Abstract
Vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF) is implicated as a molecular mediator for adult neurogenesis and behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs. However, these potential roles of VEGF in the CNS have not been clarified in model animals. Here we have created transgenic mice overexpressing a short active variant of VEGF-A (VEGF120) in forebrain. Expression of VEGF120 significantly enhanced cell proliferation and angiogenesis, as exemplified by the formation of an enlarged reddish brain. Adult neurogenesis in hippocampus was markedly stimulated without affecting cell differentiation of neural progenitor cells. Hippocampal neurogenesis was particularly robust in young adult animals, but it declined with age and reduced to control levels by 20 weeks under continuous expression of VEGF120. Thus, VEGF alone is not sufficient to support the long-term enhancement of adult neurogenesis, and VEGF-induced vascularization per se does not necessarily predict increased neurogenesis. In transgenic mice, we observed significant changes in affective behaviors. VEGF was found to have not only antidepressant effects but also anxiolytic effects. In addition, we found that VEGF significantly reduced fear and aggression. In contrast, basal activities under natural conditions were not affected much. Unexpectedly, these characteristic behaviors were maintained in older transgenic mice undergoing a reduced level of cell proliferation in hippocampus, suggesting that there is potential dissociation between adult neurogenesis and mood regulation. Our data indicate that VEGF exerts strong neurogenic and angiogenic effects in postnatal brain and influences different forms of affective behaviors.
Keywords: growth factor, neurogenesis, hippocampus, depression, anxiety, fear, aggression
Introduction
Vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF) is known to be an essential angiogenic factor that stimulates proliferation of vascular endothelial cells (VECs) (Neufeld et al., 1999). In the CNS, VEGF and its receptors are widely expressed, not only in VECs but also in neurons, astrocytes, and neural progenitor cells (NPCs) (Stone et al., 1995; Maurer et al., 2003; Cao et al., 2004). Recent studies suggest that VEGF exerts pleiotropic effects on brain functions including enhancement of adult neurogenesis through direct activation of NPCs (Jin et al., 2002). VEGF is induced by various environmental factors such as voluntary exercise, enriched environment, learning tasks, and brain insults (Papavassiliou et al., 1997; Fabel et al., 2003; Cao et al., 2004). Interestingly, these stimuli are also known to significantly enhance hippocampal neurogenesis (Kempermann et al., 1997; Gould et al., 1999; van Praag et al., 1999; Magavi et al., 2000; Gage, 2002), suggesting that VEGF may promote adult neurogenesis in response to external stimuli (Yasuhara et al., 2004; During and Cao, 2006).
Recently, antidepressant effects of VEGF have been suggested. A pharmacological approach by Warner-Schmidt and Duman (2007) has shown that administration of VEGF receptor antagonists diminishes both hippocampal neurogenesis and behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs. This is in good agreement with a previous report that hippocampal neurogenesis is required for the effects of antidepressant drugs (Santarelli et al., 2003). Thus, these observations raise the possibility that VEGF may exert antidepressant effects as a result of enhanced neurogenesis. However, the correlation between adult neurogenesis and clinical depression is still under debate (Lledo et al., 2006), and the real effects of VEGF on hippocampal neurogenesis and affective behaviors have not been demonstrated in model animals.
To dissect the mechanism, we have created mice overexpressing VEGF120 in forebrain. We have used this strategy because the general or brain-specific knock-out of VEGF-A is known to be lethal (Ferrara et al., 1996; Raab et al., 2004). Although transgenic (TG) mice overexpressing human VEGF165 in brain have been reported (Wang et al., 2005), the effects of VEGF on hippocampal neurogenesis and animal behaviors remain unknown. In this study, we used a short active variant of VEGF-A, VEGF120 (VEGF121 in humans), rather than VEGF164 (VEGF165 in humans) to specify the signaling pathway through VEGF receptors (Flt-1 and Flk-1) but not neuropilins involved in many biological functions such as axon guidance (Fujisawa, 2004; Moretti et al., 2006; Staton et al., 2007). In this manner, we can also minimize the potential side effects on brain development. VEGF120 TG mice exhibited a significant increase in hippocampal neurogenesis as well as angiogenesis in forebrain. The mice showed profound differences in several forms of affective behaviors. Our data demonstrated the multiple roles of VEGF in the CNS and provided novel evidence that VEGF plays important roles in mood regulation.
Materials and Methods
TG mice
A cDNA of mouse VEGF120 was cloned by reverse transcription-PCR with total brain RNAs (C57BL/6N) as templates. After confirmation of DNA sequences, VEGF120 cDNA was subcloned into an exon–intron cassette under control of a calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKIIα) promoter (Mayford et al., 1995). The linearized DNA fragment was injected into oocytes from DBF1 mice (SLC), and four founders (F0) were obtained. Two of four lines transmitted the transgene to the F1 generation, and they were backcrossed to C57BL/6N wild-type (WT) mice at least five times. The TG mice were maintained as heterozygotes. The backcross to C57BL/6N gave rise to nearly equal percentages of WT and TG progenies. WT littermates served as controls. Both male and female mice (3, 6, 10, 20, and 50 weeks old) were used for experiments, except that only male mice (10, 15, and 30 ± 2 weeks old) were used for behavioral studies. Mice were housed at 23°C with 12 h light/dark cycles (lights on 8:00 A.M. to 8:00 P.M.) and received food and water ad libitum. Animal experiments were conducted in accordance with the institutional guidelines at Kyushu University.
Voluntary exercise
Three-week-old male mice were randomly assigned into two groups of controls and runners. Runners were transferred to cages equipped with a running wheel (cage, 25 cm wide × 35 cm high × 40 cm deep; running wheel, 16 cm diameter × 8.5 cm deep). Control mice were housed in the cages without the wheel. Runners had free access to the running wheel for voluntary exercise. After 3 weeks, both controls and runners (6 weeks old) were killed for cytochemical analyses. The number of wheel rotations was automatically counted by an infrared sensor-based electronic preset counter (Keyence). The total running distances were calculated from the number of wheel rotations. There was no difference in the total running distances between genotypes.
In situ hybridization
In situ hybridization was performed as described previously (Schaeren-Wiemers and Gerfin-Moser, 1993). Briefly, brains were quickly isolated from 6-week-old mice and frozen in OCT compound (Tissue-Tek) at −80°C. The sagittal brain sections (40 μm thickness, ∼1 mm from the midline) were prepared with a cryostat (Leica) at −20°C and mounted on glass slides. The digoxigenin (DIG)-labeled antisense and sense RNA probes corresponding to the entire mouse VEGF120 sequence (∼0.5 kbp) were synthesized in vitro (Roche) and used at a concentration of 200 ng/ml in hybridization solution. The antisense probe recognizes both the VEGF120 transgene and endogenous VEGF-A splice variants (including VEGF120, VEGF164, and VEGF188). The sense probe was used to evaluate the background signals. The mRNA signals were detected by the immuno-enzyme technique using alkaline phosphatases conjugated to anti-DIG antibodies. As a positive control, endogenous VEGF-A mRNAs were induced by hypoxia in WT mice, in which mice were placed in a nitrogen-filled chamber for 20 s for three times at 5 min intervals and then killed after 4 h. For comparisons, brain sections from WT mice, hypoxic WT mice, and line 1 and 2 TG mice were mounted on the same glass slides to minimize possible staining variation. The images shown in Figure 1A were obtained from samples on the same glass slide.
5-Bromo-2′-deoxyuridine labeling
To evaluate cell proliferation and differentiation, 5-bromo-2′-deoxyuridine (BrdU; 100 mg/kg, 10 ml/kg, i.p.) was administered to mice. For analysis of cell proliferation, mice at 3, 6, 10, 20, or 50 weeks of age were killed 2 h after a single injection of BrdU. For analysis of cell differentiation, 6-week-old mice were given four injections of BrdU (once daily, 4 consecutive days) and killed after 4 weeks (10 weeks old).
Western blot analysis
Whole brains or different brain regions (olfactory bulb, cerebral cortex, striatum, hippocampus, thalamus, cerebellum) were isolated from WT and TG mice at 6, 10, 30, and 50 weeks of age. The brain tissues were weighed, sonicated in 10× volume of 1% SDS on ice, and frozen/stored at −80°C. The equal amounts of protein samples (∼20 μg) in SDS sample buffer were subjected to SDS-PAGE [15% gel for VEGF and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF); 12% gel for Flk-1, Nestin, and β-tubulin] and blotted onto a polyvinylidene difluoride membrane (Bio-Rad). The membrane was pretreated with a blocking solution (5% skim milk, 0.1% Tween 20 in PBS) overnight at 4°C and reacted with primary antibodies against VEGF (1:1000, rabbit polyclonal; Millipore Bioscience Research Reagents), BDNF (1:250, mouse monoclonal; Promega), Flk-1 (1:2000, mouse monoclonal; Santa Cruz Biotechnology), Nestin (1:250, mouse monoclonal; Millipore Bioscience Research Reagents), and β-tubulin (1:5000, mouse monoclonal; Sigma) in blocking solution for 2 h. Membranes were washed with a washing solution (0.1% Tween 20 in PBS) six times for 5 min each and reacted with horseradish peroxidase-conjugated secondary antibodies against mouse or rabbit IgGs (1:2000; NEB) in a blocking solution for 1 h. The membrane was washed with a washing solution six times for 5 min each and rinsed with distilled water three times. The protein signals were detected by chemiluminescence [Lumi-Light Plus (Roche); exposed to x-ray film (RX-U; Fuji)].
Immunocytochemistry
Mice were fixed by intracardiac perfusion with 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS under anesthetization. Brains were removed, postfixed with 4% paraformaldehyde overnight at 4°C, cryoprotected with 30% sucrose overnight at 4°C, embedded in OCT compound (Tissue-Tek), and frozen/stored at −80°C. A series of 40 μm coronal sections from different brain regions including cerebral cortex, striatum, hippocampus, and cerebellum were obtained with a cryostat (Leica). Brain sections were collected at 120 μm intervals for cortex/striatum (corresponding region, 0.38–1.10 mm bregma; approximately six sections per pool, three pools), at 200 μm intervals for hippocampus (corresponding region, −2.70 to −1.46 mm bregma; approximately six sections per pool, five pools), and at 80 μm intervals for cerebellum (corresponding region, −6.24 to −5.80 mm bregma; approximately six sections per pool, two pools) and transferred to multiwell plates containing PBS. For BrdU staining, sections were pretreated with 50% formamide/2× SSC for 1 h at 65°C, soaked in 2× SSC for 5 min, denatured with 1N HCl for 30 min at 37°C, neutralized with 0.1 m borate buffer, pH 8.2, for 10 min, and washed three times with PBS for 5 min each. For immunofluorescent staining, free-floating brain sections were first treated with blocking solution (3% donkey serum, 0.1% Triton X-100, 0.05% Tween 20 in PBS) for 1 h and reacted with primary antibodies against BrdU (1:400, rat monoclonal; Abcam), VEGF (1:200, rabbit polyclonal; Millipore Bioscience Research Reagents), NeuN (1:400, mouse monoclonal; Millipore Bioscience Research Reagents), GFAP (1:400, rabbit polyclonal; Sigma), Nestin (1:400, mouse monoclonal; Millipore Bioscience Research Reagents), or Doublecortin (Dcx; 1:400, rabbit polyclonal; Abcam) in blocking solution for 2 d at 4°C. Sections were washed six times with PBS for 5 min each, soaked in blocking solution for 10 min, and reacted with donkey IgG secondary antibodies conjugated to FITC, Cy2, Cy3, or Cy5 against mouse, rat, or rabbit IgGs (1:200; minimal cross-species reactivity; Jackson ImmunoResearch) in blocking solution overnight at 4°C. Sections were washed six times with PBS for 5 min each and mounted on slide glasses. Cell nuclei were stained with Syto13 green (1:2000; Invitrogen), which was included in a secondary antibody solution. Immunostaining with secondary antibodies alone yielded no apparent nonspecific signals. For comparison of fluorescent intensities, brain sections from WT and TG mice were pretreated and stained together in the same wells, placed onto glass slides side by side, and imaged under the same microscope settings.
Imaging and analysis
Specimens were examined under a LSM510 laser-scanning confocal microscope (Zeiss) using argon and helium lasers at 488, 543, and 633 nm. The laser intensity, scan speed, and gain were adjusted to avoid saturation and photobleaching. For each experiment, the same optimized conditions were applied to a series of samples. Original imaging data were acquired and viewed with the LSM Image Browser (Zeiss). For counting BrdU-positive cells, immunofluorescent images were taken with a 10× lens at 40 μm optical thickness. For identification of cell types and capillaries, a series of z-sections at 1–4 μm optical thickness were taken with a 20× [numerical aperture (N.A.) 0.7] or 40× (N.A. 1.3, oil) objective lens. To create composite images for the entire brain sections (as in Fig. 2C), multiple images obtained with a 5× objective lens were aligned by Adobe Photoshop (Adobe).
Measurement of BrdU-positive cells in dentate gyrus
A series of 40 μm coronal sections were prepared from the hippocampal region (600–1800 μm region from the rostral end of hippocampus; corresponding region, −2.70 to −1.46 mm bregma). Brain sections at 200 μm intervals were pooled in multiwell plates (approximately six sections per pool, five pools) and used for immunostaining. Immunofluorescent images were acquired from both left and right hippocampi for each section (total, ∼12 images per mouse). We used a linear density of BrdU-positive cells [the number of cells per millimeter of subgranular zone (SGZ)] as an indicator for cell genesis (Kitamura et al., 2003). The length of SGZs was measured by the LMS Image Browser (Zeiss) on images of Syto13 green nuclear staining. BrdU immunofluorescent images were converted into binary formats with a threshold at 50% of the maximal intensity by ImageJ [National Institutes of Health (NIH), Bethesda, MD]. The regions of interest [including SGZ and granule cell layer (GCL), but not hilus] were first outlined on images of nuclear staining and then superimposed on BrdU immunofluorescent images, and the number BrdU-positive cells within the region were automatically counted by ImageJ. The average linear densities of BrdU-positive cells along the SGZ were calculated for each mouse (based on ∼12 images per mouse).
Measurement of capillaries
We used a lectin-based method to specifically label blood capillaries (Thurston et al., 1996). Briefly, brain sections containing cerebral cortex, hippocampus, or cerebellum were first stained with biotin-conjugated Lycopersicon esculentum (tomato) lectin (1 μg/ml; Sigma) and then detected with avidin–Cy5 (1:200; Jackson Immunoresearch). For comparison of fluorescent intensities, sections from WT and TG mice were stained in the same wells, placed onto the same glass slides, and imaged under the same conditions. For the analysis of capillary diameters, lectin-stained capillaries in polymorphic regions of dentate gyrus were randomly selected (total of 200 capillaries from three mice per genotype), and the minor axes of capillaries were measured by the LMS Image Browser (Zeiss). For the proliferating VECs, mice were labeled with BrdU for 2 h in vivo, and a series of brain slices (40 μm thickness, four to six slices per mice, 200 μm intervals in hippocampus) were triple stained with the lectin, anti-BrdU antibody, and Syto13 green. The z-series of fluorescent images were taken, and polymorphic regions of dentate gyrus were examined. BrdU/lectin-double positive cells with a smaller nucleus (∼5 μm) on capillaries were identified as proliferating VECs. After counting the number of proliferating VECs, the cell densities (cells/mm3) were then calculated.
Behavioral analysis
All behavioral studies were performed in adult male mice of indicated ages (10, 15, or 30 ± 2 weeks old). WT and TG mice were housed together (total of four to six mice per cage, equal numbers of WT and TG mice in most cases). WT littermates were used as controls. The apparatus and data acquisition/analysis software for mood-related behaviors were designed and manufactured by O'Hara & Company. The software used for the behavioral analysis (Image OF, Image EP, and Image FZ) was based on ImageJ (NIH).
Open-field test.
Exploratory drive to a novel open field was measured with an open-field apparatus (white Plexiglas; 50 cm wide × 50 cm high × 30 cm deep) under bright ambient conditions (500 lux). Mice were placed in the defined corner of the apparatus, and the activities were recorded with a video camera for 10 min. Total distance (centimeters) and time spent in the central part of the open field (seconds) were automatically analyzed using Image OF software.
Elevated plus maze.
The platform of the elevated plus maze consisted of two closed arms (white Plexiglas; 5 cm wide × 45 cm high; equipped with 15-cm-high clear Plexiglas sidewalls) and two open arms (white Plexiglas; 5 cm wide × 45 cm high; no sidewalls) that were radiating from a central platform (white Plexiglas; 5 cm wide × 5 cm high) to form a plus sign. The entire platform was elevated to a height of 55 cm above the floor level under ambient conditions (300 lux). Each mouse was placed on the central platform facing one of the open arms, and the activities were recorded for 10 min. The number of entries into open and closed arms, the stay time on the open and closed arms (seconds), and total distance traveled (centimeters) were automatically analyzed using Image EP software.
Porsolt forced swim test.
The forced swim test was performed as described previously (Porsolt et al., 1977). A clear acrylic cylinder (13 cm diameter × 30 cm high) was filled with water (25°C) up to a height of 15 cm. The cylinder was then transferred into a white acrylic box with a video camera attached above and illuminated at 30 lux. A mouse was placed in the cylinder, and its behavior was recorded for 10 min. A mouse was judged to be immobile when it was making only minimal movements to keep its head above water. Immobile time (seconds) and freezing percentages were automatically analyzed by Image FZ software.
Fear conditioning.
Fear conditioning was conducted in a conditioning chamber (transparent acrylic box; 25 cm wide × 33 cm high × 28 cm deep) equipped with electricity-conducting metal wires aligned at the bottom. The entire conditioning chamber was placed in a sound-proof box. On the first day, mice were placed in the conditioning chamber (150 lux; background noise at 50 dB), and auditory stimuli (white noise at 55 dB for 30 s) paired with electric footshocks (0.2 mA for 2 s; during the last 2 s of the auditory stimuli) were applied at 1, 3, and 5 min (indicating the onset of the tone). On the second day, mice were placed in the same conditioning chamber, and the activities were monitored. The mouse behavior during the first 2 min was used for the analysis of context-induced fear. A pair of tone and footshock (same as day 1) was then delivered at 3 min (indicating the onset of the tone), and the test ended at 4 min. On the third day, mice were transferred to a distinct chamber (white triangular acrylic box; 33 cm side × 32 cm high) under different conditions (illumination at 30 lux; background noise at 55 dB; different odor and floor texture). During the 6 min test session, the conditioned stimulus (white noise at 60 dB for 120 s) was applied between 2 and 4 min (in bins 3–4 on day 3). In each session, mouse behaviors were monitored with a video camera attached at the top of the chambers. Immobile time (seconds), freezing rate (percentage), and total distance traveled (centimeters) were automatically analyzed by Image FZ software.
Hot plate test.
To measure thermal nociception in mice, a hot plate test was performed as described previously (Mogil et al., 1999). Briefly, 30-week-old male mice were placed on a metal surface of a hot plate (TGK) maintained at 55.0 ± 0.1°C. A colorless transparent plastic cylinder (23 cm high × 16 cm diameter) was used to constrain the mice for behavioral observation. In this analysis, two behavioral responses were regarded as indicators of thermal nociception: hindpaw lick or hindpaw shake/flutter. We measured the latency (seconds) of thermal nociception. Mice were quickly removed from the hot plate right after the responses. Cutoff time for the test was 60 s.
Basal activity.
The basal locomotive activities in home cages were measured by counting the number of line-crossing (an infrared sensor-based electronic preset counter; Keyence) for 3 d. The data from days 2 and 3 were used for the analysis for the total and nighttime activities.
Statistical analysis
All data for quantitative studies are expressed as mean ± SEM and are described in the figure legends. Parametric analyses were performed with the unpaired Student's t test or Welch's t test for comparisons of two groups, or one- or two-way factorial or repeated-measure ANOVA for comparisons of more than two groups (multiple comparisons by Tukey–Kramer's or Scheffe's post hoc test, if applicable). Nonparametric analyses were performed with the Mann–Whitney U test for comparisons of two groups or the Kruskal–Wallis test for comparisons of more than two groups (multiple comparisons by Steel-Dwass's or Scheffe's post hoc test, if applicable). A value of p < 0.05 was considered to be significant.
Results
TG mice overexpressing VEGF120
To specify the VEGF receptor-mediated signaling, we have created TG mice overexpressing a short active variant of VEGF-A (mouse VEGF120) in forebrain under control of a CaMKIIα promoter. This promoter drives gene expression in excitatory neurons in forebrain from 2 to 3 weeks after birth, minimizing potential side effects during development (Mayford et al., 1995). Two of four lines (lines 1 and 2) transmitted the transgene to the F1 generation, and they were backcrossed with C57BL/6.
We first examined the mRNAs by in situ hybridization (Fig. 1A). VEGF mRNA was found to be expressed in forebrain including hippocampus, cerebral cortex, and striatum in line 1 TG mice. However, expression in line 2 was comparable to that of WT mice. As a reference, endogenous VEGF mRNAs were induced by hypoxia in WT mice, which showed increased expression in broad regions including pyramidal and GCLs in hippocampus. Western blot analysis of protein extracts showed similar results that the level of VEGF120 was higher in hippocampus, cerebral cortex, and striatum in line 1 (Fig. 1B). In contrast, VEGF expression in line 2 was similar to that of WT controls. Therefore, we used line 1 for additional studies. The protein concentration of VEGF as determined by ELISA showed a ∼25-fold increase in cerebral cortex and hippocampus in TG mice (∼80 and ∼2000 pg/mg tissue for WT and TG mice; data not shown). At a cellular level, the expression of VEGF120 was evident in the cytoplasmic compartments of cell bodies and proximal dendrites of both granule and pyramidal cells in hippocampus (Fig. 1C).
When brains were isolated, it was apparent that TG mice had an enlarged reddish forebrain with distinct vascular structures (Fig. 1D). The weight of whole brains was increased by ∼20% in TG mice (brain weights: WT mice, 464.3 ± 4.2 mg; TG mice, 570.9 ± 12.2 mg; n = 19 WT and 15 TG, 15-week-old mice; p < 0.01, unpaired Student's t test) (Fig. 1E). In contrast, the body size of TG mice was rather small (body weights: WT mice, 33.2 ± 0.8 g; TG mice, 29.7 ± 1.1 g; n = 19 WT and 15 TG, 15-week-old mice; p < 0.01, unpaired Student's t test), which resulted in a 37% increase in the brain/body weight ratio (brain/body weight ratios: WT mice, 1.43 ± 0.02%; TG mice, 1.95 ± 0.08%; n = 19 WT and 15 TG, 15-week-old mice; p < 0.01, unpaired Student's t test). Based on the fact that the onset of VEGF120 expression is 2 weeks after birth (supplemental Fig. 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), these results suggest that VEGF enhances postnatal brain development, to some extent.
Increased cell proliferation and angiogenesis in forebrain
To investigate the cellular changes in forebrain, brain sections from cerebral cortex and striatum were double stained for cell nuclei (Syto13 green) and neurons (NeuN as a marker) (Fig. 2A). We counted the number of nuclei and NeuN-positive cells and found that the cell densities in forebrain including cerebral cortex and striatum were increased by >30% in TG mice (genotype effect for total cell densities: F(1,28) = 72.60, p < 0.0001; no interaction effect; two-way factorial ANOVA; n = 8 per group, 6-week-old mice). However, the analysis of NeuN-positive cells in cerebral cortex and striatum indicated that there was no difference in the neuronal densities between WT and TG mice (genotype effect for neuronal densities: F(1,28) = 0.18, p = 0.67; no interaction effect; two-way factorial ANOVA; n = 8 per group, 6-week-old mice), although an enlargement of forebrain might have resulted in an increase in the total neuronal population (Fig. 2B). To examine cell genesis, dividing cells were specifically labeled in vivo with BrdU (100 mg/kg, i.p.). It was found that the distribution pattern of BrdU-positive cells in forebrain was similar between genotypes, where many BrdU-positive cells were localized in ependyma of lateral ventricle, external capsule, and dentate gyrus of hippocampus (Fig. 2C). In general, cell divisions are less frequent in cerebral cortex and striatum, but TG mice showed a robust threefold to fivefold increase in BrdU-positive cells in these regions (genotype effect: F(1,8) = 215.32, p < 0.0001; no interaction effect; two-way factorial ANOVA; n = 3 per group, 6-week-old mice) (Fig. 2D). Many of these BrdU-labeled cells in cerebral cortex and striatum had smaller (∼5 μm) oval-shaped nuclei and were often found along vascular-like structures, suggesting that many of these proliferating cells are VECs, one of the major targets of VEGF.
To visualize the blood vessels, brain slices were stained with L. esculentum (tomato) lectin (Thurston et al., 1996) (Fig. 2E). As expected, vascular development was evident in the TG mice. Angiogenesis was significant in dentate gyrus, whereas there was no difference in cerebellum where the CaMKIIα promoter is almost quiescent (normalized intensities: p < 0.01 for dentate gyrus and nonsignificant for cerebellum; one-way factorial ANOVA followed by Tukey–Kramer's post hoc test; n = 2–3 per group, 6-week-old mice) (Fig. 2F, top). The diameter of capillaries in polymorphic regions of dentate gyrus was also increased in TG mice (capillary diameter: WT mice, 6.1 ± 0.1 μm; TG mice, 10.2 ± 0.2 μm; n = 200 capillaries from three mice per group, 6-week-old mice; p < 0.0001, unpaired Student's t test) (Fig. 2F, bottom). The density of actively dividing VECs was doubled in dentate gyrus of TG mice (BrdU-positive VECs: WT mice, 6.02 ± 0.98 cells/mm3; TG mice, 11.35 ± 0.75 × 103 cells/mm3; n = 8 per group, 6-week-old mice; p < 0.001, unpaired Student's t test). These results suggest that VEGF120 significantly enhances angiogenesis in forebrain.
VEGF is a secreted factor so that it can diffuse into other areas. However, the physiological effect of VEGF120 (as judged by angiogenesis) was mostly restricted to forebrain. We also found no increase in the serum concentration of VEGF (examined by ELISA; data not shown), suggesting that overexpression of VEGF120 in forebrain does not affect the rest of the body.
Cell proliferation in dentate gyrus
To examine the morphology of hippocampus, isolated hippocampi were sliced from dorsal to ventral direction at 250 μm intervals and stained for cell nuclei (Fig. 3A). We found that gross anatomical structures were similar between WT and TG mice, but we noticed an expansion of the GCL and enlargement of hippocampus in TG mice. Quantitative analysis indicated that the volume of GCL and hippocampus (excluding fimbria and subiculum) was significantly increased by 57 and 24%, respectively (GCL volume: WT mice, 0.46 ± 0.02 mm3; TG mice, 0.72 ± 0.03 mm3; hippocampal volume: WT mice, 12.53 ± 0.23 mm3; TG mice, 15.49 ± 0.28 mm3; n = 4 per group, 6-week-old mice; p < 0.001, unpaired Student's t test) (Fig. 3B,C). These results suggest that vigorous cell genesis and/or angiogenesis have resulted in an enlargement of hippocampus.
Cell proliferation in hippocampus was examined by the in vivo labeling with BrdU in 6-week-old mice. Many BrdU-positive cells were identified in SGZ of dentate gyrus, where NPCs are especially enriched (Fig. 3D). We counted the number of BrdU-positive cells and found that the density of proliferating cells in SGZ was almost doubled in TG mice (BrdU-positive cell densities; WT mice, 4.98 ± 0.58 cells/mm SGZ; TG mice, 10.47 ± 1.63 cells/mm SGZ; n = 10 per group, 6-week-old mice; p < 0.01, Welch's t test). Thus, overexpression of VEGF120 indeed stimulated cell proliferation of NPCs in hippocampus.
To evaluate the level of enhancement, we attempted to further stimulate the cell division of NPCs by the running-wheel exercise, voluntary exercise for rodents known to efficiently enhance hippocampal neurogenesis (van Praag et al., 1999; Kitamura et al., 2003) (Fig. 3E). The running-wheel exercise for 3 weeks (between 3 and 6 weeks old) significantly enhanced the BrdU incorporation by >40% in WT mice (WT controls, 4.43 ± 0.57 cells/mm SGZ; WT runners, 6.35 ± 0.29 cells/mm SGZ; n = 6 and 7, 6-week-old mice; p < 0.01, unpaired Student's t test), whereas no significant enhancement was observed in TG mice (TG controls, 11.20 ± 2.67 cells/mm SGZ; TG runners, 11.93 ± 1.65 cells/mm SGZ; n = 6 and 5, 6-week-old mice; p = 0.83, unpaired Student's t test). The level of cell proliferation in TG mice turned out to be much higher than that of WT mice with exercise (p < 0.01, Kruskal–Wallis test for all groups and Mann–Whitney U test between exercised WT mice and control TG mice). This result suggests that the rate of cell divisions in SGZ may be at near-saturation in TG mice. Therefore, a short active variant of VEGF-A (i.e., activation of Flk-1 or Flt-1) sufficiently enhances cell division of NPCs in adult hippocampus. In this analysis, we also found in WT mice that ∼60% of BrdU-positive cells were localized near capillaries in SGZ and that the running-wheel exercise increased cell proliferation near capillaries (the percentages of BrdU-positive cells along capillaries in controls and runners were 59.2 ± 1.8 and 70.0 ± 1.3%, respectively; n = 6 and 5 per group, 6-week-old WT mice; p < 0.01, unpaired Student's t test; examined by the nucleus/BrdU/lectin triple staining). Thus, cell proliferation of NPCs or VECs along capillaries is preferentially stimulated by voluntary exercise.
VEGF enhances neurogenesis but does not affect cell differentiation in dentate gyrus
To visualize NPCs in dentate gyrus, brain sections were immunostained for Nestin (Lendahl et al., 1990) (Fig. 4A). The Nestin immunoreactions were apparently stronger in TG mice. Nestin-positive cells are often found along SGZ in WT mice, whereas they are also detected in the polymorphic regions between the blades of GCL in TG mice. Consistently, Western blot analysis showed a marked increase of Nestin in TG mice (Fig. 4B).
We next examined the immature neurons using Dcx (Francis et al., 1999) (Fig. 4C). Immunostaining of Dcx revealed the cell morphology of immature neurons in GCL. Dcx signals were also found to be stronger in TG mice, and the dendritic arbor of immature neurons appeared to be more developed in TG mice (see supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material, for enlarged images). Usually, Dcx-positive immature neurons are localized in the innermost layer of GCL. However, many of these cells were inserted into the middle or even outer layer of GCL in TG mice. Similarly, most of the 4-week-old young neurons (BrdU/NeuN double-positive cells) in WT mice were localized in the inner layer of GCL. In contrast, many young neurons were found in the middle or outer layer of GCL in TG mice (see supplemental Fig. 3, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material, for enlarged images).
To evaluate the effects of VEGF on cell differentiation, we examined the cell types of newborn cells in GCL. Proliferating cells in 6-week-old mice were labeled with BrdU and examined after 4 weeks (newborn cells were allowed to differentiate for 4 weeks). The brain sections were triple stained for BrdU, NeuN (neuronal marker), and GFAP (astrocyte marker), and double-positive cells (BrdU+/NeuN+- or BrdU+/GFAP+-positive cells) in GCL were counted (Fig. 4D). The percentages of neurons or astrocytes among BrdU-labeled cells in GCL were found to be similar between WT and TG mice (neurons: WT mice, 80.2 ± 2.3%; TG mice, 76.5 ± 2.9%; astrocytes: WT mice, 0.6 ± 0.5%; TG mice, 0.5 ± 0.3%; other cell types: WT mice, 19.2 ± 2.1%; TG mice, 23.0 ± 3.0%; n = 16 slices from four mice per group, 10-week-old mice; no significant differences in neurons, astrocytes, or other cell types between WT and TG mice by Steel-Dwass's test) (Fig. 4E). Therefore, most of the newborn cells in GCL are indeed differentiated into neurons (∼80%; note that the actual percentage may be higher because not all BrdU-positive cells in GCL were derived from NPCs). Because NPCs in SGZ have been shown to generate neurons and astrocytes (Gage et al., 1995), this result suggests that VEGF does not modulate the differentiation of newborn cells derived from NPCs in dentate gyrus. In this analysis, we also found that the number of 4-week-survived BrdU-positive cells in GCL was doubled in TG mice (4-week-old BrdU-positive cells in GCL: WT mice, 4.58 ± 0.24 cells/mm3; TG mice, 9.78 ± 1.17 × 103 cells/mm3; n = 4 per group, 10-week-old mice; p < 0.001, Welch's t test). This is in good agreement with the result from the short-term BrdU administration (Fig. 3D). Together, these data indicate that VEGF exerts a strong neurogenic effect in adult hippocampus.
VEGF alone is not sufficient for long-term enhancement of adult neurogenesis
We speculated that if VEGF directly promotes proliferation of NPCs, a considerable number of dividing cells would be detected even in aged animals. To test this hypothesis, we labeled the proliferating cells with BrdU for 2 h at different ages (3, 6, 10, 20, and 50 weeks after birth) and compared the densities of BrdU-positive cells along SGZ (Fig. 5A). We found that the cell proliferation in SGZ was markedly promoted by VEGF120 at 3–10 weeks after birth, which confirmed our previous observations (3 weeks old: WT mice, 15.50 ± 1.43 cells/mm SGZ; TG mice, 30.65 ± 8.02 cells/mm SGZ; n = 9 per group; p < 0.05, Mann–Whitney U test; 6 weeks old: WT mice, 4.98 ± 0.58 cells/mm SGZ; TG mice, 10.47 ± 1.63 cells/mm SGZ; n = 10 per group; p < 0.01, Welch's t test; 10 weeks old: WT mice, 4.74 ± 0.46 cells/mm SGZ; TG mice, 8.70 ± 1.48 cells/mm SGZ; n = 7 per group; p < 0.05, Welch's t test) (Fig. 5B). However, the rate of cell genesis in TG mice rapidly declined with age and reduced to the control levels by 20 weeks of age (WT mice, 2.51 ± 0.20 cells/mm SGZ; TG mice, 2.24 ± 0.81 cells/mm SGZ; n = 6 per group; p = 0.11, Mann–Whitney U test). At 50 weeks old, a minimal level of cell proliferation was observed in both groups (WT mice, 0.68 ± 0.19 cells/mm SGZ; TG mice, 0.96 ± 0.34 cells/mm SGZ; n = 4 per group; p = 0.50, unpaired Student's t test) (Fig. 5B). This result suggests that cell proliferation of NPCs in dentate gyrus declines in a time-dependent manner in both WT and TG mice (Kuhn et al., 1996).
We wondered whether a failure to stimulate cell division of NPCs in older TG mice might be attributable to a reduction in VEGF120 expression. However, Western blot analysis of hippocampal extracts indicated that the level of VEGF120 was similar at 6, 10, 30, and 50 weeks of age in TG mice (Fig. 5C). In addition, VEGF mRNAs were still abundant in hippocampus at 30 weeks of age (in situ hybridization; data not shown). Thus, the declined cell proliferation in older TG mice was not the result of reduced expression of VEGF120. We also found that Flk-1 (VEGF receptor 2) is constantly expressed in hippocampus (Fig. 5C), suggesting that the downregulation of Flk-1 may not be the case. In contrast, BDNF and Nestin tended to decrease with age (see supplemental Fig. 4, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material, for quantitative analysis). Interestingly, Nestin expression was still higher at 50 weeks of age, implying that a large population of NPCs may be quiescent in older TG mice. These results suggest that in addition to VEGF, the other factor(s) may be required to stimulate the cell division of NPCs in older mice. Although VEGF is a potent neurogenic factor, VEGF alone is not sufficient to support adult neurogenesis for prolonged periods.
Anxiolytic-like effects of VEGF
To investigate the effects of VEGF120 on affective behavior, we first examined the anxiety-related behavior in an open-field test (Fig. 6A). In a bright open field, mice tend to stay in the peripheral area by the enclosure and spend less time in the center area (or unprotected area). When mice were placed in the open field, TG mice spent more time in the center area than WT mice (time in center: WT mice, 86.6 ± 6.7 s; TG mice, 124.3 ± 15.9 s; n = 19 and 18, 10-week-old mice; p < 0.05, unpaired Student's t test), although the total distance traveled during the 10 min test period was similar (total distance: WT mice, 3675 ± 152 cm; TG mice, 3302 ± 249 cm; n = 19 and 18, 10-week-old mice; p = 0.38, unpaired Student's t test). This result suggests that TG mice have reduced anxiety-like behavior.
Anxiety-like behavior was also analyzed in the elevated plus maze (Fig. 6B). When mice are placed onto an elevated platform to induce natural aversion to height and open space, mice tend to stay within closed arms (safe area, equipped with sidewalls) and spend less time on open arms (risky area, without sidewalls). We found that TG mice more often explored open arms than WT controls (time on open arms: WT mice, 3.63 ± 1.2%; TG mice, 24.2 ± 4.9%; n = 18 and 15, 10-week-old mice; p < 0.0001, Mann–Whitney U test), whereas the total distance traveled during the 10 min test period did not change (total distance: WT mice, 1583 ± 82 cm; TG mice, 1675 ± 107 cm; n = 18 and 15, 10-week-old mice; p = 0.49, unpaired Student's t test). Therefore, this result also suggests that TG mice show reduced anxiety-like behavior than WT mice.
Antidepressant-like effects of VEGF
In clinical cases of mood disorders, depression often accompanies anxiety and vice versa (Paul, 1988). Therefore, we attempted to analyze the level of depression-like behaviors in the Porsolt forced swim test that has been widely used for the assessment of antidepressant drugs (Porsolt et al., 1977) (Fig. 7A). Mice were placed in an inescapable cylinder half-filled with water to induce the behavioral state of despair. We measured the periods of immobile status (“freezing”), in which longer freezing periods indicate higher degrees of depression-like status. We found that TG mice had significantly less freezing time compared with controls (freezing time: WT mice, 63.4 ± 2.0%; TG mice, 40.9 ± 4.0%; n = 20 and 19, 10-week-old mice; p < 0.0001, Welch's t test). This result suggests that TG mice may be more resistant to depression-like status, which is in good agreement with a recent report that a pharmacological application of VEGF receptor antagonists suppressed the behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs in rats (Warner-Schmidt and Duman, 2007).
VEGF reduces fear-related behavior
We have further examined fear-related behaviors in TG mice (Fig. 7B). On the first day, mice were placed in a conditioning chamber, and auditory stimuli [conditioned stimulus (CS): white noise, 55 dB, 30 s] paired with electric footshocks [unconditioned stimulus (US): 0.2 mA, 2 s; during the last 2 s of auditory stimuli] were presented three times at 1, 3, and 5 min (indicating the onset of auditory stimuli). Both WT and TG mice showed a gradual increase in fear responses (freezing behavior). However, the degree of freezing behaviors was considerably reduced in TG mice (genotype effect: F(1,22) = 18.88, p < 0.001; bin effect: F(6,132) = 68.45, p < 0.001; interaction effect of genotype × bin: F(6,132) = 7.57, p < 0.001; two-way repeated-measure ANOVA; comparisons between genotypes: p < 0.05 for bins 4–7, one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey–Kramer's post hoc test; n = 14 and 10 for 10-week-old WT and TG mice). On the second day, mice were placed in the same conditioning chamber, and context-induced fear was assessed for 2 min. Both genotypes showed freezing responses at the similar levels as observed on the first day (comparisons between bin 7 on day1 and bin 1 on day 2 were nonsignificant for both genotypes; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey–Kramer's post hoc test; n = 14 and 10 for 10-week-old WT and TG mice), although the level of freezing was consistently lower in TG mice (genotype effect: F(1,22) = 9.75, p < 0.01; no interaction or bin effect, two-way repeated measure ANOVA; n = 14 and 10 for 10-week-old WT and TG mice). On the third day, when the mice were transferred to a different chamber, they moved more freely. However, presentation of the auditory cue (white noise for 120 s, in bins 3–4) specifically induced freezing responses in both groups (genotype effect: F(1,22) = 0.91, p = 0.35; bin effect: F(5,110) = 24.80, p < 0.0001; interaction effect of genotype × bin: F(5,110) = 1.19, p = 0.32; two-way repeated-measure ANOVA; comparisons between bins 1 and 3: p < 0.01 for both genotypes, one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey–Kramer's post hoc test; n = 14 and 10 for 10-week-old WT and TG mice). Together, the induction of fear may be reduced in TG mice, whereas the associative learning per se remains intact.
VEGF120 TG mice are less aggressive
During the routine maintenance of mouse colonies, we have noticed that TG mice are more passive and seldom participate in fighting. Therefore, we performed a resident–intruder aggression assay to quantitatively evaluate aggressive behaviors in 15-week-old mice (supplemental Fig. 5, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). WT resident mice that were individually housed for 4 weeks before testing exhibited territory-protecting aggression against intruder mice (e.g., biting, clawing, lunging, and aggressive pinning). As expected, TG mice seldom attacked the intruder mice as indicated by the number of attacks, duration of attacks, and latency to initiate attacks (data and statistical analyses are described in the figure legend for supplemental Fig. 5, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). In TG mice, the social behaviors such as sniffing or following the intruders were normal. These results suggest that VEGF efficiently suppresses aggressive behaviors.
Basal locomotive activities are normal
Because of distinct alterations in mood-related behavior, we wonder whether TG mice also act differently under natural conditions. Therefore, we attempted to monitor the basal locomotive activities in home cages by counting the number of line-crossings for 3 consecutive days (Fig. 7C). However, we found no significant difference between WT and TG mice in the total locomotive activities in their home cages (total line-crossings on days 2–3: WT mice, 1.30 ± 0.19 × 103 counts; TG mice, 1.71 ± 0.38 × 103 counts; n = 9 per group, 10-week-old mice; p = 0.07 by unpaired Student's t test), although a slight increase in nighttime activities was observed in TG mice (activities at night: WT mice, 67.7 ± 5.8%; TG mice, 84.0 ± 2.8%; n = 9 per group, 10-week-old mice; p < 0.05, unpaired Student's t test). Thus, the basal activities under natural conditions were found to be similar in WT and TG mice.
Dissociation between the level of adult neurogenesis and mood-related behaviors
To investigate the relationships between hippocampal neurogenesis and mood-related behaviors, we also examined 30-week-old mice (“older mice”) in the elevated plus maze, forced swim test, and fear conditioning (Fig. 8). Because hippocampal neurogenesis in TG mice declines to the control levels by 20 weeks of age (Fig. 5B), we have speculated that anxiolytic-like or antidepressant-like effects of VEGF might be reduced or diminished in 30-week-old mice.
In the elevated plus maze (Fig. 8A), contrary to our expectation, TG mice still displayed a higher tendency to explore open arms than WT controls (time on open arms: WT mice, 6.3 ± 2.1%; TG mice, 29.6 ± 6.9%; n = 20 and 15, 30-week-old mice; p < 0.01, Mann–Whitney U test), whereas the total distances were not different (total distance: WT mice, 1326 ± 88 cm; TG mice, 1548 ± 112 cm; n = 20 and 15, 30-week-old mice; p = 0.12, unpaired Student's t test). This result suggests that the anxiolytic-like effect of VEGF is maintained in older TG mice.
Similarly, in the forced swim test (Fig. 8B), we found that the total freeze time (percentage) was significantly reduced in TG mice (total freeze time: WT mice, 68.1 ± 1.6%; TG mice, 44.3 ± 5.3%; n = 20 and 18, 30-week-old mice; p < 0.001, Mann–Whitney U test), suggesting that VEGF continuously exerts antidepressant-like effects in older mice.
In fear conditioning (Fig. 8C), we also observed the similar behaviors in older TG mice. When a series of electric footshocks was delivered on day 1, a gradual increase in freezing responses was observed in older WT mice but not in older TG mice (comparisons between genotypes, p < 0.01 for bins 6 and 7; comparisons between bins 1 and 7, p < 0.01 for WT mice and nonsignificant for TG mice; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey–Kramer's post hoc test; n = 20 and 14 for 30-week-old WT and TG mice). Although the older TG mice failed in this classical conditioning, we examined context-induced fear on the following day. The freezing percentages on day 2 were found to be similar to those at the end of conditioning on day 1 in WT and TG mice (genotype effect: F(1,32) = 101.79, p < 0.001; no bin or interaction effect; two-way repeated-measure ANOVA; comparisons between bin 7 on day1 and bin 1 on day 2: nonsignificant for both genotypes, one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey–Kramer's post hoc test; n = 20 and 14 for 30-week-old WT and TG mice). Thus, the reduction in freezing behavior is consistent in TG mice, but it seems more exaggerated at 30 weeks of age. Although behavioral responses to the electric footshocks (such as jumping) looked similar in both genotypes, attenuation of neuropathic pain might account for the reduced freezing behavior in TG mice. To clarify this issue, we performed a hot plate test at 55°C and measured the latency to the hindpaw lick or shake/flutter as an indicator of thermal nociception (Fig. 8D). The data suggest that TG mice are not insensitive but are slightly more sensitive to thermal nociception (latency of thermal response: WT mice, 18.1 ± 1.21 s; TG mice, 10.7 ± 1.78 s; n = 20 and 14, 30-week-old mice; p < 0.01, unpaired Student's t test). Therefore, the reduced freezing response in TG mice is not attributed to reduced neuropathic pain.
Collectively, these behavioral results suggest that the characteristic behavioral phenotypes are maintained in older TG mice. Therefore, it indicates that the degree of hippocampal neurogenesis is not always correlated with mood-related behaviors. A similar implication can be drawn from the data of WT mice that the anxiety- or depression-like behavior seems to be unchanged between 10 and 30 weeks of age despite differences in hippocampal neurogenesis (Figs. 5–8). Thus, the degree of adult neurogenesis and affective behaviors may not be correlated directly.
Discussion
In the present study, we have demonstrated the effects of VEGF on hippocampal neurogenesis and affective behaviors for the first time in genetically modified mice. Our data provided novel insights into the functional roles of VEGF in hippocampal neurogenesis, affective behaviors, and their correlation.
VEGF for hippocampal neurogenesis
VEGF and its receptors are expressed in a variety of cells in brain including neurons, astrocytes, and NPCs (Stone et al., 1995; Maurer et al., 2003; Cao et al., 2004). VEGF has been shown to directly stimulate cell proliferation of NPCs in cell cultures (Jin et al., 2002), and more recently, the pharmacological application or virus-mediated gene transfer of VEGF has shown to enhance hippocampal neurogenesis in animals (Cao et al., 2004; Warner-Schmidt and Duman, 2007). Consistently, in this study, the characterization of TG mice overexpressing VEGF120 in forebrain has clearly demonstrated that VEGF exerts a strong neurogenic effect in adult brain (Fig. 4). Adult neurogenesis in hippocampus was almost doubled, and the development of immature neurons was accelerated in these mice.
Unexpectedly, however, we found that hippocampal neurogenesis in TG mice rapidly declined with age and reduced to the control levels by 20 weeks of age (Fig. 5B). This result indicates that VEGF alone is not sufficient to support long-term enhancement of adult neurogenesis and that angiogenesis per se does not necessarily predict enhanced neurogenesis. A failure to support prolonged enhancement of adult neurogenesis was not attributable to a reduction in VEGF120 expression nor downregulation of Flk-1 in hippocampus (Fig. 5C). One interpretation of these results is that activation of multiple signaling pathways is necessary to sustain prolonged proliferation of NPCs in hippocampus. Other growth factors such as BDNF and insulin-like growth factor 1 are also known as potent enhancers of adult neurogenesis (Trejo et al., 2001; Rossi et al., 2006). Therefore, it is possible that these growth factors may act together to support proliferation of NPCs in adult brain. Age-dependent declines of these factors might partly explain a reduced level of neurogenesis in elders. Indeed, a gradual reduction in BDNF expression has been observed in TG mice (Fig. 5C). An alternative interpretation of our results is that NPCs in hippocampus do not have self-replicating abilities or naturally lose such abilities with age. This possibility is suggested by a recent report that hippocampal NPCs may not be neural stem cells capable of self-replication (Bull and Bartlett, 2005). Without replenishment of NPCs, chronic stimulation of adult neurogenesis would eventually exhaust NPCs. In both interpretations, intrinsic properties of hippocampal NPCs seem to be most critical. With the aim of long-lasting enhancement of adult neurogenesis, it is a key issue to endow NPCs with self-replicating abilities for extended periods.
VEGF for affective behaviors
This study has also provided direct evidence that VEGF influences mood-related behaviors. Overexpression of VEGF120 in forebrain has resulted in antidepressant-like behavior (Fig. 7A), which is in good agreement with a recent pharmacological report showing that antagonists of VEGF receptors block behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs (Warner-Schmidt and Duman, 2007). In addition to antidepressant-like effects, we have demonstrated for the first time that VEGF reduces anxiety-like behavior as well as induction of fear (Figs. 6, 7B). Furthermore, we also provided evidence that VEGF suppresses aggressive behaviors (supplemental Fig. 5, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Despite these dramatic alterations in affective behaviors, the basal activities under natural conditions (Fig. 7D) and the social activities observed during the aggression assay (supplemental Fig. 5, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) were similar between genotypes.
Clinical cases of mood disorders in humans suggest that affective behaviors such as depression and anxiety are interrelated. Similarly, we found that VEGF alters several forms of affective behaviors (e.g., depression- and anxiety-like behaviors, aggression, and fear). However, the neural mechanisms that explain such concurrent changes remain unknown. Subcortical structures including amygdala and hippocampus are known to be involved in regulation of mood. Because VEGF120 is overexpressed in both amygdala and hippocampus, normal functions of these structures might be affected in TG mice. However, we at least found that TG mice were able to express conditioned fear induced by contexts or auditory cues, suggesting that some functional aspects of amygdala or hippocampus remain intact (Fig. 7B). Because behavioral analyses may be insufficient to dissect out the precise neural correlates, different approaches are necessary to address this issue.
We are currently investigating the molecular mechanism how VEGF affects mood. The hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis of hormonal system is known to participate in stress responses and modulation of mood (Young, 2004). External stresses increase the level of glucocorticoids (e.g., cortisol), and these stress hormones are often elevated in depressed patients. We have tested this possibility by measuring the plasma contents of cortisol in the basal state, under constraint stress and after the recovery from stress in mice (supplemental Fig. 6, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). In both genotypes, plasma contents of cortisol significantly increased after restraint stress and reduced to near basal levels 1 h after the end of stress. The cortisol levels between genotypes were similar in any of these measures (see supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material, for data and statistical analyses). Thus, the altered behaviors in TG mice may not be explained by the changes in neuroendocrine system for stress response.
We also examined the tissue contents of serotonin (5-HT) and norepinephrine (NE) because a deficiency of monoamines such as 5-HT and NE in brain has been suggested to be an etiology of clinical depression (Schildkraut, 1965; Coppen, 1967). We expected that the level of monoamines might be higher in TG mice, but our preliminary results from ELISA showed that tissue contents of 5-HT or NE were not increased in TG mice (data not shown). Thus, the behavioral consequence of VEGF expression may not be attributed to the elevation of monoamine contents. Although the molecular mechanisms of VEGF in affective behaviors have yet to be elucidated, it is tempting to speculate that VEGF might be a common signaling molecule for different forms of affective behaviors.
Correlation of hippocampal neurogenesis and affective behavior
Finally, this study also provided insights into the correlation of adult neurogenesis and depression-like behavior. A role of adult neurogenesis in antidepressant actions has been implicated by a number of studies (for review, see Sahay and Hen, 2007). A pharmacological analysis by Warner-Schmidt and Duman (2007) has shown that an inhibition of Flk-1-mediated VEGF signaling blocked both neurogenesis and antidepressant effects. Consistent with this report, we observed enhanced hippocampal neurogenesis as well as antidepressant-like behavior in TG mice (Figs. 4, 7B). Unexpectedly, however, antidepressant-like behavior was still present under a minimal level of hippocampal neurogenesis in aged TG mice (Fig. 8B). In fact, even in older WT mice, declined hippocampal neurogenesis does not seem to result in elevated levels of depression-like behavior or anxiety-like behavior (Figs. 7, 8). Thus, our data indicate that the level of adult neurogenesis and the expression of antidepressant-like behavior are not correlated directly. Although these findings seem to conflict with a previous report showing that adult neurogenesis is required for the behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs (Santarelli et al., 2003), our present data have not excluded the possibility that neurons generated during juvenile stages might have contributed to antidepressant-like behavior in older animals. Therefore, additional analyses are necessary to address the issue whether VEGF has bifurcated (or independent) pathways for adult neurogenesis and antidepressant behavior.
In summary, we have demonstrated that VEGF exerts pleiotropic effects on brain functions including enhancement of adult neurogenesis and modulation of mood. Our findings provide valuable insights into the molecular mechanisms underlying affective behaviors.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (Grant 18500297). We thank Dr. T. Ishihara for the critical review of this manuscript, Dr. T. Kitamura for helpful discussion, and Dr. Y. Toh for the use of the equipment.
References
- BullND, Bartlett PF (2005) The adult mouse hippocampal progenitor is neurogenic but not a stem cell. J Neurosci 25:10815–10821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- CaoL, Jiao X, Zuzga DS, Liu Y, Fong DM, Young D, During MJ (2004) VEGF links hippocampal activity with neurogenesis, learning and memory. Nat Genet 36:827–835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- CoppenA (1967) The biochemistry of affective disorders. Br J Psychiatry 113:1237–1264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DuringMJ, Cao L (2006) VEGF, a mediator of the effect of experience on hippocampal neurogenesis. Curr Alzheimer Res 3:29–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- FabelK, Tam B, Kaufer D, Baiker A, Simmons N, Kuo CJ, Palmer TD (2003) VEGF is necessary for exercise-induced adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Eur J Neurosci 18:2803–2812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- FerraraN, Carver-Moore K, Chen H, Dowd M, Lu L, O'Shea KS, Powell-Braxton L, Hillan KJ, Moore MW (1996) Heterozygous embryonic lethality induced by targeted inactivation of the VEGF gene. Nature 380:439–442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- FrancisF, Koulakoff A, Boucher D, Chafey P, Schaar B, Vinet MC, Friocourt G, McDonnell N, Reiner O, Kahn A, McConnell SK, Berwald-Netter Y, Denoulet P, Chelly J (1999) Doublecortin is a developmentally regulated, microtubule-associated protein expressed in migrating and differentiating neurons. Neuron 23:247–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- FujisawaH (2004) Discovery of semaphorin receptors, neuropilin and plexin, and their functions in neural development. J Neurobiol 59:24–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- GageFH (2002) Neurogenesis in the adult brain. J Neurosci 22:612–613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- GageFH, Coates PW, Palmer TD, Kuhn HG, Fisher LJ, Suhonen JO, Peterson DA, Suhr ST, Ray J (1995) Survival and differentiation of adult neuronal progenitor cells transplanted to the adult brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 92:11879–11883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- GouldE, Beylin A, Tanapat P, Reeves A, Shors TJ (1999) Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. Nat Neurosci 2:260–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- JinK, Zhu Y, Sun Y, Mao XO, Xie L, Greenberg DA (2002) Vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF) stimulates neurogenesis in vitro and in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:11946–11950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- KempermannG, Kuhn HG, Gage FH (1997) More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature 386:493–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- KitamuraT, Mishina M, Sugiyama H (2003) Enhancement of neurogenesis by running wheel exercises is suppressed in mice lacking NMDA receptor epsilon 1 subunit. Neurosci Res 47:55–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- KuhnHG, Dickinson-Anson H, Gage FH (1996) Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat: age-related decrease of neuronal progenitor proliferation. J Neurosci 16:2027–2033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LendahlU, Zimmerman LB, McKay RD (1990) CNS stem cells express a new class of intermediate filament protein. Cell 60:585–595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LledoPM, Alonso M, Grubb MS (2006) Adult neurogenesis and functional plasticity in neuronal circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:179–193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MagaviSS, Leavitt BR, Macklis JD (2000) Induction of neurogenesis in the neocortex of adult mice. Nature 405:951–955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MaurerMH, Tripps WK, Feldmann REJ, Kuschinsky W (2003) Expression of vascular endothelial growth factor and its receptors in rat neural stem cells. Neurosci Lett 344:165–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MayfordM, Wang J, Kandel ER, O'Dell TJ (1995) CaMKII regulates the frequency-response function of hippocampal synapses for the production of both LTD and LTP. Cell 81:891–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MogilJS, Wilson SG, Bon K, Lee SE, Chung K, Raber P, Pieper JO, Hain HS, Belknap JK, Hubert L, Elmer GI, Chung JM, Devor M (1999) Heritability of nociception I: responses of 11 inbred mouse strains on 12 measures of nociception. Pain 80:67–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MorettiS, Boemi M, Catalano A (2006) Neuronal semaphorins regulate a primary immune response. Curr Neurovasc Res 3:295–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NeufeldG, Cohen T, Gengrinovitch S, Poltorak Z (1999) Vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF) and its receptors. FASEB J 13:9–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- PapavassiliouE, Gogate N, Proescholdt M, Heiss JD, Walbridge S, Edwards NA, Oldfield EH, Merrill MJ (1997) Vascular endothelial growth factor (vascular permeability factor) expression in injured rat brain. J Neurosci Res 49:451–460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- PaulSM (1988) Anxiety and depression: a common neurobiological substrate? J Clin Psychiatry 49:13–16. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- PorsoltRD, Bertin A, Jalfre M (1977) Behavioral despair in mice: a primary screening test for antidepressants. Arch Int Pharmacodyn Ther 229:327–336. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- RaabS, Beck H, Gaumann A, Yuce A, Gerber HP, Plate K, Hammes HP, Ferrara N, Breier G (2004) Impaired brain angiogenesis and neuronal apoptosis induced by conditional homozygous inactivation of vascular endothelial growth factor. Thromb Haemost 91:595–605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- RossiC, Angelucci A, Costantin L, Braschi C, Mazzantini M, Babbini F, Fabbri ME, Tessarollo L, Maffei L, Berardi N, Caleo M (2006) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is required for the enhancement of hippocampal neurogenesis following environmental enrichment. Eur J Neurosci 24:1850–1856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SahayA, Hen R (2007) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in depression. Nat Neurosci 10:1110–1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SantarelliL, Saxe M, Gross C, Surget A, Battaglia F, Dulawa S, Weisstaub N, Lee J, Duman R, Arancio O, Belzung C, Hen R (2003) Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science 301:805–809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaeren-WiemersN, Gerfin-Moser A (1993) A single protocol to detect transcripts of various types and expression levels in neural tissue and cultured cells: in situ hybridization using digoxigenin-labelled cRNA probes. Histochemistry 100:431–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SchildkrautJJ (1965) The catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorders: a review of supporting evidence. Am J Psychiatry 122:509–522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- StatonCA, Kumar I, Reed MW, Brown NJ (2007) Neuropilins in physiological and pathological angiogenesis. J Pathol 212:237–248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- StoneJIA, Alon T, Pe'er J, Gnessin H, Chan-Ling T, Keshet E (1995) Development of retinal vasculature is mediated by hypoxia-induced vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF) expression by neuroglia. J Neurosci 15:4738–4747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- ThurstonG, Baluk P, Hirata A, McDonald DM (1996) Permeability-related changes revealed at endothelial cell borders in inflamed venules by lectin binding. Am J Physiol 271:H2547–H2562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- TrejoJL, Carro E, Torres-Aleman I (2001) Circulating insulin-like growth factor I mediates exercise-induced increases in the number of new neurons in the adult hippocampus. J Neurosci 21:1628–1634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van PraagH, Kempermann G, Gage FH (1999) Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat Neurosci 2:266–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- WangY, Kilic E, Kilic U, Weber B, Bassetti CL, Marti HH, Hermann DM (2005) VEGF overexpression induces post-ischaemic neuroprotection, but facilitates haemodynamic steal phenomena. Brain 128:52–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warner-SchmidtJL, Duman RS (2007) VEGF is an essential mediator of the neurogenic and behavioral actions of antidepressants. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:4647–4652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- YasuharaT, Shingo T, Date I (2004) The potential role of vascular endothelial growth factor in the central nervous system. Rev Neurosci 15:293–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- YoungAH (2004) Cortisol in mood disorders. Stress 7:205–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]