Abstract
Objective
The goal of this study was to examine the independent and interactive roles of harsh-intrusive maternal behaviors and children's executive function in the development of internalizing behaviors across the first years of school.
Method
A diverse sample (58% African American; 42% European American) of 137 children (48% females) was followed from kindergarten (age 5 years) through school entry (ages 6 and 7 years). At age 5 years, maternal harsh-intrusive parenting behaviors were rated from a mother-child structured play task and children completed three executive function tasks that measured inhibitory control, working memory and attention set-shifting. Teachers reported on children's internalizing behaviors at ages 5, 6 and 7 years.
Results
Harsh-intrusive parenting behaviors were positively related to internalizing behaviors, whereas high executive function abilities were related to lower internalizing behaviors at school entry. Additionally, executive function buffered the association between parenting behaviors and internalizing behaviors such that the link between harsh-intrusive parenting and child internalizing behaviors was evident only among children with low executive function, and not among children with high executive function.
Conclusions
Interventions that focus on reducing negative parenting behaviors and improving children's executive function may prevent internalizing behaviors from increasing during times of social and academic challenge.
Keywords: Internalizing behaviors, intrusive parenting, executive function, risk/protective factors
Internalizing behaviors (IBs; anxiety, depression, withdrawal) are among the most common forms of psychopathology during childhood and adolescence (Brumariu & Kerns, 2010). These behaviors may originate in early childhood (Tandon, Cardeli, & Luby, 2009), and their prevalence increases over time (Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, 2003). Because IBs are related to multiple aspects of maladaptive functioning, including decreased psychosocial and academic functioning and an increased risk for substance abuse and suicide (Birmaher et al., 1996), research that elucidates risk and resilience factors in the developmental pathways of these behaviors is of critical importance. IBs often begin to present in early childhood, and longitudinal studies show that highly controlling, harsh, and intrusive parenting behaviors contribute to their emergence and stability (Hastings et al., 2015; McLeod, Weisz, & Wood, 2007; McLeod, Wood, & Weisz, 2007; Rubin, Burgess, & Hastings, 2002), and increase the risk of clinical presentations such as social anxiety disorder (Lewis-Morrarty et al., 2012). Thus, targeting parenting behaviors and factors that may mitigate their negative effects during this period may prevent IBs from increasing during times of social and academic challenge, such as the transition to formal schooling. What remains under-studied in the developmental and clinical literature is the extent to which child characteristics may moderate the link between parenting and eventual IBs, a line of study which has potential for informing and developing critical intervention and prevention programs.
Efficient self-regulation abilities may serve to protect children and promote resilience under conditions of risk (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). Previous research has found that temperamental qualities (e.g., high effortful control, high positive emotionality, low fearfulness), cognitive abilities (e.g., high verbal ability), and physiological regulation (e.g., Autonomic Functioning) can mitigate the negative effects of environmental risk on the development of IBs (Flouri, Midouhas & Joshi, 2014; Gallagher, 2002; Lenuga, Bush, Long, Kovacs, & Trancik, 2008; Lengua , Wolchik , Sandler, & West, 2000; Muhtadie, Zhou, Eisenberg, & Wang, 2013; Wagner, Propper, Gueron-Sela, & Mills-Koonce, 2016). However, to the best of our knowledge the role of children’s executive function (EF) as a protective factor has not yet been explored. EF abilities are related to successful behavioral and emotional adaptation during the transition to school (Hughes & Ensor, 2011). Furthermore, EF is considered a potentially modifiable protective factor that can be effectively improved by the implementation of intervention programs in the school context (Diamond & Lee, 2011) Thus, in the current study we will examine the role of children's EF as a buffer against the negative implications of harsh-intrusive parenting behaviors on children’s IBs during the first years of school.
Parenting Behaviors and IBs
Parental behaviors such as withdrawal, hostility, negative emotional expressivity and low autonomy granting have been related to higher levels of children’s IBs (McLoad et al., 2007; Muhtadie et al., 2013; Valiente et al., 2006). These behaviors may evoke distress in children, and lead to the development of negative cognitions such as reduced control over threat and a perception of social relationships as untrustworthy and dangerous (Bayer, Sanson, & Hemphill, 2006). Parental harshness and intrusion in particular may maintain and aggravate children’s IBs by denying opportunities for exploration, constraining the growth of autonomy, and restricting the acquisition of adaptive coping skills with stressful events (Barlow, 1988). The effects of these negative parental behaviors on the course of IBs may be particularly salient during the first years of school, a time period characterized by increased social and academic demands that require children to frequently employ coping skills and self-regulatory abilities (Blair et al., 2007). Indeed, research has shown that harsh-intrusive parenting behaviors during the preschool period were associated with exacerbation of IBs over the transition from preschool- to school-age (Hastings et al., 2015).
Child EF and IBs
There is also evidence that certain self-regulatory abilities are related to children's IBs (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2009; Hughes & Ensor, 2011; Muhtadie et al., 2013; Valiente et al., 2006). For example, effortful control (EC), a temperamental quality that refers to the ability to inhibit a dominant response and initiate a subdominant response (Posner & Rothbart, 2000), has been related to children's IBs (Eisenberg et al., 2009; Muhtadie et al., 2013; Valiente et al., 2006). Specifically, deficits in EC have been related to more IBs over time, indicating that poor regulatory abilities may increase initial levels of IBs (Eisenberg et al., 2009; Valiente et al., 2006).
Less is known about the links between children's EF and IBs. EF refer to a set of cognitive control processes (i.e., inhibitory control, working memory and set-shifting) which regulate lower level automatic processes, allowing individuals to plan, prioritize and sequence their actions (Miyake & Friedman, 2012). Individual differences in EF are considered to be genetic in origin (Miyake & Friedman, 2012), although there is a growing body of evidence suggesting that early caregiving experiences also play an important role in the development of children's EF. For example, higher levels of maternal sensitivity during toddlerhood were related to later improved performance on EF tasks (e.g., Bernier, Carlson, Deschênes, & Matte-Gagné, 2012; author citation), whereas harsh-intrusive mother-child interactions were negatively related to children’s EF (e.g., Blair et al., 2011).
There are multiple domains that fall under the category of EF, including inhibitory control (i.e., the ability to inhibit automatic, or prepotent responses to facilitate task completion), which shares common characteristics with EC described above. Both constructs have been related to children’s self-regulation and adaptive social functioning (Zhou, Chen, & Main, 2012). However, EF includes two additional cognitive control processes that may facilitate children’s ability to utilize adaptive self-regulatory skills. The first is working memory, the ability to maintain and manipulate information over brief periods of time. Working memory and inhibitory control support one another such that the ability to hold a rule in mind and manipulate it, based on environmental demands, increases the likelihood that this information will guide children’s behavior and decrease inhibitory error (Diamond, 2013). The second additional EF process is set-shifting, or cognitive flexibility, which refers to the ability to shift between mental states, operations, or tasks to adjust to new demands (Diamond, 2013; Miyake & Friedman, 2012). High set-shifting ability may facilitate goal directed behavior and self-regulation by allowing individuals to abandon suboptimal means (e.g., obstructed or costly means) and pursue alternative means to reach a desired goal (Hofmann, Schmeichel, & Baddeley, 2012).
To the best of our knowledge only one study has tested the longitudinal associations between child EF and IBs across the transition to formal schooling (Hughes & Ensor, 2011). This study found that EF improvements in children's EF from age 4 to 6 years were related to less IBs at school entry (Hughes & Ensor, 2011). However, this study did not control for earlier levels of children's IBs, precluding the examination of change in children's IBs over time. In this study we expand the findings reported by Hughes & Ensor (2011) by testing whether high EF prior to school entry are related to a decrease in IBs across the transition to school. We suggest that because high EF abilities enable better adaptation to both social and academic aspects of the school context (Blair, 2002), they can consequently improve behavioral adjustment and decrease IBs.
The Moderating Role of EF
Research indicates that self-regulatory abilities may also protect children against the negative effect of environmental risk factors on IBs (Flouri et al., 2014; Lenuga et al., 2008; Muhtadie et al., 2013), suggesting that the interaction between dispositional and environmental factors may contribute to the stability or exacerbation of IBs over time (Hastings et al., 2015). For example, Lenuga et al. (2008) found that maternal risk (e.g., adolescent parent status, maternal depression) and environmental risk (i.e., quality of the home and neighborhood environment) predicted children’s mean level and growth in IBs through middle childhood only for children with low EC, and not for children with high EC (Lenuga et al., 2008). These findings suggest that children with high effortful control may be better able to employ adaptive coping strategies in the face of stress that mitigate the negative effects of high risk environments, reducing the negative emotions elicited by such risk.
The role of EF as a protective factor against the negative effects of environmental risk on IBs has not been examined thus far. However, there is evidence that EF processes can further support the ability to gain control over reactivity to stressful experiences, and as such buffer the negative implications of harsh-intrusive caregiving environments on behavioral adjustment. For example, set-shifting abilities have been negatively correlated with rumination, the tendency to passively focus on negative mood and problems (Lissnyder, Koster, Derakshan, & De Raedt, 2010). Similarly, higher working memory capacity has been related to an increased ability to suppress ruminative thoughts and to down-regulate undesired and inappropriate affective expressions (Hofmann et al., 2012). Thus, children with high EF abilities may be better able to modulate their negative affect associated with exposure to harsh-intrusive caregiving environments by preventing them from carrying over their perceptions of danger and threat from the home environment and enabling them to flexibly change their expectations and behaviors in other contexts, such as the school environment.
The Current Study
The current study aimed to expand the literature by examining the link between child EF, a broad measure of cognitive regulatory abilities, and the development of IBs across the first years of school. We examined both the direct effect of EF on children's IBs and the role of EF as a protective factor against the negative effects of harsh-intrusive parenting on subsequent child IBs. Further, the current study included independent observers of child behavior and parenting in order to avoid maternal reporting bias. The inclusion of observed parenting behaviors at age 5 years and teacher report of child IBs at Kindergarten, 1st, and 2nd grade, provides a unique and objective assessment of these relationships over time.
We hypothesized that mothers’ harsh-intrusive parenting behaviors observed while children were in Kindergarten (age 5 years) will be associated with increased levels of IBs from Kindergarten to the first years of school (1st and 2nd grade; ages 6 and 7 years). In addition, children's EF in Kindergarten will associated with decreased levels of IBs in the first years of school. Finally, child EF at age 5 years will moderate the link between parenting and IBs, such that harsh-intrusive parenting will predict elevated IBs only among children with low EF and not among children with high EF.
Methods
Participants
Participants in this study were a subsample of the Durham Child Health and Development Study (DCHDS), a longitudinal study of 206 socioeconomically and racially diverse families living in and around a midsized southeastern city in the US. The participants were full-term, healthy infants that were recruited at age 3 months using fliers and postings at birth and parenting centers or through phone contact via birth records. Participants were recruited according to a stratified sampling plan in an effort to assemble a sample with approximately equal numbers of European American and African American families from low- and middle-income groups. The subsample used in the current study included families in which the participating child was administered an EF battery at age 60 months (n=137). In this subsample, 48% of the children were female, 58% were African American (41.6% were European American), and approximately 42% of the sample was low income (below 200% of the poverty level). This subsample did not differ significantly from the complete sample on any of these variables. Children’s EF and maternal parenting behaviors were assessed during laboratory at age 5 years, and children’s IBs was assessed by teachers at ages 5, 6 and 7 years.
Measures
Harsh-intrusive parenting
During the lab visit at age 5 years, mothers and their children completed two interactive tasks which lasted a total of 15 minutes. The first task involved building towers with wooden blocks and the second was a card game called “Slap Jack” in which mothers and their child competed to win cards. Interactions were videotaped and coded based on coding schemes that were used in additional studies with ethnically and socio-economically diverse samples, such as the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD; 1997) and the Family Life Project (Blair et al., 2011). The current study used the negative regard / hostility and the respect for child autonomy scales, that were both rated on a 7-point scale ranging from “1” (very low) to “7” (very high). The negative regard scale rated mother's frequency and intensity of negative affect toward the child. Some markers of negative regard included: disapproval, negative voice when correcting, tense facial muscles and strained expression, threatening the child or punishing without explanation, roughness and calling the child unflattering names. The respect for child autonomy scale rated the degree to which the mother acted in a way that recognized and respected the validity of the child's individuality, motives, and perspectives. A high score on this scale represented mothers who interacted with their child in a way that acknowledged the validity of the child’s perspective, encouraged the child to acknowledge his/her intentions, and to negotiate the course of interactions in the session. A low score on this scale represented mothers who denied the child’s individuality and displayed minimal support and/or pervasive intrusion (e.g., interrupted the child, did things before the child can on his/her own, exerted her own expectations/agenda on the child). The negative regard and respect for child autonomy scales were significantly correlated (r=-.52, p<.001). Based on previous factor analysis within this sample (Mills-Koonce et al., 2009), a composite measure of harsh-intrusive parenting was created by averaging the negative regard and respect for autonomy (reversed score) scales.
Trained coders, who were unaware of other information about the families, rated the interactions for maternal negative hostility and respect for child autonomy. Two lead graduate student coders trained all other coders until acceptable reliability (ICC > 0.80) was reached for each coder on every scale. In addition, two highly experienced coders double-coded 30% of randomly selected cases and an inter-rater reliability of ICC > 0.80 was maintained throughout the coding process. In the case of disagreements between coders in the double coded cases, consensus scores were reached by consulting with one of the two lead graduate students.
Child EF
Children’s executive function was measured with 3 widely used tasks that were administered to the child at 5 years of age. Children completed the day/night task to assess inhibitory control (DNT; Gerstadt, Hong, & Diamond, 1994), the backward digit span task to assess working memory (BS; McCarthy, 1972), and the Flexible Item Selection Task to assess set-shifting (FIST; Jacques & Zelazo, 2001). A detailed description of these tasks can be found in Gueron-Sela et al. (2017). These three tasks have been widely used to measure children's EF, including in ethnically diverse low income samples (e.g., Marcovitch et al., 2010; Blair et al., 2011).
The extant literature has characterized the structure of EF during adulthood in two main ways (Miyake & Friedman, 2012) including a unity (i.e., all three components tap on a common underlying ability) and a diversity approach (i.e., the components also show some degree of separability). Due to recent evidence suggesting that preschool EF is best described as a single unitary factor (Hughes, Ensor, Wilson, & Graham, 2009; Willoughby et al., 2012), the current study adopted the unity approach and refer to EF as a single factor that includes children's performance on tasks that assess inhibitory control, working memory and attention set-shifting. Previous factor analytic work with this sample suggests that the 3 EF tasks load on one latent EF factor in a structural equation framework (Nesbitt, Baker-Ward, & Willoughby, 2013). Thus, an unweighted mean of the z-standardized scores on the three measures was used in the current analyses.
Child internalizing behaviors
Teacher reports of children’s internalizing behaviors were assessed with the Child Behavior Checklist Teacher’s Report Form (CBCL-TRF; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) that was obtained by mail at ages 5, 6 and 7 years. The CBCL-TRF has been widely used in community studies, including samples of low income African American children (e.g., Smith, 2001). The internalizing scale included the items from the withdrawn, somatic complaints, and anxious/depressed scales.
The correlation between the IBs scores at 6 and 7 years was r = .40, p =.002. To capture overall IBs during the early school years and to improve the reliability of measurement, we averaged children's raw IBs scores from ages 6 and 7 years to create a composite of IBs at this time period (n = 101). Children's IBs at age 5 years was included in the analysis to control for initial levels of IBs.
Covariates
Because the sample was diverse in terms of SES and ethnicity, we included child ethnicity and family income-to-needs ratio (determined using the mother’s or primary caregiver’s report of the total family yearly income at the first grade visit, the size of the family, and the 2003 federal poverty guidelines) as covariates in all analyses. We also chose to control for child sex because it has also been related to all key variables in the study including IBs, negative parenting behvaiors and child EF (Hastings et al., 2015; Willoughby & Blair, 2016).
Missing Data
Of the 137 children who consisted the sample of the current study, teacher reports on IBs were obtained for 59 children at age 5 years, 76 children at age 6 years and 83 children at age 7 years. Because the age 6 and age 7 assessments of IBs were combined, a total of 101 children had an age 6-7 years IBs composite score. The main reasons for missing data were difficulty in contacting the teachers and their unwillingness to fill out the questionnaires. Children with missing teacher reports at ages 5 and 7 years had significantly lower family income-to-needs ratios compared to children without missing data t(134)=2.88, p=.005; t(134)=2.95, p=.004, respectively. No differences were found between children with and without missing teacher data in terms of ethnicity and child sex distributions. Further, 124 children had data on harsh-intrusive parenting. No differences were found between children with and without missing mother-child interaction data in terms of family income, ethnicity and child sex distributions.
To account for missing data, we utilized a full maximum likelihood (FIML) estimator for all analyses. FIML is well recognized as an effective method for analyzing longitudinal data with moderate to large amounts of missing data, and has been demonstrated to provide less biased parameter estimates than other commonly used techniques, such as listwise deletion (Enders, 2013; Widaman, 2006). FIML is particularly effective and recommended compared to other missing data procedures when variables related to missingness can be included in analytic models (e.g., family income; Widaman, 2006). Because FIML procedures allow for the use of all available data from each participant, the full sample of n = 137 was retained in all primary analyses.
Analytic Strategy
The primary analytic strategy involved estimating a series of regression models using AMOS 23 software. Continuous predictors were centered prior to the creation of the interaction terms. Significant interactions were probed using the online utility and computational tools for probing interactions (Roisman et al., 2012). Specifically, significant interactions were probed by estimating simple slopes at ±1 SD of child EF, followed by regions of significance (RoS) analysis. The RoS analysis computes and graphically represents the specific upper and lower values of both the moderator variable (i.e., child EF) and the predictor (i.e., harsh-intrusive parenting) in which the simple slopes are significantly different from zero. Consequently, this approach allows for much greater precision in identifying the moderating effects than the conventional representation of the slopes (Roisman et al., 2012). For example, the RoS enable to detect the specific level of EF abilities that are required to buffer the effects of harsh-intrusive parenting on IBs.
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Table 1 presents the bivariate correlations, means, and standard deviations for the model covariates and variables of interest. Harsh-intrusive parenting at age 5 years was positively correlated with children’s IBs at ages 6 and 7 years and with family income-to-needs. Further, harsh-intrusive parenting was positively correlated with child ethnicity, such that mothers of African American children had higher scores on the harsh-intrusive parenting scale than mothers of European American children. Children’s EF at age 5 years was negatively related to later IBs and to child ethnicity, such that African American children had lower EF scores that European American children. Finally, family income-to-needs was positively related to children's EF.
Table 1. Zero-order bivariate correlations between study variables and covariates.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Child Ethnicity (0 = European-American ) | - | ||||||
2. Child Sex (0=male) | .08 | - | |||||
3. Household Income to Needs Ratio | -.30*** | -.03 | - | ||||
4. EF composite (60m) | -.29*** | -.00 | .24** | - | |||
5. Intrusive Parenting (60m) | .37*** | -.00 | -.26** | -.15' | |||
6. IB (60m) | -.06 | -.06 | -.07 | -.07 | -.00 | ||
7. IB (72m, 84m) | .04 | -.13 | -.11 | -.33** | .24* | .46** | |
N | 137 | 137 | 136 | 137 | 124 | 59 | 101 |
Mean | - | - | 4.55 | .00 | 2.81 | 3.06 | 3.47 |
Standard Deviation | - | - | 3.48 | .69 | 1.39 | 4.05 | 3.56 |
Notes.
p<.10′, p < .05*, p < .01**, p < .001***;
m=child age in months.
Model Results
We estimated a series of regression models to test the direct and interactive associations between harsh-intrusive parenting and children’s EF at age 5 years in the prediction of IBs at ages 6 to 7 years (Table 2). First, child IBs were regressed on model covariates (R2 = .34). Only child IBs at age 5 years was a significant predictor of later IB (β=.56, p<.001, 95% CI = .261, .762). Next, in model 2 intrusive parenting and the child EF were added. In this model, intrusive parenting (β = .26, p = .019, 95% CI = .191, 1.337), child EF (β = -.25, p = .008, 95% CI = -2.438, -0.283) and IBs at 60 months (β = .52, p<.001, 95% CI = .215,.713) were significant predictors of children’s later IB (R2 =.44). Finally, in model 3 the interaction term was entered, and a significant interaction was observed between child EF and intrusive parenting (β = -.20, p = .047, 95% CI = -1.725,.141) in the prediction of children’s IBs. The final model accounted for 49% of the variance in children’s IBs (see Table 2).
Table 2. Regression analysis using the EF composite as a moderator of the link between intrusive parenting and children’s later IB.
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | |
---|---|---|---|
β (SE) | β (SE) | β (SE) | |
Child Race | .10(.73) | -.01(.74) | -.00(.72) |
Child Sex | -.13(.67) | -.12(.62) | -.12(.60) |
Household Income to Needs Ratio | -.01(.09) | .08(.08) | .08(.08) |
IB (60m) | .56***(.09) | .52***(.08) | .50***(.08) |
Intrusive Parenting (60m) | .26*(.28) | .24*(.27) | |
Child EF Composite (60m) | -.25**(.49) | -.28**(.49) | |
Child EF X Intrusive Parenting | -.20*(.43) | ||
R² | .34 | .44 | .49 |
Note.
p <.05*, p < .01**, p < .001***
Simple slopes and RoS analyses
The significant interaction between child EF and harsh-intrusive parenting was probed at high (+1SD) and low (-1SD) levels of child EF (Figure 1). The positive association between intrusive parenting and children’s IBs was significant only for children with low EF (simple slope = 1.23, t = 3.09, p = .002), and not for children with high EF (simple slope = .02, t = .06, ns). Regions of significance analysis (RoS) indicated that when children had EF scores below .08 (approximately the sample mean of zero), harsh-intrusive parenting was significantly related to children's IBs. We also estimated simple slopes for high (+1SD) and low (-1SD) levels of intrusive parenting and found that the association between child EF and IBs was significant under high [simple slope = -2.65, t = 3.05, p = .003] but not under low [simple slope = -.25, t = .36, ns] levels of intrusive parenting. RoS indicated that when intrusive parenting scores were above -.54 (slightly below the mean) children with low EF had significantly more IBs than children with high EF.
Discussion
The current study sought to examine how harsh-intrusive parenting behaviors and children's EF contribute to the exacerbation or amelioration of IBs across the first years of formal schooling, in an ethnically and socio-economically diverse sample of mothers and their children. Given that IBs are related to multiple aspects of maladaptive functioning during adolescence (Birmaher et al., 1996), identifying dysfunctional parenting behaviors and factors that may buffer their negative effects during early childhood is an important step for preventing IBs from increasing during challenging transitions across development. This study focused on the protective role of children's EF, because they enable self-regulation and self-directed behaviors that allow individuals to adaptively cope with novel and possibly stressful situations (Snyder, Miyake, & Hankin, 2015).
Consistent with previous literature (Eisenberg et al., 2009; Hughes & Ensor, 2011; McLoad et al., 2007), higher levels of harsh-intrusive parenting in Kindergarten were related to increased levels of IBs at school entry, whereas high EF abilities were related to a decrease in IBs. However, EF also moderated the association between early harsh-intrusive parenting and IBs across the first years of school. Specifically, the link between maternal harsh-intrusive parenting behaviors and children's later IBs was evident only among children with low EF abilities, but not among children with high EF abilities. As demonstrated in Figure 1, whereas children with high EF exposed to harsh parenting exhibited no IBs, children with low EF exhibited approximately 1.5 SD more IBs, which is approximately 1SD above the mean of the current sample. In a non-clinical sample differences of 1 SD above the mean may be meaningful and are likely to pose challenges in the context of typical development including peer difficulties. Our findings are in line with previous studies that identified self-regulatory abilities such as EC as protective factors against the negative effect of environmental risk factors on IBs (Lenuga et al., 2008). Children with high EF abilities may be better able to regulate their negative emotions elicited by harsh-intrusive parenting behaviors and employ more adaptive coping strategies. For example, high working memory abilities are related to a better ability to suppress ruminative thoughts, and down-regulate negative affect by supporting active representations of goals (Hofmann, Schmeichel, & Baddeley, 2012). Children with high EF abilities may be able to actively maintain adaptive social goals, facilitating their ability to overcome feelings of depression and anxiety that are associated with harsh-intrusive parenting, and pursue adaptive social bonds in the school context. In addition, EF are related to the ability to flexibly shift attention in response to environmental demands, which is thought to play an important role in adaptive regulation of negative emotions (White, McDermott, Degnan, Henderson, & Fox, 2011). Efficient set-shifting abilities can enable children to abandon dysfunctional social cognitions and behaviors that characterize the mother-child social dynamic and pursue alternative behaviors that are more adaptive for establishing adaptive social relations with peers.
It is also likely that children with low EF are more vulnerable to the negative effects of harsh-intrusive parenting behaviors on the development of IBs. This notion is consistent with the diathesis-stress model (Monroe & Simons, 1991), suggesting that children with inherent vulnerabilities are likely to exhibit more difficulties when raised in adverse contexts, but will develop similarly to children without vulnerabilities in the absence of environmental adversity. Indeed, the RoS analysis indicates that children with low EF had significantly more IBs than children with high EF when exposed to high levels of harsh-intrusive parenting, but these differences were no longer present when exposed to low levels of harsh-intrusive parenting. These findings are consistent with previous studies showing that children with low EC are particularly vulnerable to the effects of adverse rearing environments on behavior problems (Choe, Olson, & Sameroff, 2014).
An additional explanation for these findings may be that for children with good EF, the transition to school opens up new opportunities to incorporate their EF skills into their daily behaviors in the school context, such as following directions, controlling motor activity, and attending to lessons. Children who were high on IBs in Kindergarten but have high EF abilities may thus experience the transition to school as a positive, confidence-promoting experience that reduces IB in the school context. Conversely, IBs could be exacerbated in the face of the increasing social and academic demands for self-regulated behaviors among children with low EF abilities. Examining the role of self-esteem and self-efficacy in the link between EF and IBs could be an important next step in understanding this process.
The results from the current study should be considered in light of a few limitations. First, EF abilities were measured using “cold” EF tasks, that introduce neutral or very low emotional salience. Although it is possible that these EF abilities generalize to situations in which social-emotional content is present, future research should further assess how EF in an emotional context (i.e., “hot” EF tasks) may be related to the reduction of IBs for children exposed to harsh-intrusive parenting environments. Second, there was considerable amount of missing data in the teachers' report of children's IBs., particularly at the first time point (age 5 years). Although we employed appropriate statistical measures to account for the missing data, including the use of a FIML estimator in the analysis and inclusion of demographic covariates that were associated with missing data, it is possible that additional factors that were not measured in the study were related to rates of missing data. For example, teacher characteristics such as burn out and fatigue may have precluded teachers from filling out the questionnaires and as a result only children with highly motivated teachers had full IBs data. Finally, the current study used a community sample rather than a clinical sample of children with IBs. While the use of a community sample in this study expands generalizability, our ability to predict IBs at clinically meaningful levels, as well as the extent to which the findings can be directly compared to and integrated with studies using clinical samples is limited. It is therefore imperative to examine whether children's EF have a protective role in the context of clinical levels of IBs. This would be a necessary step in translating these findings into intervention programs.
Conclusions and Clinical Implications
Findings from this study demonstrate that both endogenous and exogenous factors are related to children's development of IBs, suggesting that there are multiple ports of entry for intervention that may be leveraged. For example, the Turtle Program (Chronis-Tuscano et al., 2015), which involves both in vivo coaching of parents and social-skills training for children in the context of a peer group is a promising new intervention program that specifically targets children at risk for IBs (Chronis-Tuscano et al., 2015). Children at risk for IBs may also benefit from intervention programs that specifically improve EF. Diamond & Lee (2011) suggest that children's EF can be effectively improved by targeting EF abilities in the context of emotional and social development (e.g., verbalizing feelings and practicing conscious self-control strategies) and incorporating physical activities (e.g., aerobics, martial arts, and yoga) that emphasize self-control, perseverance and planning. These interventions could be effective in setting children with early IBs on a different trajectory towards better social-emotional functioning.
In summary, this study extends the extant literature by using multi-method longitudinal data to test the premise that children’s EF moderate the relations between mothers’ harsh-intrusive parenting and children’s IBs at the first years of formal schooling. The findings demonstrate that children's high EF may act as a protective factor against the negative implications of unsupportive caregiving on the development of IBs. The pattern of findings raises a number of additional questions. For example, it is well established that temperamental traits, such as behavioral inhibition, are also related to the later development of IBs and that behaviorally inhibited children are particularly vulnerable to the effects of parenting behaviors on changes in IBs over time (e.g., Rubin et al., 2002; Williams et al., 2009). In future work it would be useful to examine how the interactions between behavioral inhibition, EF and harsh-intrusive parenting impact the development of IBs.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the North Carolina Child Development Research Collaborative, funded by the National Science Foundation through a Children’s Research Initiative grant #BCS-0126475. Follow-up data collection from the sample has been funded by an Integrative Research Activities for Developmental Science (IRADS) grant from the National Science Foundation (BCS-0720660).
References
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Bayer JK, Sanson AV, Hemphill SA. Parent influences on early childhood internalizing difficulties. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2006;27:542–559. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.08.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow DH. Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic. New York: Guilford press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Bernier A, Carlson SM, Deschênes M, Matte-Gagné C. Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: A closer look at the caregiving environment. Developmental Science. 2012;15:12–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01093.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birmaher B, Ryan ND, Williamson DE, Brent DA, Kaufman J, Dahl RE, et al. Nelson B. Childhood and adolescent depression: a review of the past 10 years. Part I. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35:1427–1439. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199611000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair C. School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. American Psychologist. 2002;57:111–127. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair C, Granger DA, Willoughby M, Mills-Koonce R, Cox M, Greenberg MT, et al. Fortunato CK. Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Development. 2011;82:1970–1984. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brumariu LE, Kerns KA. Parent-child attachment and internalizing symptoms in childhood and adolescence: A review of empirical findings and future directions. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:177–203. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choe DE, Olson SL, Sameroff AJ. Effortful control moderates bidirectional effects between children's externalizing behavior and their mothers' depressive symptoms. Child Development. 2014;85:643–658. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chronis-Tuscano A, Rubin KH, O’Brien KA, Coplan RJ, Thomas SR, Dougherty LR, et al. Menzer M. Preliminary evaluation of a multimodal early intervention program for behaviorally inhibited preschoolers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2015;83:534–540. doi: 10.1037/a0039043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costello EJ, Mustillo S, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Angold A. Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:837–844. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond A. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology. 2013;64:135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333:959–964. doi: 10.1126/science.1204529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Lissnyder E, Koster EH, Derakshan N, De Raedt R. The association between depressive symptoms and executive control impairments in response to emotional and non-emotional information. Cognition and Emotion. 2010;24:264–280. doi: 10.1080/02699930903378354. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Spinrad TL, Cumberland A, Liew J, Reiser M, et al. Losoya SH. Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45:988–1008. doi: 10.1037/a0016213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enders CK. Dealing with missing data in developmental research. Child Development Perspectives. 2013;7:27–31. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Flouri E, Midouhas E, Joshi H. Family poverty and trajectories of children’s emotional and behavioural problems: the moderating roles of self-regulation and verbal cognitive ability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2014;42:1043–1056. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9848-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher KC. Does child temperament moderate the influence of parenting on adjustment? Developmental Review. 2002;22:623–643. doi: 10.1016/S0273-2297(02)00503-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gerstadt CL, Hong YJ, Diamond A. The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 312–7 years old on a stroop-like day-night test. Cognition. 1994;53:129–153. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gueron-Sela N, Wagner NJ, Propper CB, Mills-Koonce WR, Moore GA, Cox MJ. The interaction between child respiratory sinus arrhythmia and early sensitive parenting in the prediction of children’s executive functions. Infancy. 2017;22(2):171–189. doi: 10.1111/infa.12152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hastings PD, Helm J, Mills RS, Serbin LA, Stack DM, Schwartzman AE. Dispositional and environmental predictors of the development of internalizing problems in childhood: Testing a multilevel model. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2015;43:831–845. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9951-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16:174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes C, Ensor R. Does executive function matter for preschoolers’ problem behaviors? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:1–14. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9107-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes C, Ensor R. Individual differences in growth in executive function across the transition to school predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors and self-perceived academic success at 6 years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011;108:663–676. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes C, Ensor R, Wilson A, Graham A. Tracking executive function across the transition to school: A latent variable approach. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2009;35:20–36. doi: 10.1080/87565640903325691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacques S, Zelazo P. The Flexible Item Selection Task (FIST): A measure of executive function in preschoolers. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2001;20:573–591. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2003_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lengua LJ, Bush NR, Long AC, Kovacs EA, Trancik AM. Effortful control as a moderator of the relation between contextual risk factors and growth in adjustment problems. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:509–528. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lengua LJ, Wolchik SA, Sandler IN, West SG. The additive and interactive effects of parenting and temperament in predicting adjustment problems of children of divorce. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 2000;29:232–244. doi: 10.1207/S15374424jccp2902_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis-Morrarty E, Degnan KA, Chronis-Tuscano A, Rubin KH, Cheah CS, Pine DS, et al. Fox NA. Maternal over-control moderates the association between early childhood behavioral inhibition and adolescent social anxiety symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2012;40:1363–1373. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9663-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS, Coatsworth JD. The development of competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: Lessons from research on successful children. American Psychologist. 1998;53:205. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcovitch S, Leigh J, Calkins SD, Leerks EM, O'brien M, Blankson AN. Moderate vagal withdrawal in 3.5-year-old children is associated with optimal performance on executive function tasks. Developmental Psychobiology. 2010;52:603–608. doi: 10.1002/dev.20462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy D. Manual for the McCarthy scales of children's abilities. Psychological Corporation; 1972. [Google Scholar]
- McLeod BD, Weisz JR, Wood JJ. Examining the association between parenting and childhood depression: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2007;27:986–1003. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLeod BD, Wood JJ, Weisz JR. Examining the association between parenting and childhood anxiety: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2007;27:155–172. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake A, Friedman NP. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2012;21:8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mills-Koonce WR, Propper C, Gariepy JL, Barnett M, Moore GA, Calkins S, Cox MJ. Psychophysiological correlates of parenting behavior in mothers of young children. Developmental Psychobiology. 2009;51(8):650–661. doi: 10.1002/dev.20400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monroe SM, Simons AD. Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life-stress research: Implications for depressive disorders. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;110:406–425. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.110.3.406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muhtadie L, Zhou Q, Eisenberg N, Wang Y. Predicting internalizing problems in Chinese children: The unique and interactive effects of parenting and child temperament. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25:653–667. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NICHD SECCYD. The effects of infant child care on infant-mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD [National Institute of Child Health and Human Development] Study of Early Child Care. Child Development. 1997;68:860–879. doi: 10.2307/1132038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nesbitt KT, Baker-Ward L, Willoughby MT. Executive function mediates socio-economic and racial differences in early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2013;28:774–783. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.07.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:427–441. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400003096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roisman GI, Newman DA, Fraley RC, Haltigan JD, Groh AM, Haydon KC. Distinguishing differential susceptibility from diathesis–stress: Recommendations for evaluating interaction effects. Development and Psychopathology. 2012;24:389–409. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin KH, Burgess KB, Hastings PD. Stability and social–behavioral consequences of toddlers’ inhibited temperament and parenting behaviors. Child Development. 2002;73:483–495. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith M. Social and emotional competencies: Contributions to young African-American children's peer acceptance. Early Education and Development. 2001;12:49–72. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed1201_4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder HR, Miyake A, Hankin BL. Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:328. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tandon M, Cardeli E, Luby J. Internalizing disorders in early childhood: A review of depressive and anxiety disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2009;18:593–610. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valiente C, Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Reiser M, Cumberland A, Losoya SH, Liew J. Relations among mothers' expressivity, children's effortful control, and their problem behaviors: A four-year longitudinal study. Emotion. 2006;6:459–472. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner NJ, Propper C, Gueron-Sela N, Mills-Koonce WR. Dimensions of maternal parenting and infants’ autonomic functioning interactively predict early internalizing behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2016;44:459–470. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0039-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White LK, McDermott JM, Degnan KA, Henderson HA, Fox NA. Behavioral inhibition and anxiety: The moderating roles of inhibitory control and attention shifting. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39:735–747. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9490-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Widaman KF. III. Missing data: What to do with or without them. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2006;71:42–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2006.00404.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Williams LR, Degnan KA, Perez-Edgar KE, Henderson HA, Rubin KH, Pine DS, Steinberg L, Fox NA. Impact of behavioral inhibition and parenting style on internalizing and externalizing problems from early childhood through adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37:1063–1075. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9331-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willoughby MT, Blair CB. Longitudinal measurement of executive function in preschoolers. In: Griffen JA, McCardle P, Freund L, editors. Executive function in preschool age children: Integrating measurement, neurodevelopment, and translational research. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Willoughby MT, Wirth RJ, Blair CB. Executive function in early childhood: longitudinal measurement invariance and developmental change. Psychological Assessment. 2012;24:418. doi: 10.1037/a0025779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Q, Chen SH, Main A. Commonalities and differences in the research on children’s effortful control and executive function: A call for an integrated model of self- regulation. Child Development Perspectives. 2012;6:112–121. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00176.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]