Abstract
In spatial working memory tasks, pyramidal cells in the relevant cortical circuit receive massive inputs to shape stimulus location-selective sustained activity. A significant part of those inputs are applied onto the dendrites. Considering the dendritic morphology and circuit anatomy together with recently suggested branch-specific plasticity rules, external inputs transmitting the information of the stimulus location would be mapped onto some portion of the dendritic branches, rather than uniformly distributed over the branches. Meanwhile, recent studies revealed that each dendritic branch of pyramidal cell functions as a compartmentalized integration subunit through local spike generation and branch-specific excitation–inhibition interaction. I have examined how such nonlinear dendritic integration, combined with the nonuniform distribution of the external input, affects the behavior of the whole circuit by constructing a rate-coding model incorporating multiple dendritic branches of the individual pyramidal cell. Simulations varying the nature of dendritic nonlinearity and the configuration of somatically and dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition revealed that dendritic compartmentalization potentially enables the circuit to form an accurate memory depending on the contrast of the external input, but insensitively to its intensity, under certain conditions; in particular, when there exists tuned global dendritic recurrent inhibition or local dendritic inhibition coupled with global somatic inhibition. The model suggests that, when the circuit receives low-contrast or background input, only a small portion of dendritic branches of each pyramidal cell can overcome the local threshold so as to contribute to the somatic low-frequency firing, which in turn stabilizes the low-activity state of the circuit by recruiting recurrent inhibition.
Keywords: dendritic arborization, local circuit, recurrent inhibition, sustained activity, working memory, decision making, dendritic inhibition, microcircuit, neural network model, dendrite, spatial
Introduction
In working memory tasks, pyramidal cells in the relevant cortical circuit receive feedforward sensory inputs and recurrent inputs to shape stimulus-selective sustained activity. To reveal cellular mechanisms of working memory, it seems essential to understand how different types of inputs are integrated in individual pyramidal cells. Most excitatory inputs are applied onto the pyramidal dendrites. Regarding inhibition, although some types of inhibitory interneurons almost exclusively target cell bodies or axon initial segments of the pyramidal cells, many types target the dendrites, including the distal parts and the spines (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997; Somogyi et al., 1998; Markram et al., 2004). Although detailed distribution of inhibitory, as well as excitatory, inputs on the dendrites of the pyramidal cells in the working memory circuit remains elusive, existence of dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition has been suggested (González-Burgos et al., 2005).
Pyramidal dendrites have multiple branches, and synaptic integration thereon has been extensively studied (Rall, 1964; Koch et al., 1983; Segev and Parnas, 1983; Shepherd and Brayton, 1987; Mel, 1993; Segev, 1995; London and Häusser, 2005). An emerging view, originally advanced on theoretical grounds (Koch et al., 1983; Shepherd and Brayton, 1987; Poirazi et al., 2003a,b; Mel, 2007) but now supported by a considerable amount of experimental results (Häusser and Mel, 2003; Spruston et al., 2007), is that individual dendritic branches can operate as computational subunits, each of which implements compartmentalized nonlinear input integration by virtue of dendritic spike generation (Schiller et al., 2000; Polsky et al., 2004; Losonczy and Magee, 2006; Nevian et al., 2007) and branch-specific inhibition (Liu, 2004; Rhodes, 2006). Such dendritic compartmentalization in individual neurons can generally affect the collective behavior of the network activity (Spratling and Johnson, 2001, 2002; Goldman et al., 2003; Morita et al., 2007), and thus appears likely to affect the computational property of the cortical circuit for working memory.
Among various types of working memory, spatial working memory is one of the best studied systems. Based on the experimental results (Funahashi et al., 1989), fundamental organization of the underlying cortical circuit has been proposed (Goldman-Rakic, 1995) wherein the pyramidal cells with similar selectivity strongly excite each other to sustain their activity and simultaneously inhibit other cells via inhibitory interneurons. By incorporating such a basic network architecture, numerous modeling studies have elaborated the detailed biological mechanisms (Camperi and Wang, 1998; Compte et al., 2000; Renart et al., 2003; Constantinidis and Wang, 2004; Carter and Wang, 2007). However, thus far, possible effects of pyramidal dendritic compartmentalization have not been examined. Although some studies considered the pyramidal dendrites (Tegnér et al., 2002; Wang et al., 2004), they did not consider multiple branches. In this study, I have addressed this issue by constructing a neural circuit model incorporating pyramidal dendritic arborization and branch-specific nonlinear input integration. Through simulations, I show that dendritic compartmentalization potentially endows the circuit with the ability to form an accurate spatial memory depending on the contrast of the external feedforward input to the circuit, but insensitively to its intensity. I describe desired conditions, explore the underlying mechanism so as to derive testable predictions, and discuss possible behavioral relevance.
Materials and Methods
Network architecture.
I assumed that a neuronal circuit for spatial working memory exists in the cerebral cortex (Goldman-Rakic, 1995). The concerned excitatory pyramidal cells (see Fig. 1A–E,G, pyramids at bottom) were presumed to receive feedforward excitation (see Fig. 1A, red arrows) from the neurons in the preceding brain areas such as higher-order visual cortices. These “input layer neurons” (see Fig. 1A–E,G, circles at top) were assumed to have their own “preferred angles” (see Fig. 1A–E,G, arrows in the circles); thus, each neuron becomes transiently active on stimulus presentation at an angle that is close to its preferred angle. I referred to the population activity of these input layer neurons as the “input to the memory circuit” or the “external/feedforward input” (see Fig. 1A, orange bars at top), which therefore peaks around the stimulus angle. Through the feedforward connectivity that was assumed to be topographically organized in a functional sense, but not necessarily in a physical sense, the pyramidal cells in the memory circuit also have their own preferred angles (see Fig. 1A–E,G, arrows in the pyramids) (for more details, see below). Between the pyramidal cells, recurrent excitation via axonal collaterals (see Fig. 1B) and recurrent inhibition either via soma-targeting interneurons (see Fig. 1C) or via dendrite-targeting interneurons (see Fig. 1D) were assumed to exist (single cell population would target both the soma and the dendrite) (see below). Recurrent excitation was assumed to be organized such that the strength of synaptic coupling between two pyramidal cells decreases with the difference in their preferred angles (for details, see below). Somatically mediated recurrent inhibition was assumed to be unstructured (global). Dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition was assumed to be either global or local (see below). Inhibitory interneurons were not explicitly modeled but instead represented by negative signs preceding the connection strengths. Although soma-targeting and dendrite-targeting interneurons are schematically illustrated as separate cell populations in the figures, it might be more likely that a single cell population could implement both of them. Somatically mediated global recurrent inhibition could be implemented by large basket cells as assumed in a previous study (Wang et al., 2004). As for dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition, there seem to be many possibilities, given that there is a huge variety of neocortical GABAergic interneurons (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997; Somogyi et al., 1998; Markram et al., 2004) and most of them target pyramidal dendrites with various degree of preference [Kubota et al. (2007), their Fig. 4]. Among them, I would propose that basket cells (Gupta et al., 2000; Wang et al., 2002), at least certain subtypes of them, can also implement the dendritic (in addition to somatic) recurrent inhibition such as assumed in the model of the present study, despite their being known as soma-targeting cells. Basket cells target pyramidal somata, because it is a defining feature, but they also generally target proximal dendrites, which would include the whole basal dendrite; it was shown that at least certain subtypes of basket cells prevalently target medial or even distal portion of pyramidal basal dendrites [Wang et al. (2002), their Fig. 5, for the nest basket cell; the most preferable target cite was the third-order basal dendritic branches; or Gupta et al. (2000), their Fig. 6C, for the small basket cell] (however, it was also shown that at least some basket cells made synapses exclusively on the soma or the most proximal basal dendrites [Tamás et al. (1997), their Fig. 2C]); whether or not to target medial/distal basal branches therefore seems to depend on the subtype, brain region, etc. (Kubota et al., 2007). Recent studies have revealed that local dendritic spikes (NMDA spikes) can be generated independently in each branch of the pyramidal basal dendrites at a location >70 μm away from the soma (Schiller et al., 2000; Polsky et al., 2004; Nevian et al., 2007), and it was thus suggested (Nevian et al., 2007) that basal dendrites, despite their proximity to the soma, do not form a single basal-somatic region but rather should be considered as a separate integrative compartment. Although a direct experiment regarding the effect of inhibitory input applied onto the pyramidal basal dendrite with single branch resolution has not so far been done to my knowledge, a recent detailed simulation study (Rhodes, 2006) has shown that inhibitory input applied onto the basal dendritic branch close to (∼20 μm) the cite of NMDA spike generation (or more distal toward the end of the branch) vetoes the NMDA spikes in a branch-specific manner. The average distance of the basket-to-pyramidal synapses from the pyramidal cell body was shown to be 97.06 ± 41.49, 68.28 ± 13.14, or 78.83 ± 20.61 μm for the large, nest, and small basket cells, respectively [Markram et al. (2004), their supplemental information S2], and therefore it seems likely that certain portion of the synapses made by those cells onto the pyramidal basal dendrites (and possibly also proximal apical/oblique dendrites) can perform the branch-specific veto operation as mentioned above. In the case in which excitatory inputs are rather weak and temporally and/or spatially dispersed, NMDA spikes would not be generated and location-independent summation of the excitatory inputs would instead occur (Nevian et al., 2007) (see also Cash and Yuste, 1999; Gasparini and Magee, 2006); however, inhibition applied onto the branch would still have a branch-specific effect in such a subthreshold regimen via shunting, as theoretically predicted (Koch et al., 1983; Shepherd and Brayton, 1987) and experimentally demonstrated (Liu, 2004) (although not specifically in the neocortical pyramidal basal dendrites but in the cultured hippocampal neurons). Regarding the distal apical dendrites, although basket cells would also make synapses on the apical, oblique, and even tuft dendrites (e.g., it was shown that axodendritic synapses made by nest basket cells are formed onto three-quarters of the pyramidal dendritic tree [Wang et al. (2002), their Fig. 5]), other types of interneurons, such as (subtypes of) bitufted cells (Markram et al., 2004) or calbindin-positive interneurons with ascending axons observed in the monkey prefrontal cortex (Zaitsev et al., 2005), for example, would implement branch-specific inhibition on the distal apical/tuft dendrites. Martinotti cells, the most well-known cell type known to target pyramidal apical/tuft dendrites (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997; Markram et al., 2004), are however not considered to be a candidate for the source of such dendritic inhibition as assumed in this study, because it was shown that pyramidal-to-Martinotti synapses show prominent short-term facilitation, different from many other pyramidal-to-interneuron synapses, so that the Martinotti cell becomes active only after it receives high-frequency input from a single presynaptic pyramidal cell (Kapfer et al., 2007; Silberberg and Markram, 2007), which seems incompatible with the model of the present study (for details, see Results, On the nature of recurrent inhibition). By virtue of those excitatory and inhibitory recurrent connections, it is expected that a small portion of the pyramidal cells whose preferred angles are close to the stimulus angle sustain their activity after stimulus extinction while the others are inactivated, thereby representing the memory of the stimulus angle (Goldman-Rakic, 1995).
Distributions of the inputs on the dendritic branches.
I considered a model of individual pyramidal cells with multiple dendritic branches (see Fig. 1). As for the distribution of the synaptic inputs over the branches, I have made the following assumptions, based on recent experimental findings about the morphology, plasticity, and circuit anatomy regarding the pyramidal cellular dendrites (for explanation, see Results). I assumed that different dendritic branches of a given pyramidal cell receive feedforward (external) inputs from different input layer neurons (see Fig. 1G). For the sake of simplicity, I further assumed the one-to-one connectivity as shown in the figure; however, I have confirmed that such an exact one-to-one connectivity seems not to be important for the main results of the present study (see Fig. 9). The connection strength between each dendritic branch of a given pyramidal cell and the input layer neuron connected to the branch was assumed to have a bell-shaped distribution peaked at the connection to the input layer neuron with the same angle preference as the pyramidal cell (see Fig. 1G, orange line). Note that, because of this assumption, effectively only some portion of the dendritic branches receives a significant amount of feedforward inputs whereas other branches receive little feedforward excitation. In other words, almost all the feedforward inputs converge onto some portion of dendritic branches. Recurrent excitation, and recurrent inhibition mediated by dendrite-targeting interneurons when it was considered, was assumed to be evenly applied onto all the dendritic branches in most simulations, although nonuniformly distributed recurrent excitation (see Fig. 1G, red dashed line) was also tested in some simulations (see Fig. 13D–F and the corresponding text in Results). If intradendritic input distributions are quite different from those assumed ones, results (circuit behaviors) may differ. Nevertheless, I would like to argue that the main point of the present study, namely, a feature of the circuit behavior (see Results), seems not to be a direct outcome of the particular intradendritic input distributions as assumed above, but rather a more generic phenomenon, because similar feature appeared in a related simpler model without such an assumption (Morita et al., 2007) (although that model included only self-excitation, but not mutual excitation). More specifically, now let us consider the simplest situation, for the sake of explanation, in which the dendritic threshold is 0, dendritic saturation is absent, and dendritic inhibition operates as a subtraction. If all the inputs, namely, feedforward excitation, recurrent excitation, and recurrent inhibition have exactly the same intradendritic distribution, it is equivalent to have only a single branch because relative ratios of each type of inputs between the branches are constant all the time (i.e., activity of every branch changes proportionally; for example, thresholding is simultaneously imposed in every branch). However, if the distributions are different between input types, thresholding is expected to be imposed at different times from branch to branch, and thus, an effect of having multiple branches could appear. How strongly such an effect appears, as well as what type of effect appears, would depend on the particular shapes of the distributions. In the present study, I have assumed bell-shaped external excitation, uniform or weakly bell-shaped recurrent excitation, and uniform recurrent inhibition, based on the biological findings as mentioned above, and have found that a particular feature appears under such an assumption (see Results). A similar feature, as well as another feature yet to be known, might appear, at least theoretically, under different intradendritic distributions.
Dynamics of the pyramidal cellular activity.
Let us denote the activity (firing rate) of a pyramidal cell with preferred angle (memory/response field) θ at time t by x(t,θ). I described the temporal evolution of activities of the pyramidal cells by the following set of differential equations:
Here, J(ϕ,θ) represents the total input activity, which comprises feedforward excitation, recurrent excitation, and dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition (see below), on the dendritic branch of the pyramidal cell with preferred angle θ, which receives feedforward excitation from the input layer neuron with the preferred angle ϕ. S(t) ≡ Σθ′x(t,θ′) is the summation of the activities of all the pyramidal cells, and the negative term −asS(t) represents somatically mediated recurrent inhibition with as representing the strength. The function
represents the nonlinear input integration on each dendritic branch (see Fig. 1Ha); βd is the dendritic threshold; αd is the slope; and ηd is the upper bound (saturation). The function
represents the threshold nonlinearity imposed on the summation of all the branch outputs and somatic inhibition at the soma. This represents the transformation from the input activity to the output firing rate through spike generating mechanisms at the soma. The threshold nonlinearity would correspond to that the neuron starts firing just when the mean current averaged over a short time duration it receives is equal to zero, although our model is based on the firing rate description so that such a direct interpretation might not be appropriate. It has been shown that, in the case of pyramidal cells receiving a mixture of fluctuating excitatory and inhibitory inputs, the minimum mean current necessary for firing would be around zero, or could be slightly negative, because spikes can be driven by the fluctuation of the current (Hô and Destexhe, 2000; Chance et al., 2002; Shu et al., 2003b). The main findings in the present study continue to hold at least to a certain extent with a negative somatic threshold (data not shown). However, if the somatic threshold is positive, competition between pyramidal cells is further promoted so that inaccurate memory would become more easily induced even with compartmentalized input integration in the dendritic branches (data not shown).
Inputs onto each dendritic branch.
As described above, the pyramidal cells (see Fig. 1A–E,G, bottom pyramids) were assumed to be conceptually, but not necessarily physically, distributed in a ring according to their own preferred angles (θ; −180° ≤ θ < 180°; indicated by arrows in the pyramids in Fig. 1A–E,G). They were assumed to receive feedforward excitation from the input layer neurons in the preceding brain area (see Fig. 1A–E,G, top circles). The activity of the input layer neuron with preferred angle ϕ (see Fig. 1A–E,G, arrow in the circles) under the presentation of the visual stimulus at the angle ϕ0, denoted by I(ϕ;ϕ0), was assumed to be the sum of the following three components:
where (1) I0 is the background activity, (2) Is(ϕ;ϕ0) is the “stimulus angle-correlated component” that depends on the location, or angle, of the presented visual stimulus (ϕ0) and was assumed to be represented by a bell-shaped function centered at ϕ0 with the height Is max, specifically, the following:
and (3) ξ(ϕ) is Gaussian random noise satisfying =εl0 (ε, noise level). In most simulations, ξ(ϕ) was not assumed to change with time (static noise). However, I have also performed simulations with time-varying noise [dynamic noise, ξ(t,ϕ)] by applying independent Gaussian noise at every time step of the simulation (fourth-order Runge–Kutta method), confirming that main results mostly did not change (see Fig. 14). Note that, because the model describes the dynamics of the firing rate in an abstract manner, these noises do not directly correspond to biophysical processes. The ratio of the stimulus angle-correlated component to the background (Is max/I0) is referred to as an “input contrast” (see Fig. 2Ba). Feedforward excitatory connections (see Fig. 1A, red arrows) were assumed to be topographically organized in a functional sense, but not necessarily in a physical sense, such that the input layer neuron and the pyramidal cell with similar preferred stimulus angles have strong connections. Specifically, it was represented as follows:
where Fmax and δf represent the maximum strength and the width of the convergence–divergence tuning of the connection, respectively. Recurrent excitation (see Fig. 1B, red arrows) was organized such that the strength of synaptic coupling between two pyramidal cells decreases with the difference in their preferred angles. Specifically, the strength of the recurrent excitation between two pyramidal cells with preferred angles θ and θ′ was described as follows:
where Emax and δre represent the maximum strength and the width of the connection tuning, respectively. Although recurrent excitation was assumed to be uniformly distributed over branches in most simulations, nonuniformly distributed recurrent excitation was also tested in some simulations (see Fig. 13D–F and the corresponding text in Results). In those cases, recurrent excitation from the neuron θ′ to the neuron θ was assigned more onto the branch that receives feedforward input from the input layer neuron with the same angle preference θ′ (see Fig. 1G, red dashed line) according to the following:
where ψ represents the preferred angle of the input layer neuron connected to each branch and δre_branch represents the range (width) of such nonuniformity of recurrent excitation. In the simulations, I used c1 = 1, c2 = 0.5, δre_branch = π/4 (see Fig. 1G, red dashed line). c3 is a normalization factor:
Dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition (see Fig. 1D, blue arrows) was assumed to be either global:
where ad represents the strength, or local in a similar manner to the recurrent excitation (see Fig. 11A):
where Dmax and δri represent the maximum strength and the width of the connection tuning, respectively. In some simulations (see Fig. 13B,C), positive threshold for dendritic recurrent inhibition was considered as follows:
where cri represents the threshold (set to 0.05) and the superscript “plus” means rectification. As for the effect of dendritically mediated inhibition, I considered two cases: subtraction from the excitatory inputs or division on the excitatory inputs (see Fig. 1Hb,Hc and the corresponding text in Results). Note that inhibitory interneurons were not explicitly modeled but instead represented by negative signs in both cases. The total input activity on a dendritic branch was thereby the sum of feedforward excitation and recurrent excitation, subtracted or divided by dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition:
or
Simulation.
In the simulations, I assumed n pyramidal cells and m dendritic branches per pyramidal cell, whose preferred angles are evenly spaced in −180° ≤ θ < 180° and −180° ≤ ϕ < 180°. In most simulations, I set n = 100 and m = 100, except for those varying the number of branches (see Results) (see Fig. 9). Visual stimulus was assumed to appear at time 0 and disappear at time 100 (for the simulations in Figs. 3, 4, 9Bb, and 10B: the cases with “moderately nonlinear dendrite”) (see below) or at time 200 (for others: the cases with “highly nonlinear dendrite”). Probability of memory formation and the accuracy was calculated at time 200 (for the simulations in Figs. 3, 4, 9Bb, and 10B) or at time 400 (for others). Other parameters were set as follows, unless otherwise mentioned. Common to all the cases were the following: δinput = 15°, Fmax = 1, δf = 15°, Emax = 15, δre = 15°, ηd = , and ε = 0.1. Different values for the strength of recurrent excitation Emax were also examined in some simulations: see Figure 13, E and F (see below for more details about the parameter dependence). I considered two different types of dendritic nonlinearity, which I refer to as the moderately nonlinear dendrite (see Fig. 1Hb) and the highly nonlinear dendrite (see Fig. 1Hc), respectively (see Results). Parameters for the moderately nonlinear dendrite are as follows: αd = 1, βd = 0, I0 = 0.1, as = 2 in the case of somatic inhibition (see Figs. 3, 5) or ad = in the case of dendritic inhibition (see Figs. 4, 5), unless otherwise described. Parameters for the highly nonlinear dendrite are as follows: αd = 5, βd = , and as = 5 in the case of somatic inhibition or ad = 12 in the case of global dendritic inhibition (see Figs. 6, 7), unless otherwise described. In the simulations in which the threshold of the moderately nonlinear dendrite was subject to random positive deviation (see Fig. 10B), the threshold βd was assumed to be distributed according to the following half-Gaussian distribution (see Fig. 10Aa):
where σ (SD of the original Gaussian) was set to 0.02 (see Fig. 10Aa, green curve) (results are shown in Fig. 10Bb) or 0.04 (see Fig. 10Aa, blue curve) (results are shown in Fig. 10Bc). In the simulations in which the threshold (βd), slope (αd), and upper bound (ηd) of the highly nonlinear dendrite were subject to random variations (see Fig. 10C), they were assumed to be distributed according to the Gaussian distributions whose SDs were set to 0.1 or 0.2 times the means (, 5, and for βd, αd, and ηd, respectively) (see Fig. 10Ab,Ac) (results are shown in Fig. 10Ca,Cb) or varied from 0 to 0.2 of the means (see Fig. 10Ce,Cf). In the simulations in which locally operated dendritic recurrent inhibition was considered (see Figs. 11–13), the peak strength (Dmax) was varied as described in the figures. The width (δri) was set to 45° (see Fig. 11Aa), 30° (Ab), 22.5° (Ac), or 15° (Ad) (see Results). In the simulations in which both global somatic recurrent inhibition and global or local dendritic inhibition were considered (see Figs. 12, 13), the values for as, ad, Dmax, and δri were varied as described in the figures. The initial activities of the pyramidal cells [x(0,θ)] were taken from a uniform distribution of [0 0.05] in all the simulations. Simulations were performed by MATLAB (Mathworks). Specifically, ordinal differential equations were numerically solved by using the “ode45” function that uses a varying time step, except for those considering dynamic noise (see Fig. 14), which were solved by the fourth-order Runge–Kutta method with a fixed time step of 0.1 [I have also done some simulations with static noise by using the Runge–Kutta method instead of ode45, confirming that the results looked similar (data not shown)]. Unless otherwise described, simulations were repeated for 50 times (for Fig. 14) or 100 times (for all the others) with the same condition except for the presumed randomness (initial activity of the cells and static or dynamic noise in the input) so as to calculate the probability of memory formation as well as the memory accuracy (for the definition of those terms, see Results).
Dependence on the parameters.
In most simulations, the strength of recurrent excitation was fixed, whereas the magnitude of feedforward input and the strength of somatic and/or dendritic recurrent inhibitions were systematically varied. Therefore, in effect, relative magnitudes between those different kinds of inputs/connections were systematically varied, whereas the relationship between them (connection strengths) and the dendritic threshold and saturation was not. (Note that the model is otherwise scalable; especially, the model is scalable except for the dendritic saturation in the moderately nonlinear dendrite case.) I have, however, performed simulations with different strength of recurrent excitation (33% increased or 20% decreased) under several (although not all) configurations regarding the type of dendritic nonlinearity and recurrent inhibition, and confirmed that the circuit behavior that I propose as a feature of the model incorporating dendritic compartmentalization still appeared in such conditions (inhibition strength should also be changed, although there are certain margins in the excitation–inhibition ratio) (see Results). Examples under a particular configuration are shown in Figure 13, E and F. As shown in the figures and explained in the corresponding text, increasing recurrent excitation, which corresponds to decreasing the dendritic threshold relatively, resulted in better performance in terms of our criteria (explained in Results), whereas decreasing recurrent excitation led to the opposite result. Such a tendency was also observed in simulations under other configurations.
Possible extension of the model and the relationship to a previous model.
The model of the present study described above is related to a model considered in a previous work by Morita et al. (2007), which studied how the behavior of the competitive neural network (Amari and Arbib, 1977; Fukai and Tanaka, 1997; Hahnloser et al., 2000; Xie et al., 2002) changes when multiple dendritic branches with independent nonlinearity are incorporated. The most prominent difference between the previous study and the present one is that the previous model (Morita et al., 2007) included only self-excitation of neurons, whereas the model in this study includes mutual (recurrent) excitation between pyramidal cells, which plays an essential role in the formation of a sustained bump of activity that represents a spatial memory. It might be possible to regard the previous model (Morita et al., 2007) as a model of object working memory, although that model was much simpler than the model in this study so that direct correspondence to biological circuits would be more difficult. However, taking advantage of the simplicity, we could perform mathematical analyses in the previous model by theoretically considering the limit at which every neuron has an infinite number of dendritic branches. In fact, mathematical analysis could also be done for the model of this study. If there are a large number of pyramidal cells, they can be represented as a “neural field” (Amari, 1977), in which each cell is continuously parameterized by the preferred angle (θ). Thus, the above-mentioned set of ordinary differential equations can be represented as an integro-partial-differential equation as follows:
where
Likewise, assuming a large number of dendritic branches per pyramidal cell, the branches can be represented as a “dendritic field” in which each branch is continuously parameterized by the preferred angle of the input layer neuron (ϕ), from which it receives feedforward excitation. Consequently, the equation can be further modified as follows:
where
or
which can be referred to as “neuro-dendritic field equation” (Morita and Amari, 2007) and could be further analyzed mathematically.
Results
Modeling spatial working memory circuit incorporating pyramidal dendritic compartmentalization
I simulated a spatial working memory task such that a visual stimulus (visual cue) was transiently presented at an arbitrary location on a circle, and the animal was expected to memorize the location or “angle” of the stimulus (Funahashi et al., 1989) (see Fig. 2A). It has been hypothesized that there is a neural circuit underlying such spatial working memory in the neocortex, which consists of topographically organized feedforward excitatory connections from sensory (visual) cortices to the principal (pyramidal) cells, recurrent excitation between pyramidal cells with the strength correlating with the similarity in the spatial selectivity (i.e., location/angle preference, or memory/response field), and recurrent inhibition via interneurons (Goldman-Rakic, 1995). Assuming such a basic network architecture, many computational models have been developed to explore cellular mechanisms of spatial working memory (Constantinidis and Wang, 2004). I constructed a model based on a similar network architecture, but incorporated multiple dendritic branches of the individual pyramidal cells (Fig. 1E) (see below) (for details, see also Materials and Methods). Pyramidal cellular activity was described as a scalar variable representing the firing rate, which was driven by the inputs on the dendritic branches as well as on the soma. Inhibitory interneurons were not explicitly modeled but instead represented by negative signs of the connection strength.
To incorporate multiple dendritic branches, distributions of various types of inputs over the branches should be determined. Recent experiments in the hippocampus have revealed that synaptic potentiation could be induced in the pyramidal cells by locally generated dendritic spikes coupled with presynaptic cellular firings, even without postsynaptic somatic action potential generation (Golding et al., 2002). Moreover, electrical (Mel, 1993) and biochemical (Govindarajan et al., 2006) cooperativity within individual dendritic branches was proposed (Poirazi and Mel, 2001; Mehta, 2004; Govindarajan et al., 2006), and actually demonstrated in the hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells (Harvey and Svoboda, 2007), to define the dendritic branch as a functional unit of synaptic plasticity. It was also shown that the excitability of the dendrite can be changed and stored via activity-dependent regulation of potassium channels in a branch-specific manner in the CA1 pyramidal cells (Losonczy et al., 2008). Although there has been no evidence for or against that similar rules can also be applied to other brain areas such as the neocortex and it should be noted that things could potentially differ (Matsuzaki, 2007), I assumed that such dendritic branch-specific plasticity rules operate in the spatial working memory circuit so that the external space is mapped, not only onto a population of neurons constituting the circuit (Fig. 1F, left) but also onto a dendritic tree of individual component neurons (Fig. 1F, right), in analogy with what has been proposed for visual cortical neurons (Mel et al., 1998; Archie and Mel, 2000). Specifically, I assumed that different dendritic branches of a given pyramidal cell receive feedforward inputs from different neurons in the preceding area (referred to as the input layer neurons) (Fig. 1G) (for details, see Materials and Methods). In addition, I assumed that such an intradendritic self-organization (Fig. 1F, right) occurs in parallel with the network level self-organization (Fig. 1F, left), namely, the topographical organization of the feedforward connectivity as considered in the existing models of spatial working memory, so that the connection strength between each dendritic branch of a given pyramidal cell and the input layer neuron has a bell-shaped distribution peaked at the input layer neuron with the same angle preference as the pyramidal cell (Fig. 1G, orange line). With this assumption, most feedforward inputs are mapped onto some portion of the dendritic branches, whereas other branches mainly receive recurrent inputs (see below). If the same plasticity rule is applied to recurrent excitatory inputs, they would also be mapped onto the branches in a similar nonuniform manner. However, the situation is not the same as that of the feedforward inputs, because the pyramidal cells in the memory circuit are considered to fire randomly in the beginning of the circuit formation, contrary to the input layer neurons. Therefore, nonuniformity of the distribution of the recurrent excitatory inputs, if it exists, is expected to be shaped later and/or to less extent than that of the feedforward inputs. Considering that, in most simulations, I assumed that recurrent excitation is evenly applied onto all the dendritic branches (Fig. 1G, red solid line). I also performed several simulations with recurrent excitation that is nonuniformly distributed in a similar manner to the feedforward inputs, although to less extent (Fig. 1G, red dashed line) (for details, see Materials and Methods), confirming that the circuit feature of our interest (explained later) is mostly preserved (see Fig. 13D–F and the corresponding text). If the intradendritic distributions of the feedforward and recurrent excitation are quite different from those assumed ones, however, circuit behavior may differ (for additional discussion, see Materials and Methods). Looking into additional details, dendrites of pyramidal cells can usually be divided into two morphologically distinct parts, namely, apical and basal dendrites. However, because exactly how various types of inputs are distributed onto those two parts seems to remain elusive, I did not explicitly incorporate such a distinction into the model. Nevertheless, it has been generally considered that basal and proximal apical/oblique dendrites of pyramidal cells tend to receive inputs from nearby cortical cells, whereas distal apical/tuft dendrites tend to receive long-range corticocortical as well as thalamocortical inputs (Spruston, 2008). If such a principle, supposedly determined by genetic factors, is applicable to the pyramidal cell in the spatial working memory circuit, feedforward inputs are expected to be mapped onto the distal apical dendrite, whereas recurrent excitation is expected to be applied onto the proximal/basal dendrites. The above-mentioned model assumption (Fig. 1G) might also be approximately in line with such an apical–basal/proximal distinction, with the branches receiving a considerable amount of feedforward inputs corresponding to the apical/distal dendrites and the rest representing the basal/proximal dendrites, although more precise modeling would be desired when detailed data become available in the future. Regarding recurrent inhibition, I considered a somatically mediated one (Fig. 1C) and a dendritically mediated one (Fig. 1D). Dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition, when it was considered, was assumed to be evenly applied onto all the dendritic branches (although inhibitory inputs can also be nonuniformly distributed in reality, exact distribution for recurrent inhibition is not known at the moment so that the uniform distribution was assumed). I initially assumed that recurrent inhibition, either somatic or dendritic, is recruited by the activity of all the pyramidal cells and, in turn, equally affects every pyramidal cell (i.e., operates as global recurrent inhibition). However, later I also examined the case in which dendritic inhibition is more localized (see Fig. 11A) (see Materials and Methods). Although soma-targeting and dendrite-targeting interneurons are schematically illustrated as separate cell populations in Figure 1, C and D, it might be more likely that a single-cell population could implement both of them (for details, see below, On the nature of recurrent inhibition, and above, Materials and Methods, Network architecture).
I modeled the nonlinearity of the input integration on each branch (Fig. 1E, black square) as a piecewise linear function, a mathematical formula characterized by threshold, slope, and upper bound (saturation) (Fig. 1Ha). Two different cases were examined (Fig. 1Hb,Hc). First, if the dendritic branch is relatively passive and short, integration of the excitatory inputs is expected to be approximately linear except for the saturation attributable to shunting (Prescott and De Koninck, 2003), and voltage attenuation could be ignored. Moreover, inhibition applied onto a branch is expected to effectively shunt the excitatory inputs on the same branch but hardly affect the other branches because of the nature of shunting inhibition, as theoretically predicted (Koch et al., 1983; Shepherd and Brayton, 1987) and experimentally demonstrated (Liu, 2004). Therefore, the relationship between the input to the branch and the output current from the branch to the soma could be approximated by a threshold-linear function with a saturation (Fig. 1Hb, solid line), in which the threshold at 0 (i.e., rectification) represents the branch restrictiveness of the shunting inhibition [how positive deviation of the threshold affects the circuit behavior will be considered later (see Fig. 10Aa,B) (see Materials and Methods)]. The effect of inhibition on the somatic firing rate was described as a subtraction, based on a known result (Holt and Koch, 1997) (Fig. 1Hb, blue arrow). Next, however, if the dendritic branch is much more active, local dendritic spikes are likely to be generated when the excitation onto the branch exceeds a certain level within certain temporal and spatial windows (Schiller et al., 2000; Wei et al., 2001; Polsky et al., 2004; Gasparini and Magee, 2006; Losonczy and Magee, 2006; Milojkovic et al., 2007; Nevian et al., 2007), resulting in supralinear input integration above the threshold level. Moreover, if the branch is relatively long, small EPSPs are attenuated on the way to the soma, and thereby the branch can affect somatic firing almost exclusively via generating dendritic spikes. The input–output relationship of such a highly active branch could therefore be better approximated by a function with a positive threshold, rather than the rectification (zero threshold), and a steep slope (Fig. 1Hc, solid line). Random variation on the threshold, slope, and saturation will be considered later (see Fig. 10Ab,Ac,C) (see Materials and Methods). Inhibition applied onto such an active branch is again expected to have branch-specific effect [i.e., prevention of the dendritic spike generation on the same branch, as predicted by detailed numerical simulations (Rhodes, 2006)]. However, its effect on the somatic firing rate might not be described as a subtraction as considered above, but instead as a division on the excitatory input to the branch, which results in a combination of divisive and subtractive effect on the output firing rate, when inhibition is asynchronous (Jadi and Mel, 2007) (Fig. 1Hc, blue arrows). I will refer to the above-mentioned two cases as moderately nonlinear dendrite (Fig. 1Hb) and highly nonlinear dendrite (Fig. 1Hc), respectively. It was suggested that the degree of nonlinearity of the dendritic integration would depend on the spatiotemporal property of the synaptic inputs (Gasparini and Magee, 2006); however, because individual synaptic inputs were not described in our model, considering temporal features would be beyond the scope of the current study (additional discussion on this matter, however, will be provided later). In both moderately and highly nonlinear dendrite cases, output current from all the branches were assumed to be linearly summated, and then somatically mediated recurrent inhibition was implemented as a subtraction. Finally, this resulting value was subject to a threshold nonlinearity representing action potential generation at the soma (i.e., the value was rectified to zero if it was negative).
I examined the behavior of the circuit model in response to a transiently presented visual stimulus (Fig. 2A). Specifically, it was assumed that a visual stimulus was transiently presented at the angle 0° (for simplicity), and the working memory circuit received a visually evoked feedforward input from preceding brain areas during this duration. I considered two distinct parameters that could characterize the feedforward input to the memory circuit from preceding visual regions, namely, input contrast (Fig. 2Ba), which is the ratio of the stimulus-location (angle)-correlated component to the background, and “input intensity” (Fig. 2Bb) (for details, see Materials and Methods). I examined the circuit behavior with systematically varying those two parameters under various conditions regarding the nature of dendritic nonlinearity and recurrent inhibition.
Behavior of the circuit with moderately nonlinear dendritic branches and somatically mediated recurrent inhibition
First, I examined the behavior of the circuit with moderately nonlinear dendritic branches (Fig. 1Hb) under the assumption that globally operated recurrent inhibition was mediated by soma-targeting interneurons. Figure 3A shows the input to the memory circuit (panels in the top row), resulting temporal evolution of the pyramidal cellular activities (second row), and the activities at a time after stimulus extinction (i.e., in the “delay period”) (bottom row). The results for three different input contrasts, 0.8 (Fig. 3Aa), 0.4 (Ab), and 0 (Ac), are shown. When the input contrast was high (Fig. 3Aa), the pyramidal cells whose preferred angles were in proximity to the stimulus angle (0°) became highly active immediately after the appearance of the visual stimulus, whereas the other cells remained silent. This activity “bump” was sustained after the disappearance of the stimulus (Fig. 3Aa, bottom); thus, it can be considered to represent the “memory” of the stimulus location (angle). When the input contrast was decreased, the activity bump was still formed and sustained (Fig. 3Ab). However, there was greater trial-to-trail variation in the center of the bump, which corresponded to the “memorized angle,” as shown for two trials in Figure 3Ab. Even in the case in which the input contrast was 0, thereby indicating that the input to the memory circuit did not include stimulus angle-correlated component (Fig. 3Ac), the activity bump was still formed and sustained. However, the location of the bump was randomly determined, thereby indicating compulsive formation of spurious memory. Figure 3B summarizes the results of many trials at four different input contrasts. Histograms (Fig. 3B, left) show the distributions of the center of the activity bump (i.e., memorized stimulus angle). When the input contrast was high (Fig. 3B, left, top), the distribution peaked sharply at the stimulus angle (0°), indicating accurate memory formation. The distribution continued to peak despite the decrease in the input contrast (Fig. 3B, left, second and third); however, the variation increased significantly. When the input contrast was 0 (Fig. 3B, left, bottom), the distribution was flat, thereby indicating again the formation of random spurious memory. The circles (Fig. 3B, right) show instances of the memorized angle (red lines: <250 trials are drawn) and its vector average (thick black line: averaged over all the trials), whose length is considered to indicate the accuracy with which the memory provides information regarding the stimulus angle on average; therefore, I refer to it as “accuracy.” Figure 3C, solid line, indicates the accuracy. As shown in the figure, the accuracy decreased with decrease in the input contrast. I examined how such dependence of the memory accuracy on the input contrast changed when the intensity of the input to the memory circuit (Fig. 2Bb) was varied. Figure 3C, dashed and dotted lines, indicates the accuracy of memory in the cases in which the input intensity was doubled or halved (i.e., the background and the stimulus angle-correlated component of the input to the memory circuit were simultaneously doubled or halved, respectively). As shown in the figure, the accuracy decreased along with the decrease in the input contrast in all cases.
Behavior of the circuit with moderately nonlinear dendritic branches and dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition
Next, I examined the case in which recurrent inhibition operated again globally, but was mediated by dendrite-targeting interneurons rather than by soma-targeting cells. Figure 4 shows the results. When the input contrast was high (Fig. 4Aa), the circuit behavior was similar to that observed for somatically mediated recurrent inhibition (Fig. 3Aa) (i.e., the activity bump was formed and sustained after stimulus extinction). However, as the input contrast decreased (Fig. 4Ab), the circuit behavior varied from the somatic inhibition case (Fig. 3Ab). The activity bump was formed and sustained in some trials (Fig. 4Ab, trial 1). However, in others (trial 2), the pyramidal cells shared small activities (indicated by light blue color in the second panel) during the stimulus presentation, and the activity was not sustained after stimulus extinction. When the input contrast was zero, the activity bump was never formed nor sustained (Fig. 4Ac). The probability of the formation and sustenance of the activity bump (i.e., the probability of memory formation) decreased as the input contrast decreased, as appeared in the red line in Figure 4C. More precisely, the probability decreased substantially around a certain level of input contrast, thereby suggesting the existence of a “threshold contrast” for memory formation [the curve becomes even steeper when the input has time-varying noisy component (dynamic noise) as we shall see later (see Fig. 14)]. I examined how such a dependence of the memory formation on the input contrast changed when the input intensity was varied (Fig. 2Bb). Figure 4C, black dashed and dotted lines, indicates the probability of memory formation in the cases in which the input intensity was doubled or halved, respectively. As shown in the figure, these lines are so close to the red line (result for the original input intensity) that they are almost hidden, indicating that the contrast dependence of the memory formation was hardly affected by the change in the input intensity. In other words, memory formation was shown to be almost solely contrast dependent, and intensity invariant at least in a certain range. Figure 4B shows the distributions of the memorized angle over many trials at four different input contrasts. As shown in the figures, there was little variation in the memorized angle between the trials. Indeed, the variation was substantially smaller than that observed with somatic inhibition (Fig. 3B), even when the input contrast was so low that the probability of memory formation was quite low (Fig. 4B, third row). Thus, whenever the activity bump was formed, its location provided accurate information regarding the angle of the presented stimulus. Spurious memory was hardly formed, unlike that observed with somatic inhibition. Figure 4C, black solid line, shows how the accuracy of the memory, defined as the length of the averaged vector of memorized angles over trials (Fig. 4B, right, thick black lines), changed with the input contrast. Indeed, as shown in the figure, the accuracy hardly decreased with the decrease in the input contrast until the latter reached the threshold contrast for memory formation.
Let us clarify the difference between the effects of somatically and dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition. As seen above, somatically mediated recurrent inhibition consistently induced sustained activity bump regardless of the input contrast, even if the input did not include any stimulus angle-correlated component (Fig. 3Ac). Actually, circuit with somatic inhibition may show contrast-dependent memory formation if the somatic threshold, which was set to zero in the above simulations, is instead set to a positive value. However, in such a case, contrast-dependent memory formation will occur only for a limited range of input intensity; if the input intensity is near the value of the positive somatic threshold, memory is expected to be induced for a high-contrast input simply because it exceeds the threshold. To realize contrast-dependent memory formation for a wider range of input intensity, certain elaborative mechanisms such as finely tuned feedforward inhibition needs to be additionally incorporated. In contrast, as far as the dendritic nonlinearity is described as the zero-threshold-linear function with a saturation (Fig. 1Hb), dendritically mediated global subtractive recurrent inhibition enabled the contrast-dependent accurate memory formation for a wide range of input intensity without any additional mechanisms, as shown above.
I examined how the circuit behavior changed if the inhibition strength, or the balance between excitation and inhibition, was varied. Figure 4D shows the dependences of the probability of memory formation (left panel) and the accuracy of memorized location (right panel) on the input contrast (horizontal axes) with varying the inhibition strength (vertical axes). As shown in the figure, for a range of inhibition strength (between 1.85 and 2.05), memory was formed only when the input contrast was higher than a certain threshold (left panel) and the memory was accurate whenever it was formed (right panel) (i.e., the circuit was capable of contrast-dependent accurate memory formation). If inhibition was much weaker, memory was induced more easily but it was no longer accurate, as indicated by yellow or light blue colors in the bottom region of Figure 4D, right panel. In other words, the circuit behavior became more similar to that in the case of somatically mediated recurrent inhibition. In contrast, if inhibition was much stronger, memory could not be induced regardless of the input contrast, as indicated by the blue color in the topmost region of Figure 4D, left panel. Note that this also occurred in the case with somatically mediated recurrent inhibition (data not shown).
Mechanism of contrast-dependent accurate memory formation via dendritic inhibition
Let us explore the origin of such contrast-dependent accurate memory formation by dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition. As shown in the simulation (Fig. 4C), when the input contrast was lower than a certain threshold (Fig. 4C, ∼0.35), pyramidal cells never developed an activity bump but, instead, shared almost uniform low activities (Fig. 5Aa,Ab). What combination of different components of input to the pyramidal dendritic branches can stabilize such a low activity state? Figure 5, Ac and Ad, shows the magnitude of each component of inputs to the dendritic branches [parameterized by the preferred angle (ϕ) of the input layer neuron that is connected to the branch (Fig. 1G)] of the pyramidal cell whose preferred angle matches the stimulus angle (0°), which is referred to as the “corresponding cell” (Fig. 5Ab, filled star, Ac), and the cell whose preferred angle is the opposite of the stimulus angle (−180°), which is referred to as the “noncorresponding cell” (Fig. 5Ab, open star, Ad), while the circuit is receiving the input with three different contrasts (three columns). Feedforward excitation (Fig. 5Ac,Ad, orange lines), recurrent excitation (red), dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition (blue), and the summation of those three (purple) are drawn. When the input contrast is very low (0.05) (Fig. 5A, left column), in most branches except those in proximity to the preferred angle of the cell [0° (c) or −180° (d)], the contribution of feedforward excitation is little. Recurrent excitation and inhibition are mostly balanced; however, the inhibition is slightly dominant; therefore, the result of summation (purple lines) is negative, even for the corresponding cell (Fig. 5Ac). Thus, the “output” of those branches is 0 because of the presumed dendritic threshold at 0 (Fig. 1Hb). In the results, the soma collects positive current only from a small portion of the branches that receive considerable amount of feedforward excitation; thus, the cellular activity remains small, never reaching a level that can be sustained after stimulus extinction. As the input contrast increases (Fig. 5A, left to middle), the feedforward as well as the recurrent excitation that the corresponding cell receives increases accordingly (Fig. 5Ac, orange and red lines, respectively). Consequently, the total input increases but it still remains negative for most branches (Fig. 5Ac, middle, purple line); thus, their dendritic outputs remain 0, thereby preventing memory formation. As the input contrast increases more (Fig. 5Ac, right), at a certain point, for most branches of the corresponding cell, the magnitudes of excitation and inhibition become equal (i.e., the inhibition is just necessary and sufficient for nullifying the excitatory effect, as indicated by a total input of 0) (Fig. 5Ac, right, the purple line is around 0 in most parts). If the input contrast increases any further, the total input will become positive in every branch of the corresponding cell (Fig. 5B). Subsequently, the cellular activity will increase significantly, thereby causing positive feedback via the recurrent excitation between the adjacent cells and resulting in memory formation. Therefore, the input contrast in the case of the right panels of Figure 5A (0.35) is considered to be approximately a critical value, which in fact well matches the threshold contrast for memory formation observed in the simulation (Fig. 4C). Note that, when the memory is formed (Fig. 5B), every dendritic branch of the corresponding cell, whose location preference perfectly matches that of the stimulus by definition and whose activity is at the maximum of the bump (Fig. 5Bb), is at the saturated level (Fig. 5Bc) that would represent successive or prolonged dendritic spike generation, whereas the branches of the cell whose preference is less similar to the stimulus (on the left and right slopes of the activity bump in Fig. 5Bb) receive suprathreshold inputs but are not necessarily saturated.
Now let us explore another way of explanation. When the input contrast is low, inhibition is more than sufficient for completely inactivating the branch in most dendritic branches (Fig. 5Ac,Ad). Because of the presumed branch specificity of the dendritic inhibition, such “excessive” inhibition cannot be used for other more active branches, but just almost “wasted” (Fig. 5C). Because inhibition dominance is more prominent in the noncorresponding cell than the corresponding cell (Fig. 5Ad, right, light purple dashed circle), such a waste occurs more significantly in the noncorresponding cell, and subsequently, the “effective” amount of inhibition is greater in the corresponding cell. This is a form of negative feedback, which is especially prominent when the input contrast is just below the threshold for memory formation, or conversely, which defines such a threshold.
Why does such a contrast-dependent circuit behavior occur over a wide range of input intensity (Fig. 4C, dashed and dotted lines)? If the input intensity is varied (i.e., both the background and the stimulus angle-correlated components of the feedforward excitation are coordinately changed) (Fig. 2Bb), recurrent excitation and recurrent inhibition applied onto dendritic branches should also be scaled accordingly, except for the effect of dendritic saturation. Therefore, the relative ratios between any input components on each dendritic branch are kept constant so that the above-mentioned mechanism of the contrast-dependent memory formation continues to hold, realizing the input intensity semiinvariance.
Behavior of the circuit with highly nonlinear dendritic branches
So far, I have assumed that the nonlinearity of the input integration on the dendritic branches was moderately nonlinear (Fig. 1Hb). Next, I examined the cases in which the dendritic nonlinearity was much more nonlinear (Fig. 1Hc). Figure 6 shows the behavior of the circuit with such highly nonlinear branches, for the case with somatically mediated global subtractive recurrent inhibition (Fig. 6A) or with dendritically mediated (also global) divisive recurrent inhibition (Fig. 6B). As shown in the figure, the circuit showed contrast-dependent accurate memory formation not only when recurrent inhibition was mediated dendritically (Fig. 6B) but also when it was mediated somatically (Fig. 6A), contrary to the previously examined cases in which the dendritic nonlinearity was assumed to be moderate. Figure 7 shows individual components of the input to the pyramidal dendritic branches during the stimulus presentation for the case with somatically (Fig. 7A) or dendritically (Fig. 7B) mediated recurrent inhibition. The purple lines in the panels in c and d indicate the total input activity on the branches [i.e., summation of the feedforward excitation and the recurrent excitation (Fig. 7A) or that summation divided by dendritic inhibition (Fig. 7B)]. For both types of recurrent inhibition, when the input contrast is low (Fig. 7A,B, left columns), the total input activity is smaller than the dendritic threshold (indicated by light blue lines) in most branches except for those receiving significant feedforward excitation, even in the cell whose preferred angle is close to the stimulus location [corresponding cell (c)]. Consequently, those branches cannot contribute to somatic firing, preventing the formation of activity bump. It can be said, therefore, that the positive dendritic threshold plays a similar role to subtractive dendritic inhibition in the moderately nonlinear dendrite case in which the dendritic threshold was zero, realizing contrast-dependent accurate memory formation even if recurrent inhibition is mediated somatically (Fig. 5).
Because of the existence of the positive dendritic threshold, however, the circuit behavior should inevitably depend on the magnitude relationship between the threshold and the input intensity, contrary to the moderately nonlinear dendrite cases, and therefore the range of the input intensity for which contrast-dependent accurate memory formation holds would be limited. However, if recurrent inhibition is mediated dendritically, it is expected to partially compensate the change in the amount of feedforward excitation on each dendritic branch so that the contrast-dependent memory formation holds for a certain range of input intensity. Figure 8 shows the dependence of the circuit behavior on the inhibition strength. As shown in the figure, contrast-dependent accurate memory formation was observed for a range of input intensity with either type of recurrent inhibition; the range was generally wider in the case with dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition (Fig. 8B), especially when the inhibition strength was finely tuned (strength 10: second panel from the left), as expected above. When the inhibition strength was too weak, the width of the sustained activity bump became too wide (data not shown), and inaccurate memory could be induced (indicated by white crosses in the leftmost panel of Fig. 8B). In contrast, when the inhibition strength was too strong, memory became hard to be formed.
On the number and heterogeneity of the dendritic compartments
The number of independent electrical processing units, or “compartments,” operating inside the neuron has been one of the major topics (Häusser and Mel, 2003). A study using a detailed model with real morphology of the pyramidal cell (Poirazi et al., 2003b) has indicated that several dozen long thin terminal branches comprise independent sigmoidal subunits. In the simulations I have so far shown, it was assumed that each pyramidal cell has 100 dendritic branches and each of those branches has its own nonlinearity (Fig. 1G). This value (100) seems to be in line with the above-mentioned study. However, the number of compartments could vary depending on the type of cells as well as on the state of synaptic inputs (Williams, 2004). To examine how the behavior of our circuit model depends on the number of compartments in individual pyramidal cells, I performed simulations with systematically varying the number of compartments. Specifically, I considered four cases. In all of them, the number of pyramidal dendritic branches, each of which receives feedforward input from an input layer neuron, were fixed to 135. Thereafter, it was assumed that neighboring 1, 5, 15, or 45 “terminal” branches were grouped into a single compartment, that is, all the excitatory inputs applied onto those 1, 5, 15, or 45 branches were at first linearly summated and then inhibition, saturation, and thresholding were imposed, so that the pyramidal cell eventually had 135, 27, 9, or 3 compartments for independent nonlinear input integration, respectively (Fig. 9A). The connection strength between a single terminal branch and the input layer neuron was assumed to have a bell-shaped distribution [Fig. 9Ba(i)] in the same manner as before (Fig. 1G), and thus, in the case of total nine compartments [Fig. 9Ba(iii)], only three compartments received significant amount of feedforward excitatory inputs (one compartment receives most of them and the neighboring two receive the rest), or in the case of total three compartments [Fig. 9Ba(iv)], only a single compartment received almost all the feedforward excitation.
Figure 9Bb shows how the circuit behavior changed depending on the number of compartments in the case with moderately nonlinear dendrite and dendritically mediated global recurrent inhibition. Probability of memory formation (left column) and accuracy of memorized location (right column) are plotted against the input contrast (horizontal axes) and the inhibition strength (vertical axes) for the cases with 135, 27, 9, or 3 compartments [(i), (ii), (iii), and (iv), respectively]. As the number of compartments decreased [from (i) to (iv)], the threshold contrast for memory formation slightly decreased (left column) while the minimum amount of inhibition necessary for preventing the formation of inaccurate memory slightly increased (right column). However, the overall tendency did not primarily change, demonstrating that the main feature described above holds true even when the pyramidal cell has only a few “parallel” dendritic compartments for independent nonlinear input integration.
Figure 9Bc shows the highly nonlinear dendrite cases with either somatically mediated subtractive recurrent inhibition [(i)–(iv), top rows] or dendritically mediated divisive recurrent inhibition (bottom rows). Color indicates the probability of memory formation depending on the input contrast (horizontal axes) and the input intensity (vertical axes: log scale) with varying the inhibition strength (five columns), and white crosses indicate the formation of inaccurate memory (Fig. 8). As the number of compartments decreased from 135 to 27 [(i) to (ii)], the circuit behavior did not so much change. The circuit was still capable of contrast-dependent accurate memory formation for a range of input intensity; over twice difference for the case of somatically mediated inhibition (top rows) and up to eight times difference for the case of dendritically mediated inhibition (bottom rows). Even if the number of compartments was further decreased to nine [Fig. 9Bc(iii)], the behavior of the circuit with dendritically mediated inhibition was mostly conserved (bottom row). However, in the case with somatically mediated inhibition (top row), contrast-dependent accurate memory formation was possible only for a limited range of input intensity (∼0.2); memory was not formed or inaccurate memory could be induced for the input with different intensity. When the pyramidal cell had only three dendritic compartments [Fig. 9Bc(iv)], contrast-dependent accurate memory formation occurred only for a limited range of input intensity in both somatically or dendritically mediated inhibition cases. What caused such differences in the circuit behavior along with the decrease in the number of compartments? As I have shown above (Fig. 7), to prevent the formation of inaccurate memory in response to a low-contrast input, it seems essential that “most of” the dendritic branches do not have impact on somatic firing so that cell-to-cell difference in the amount of excitation onto those branches can be canceled. In the case in which the pyramidal cell had 27 compartments [Fig. 9Ba(ii)], the number of branches that received significant amount of feedforward excitation (visible in the figure) was 7, and thus, such cancellation occurred on at least 20 of 27 (possibly 22 of 27) branches, that is, >74% (possibly 81%) of the branches. In contrast, if the pyramidal cell had only three compartments [Fig. 9Ba(iv)], the cancellation could occur on only two of three branches, or 67% of the branches, which is thereby supposed to be insufficient to prevent the undesirable competition. Note that, however, those percentage values themselves do not have direct meanings because our model is very simple, in particular, branches were all identical.
What happens if the branches are not identical? I examined the effect of the intercompartment heterogeneity in the model [the number of the compartments (branches) was again set to the original value (100) in the following]. Figure 10B shows the circuit behaviors when the threshold of the moderately nonlinear dendrite, which was originally set to 0 (Fig. 1Hb), was subject to random positive deviation (Fig. 10Aa, green and blue curves). Subtractive dendritic recurrent inhibition was assumed as same as in the original simulations (Fig. 4). When the threshold was exactly at 0 (i.e., the circuit had the original moderately nonlinear dendrite), the circuit behavior was completely invariant for arbitrary input intensity weaker than a certain level (Fig. 10Ba) [above which the behavior varied because of the effect of the dendritic saturation, as expected above (Fig. 5)]. When the random positive deviation of the threshold was incorporated, such a genuine intensity semiinvariance could no longer hold (Fig. 10Bb,Bc), although contrast-dependent memory formation was shown to still occur for a certain range of intensity. It turned out, therefore, that in order for the ideal contrast-dependent intensity-invariant property to be realized, the dendritic threshold should be rather strictly tuned to 0. It has been shown that summation of excitatory inputs applied onto the dendrite could become nearly linear possibly except for rectification attributable to branch-specific inhibition (Cash and Yuste, 1999), in particular, in the case with spatially dispersed and temporally asynchronous inputs (Gasparini and Magee, 2006), presumably by virtue of appropriately tuned dendritic active conductance (Cash and Yuste, 1999; Gasparini and Magee, 2006). Our model, however, describes the relationship between the inputs onto the dendritic branches and the output firing rate of the cell in a rather abstract manner, and therefore, whether such active linearization mechanisms could realize the required tuning of the dendritic threshold in our model to 0 would need to be studied by using spiking neuron models. Figure 10C shows the behavior of the circuit with the highly nonlinear dendrite including intercompartment heterogeneity, that is, when the threshold, slope, and upper bound (saturation) of the dendritic nonlinearity were all subject to random variations as shown in Figure 10, Ab and Ac. Either somatically (Fig. 10Ca) or dendritically (Fig. 10Cb) mediated global recurrent inhibition was considered. In both cases, as the variation in the dendritic nonlinearity increased (from the top rows to the bottom rows in Fig. 10Ca,Cb), memory became easier to be formed, and the tuning curves of individual neurons became heterogeneous as shown in Figure 10, Cc and Cd. Meanwhile, the accuracy of the memory was decreased, as indicated by the black and white crosses in Figure 10, Ca and Cb [<0.95 (but >0.9) and <0.9 accuracy, respectively]. However, the decrease in the accuracy appeared to be gradual, rather than abrupt, as shown in Figure 10, Ce and Cf. Therefore, the overall feature of the circuit with the highly nonlinear dendrite discussed above can be said to be fairly robust against such intercompartment heterogeneity.
On the nature of recurrent inhibition
Regarding the source of recurrent inhibition during working memory-associated stimulus-selective sustained activity, it was suggested that the fast-spiking (FS) cell could be a candidate (Rao et al., 1999, 2000; Constantinidis et al., 2002). However, based on the findings that synapses arriving at or coming from FS cells show strong short-term depression whereas synapses for certain types of non-FS cells rather show short-term facilitation (Reyes et al., 1998; Gibson et al., 1999; Beierlein et al., 2003; Gao et al., 2003; González-Burgos et al., 2004, 2005), it was also proposed that those non-FS interneurons could be an ideal candidate for the source of recurrent inhibition in the working memory circuit (González-Burgos et al., 2005). However, the possibility for the FS cells has not be denied (Galarreta and Hestrin, 1998; Goldman et al., 2002), and thus the exact cell type(s) that mediate recurrent inhibition in the spatial working memory circuit seems to remain elusive. Because there is a huge variety of GABAergic interneurons, some of which target the soma of pyramidal cells and most of which target various domains of the pyramidal dendrites with various degree of preference (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997; Somogyi et al., 1998; Markram et al., 2004), there seem to be many possibilities (see below for more specific arguments). One point regarding the nature of recurrent inhibition that was not considered well in the above simulations is the effective range. I have so far assumed that both somatically and dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition operate globally (i.e., the same impact to all the pyramidal cells). Whereas certain types of soma-targeting cells such as large basket cells have horizontally and densely extended axonal arborization that is considered to be able to mediate recurrent inhibition over the wide range, other interneurons, especially, those targeting dendrites would have more vertically oriented axons so that their effects would be more local. Therefore, next I examined how the circuit behaves if dendritic recurrent inhibition operates more locally. Figure 11Ba shows the circuit behavior when dendritic recurrent inhibition operates locally with the range three times wider than that of the feedforward and recurrent excitation, as shown in the blue line (a) in Figure 11A. With such moderately local dendritic recurrent inhibition, although multiple bumps (Fig. 11C) were shown to be easily formed under certain conditions (indicated by white diamonds in Fig. 11B), the circuit feature that I have so far discussed, namely, contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation was shown to still occur when the inhibition strength was appropriately tuned (26). It would be reasonable; if the circuit is in a state in which every pyramidal cell has comparable level of small activity, dendritic recurrent inhibition that each cell receives is expected to become comparable regardless of whether it is driven by all the pyramidal cells or instead by some portion of them, unless their number is too small. When the inhibition range became narrower, namely, twice wider than the excitation (Fig. 11Ab), however, multiple bumps were more easily formed and the contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation could no longer occur (Fig. 11Bb).
In all the simulations I have thus far shown, either somatically or dendritically mediated recurrent inhibition was considered only in an isolated manner. In the actual circuit in the brain, however, it would be more likely that those two types of recurrent inhibitions exist together. Therefore, here let us see how the circuit behaves with such a combination of somatic and dendritic recurrent inhibition. When globally operated somatic recurrent inhibition and also globally operated dendritic recurrent inhibition coexist in the circuit with the highly nonlinear dendrite (Fig. 12A), the circuit behavior appeared to interpolate those in the extremes cases in which only one of those two types of inhibition exists (Fig. 8A,B). It was therefore confirmed that the phenomena I have shown in the above are not specific to biologically unrealistic situations in which exclusively somatic or dendritic recurrent inhibition exists. It would be more intriguing to see what happens when globally operated somatic recurrent inhibition, presumably mediated by certain types of FS cells, is coupled with locally operated dendritic one. Figure 12B shows the circuit behavior when local dendritic inhibition whose width is three times wider than the excitation (Fig. 11Aa) (same as considered in Fig. 11Ba) was coupled with global somatic inhibition. As shown in Figure 13B, contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation occurred under certain conditions. Notably, the permitted range of the dendritic inhibition strength for such a desirable response appeared wider than the case with local dendritic inhibition only (Fig. 11Ba). It seems that there are functional distinctions; whereas globally operated somatic recurrent inhibition ensures the uniqueness of the bump (i.e., avoids the appearance of multiple bumps), locally operated dendritic recurrent inhibition mainly serves for preventing the formation of inappropriate bump (inaccurate memory) in the case of low-contrast input. Even narrower dendritic recurrent inhibition (Fig. 11Ac,Ad), which by itself could not realize contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation (Fig. 11Bb), was shown to realize such a response when it was coupled with global somatic recurrent inhibition (Fig. 12C,D). Note that, when the inhibition strength (especially that of dendritic inhibition) was weak, although contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation was observed, the sustained activity bump appeared to be too wide (black circles in the figures indicate the case in which the bump included more than the one-half of the pyramidal cells).
Figure 13A shows the circuit behavior in the same condition as Figure 12C except that the variation in the dendritic nonlinearity such as shown in Figure 10Ab was incorporated. In consequence, the memory accuracy was decreased, but not drastically; the accuracy was shown to be >0.9 in most conditions [the black crosses indicate >0.9 (but <0.95) accuracy in the figure]. Figure 13B shows the circuit behavior also in the same condition as Figure 12C except that the local dendritic recurrent inhibition was assumed to be recruited only above a certain threshold (for details, see Materials and Methods), which may exist but has not so far been incorporated in our model. In the results, memory became slightly easier to be formed as expected, but the capability of contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation was preserved. It was also mostly preserved when both the variation in the dendritic nonlinearity and the threshold for dendritic recurrent inhibition were incorporated (Fig. 13C). It should be noted, however, that the threshold for inhibition assumed in those simulations (Fig. 13B,C) was not very high so that recurrent inhibition was recruited not only when the localized high-level activity bump was formed but also when the most pyramidal cells had comparable relatively low or moderate-level activities, and importantly, that such a recruitment in the latter case appeared to be essential for preventing inappropriate memory formation in response to high-intensity low-contrast input and thereby realizing intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation (Figs. 5, 7). For this reason, it is unlikely that the dendritic recurrent inhibition considered in the present study represents Martinotti or somatostatin-expressing cell-mediated inhibition that was recently found in the somatosensory cortex (Kapfer et al., 2007; Silberberg and Markram, 2007), because Martinotti cell was shown to be recruited only after it receives high-frequency input from single pyramidal cell via prominently facilitating synapses. Instead, dendritic recurrent inhibition assumed in the present study can be implemented by many other cell types, considering that there is a huge variety of neocortical GABAergic interneurons (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997; Somogyi et al., 1998; Markram et al., 2004) and most of them target pyramidal dendrites with various degrees of preference [Kubota et al. (2007), their Fig. 4] and also many of them have depressing synapses (Gupta et al., 2000), contrary to Martinotti cells. Among them, I would like to suggest that basket cells can also be a candidate, given that many of them receive excitation from pyramidal cells via depressing synapses, and moreover, despite their being known as as soma-targeting cells, many basket cells also prevalently target basal/proximal dendrites of pyramidal cells, including medial or even distal branches [Wang et al. (2002), their Fig. 5; or Gupta et al. (2000), their Fig. 6C; but see Somogyi et al. (1998), their Fig. 7B; whether or not to target medial/distal branches would depend on the subtype, brain region, etc.]. Considering that inhibitory input applied onto a basal dendritic branch was shown to have a branch-specific effect by a detailed simulation study (Rhodes, 2006), together with an indication derived from a direct experiment in the pyramidal basal dendrites (Nevian et al., 2007) that the basal dendrites should be considered as a separate integrative compartment, rather than being regarded as a single basal-somatic region, despite their proximity to the soma (although inhibition was not examined in that study), it seems likely that certain portion of the synapses made by basket cells onto the pyramidal basal dendrites can have branch-specific effect, and therefore, that basket cells can implement not only somatic inhibition but also dendritic inhibition assumed in the present study at least for the basal dendrite. Notably, it is possible that the basal dendrite is the main target of recurrent excitatory inputs as discussed before, and branch-specific inhibition of recurrent excitation is crucial for the intensity-insensitive contrast-dependent accurate memory formation in the model of the present study (Figs. 5, 7). Speaking more specifically about the cell type, large basket cells could implement global somatic and dendritic recurrent inhibition, whereas nest and/or small basket cells could implement local recurrent inhibition. Regarding the apical dendrites, although basket cells do make synapses on the apical, oblique, and even tuft dendrites [Wang et al. (2002), their Fig. 5], other types of interneurons, such as (subtypes of) bitufted cells (Markram et al., 2004) or calbindin-positive interneurons with ascending axons observed in the monkey prefrontal cortex (Zaitsev et al., 2005), for example, would implement branch-specific inhibition on the distal apical/tuft dendrites (for more detailed description on this matter, see Materials and Methods, Network architecture). Figure 13D shows the circuit behavior when recurrent excitation was assumed to be nonuniformly distributed over the branches (Fig. 1G, red dashed line) (for details, see Materials and Methods), exploring the possibility that the dendritic plasticity rule similar to what was assumed for the feedforward excitation is also applied to the recurrent excitation, although to less extent (as discussed before). As shown in Figure 13D, the capability of contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation was preserved. It was also preserved when the strength of recurrent excitation was increased (Fig. 13E) or decreased (Fig. 13F), with the permitted ranges of inhibition strengths changed accordingly. More precisely, increasing recurrent excitation, which means decreasing the dendritic threshold relatively, resulted in better performance of the circuit in terms of contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation (Fig. 13E), whereas decreasing recurrent excitation appeared to have the opposite effect (Fig. 13F).
As shown in the above, when local dendritic recurrent inhibition was combined with global somatic recurrent inhibition, contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation was realized as easily as the case with global dendritic recurrent inhibition (Fig. 8B). It is possibly because such local dendritic inhibition can effectively balance out local activity peaks, unless the peak is really significant, so as to prevent inappropriate bump formation (inaccurate memory). Considering that global dendritic recurrent inhibition may cost much in terms of energy consumption and wiring, combining local dendritic inhibition with global somatic inhibition would be a better strategy. In the case of high-contrast input, however, local inhibition seems problematic for the formation and/or the maintenance of a correct memory. It could be ideally overwhelmed by local disinhibition (Wang et al., 2004; Carter and Wang, 2007), especially if the threshold for disinhibition is higher than that for inhibition (i.e., if local disinhibition is recruited only when a bump is formed, possibly via facilitating synaptic transmissions, whereas local inhibition is recruited even when, or possibly only when, a bump is not formed, via strong, reliable, but depressing synapses such as those observed between pyramidal cells and certain basket cells as well as other interneurons) (Gupta et al., 2000). However, Martinotti cell-mediated recurrent inhibition, which was recently found in the somatosensory cortex (Kapfer et al., 2007; Silberberg and Markram, 2007) and might also exist in the prefrontal/parietal spatial working memory circuits, seems to differ from such a “low-threshold” dendritic recurrent inhibition, because of the facilitating nature of the pyramidal-to-Martinotti synapses as mentioned above. Instead, considering that most Martinotti cells dominantly target apical, rather than basal, dendrites of pyramidal cells and that there might be an input domain specification such that external inputs mainly target apical/distal dendrites whereas recurrent inputs target basal/proximal dendrites as discussed before (Fig. 1G), Martinotti cell-mediated recurrent inhibition might specifically block the external input that comes after an activity bump (a memory) is formed, so as to increase the robustness of the memory against forthcoming distracting stimuli, as suggested by a previous study (Wang et al., 2004) with even more specificity to the external inputs given the spatial separation of the apical and basal dendritic compartments. Another possibility, emerging from the recent finding that Martinotti cells frequently make synapses on the pyramidal dendritic spines that receive thalamocortical, rather than intracortical, inputs (Kubota et al., 2007), is that they regulate inputs from the thalamus and thus are involved in the control of the working memory circuit by the basal ganglia or other subcortical areas via thalamus. Those features are expected to be incorporated into a model in the future to fully elucidate the functional repertoire conferred by the huge variety of inhibitory interneurons.
Discussion
By modeling the spatial working memory circuit incorporating multiple dendritic branches of individual pyramidal cells, with the assumption that external feedforward inputs are applied onto some portion of branches while recurrent inputs are distributed over branches, I have shown that dendritic compartmentalization could endow the circuit with the ability of contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation. If the dendritic integration follows moderate nonlinearity with the threshold finely tuned at 0 and there exists globally operated dendritic recurrent inhibition, genuine intensity semiinvariance was realized (Figs. 4, 10Ba), although whether and how those tunings could be biologically implemented remains to be seen. Even out of such an ideal condition, however, I have shown that the circuit with dendritic compartmentalization could realize contrast-dependent accurate memory formation for at least a certain range of intensity under rather wide conditions, in particular when local dendritic recurrent inhibition is coupled with global somatic recurrent inhibition (Figs. 12, 13). [In the above simulations, external input was assumed not to change with time. I have also examined the case in which input has time-varying noisy component, confirming that the results mostly did not change (Fig. 14) (see Materials and Methods).]
Without dendritic compartmentalization, recurrent inhibition itself possesses the ability to implement input normalization, as proposed in the models of sensory processing circuits (Carandini and Heeger, 1994; Hahnloser et al., 2000). Different from sensory processing, however, working memory is undebatably an attractor-based strongly nonlinear process, and thus, such conventionally considered mechanisms seem unable to work without additional elaboration, for which I propose dendritic compartmentalization here. Richer branches or spines of the pyramidal cellular dendrites in the prefrontal and higher-order visual cortices than in the primary visual cortex (Elston, 2003) might reflect such a functional difference. Alternatively, feedforward inhibition (cf. Troyer et al., 1998), which is not included in our model, might also be useful for the intensity invariance. Although which of them is, or whether both of them are, actually used requires future examinations, a potential advantage to use recurrent, rather than feedforward, inhibition would be efficiency; the same circuitry can be used for both competition and normalization in the case of global recurrent inhibition, or long-distance wiring is not necessary in the case of local recurrent inhibition. This might especially be the case for the prefrontal working memory circuit, which is considered to receive inputs from widely spread multiple brain regions.
Apart from the intensity invariance, contrast detection could be achieved not only by dendritic thresholding but also by somatic thresholding (i.e., just as a comparison between the strength of the external input and a positive somatic threshold). What is, if any, an advantage for the dendritic thresholding? If there is only a single threshold at the soma, recurrent excitation applied to a neuron always contributes to the neuronal output activity as long as the neuron is not completely silent. This appears to be not stable; once some neurons become slightly more active than the neighbors because of fluctuation, positive feedback inevitably occurs because of recurrent excitation so that the circuit converges to a winner(s)-take-all state. If there is dendritic thresholding, however, recurrent excitation can hardly boost the neuronal output activity unless it is indeed significant. Meanwhile, the cell can still fire at a low frequency, because of the current flow from some portion of the branches that receive strong external inputs and generate local spikes, so as to recruit recurrent inhibition, which in turn stabilizes such a low-activity state of the circuit.
Possible link to animal behavior
In the primate brain, spatial working memory circuits are considered to exist in the prefrontal cortex and also in other areas such as the parietal cortex or the superior colliculus. It has been observed that spatially tuned neural activity in those regions can be modulated by the saliency, uncertainty, or the context of the stimulus, as well as by the animal's attention or expectation (Basso and Wurtz, 1997; Sereno and Maunsell, 1998; Platt and Glimcher, 1999; Constantinidis and Steinmetz, 2001; Bisley and Goldberg, 2003; Gold and Shadlen, 2007). Given that those areas are interconnected with each other, this would indicate that the input to the spatial working memory circuit can be modulated by those factors. An intriguing possibility is that the input contrast and the intensity (Fig. 2B) are modulated distinctively; for example, the contrast could represent the overall certainty of the stimulus location integrated from multiple sensory and mental modalities, whereas the intensity could reflect how many modalities were integrated or how long the integration lasted. Contrast-dependent intensity-insensitive accurate memory formation, enabled by dendritic compartmentalization as suggested in the present study, could then be useful for the animal to execute appropriate behavior, especially in a complex environment in which the animal needs to integrate information from multiple modalities. To verify the cellular mechanism proposed in the present study, it would be desired, although technically demanding, to measure the input to the pyramidal cell that shows sustained activity in the working memory task, which simulates such a complex environment, and how it varies according to the parameters of the task.
Conflicting demands on neural modeling
To make a neuron model closer to real neurons, there are two demands. One is the temporal aspect: how to describe the dynamics of action potential generation; the other, the spatial aspect: how to implement spatial extension of dendrites and linear and nonlinear processes thereon. For the simulation of large neuronal networks such as spatial working memory circuits, implementing both aspects is extremely difficult because of the limited computer power. Thus, although the description of firing dynamics has been drastically deepened from the rate-coding unit (Amari, 1977; Camperi and Wang, 1998; Fall and Rinzel, 2006) to the integrate-and-fire model (Compte et al., 2000; Tanaka, 2002; Renart et al., 2003; Carter and Wang, 2007) and the conductance-based models (Gutkin et al., 2001; Tegnér et al., 2002; Wang et al., 2004), most studies have considered at most only a few serially connected compartments without branching. However, although there was an important indication that two-compartment models could well reproduce several phenomena from single neuronal firing patterns (Mainen and Sejnowski, 1996) to network rhythmogenesis (Pinsky and Rinzel, 1994), the recently established notion of the compartmentalized input integration in individual pyramidal dendritic branches cannot be explored by such models. The present study has implemented a complementary approach: modeling multiple dendritic branches while using simple rate-coding description. Such an approach to use “branching neuron model” as a component of the neural circuit has been proposed in several recent studies (Spratling and Johnson, 2001, 2002; Goldman et al., 2003; Morita and Aihara, 2005; Herz et al., 2006; Morita et al., 2007) and is expected to be applied to other systems.
Ideally, both the temporal and the spatial aspects should be taken into account at once. Features having slow time courses such as short-term synaptic plasticity could be incorporated under the current scheme of modeling. Because it is known that different interneuron subtypes show different short-term plasticity on their synapses (Reyes et al., 1998; Gibson et al., 1999), they are expected to be considered together in the model. Fast temporal features, namely, neuronal spiking should also matter, especially considering an indication that the degree of nonlinearity of the dendritic integration, which affected the circuit behavior in our model, would depend on the temporal and spatial profiles of the synaptic inputs (Gasparini and Magee, 2006). One possible direction is to consider a spiking neuron model with the minimum number of dendritic compartments. I have shown that a small number of branches are enough for reproducing at least some of the phenomena described in this study (Fig. 9). This would suggest the usefulness of considering a reduced spiking neuron model with a few parallel compartments, each of which receives inputs from different sources, instead of or in addition to widely considered tandem compartments.
Experimentally testable predictions
The model in this study would predict that when the spatial working memory circuit receives a low-contrast or background input, most dendritic branches of every pyramidal cell are below the threshold for dendritic spike generation so that they can hardly affect the somatic activity whereas a small portion of the branches do generate local spikes that make the cell fire at a low frequency (Figs. 5Ac,Ad; 7Ac,Ad, left; Bc,Bd, left), which in turn stabilizes the circuit by recruiting recurrent inhibition. When the circuit receives a high-contrast input, however, most dendritic branches of the pyramidal cell whose preference matches the presented stimulus are expected to receive suprathreshold inputs (Figs. 5Bc; 7Ac, right; Bc, right). More precisely, those branches can be even at the saturated level, which would represent successive or prolonged dendritic spike generation. Recently, it was shown (Milojkovic et al., 2005) that if a single basal dendrite of the rat prefrontal pyramidal cell is stimulated strongly enough to generate a dendritic spike, the soma shows depolarization like the “UP” state (Steriade et al., 1993). Given that the in vitro UP state (Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000; Shu et al., 2003a) would be an analog of the neuronal state in the awake animal and could correspond to both the high-level delay period activity and the low-level spontaneous firing, the results by Milojkovic et al. can be in line with the behavior of the present model. To further verify the validity of the model, it would be desired to test, in vitro or hopefully also in vivo, whether there exists such a correlation between the number/ratio of spike-generating branches and the neuronal firing rate as predicted above by simultaneously stimulating and/or recording multiple branches. At the macroscopic level, topographic organization of the spatial working memory circuits can now be visualized by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in the parietal (Sereno et al., 2001; Schluppeck et al., 2005, 2006) and frontal (Kastner et al., 2007) cortices. Considering that the blood oxygen level-dependent signal of fMRI would be better correlated with the input to, rather than the output from, the neural population (Logothetis and Wandell, 2004), the central prediction of the present study that the working memory circuit possesses a potential to detect the signal-to-noise contrast of the external input, regardless of the absolute intensity, might become able to be tested, to some extent, by imaging studies in the future.
Footnotes
This work was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists (B) 19700310 from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology of Japan. I thank Dr. S. Amari for helpful discussion. I also thank anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments.
References
- Amari, 1977.Amari S. Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields. Biol Cybern. 1977;27:77–87. doi: 10.1007/BF00337259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amari and Arbib, 1977.Amari S, Arbib MA. Competition and cooperation in neural nets. In: Metzler J, editor. Systems neuroscience. New York: Academic; 1977. pp. 119–165. [Google Scholar]
- Archie and Mel, 2000.Archie KA, Mel BW. A model for intradendritic computation of binocular disparity. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:54–63. doi: 10.1038/71125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basso and Wurtz, 1997.Basso MA, Wurtz RH. Modulation of neuronal activity by target uncertainty. Nature. 1997;389:66–69. doi: 10.1038/37975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beierlein et al., 2003.Beierlein M, Gibson JR, Connors BW. Two dynamically distinct inhibitory networks in layer 4 of the neocortex. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90:2987–3000. doi: 10.1152/jn.00283.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bisley and Goldberg, 2003.Bisley JW, Goldberg ME. Neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention. Science. 2003;299:81–86. doi: 10.1126/science.1077395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Camperi and Wang, 1998.Camperi M, Wang XJ. A model of visuospatial working memory in prefrontal cortex: recurrent network and cellular bistability. J Comput Neurosci. 1998;5:383–405. doi: 10.1023/a:1008837311948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carandini and Heeger, 1994.Carandini M, Heeger DJ. Summation and division by neurons in primate visual cortex. Science. 1994;264:1333–1336. doi: 10.1126/science.8191289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter and Wang, 2007.Carter E, Wang XJ. Cannabinoid-mediated disinhibition and working memory: dynamical interplay of multiple feedback mechanisms in a continuous attractor model of prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(Suppl 1):i16–i26. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cash and Yuste, 1999.Cash S, Yuste R. Linear summation of excitatory inputs by CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neuron. 1999;22:383–394. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81098-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chance et al., 2002.Chance FS, Abbott LF, Reyes AD. Gain modulation from background synaptic input. Neuron. 2002;35:773–782. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00820-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compte et al., 2000.Compte A, Brunel N, Goldman-Rakic PS, Wang XJ. Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:910–923. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.9.910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantinidis and Steinmetz, 2001.Constantinidis C, Steinmetz MA. Neuronal responses in area 7a to multiple-stimulus displays: I. Neurons encode the location of the salient stimulus. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:581–591. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.7.581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantinidis and Wang, 2004.Constantinidis C, Wang XJ. A neural circuit basis for spatial working memory. Neuroscientist. 2004;10:553–565. doi: 10.1177/1073858404268742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantinidis et al., 2002.Constantinidis C, Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS. A role for inhibition in shaping the temporal flow of information in prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:175–180. doi: 10.1038/nn799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elston, 2003.Elston GN. Cortex, cognition and the cell: new insights into the pyramidal neuron and prefrontal function. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:1124–1138. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fall and Rinzel, 2006.Fall CP, Rinzel J. An intracellular Ca2+ subsystem as a biologically plausible source of intrinsic conditional bistability in a network model of working memory. J Comput Neurosci. 2006;20:97–107. doi: 10.1007/s10827-006-4791-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukai and Tanaka, 1997.Fukai T, Tanaka S. A simple neural network exhibiting selective activation of neuronal ensembles: from winner-take-all to winners-share-all. Neural Comput. 1997;9:77–97. doi: 10.1162/neco.1997.9.1.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Funahashi et al., 1989.Funahashi S, Bruce CJ, Goldman-Rakic PS. Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1989;61:331–349. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.2.331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galarreta and Hestrin, 1998.Galarreta M, Hestrin S. Frequency-dependent synaptic depression and the balance of excitation and inhibition in the neocortex. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:587–594. doi: 10.1038/2822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao et al., 2003.Gao WJ, Wang Y, Goldman-Rakic PS. Dopamine modulation of perisomatic and peridendritic inhibition in prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2003;23:1622–1630. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-05-01622.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gasparini and Magee, 2006.Gasparini S, Magee JC. State-dependent dendritic computation in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. 2006;26:2088–2100. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4428-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson et al., 1999.Gibson JR, Beierlein M, Connors BW. Two networks of electrically coupled inhibitory neurons in neocortex. Nature. 1999;402:75–79. doi: 10.1038/47035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold and Shadlen, 2007.Gold JI, Shadlen MN. The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:535–574. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golding et al., 2002.Golding NL, Staff NP, Spruston N. Dendritic spikes as a mechanism for cooperative long-term potentiation. Nature. 2002;418:326–331. doi: 10.1038/nature00854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman et al., 2002.Goldman MS, Maldonado P, Abbott LF. Redundancy reduction and sustained firing with stochastic depressing synapses. J Neurosci. 2002;22:584–591. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-02-00584.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman et al., 2003.Goldman MS, Levine JH, Major G, Tank DW, Seung HS. Robust persistent neural activity in a model integrator with multiple hysteretic dendrites per neuron. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:1185–1195. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic, 1995.Goldman-Rakic PS. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron. 1995;14:477–485. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- González-Burgos et al., 2004.González-Burgos G, Krimer LS, Urban NN, Barrionuevo G, Lewis DA. Synaptic efficacy during repetitive activation of excitatory inputs in primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:530–542. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- González-Burgos et al., 2005.González-Burgos G, Krimer LS, Povysheva NV, Barrionuevo G, Lewis DA. Functional properties of fast spiking interneurons and their synaptic connections with pyramidal cells in primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93:942–953. doi: 10.1152/jn.00787.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Govindarajan et al., 2006.Govindarajan A, Kelleher RJ, Tonegawa S. A clustered plasticity model of long-term memory engrams. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:575–583. doi: 10.1038/nrn1937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gupta et al., 2000.Gupta A, Wang Y, Markram H. Organizing principles for a diversity of GABAergic interneurons and synapses in the neocortex. Science. 2000;287:273–278. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutkin et al., 2001.Gutkin BS, Laing CR, Colby CL, Chow CC, Ermentrout GB. Turning on and off with excitation: the role of spike-timing asynchrony and synchrony in sustained neural activity. J Comput Neurosci. 2001;11:121–134. doi: 10.1023/a:1012837415096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser et al., 2000.Hahnloser RH, Sarpeshkar R, Mahowald MA, Douglas RJ, Seung HS. Digital selection and analogue amplification coexist in a cortex-inspired silicon circuit. Nature. 2000;405:947–951. doi: 10.1038/35016072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey and Svoboda, 2007.Harvey CD, Svoboda K. Locally dynamic synaptic learning rules in pyramidal neuron dendrites. Nature. 2007;450:1195–1200. doi: 10.1038/nature06416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Häusser and Mel, 2003.Häusser M, Mel BW. Dendrites: bug or feature? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:372–383. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00075-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herz et al., 2006.Herz AV, Gollisch T, Machens CK, Jaeger D. Modeling single-neuron dynamics and computations: a balance of detail and abstraction. Science. 2006;314:80–85. doi: 10.1126/science.1127240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hô and Destexhe, 2000.Hô N, Destexhe A. Synaptic background activity enhances the responsiveness of neocortical pyramidal neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:1488–1496. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.3.1488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holt and Koch, 1997.Holt GR, Koch C. Shunting inhibition does not have a divisive effect on firing rates. Neural Comput. 1997;9:1001–1013. doi: 10.1162/neco.1997.9.5.1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jadi and Mel, 2007.Jadi M, Mel BW. Synchrony and location dependent effects of inhibition in a pyramidal neuron model. the 2007 Computational and Systems Neuroscience Meeting; February; Salt Lake City, UT. 2007. Paper presented at. [Google Scholar]
- Kapfer et al., 2007.Kapfer C, Glickfeld LL, Atallah BV, Scanziani M. Supralinear increase of recurrent inhibition during sparse activity in the somatosensory cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:743–753. doi: 10.1038/nn1909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kastner et al., 2007.Kastner S, DeSimone K, Konen CS, Szczepanski SM, Weiner KS, Schneider KA. Topographic maps in human frontal cortex revealed in memory-guided saccade and spatial working-memory tasks. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:3494–3507. doi: 10.1152/jn.00010.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997.Kawaguchi Y, Kubota Y. GABAergic cell subtypes and their synaptic connections in rat frontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7:476–486. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.6.476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koch et al., 1983.Koch C, Poggio T, Torre V. Nonlinear interactions in a dendritic tree: localization, timing, and role in information processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1983;80:2799–2802. doi: 10.1073/pnas.80.9.2799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kubota et al., 2007.Kubota Y, Hatada S, Kondo S, Karube F, Kawaguchi Y. Neocortical inhibitory terminals innervate dendritic spines targeted by thalamocortical afferents. J Neurosci. 2007;27:1139–1150. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3846-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu, 2004.Liu G. Local structural balance and functional interaction of excitatory and inhibitory synapses in hippocampal dendrites. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:373–379. doi: 10.1038/nn1206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logothetis and Wandell, 2004.Logothetis NK, Wandell BA. Interpreting the BOLD signal. Annu Rev Physiol. 2004;66:735–769. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.66.082602.092845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- London and Häusser, 2005.London M, Häusser M. Dendritic computation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2005;28:503–532. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Losonczy and Magee, 2006.Losonczy A, Magee JC. Integrative properties of radial oblique dendrites in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neuron. 2006;50:291–307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Losonczy et al., 2008.Losonczy A, Makara JK, Magee JC. Compartmentalized dendritic plasticity and input feature storage in neurons. Nature. 2008;452:436–441. doi: 10.1038/nature06725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mainen and Sejnowski, 1996.Mainen ZF, Sejnowski TJ. Influence of dendritic structure on firing pattern in model neocortical neurons. Nature. 1996;382:363–366. doi: 10.1038/382363a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mainen et al., 1995.Mainen ZF, Joerges J, Huguenard JR, Sejnowski TJ. A model of spike initiation in neocortical pyramidal neurons. Neuron. 1995;15:1427–1439. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90020-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markram et al., 2004.Markram H, Toledo-Rodriguez M, Wang Y, Gupta A, Silberberg G, Wu C. Interneurons of the neocortical inhibitory system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:793–807. doi: 10.1038/nrn1519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsuzaki, 2007.Matsuzaki M. Factors critical for the plasticity of dendritic spines and memory storage. Neurosci Res. 2007;57:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2006.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehta, 2004.Mehta MR. Cooperative LTP can map memory sequences on dendritic branches. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:69–72. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2003.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mel, 1993.Mel BW. Synaptic integration in an excitable dendritic tree. J Neurophysiol. 1993;70:1086–1101. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.3.1086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mel, 2007.Mel BW. Why have dendrites? A computational perspective. In: Stuart GJ, Spruston N, Häusser M, editors. Dendrites. Ed 2. Oxford: Oxford UP; 2007. pp. 351–399. [Google Scholar]
- Mel et al., 1998.Mel BW, Ruderman DL, Archie KA. Translation-invariant orientation tuning in visual “complex” cells could derive from intradendritic computations. J Neurosci. 1998;18:4325–4334. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-11-04325.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milojkovic et al., 2005.Milojkovic BA, Radojicic MS, Antic SD. A strict correlation between dendritic and somatic plateau depolarizations in the rat prefrontal cortex pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3940–3951. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5314-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milojkovic et al., 2007.Milojkovic BA, Zhou WL, Antic SD. Voltage and calcium transients in basal dendrites of the rat prefrontal cortex. J Physiol. 2007;585:447–468. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.142315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morita and Aihara, 2005.Morita K, Aihara K. A network model with pyramidal cells and GABAergic non-FS cells in the cerebral cortex. Neurocomputing. 2005;65:697–707. [Google Scholar]
- Morita and Amari, 2007.Morita K, Amari S-I. A field equation model of the neuronal network dynamics incorporating two stages of nonlinear input integrations in individual neurons. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2007;33:753–6. [Google Scholar]
- Morita et al., 2007.Morita K, Okada M, Aihara K. Selectivity and stability via dendritic nonlinearity. Neural Comput. 2007;19:1798–1853. doi: 10.1162/neco.2007.19.7.1798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nevian et al., 2007.Nevian T, Larkum ME, Polsky A, Schiller J. Properties of basal dendrites of layer 5 pyramidal neurons: a direct patch-clamp recording study. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:206–214. doi: 10.1038/nn1826. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinsky and Rinzel, 1994.Pinsky PF, Rinzel J. Intrinsic and network rhythmogenesis in a reduced Traub model for CA3 neurons. J Comput Neurosci. 1994;1:39–60. doi: 10.1007/BF00962717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Platt and Glimcher, 1999.Platt ML, Glimcher PW. Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature. 1999;400:233–238. doi: 10.1038/22268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poirazi and Mel, 2001.Poirazi P, Mel BW. Impact of active dendrites and structural plasticity on the memory capacity of neural tissue. Neuron. 2001;29:779–796. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00252-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poirazi et al., 2003a.Poirazi P, Brannon T, Mel BW. Arithmetic of subthreshold synaptic summation in a model CA1 pyramidal cell. Neuron. 2003a;37:977–987. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00148-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poirazi et al., 2003b.Poirazi P, Brannon T, Mel BW. Pyramidal neuron as two-layer neural network. Neuron. 2003b;37:989–999. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00149-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polsky et al., 2004.Polsky A, Mel BW, Schiller J. Computational subunits in thin dendrites of pyramidal cells. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:621–627. doi: 10.1038/nn1253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prescott and Koninck, 2003.Prescott SA, De Koninck Y. Gain control of firing rate by shunting inhibition: roles of synaptic noise and dendritic saturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:2076–2081. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0337591100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rall, 1964.Rall W. Theoretical significance of dendritic trees for neuronal input–output relations. In: Reiss RF, editor. Neural theory and modeling. Stanford, CA: Stanford UP; 1964. pp. 73–97. [Google Scholar]
- Rao et al., 1999.Rao SG, Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Isodirectional tuning of adjacent interneurons and pyramidal cells during working memory: evidence for microcolumnar organization in PFC. J Neurophysiol. 1999;81:1903–1916. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.4.1903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rao et al., 2000.Rao SG, Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Destruction and creation of spatial tuning by disinhibition: GABAA blockade of prefrontal cortical neurons engaged by working memory. J Neurosci. 2000;20:485–494. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00485.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renart et al., 2003.Renart A, Song P, Wang XJ. Robust spatial working memory through homeostatic synaptic scaling in heterogeneous cortical networks. Neuron. 2003;38:473–485. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00255-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reyes et al., 1998.Reyes A, Lujan R, Rozov A, Burnashev N, Somogyi P, Sakmann B. Target-cell-specific facilitation and depression in neocortical circuits. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:279–285. doi: 10.1038/1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes, 2006.Rhodes P. The properties and implications of NMDA spikes in neocortical pyramidal cells. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6704–6715. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3791-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000.Sanchez-Vives MV, McCormick DA. Cellular and network mechanisms of rhythmic recurrent activity in neocortex. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1027–1034. doi: 10.1038/79848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller et al., 2000.Schiller J, Major G, Koester HJ, Schiller Y. NMDA spikes in basal dendrites of cortical pyramidal neurons. Nature. 2000;404:285–289. doi: 10.1038/35005094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schluppeck et al., 2005.Schluppeck D, Glimcher P, Heeger DJ. Topographic organization for delayed saccades in human posterior parietal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:1372–1384. doi: 10.1152/jn.01290.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schluppeck et al., 2006.Schluppeck D, Curtis CE, Glimcher PW, Heeger DJ. Sustained activity in topographic areas of human posterior parietal cortex during memory-guided saccades. J Neurosci. 2006;26:5098–5108. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5330-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segev, 1995.Segev I. Dendritic processing. In: Arbib M, editor. The handbook of brain theory and neuronal networks. Cambridge, MA: MIT; 1995. pp. 282–289. [Google Scholar]
- Segev and Parnas, 1983.Segev I, Parnas I. Synaptic integration mechanisms. Theoretical and experimental investigation of temporal postsynaptic interactions between excitatory and inhibitory inputs. Biophys J. 1983;41:41–50. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(83)84404-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sereno and Maunsell, 1998.Sereno AB, Maunsell JH. Shape selectivity in primate lateral intraparietal cortex. Nature. 1998;395:500–503. doi: 10.1038/26752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sereno et al., 2001.Sereno MI, Pitzalis S, Martinez A. Mapping of contralateral space in retinotopic coordinates by a parietal cortical area in humans. Science. 2001;294:1350–1354. doi: 10.1126/science.1063695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepherd and Brayton, 1987.Shepherd GM, Brayton RK. Logic operations are properties of computer-simulated interactions between excitable dendritic spines. Neuroscience. 1987;21:151–165. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(87)90329-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shu et al., 2003a.Shu Y, Hasenstaub A, McCormick DA. Turning on and off recurrent balanced cortical activity. Nature. 2003a;423:288–293. doi: 10.1038/nature01616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shu et al., 2003b.Shu Y, Hasenstaub A, Badoual M, Bal T, McCormick DA. Barrages of synaptic activity control the gain and sensitivity of cortical neurons. J Neurosci. 2003b;23:10388–10401. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-32-10388.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silberberg and Markram, 2007.Silberberg G, Markram H. Disynaptic inhibition between neocortical pyramidal cells mediated by Martinotti cells. Neuron. 2007;53:735–746. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somogyi et al., 1998.Somogyi P, Tamás G, Lujan R, Buhl EH. Salient features of synaptic organisation in the cerebral cortex. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998;26:113–135. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00061-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spratling and Johnson, 2001.Spratling MW, Johnson MH. Dendritic inhibition enhances neural coding properties. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:1144–1149. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.12.1144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spratling and Johnson, 2002.Spratling MW, Johnson MH. Preintegration lateral inhibition enhances unsupervised learning. Neural Comput. 2002;14:2157–2179. doi: 10.1162/089976602320264033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spruston, 2008.Spruston N. Pyramidal neurons: dendritic structure and synaptic integration. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:206–221. doi: 10.1038/nrn2286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spruston et al., 2007.Spruston N, Stuart GJ, Häusser M. Dendritic integration. In: Stuart GJ, Spruston N, Häusser M, editors. Dendrites. Ed 2. Oxford: Oxford UP; 2007. pp. 351–399. [Google Scholar]
- Steriade et al., 1993.Steriade M, Nuñez A, Amzica F. A novel slow (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3252–3265. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-08-03252.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamás et al., 1997.Tamás G, Buhl EH, Somogyi P. Fast IPSPs elicited via multiple synaptic release sites by different types of GABAergic neurone in the cat visual cortex. J Physiol. 1997;500:715–738. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1997.sp022054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka, 2002.Tanaka S. Dopamine controls fundamental cognitive operations of multi-target spatial working memory. Neural Netw. 2002;15:573–582. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00050-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tegnér et al., 2002.Tegnér J, Compte A, Wang XJ. The dynamical stability of reverberatory neural circuits. Biol Cybern. 2002;87:471–481. doi: 10.1007/s00422-002-0363-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Troyer et al., 1998.Troyer TW, Krukowski AE, Priebe NJ, Miller KD. Contrast-invariant orientation tuning in cat visual cortex: thalamocortical input tuning and correlation-based intracortical connectivity. J Neurosci. 1998;18:5908–5927. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-15-05908.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang et al., 2004.Wang XJ, Tegnér J, Constantinidis C, Goldman-Rakic PS. Division of labor among distinct subtypes of inhibitory neurons in a cortical microcircuit of working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:1368–1373. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0305337101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang et al., 2002.Wang Y, Gupta A, Toledo-Rodriguez M, Wu CZ, Markram H. Anatomical, physiological, molecular and circuit properties of nest basket cells in the developing somatosensory cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:395–410. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.4.395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wei et al., 2001.Wei DS, Mei YA, Bagal A, Kao JP, Thompson SM, Tang CM. Compartmentalized and binary behavior of terminal dendrites in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Science. 2001;293:2272–2275. doi: 10.1126/science.1061198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams, 2004.Williams SR. Spatial compartmentalization and functional impact of conductance in pyramidal neurons. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:961–967. doi: 10.1038/nn1305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xie et al., 2002.Xie X, Hahnloser RH, Seung HS. Selectively grouping neurons in recurrent networks of lateral inhibition. Neural Comput. 2002;14:2627–2646. doi: 10.1162/089976602760408008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zaitsev et al., 2005.Zaitsev AV, Gonzalez-Burgos G, Povysheva NV, Kröner S, Lewis DA, Krimer LS. Localization of calcium-binding proteins in physiologically and morphologically characterized interneurons of monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:1178–1186. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]