Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2006 Sep 6;26(36):9080–9081. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2808-06.2006

Heroin Addiction: Anticipating the Reward of Heroin or the Agony of Withdrawal?

Magalie Lenoir 1, Ronald Keiflin 1,
PMCID: PMC6674500  PMID: 16958181

What drives compulsive drug use and relapse in addicts is still controversial (Robinson and Berridge, 2003). According to positive-reinforcement theories of drug addiction, the compulsion to take drugs results from an increase in the rewarding or incentive effects of drugs of abuse with chronic exposure, attributable to sensitization and/or associative conditioning. In contrast, according to negative-reinforcement theories, addicts are compelled to take drugs to avoid the unconditioned and/or conditioned negative affective consequences of drug withdrawal. The recent article by Kenny et al. (2006) in the Journal of Neuroscience provides strong support for the latter explanation by showing that withdrawal-induced decrease in brain reward drives compulsive heroin use.

Brain reward was probed using intracerebral self-stimulation in nondependent rats with stable/moderate heroin use (access to heroin limited to 1 h/d) and in dependent rats with escalating/compulsive heroin use (unlimited access to heroin). A stimulating electrode was placed in the lateral hypothalamus, a region at the heart of the brain reward system. After recovery, animals were allowed to self-stimulate this region by turning a wheel. After stabilization of the self-stimulation behavior, the intensity of the electrical stimulation was varied using the method of limits. This method allows the detection of the minimum current intensity that maintains self-stimulation. This reward threshold is an operational measure of the activity of the reward system (Kornetsky and Esposito, 1979).

In rats with daily restricted access to heroin, reward thresholds were stable across days [Kenny et al. (2006), their Fig. 1C (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/26/22/5894/F1)]. Thus, mere heroin exposure, even during several weeks, was not sufficient to alter brain reward. In contrast, in rats with prolonged access to heroin, reward thresholds gradually increased, probably because of a temporal summation of withdrawal effects [Kenny et al. (2006), their Fig. 1D (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/26/22/5894/F1)]. This elevation in reward thresholds paralleled the escalation of heroin intake [Kenny et al. (2006), their Fig. 1A (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/26/22/5894/F1)]. These observations confirm previous findings in rats with escalating cocaine use (Ahmed et al., 2002) and suggest that a withdrawal-induced decrease in reward function drives compulsive heroin use.

To assess the impact of conditioned withdrawal on heroin consumption, withdrawal was repeatedly precipitated by naloxone, a competitive μ-opioid receptor antagonist, and conditioned to a tone plus a light. As expected, in rats with restricted access to heroin, naloxone blocked heroin action on its receptors, thereby inducing a compensatory increase in heroin intake. In contrast, in heroin-dependent rats, naloxone not only blocked heroin action but also further decreased brain reward, as measured by an acute elevation in self-stimulation threshold above the already altered baseline [Kenny et al. (2006), their Fig. 3C (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/26/22/5894/F3)]. This decrease in reward was associated with a more pronounced increase in heroin consumption compared with nondependent rats [Kenny et al. (2006), their Fig. 2 (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/26/22/5894/F3)]. After conditioning, exposure to the tone-plus-light stimuli alone evoked effects similar to naloxone in dependent animals: they decreased reward activity and increased heroin self-administration (Fig. 1). These stimuli remained neutral in controls. These results clearly show that the shift to heroin dependence is associated not only with a quantitative change in heroin consumption, but also with a qualitative change in the motivation underlying heroin use, now driven by the anticipation of withdrawal.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Conditioned withdrawal drives heroin intake by decreasing brain reward activity in dependent animals only. Conditioned withdrawal was induced by the presentation of stimuli (tone plus light) previously paired with naloxone. In nondependent rats, naloxone-paired stimuli had no effect on brain reward thresholds or on heroin self-administration. In heroin-dependent rats, naloxone-paired stimuli produced an acute reward deficit that was associated with an increase in heroin consumption.

This study raises several questions for future research. First, the mechanisms underlying the motivational effects of conditioned withdrawal are not entirely clear. According to the authors, withdrawal-paired stimuli acquired motivational significance because dependent animals learned to anticipate withdrawal by taking more heroin. To fully demonstrate this point, however, it is necessary to test animals that have unlimited access to heroin but are prevented from taking heroin during naloxone conditioning. If the authors are correct, naloxone-paired stimuli alone should induce an acute decrease in brain reward, without increasing heroin consumption. This outcome will indicate that decreased reward function has no motivational power in itself; to acquire this power, dependent animals must learn that taking heroin alleviates the reward deficit. Second, the persistence of the motivational effects of conditioned withdrawal was not examined. Historically, conditioned withdrawal was proposed to account for relapses after protracted abstinence, when abstinent addicts no longer show signs of unconditioned withdrawal (Wikler, 1973). Future research should determine whether brain reward function eventually returns to normal after a prolonged period of abstinence and whether withdrawal-paired stimuli conserve motivational impact on heroin seeking in recovered addicts. Third, during conditioning, the onset of withdrawal was precipitated in an unnatural manner by rapidly blocking μ-opioid receptor using naloxone. In human addicts, heroin withdrawal is generally much slower, more gradual, and less predictable than naloxone-precipitated withdrawal in rats. The slow and gradual onset of withdrawal may reduce the likelihood of association with a specific set of environmental stimuli. Thus, it will be interesting to determine whether the authors' conclusions apply to naturally occurring drug withdrawal. Finally, it is important to recall that drug-paired stimuli (e.g., paraphernalia in human addicts) can also induce conditioned withdrawal responses in human addicts (Wikler, 1973). It would be of great interest to determine whether these stimuli alone can decrease reward function in drug-dependent individuals, as do withdrawal-paired stimuli. This issue is critical, because drug-associated stimuli are probably more prevalent than withdrawal-paired cues in human addiction.

In summary, Kenny et al. (2006) provide evidence for a profound change in the motivation underlying heroin use that may be relevant to the transition to addiction in humans. The results predict that in controlled heroin users, heroin consumption would be essentially motivated by memories of the positive rewarding effects of the drug. In dependent users, however, the motivation to use heroin would be strengthened by additional memories of the negative affective consequences of withdrawal. It remains to be established whether these memories are sufficiently persistent to explain the long-lasting vulnerability to relapse in apparently recovered addicts.

Footnotes

Editor's Note: These short reviews of a recent paper in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral fellows, are intended to mimic the journal clubs that exist in your own departments or institutions. For more information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

References

  1. Ahmed SH, Kenny PJ, Koob GF, Markou A. Neurobiological evidence for hedonic allostasis associated with escalating cocaine use. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:625–626. doi: 10.1038/nn872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Kenny PJ, Chen SA, Kitamura O, Markou A, Koob GF. Conditioned withdrawal drives heroin consumption and decreases reward sensitivity. J Neurosci. 2006;26:5894–5900. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0740-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Kornetsky C, Esposito RU. Euphorigenic drugs: effects on the reward pathways of the brain. Fed Proc; 1979. pp. 2473–2476. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol. 2003;54:25–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Wikler A. Dynamics of drug dependence. Implications of a conditioning theory for research and treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1973;28:611–616. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1973.01750350005001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES