;129:1399–1414. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillaume F, Tiberghien G. Electrophysiological study of contextual variations in a short-term face recognition task. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;22:471–487. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halgren E, Baudena P, Heit G, Clarke JM, Marinkovic K, Chauvel P, Clarke M. Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing. 2. Depth-recorded potentials in the human frontal and Rolandic cortices. J Physiol Paris. 1994;88:51–80. doi: 10.1016/0928-4257(94)90093-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itier RJ, Taylor MJ. N170 or N1? Spatiotemporal differences between object and face processing using ERPs. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:132–142. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jasper HH. The ten-twenty electrode system of the international federation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1958;10:371–375. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jemel B, Pisani M, Calabria M, Crommelinck M, Bruyer R. Is the N170 for faces cognitively penetrable? Evidence from repetition priming of Mooney faces of familiar and unfamiliar persons. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003;17:431–446. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(03)00145-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jemel B, Pisani M, Rousselle L, Crommelinck M, Bruyer R. Exploring the functional architecture of person recognition system with event-related potentials in a within- and cross-domain self-priming of faces. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:2024–2040. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klopp J, Marinkovic K, Chauvel P, Nenov V, Halgren E. Early widespread cortical distribution of coherent fusiform face selective activity. Hum Brain Mapp. 2000;11:286–293. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200012)11:4<286::AID-HBM80>3.0.CO;2-R. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopelman M, Wilson B, Baddeley A. The autobiographical memory interview. Bury St Edmunds, England: Thames Valley Test Company; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Koriat A, Goldsmith M, Pansky A. Toward a psychology of memory accuracy. Annu Rev Psychol. 2000;51:481–537. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marzi T, Viggiano MP. Interplay between familiarity and orientation in face processing: an ERP study. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;65:182–192. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mecklinger A, von Cramon DY, Matthes-von Cramon G. Event-related potential evidence for a specific recognition memory deficit in adult survivors of cerebral hypoxia. Brain. 1998;121:1919–1935. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.10.1919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesulam MM. Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology. Ed 2. Oxford: Oxford UP; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Mishkin M, Malamut BL, Bachevalier J. Memories and habits: two neural systems. In: McGaugh G, Lynch G, Weinberger NM, editors. Neurobiology of learning and memory. New York: Guilford; 1984. pp. 65–77. [Google Scholar]
- Moscovitch M, Winocur G. The frontal cortex and working with memory. In: Stuss DT, Knight R, editors. Principles of frontal lobe function. New York: Oxford UP; 2002. pp. 188–209. [Google Scholar]
- Olichney JM, Van Petten C, Paller KA, Salmon DP, Iragui VJ, Kutas M. Word repetition in amnesia. Electrophysiological measures of impaired and spared memory. Brain. 2000;123:1948–1963. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.9.1948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picton TW, van Roon P, Armilio ML, Berg P, Ille N, Scherg M. The correction of ocular artifacts: a topographic perspective. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;111:53–65. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00227-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. The orbitofrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351:1433–1443. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0128. discussion 1443–1434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:11–29. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00277-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Hornak J, Wade D, McGrath J. Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57:1518–1524. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.57.12.1518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossion B, Gauthier I. How does the brain process upright and inverted faces? Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2002;1:63–75. doi: 10.1177/1534582302001001004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rugg MD, Otten LJ, Henson RN. The neural basis of episodic memory: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2002;357:1097–1110. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Norman KA, Koutstaal W. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. Annu Rev Psychol. 1998;49:289–318. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnider A. Spontaneous confabulation and the adaptation of thought to ongoing reality. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:662–671. doi: 10.1038/nrn1179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnider A, Valenza N, Morand S, Michel CM. Early cortical distinction between memories that pertain to ongoing reality and memories that don't. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:54–61. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.1.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweinberger SR, Pickering EC, Jentzsch I, Burton AM, Kaufmann JM. Event-related brain potential evidence for a response of inferior temporal cortex to familiar face repetitions. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2002;14:398–409. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00142-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stemmer B, Segalowitz SJ, Witzke W, Schönle PW. Error detection in patients with lesions to the medial prefrontal cortex: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:118–130. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00121-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuss DT, Alexander MP, Floden D, Binns MA, Levine B, McIntosh AR, Rajah N, Hevenor SJ. Fractionation and localization of distinct frontal lobe processes: evidence from focal lesions in humans. In: Stuss DT, Knight RT, editors. Principles of frontal lobe functions. New York: Oxford UP; 2002. pp. 392–407. [Google Scholar]
- Swick D, Knight RT. Contributions of prefrontal cortex to recognition memory: electrophysiological and behavioral evidence. Neuropsychology. 1999;13:155–170. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.13.2.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JR, Olichney JM. From amnesia to dementia: ERP studies of memory and language. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2007;38:8–17. doi: 10.1177/155005940703800106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tranel D, Damasio AR. Knowledge without awareness: an autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics. Science. 1985;228:1453–1454. doi: 10.1126/science.4012303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woods DL, Knight RT. Electrophysiologic evidence of increased distractibility after dorsolateral prefrontal lesions. Neurology. 1986;36:212–216. doi: 10.1212/wnl.36.2.212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]