Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2006 Oct 18;26(42):10879–10882. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2386-06.2006

Pain Processing Is Faster than Tactile Processing in the Human Brain

Markus Ploner 1,, Joachim Gross 1, Lars Timmermann 1, Alfons Schnitzler 1
PMCID: PMC6674749  PMID: 17050725

Abstract

Pain signals threat and drives the individual into a behavioral response that significantly depends on a short stimulus-response latency. Paradoxically, the peripheral and spinal conduction velocities of pain are much slower than of tactile information. However, cerebral processing times and reaction times of touch and pain have not yet been fully assessed. Here we show that reaction times to selective nociceptive cutaneous laser stimuli are substantially faster than expected from the peripheral conduction velocities. Furthermore, by using magnetoencephalography, we found that latencies between earliest stimulus-evoked cortical responses and reaction times are ∼60 ms shorter for nociceptive than for tactile stimuli. These findings reveal that cerebral processing of pain is substantially faster than processing of tactile information and relatively compensates for the slow peripheral and spinal conduction velocities of pain. Our observation shows how the cerebral organization of pain processing enhances motor responses to potentially harmful stimuli and thereby subserves the particular behavioral demands of pain.

Keywords: pain, nociception, touch, somatosensory cortices, reaction times, magnetoencephalography

Introduction

The physical integrity of the individual critically depends on the perception of pain. Central to the protective function of pain are fast and effective behavioral responses to potentially harmful stimuli. Accordingly, pain drives the individual into a motor response aimed at achieving safety and promoting recovery.

Research during the past decades revealed that the experience of pain is subserved by physiologically specialized neural pathways termed the nociceptive system (Craig, 2003; Willis and Westlund, 2004). The peripheral part of the nociceptive system consists of thinly myelinated Aδ fibers and unmyelinated C fibers, although some nociceptive Aβ fibers have also been described (Willis and Westlund, 2004; Meyer et al., 2006). In primates, the peripheral pain pathways and the major spinal pain pathway (Ferrington et al., 1987; Kakigi and Shibasaki, 1991; Dostrovsky and Craig, 2006) mainly show paradoxically slow conduction velocities of 10–20 m/s, which may provide a slow but particular safe pathway of nociceptive information to the brain. However, because of the low peripheral and spinal conduction velocities, pain-related information from the hand arrives at the human brain not earlier than ∼100 ms after stimulus application.

At the cortical level, an extended network is associated with pain processing, which includes primary (S1) and secondary (S2) somatosensory cortices, as well as insular, anterior cingulate, and prefrontal cortices (Bushnell and Apkarian, 2006). These areas participate in different sensory, cognitive, and affective processes and, thus, differentially contribute to the experience of pain. Time courses of pain-evoked activations (Ploner et al., 1999; Kanda et al., 2000; Ohara et al., 2004) indicate that parts of this network are organized in parallel, which contrasts to the rather serial processing of other somatosensory information (Kaas, 2004). However, within this cortical network, processing times of behavioral responses to pain have remained essentially unknown. To address this question, we recorded neuromagnetic brain responses during a reaction time experiment and directly compared latencies of cortical responses and reaction times to painful and tactile stimuli. We hypothesized that the cortical organization of pain processing may allow for particularly fast processing of pain-related information to effectively prevent harm. Our results reveal that latencies between earliest stimulus-evoked cortical responses and reaction times are ∼60 ms shorter for nociceptive than tactile stimuli. However, latencies between responses from S2 and reaction times did not differ between modalities, suggesting that the direct thalamic access of pain-related information to S2 may underlie the short central processing time of pain. These observations show how the cortical organization of pain processing relatively compensates for the slow peripheral and spinal conduction velocities of pain and thereby subserves the particular behavioral demands of pain.

Materials and Methods

Twelve healthy male subjects with a mean age of 30 years (range, 22–39 years) participated in the study. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects before participation. The study was approved by the local ethics committee and was conducted in conformity with the Declaration of Helsinki. Two subjects had to be excluded from the analysis because of an extremely low signal-to-noise ratio. Thus, analysis was based on 10 subjects.

Procedure.

In a simple reaction time experiment, each of 60 painful and nonpainful stimuli was randomly applied to the right hand. Interstimulus interval was varied between 6 and 10 s. Subjects were instructed to react as fast as possible to the stimuli by pressing a button with the index finger of the left hand. Before the experiment, a practice block of at least 20 stimuli was performed.

Stimulation.

Painful stimuli were applied to the dorsum of the right hand by using cutaneous laser stimulation, which has been shown to activate selectively nociceptive Aδ and C afferents (Bromm and Treede, 1984). The laser device was a Tm:YAG laser (Carl Baasel Lasertechnik, Starnberg, Germany) with a wavelength of 2000 nm, a pulse duration of 1 ms, and a spot diameter of 6 mm. The laser beam was led through an optical fiber from outside into the recording room. Stimulation site was slightly changed within an area of 4 × 3 cm after each stimulus. Applied stimulus intensity was twice pain threshold intensity, which resulted in stimulus intensities between 550 and 700 mJ, evoking moderately painful sensations. Mean pain rating on a numerical rating scale from 0 to 10, with end points “no pain” and “worst possible pain”, was 3.5.

Tactile stimuli were constant voltage electrical pulses of 0.3 ms duration delivered to the middle and end phalanx of the right index finger by using ring electrodes. Stimulus intensity was adjusted to twice detection threshold intensity, i.e., 12–16 V, thus inducing clear and consistent nonpainful sensations.

Recordings and analysis.

Subjects were comfortably seated with eyes closed in a magnetically shielded room. Environmental noise was masked by white noise applied to both ears.

Reaction times to painful and tactile stimuli were calculated as latency between stimulus application and button press. Reaction times shorter than 100 ms or longer than 800 ms were discarded. Median reaction times were calculated, and outliers below or above 2 SDs of the median were discarded.

Cortical activity was recorded with a Neuromag-122 whole-head neuromagnetometer (Elekta, Stockholm, Sweden) containing 122 planar SQUID gradiometers. Signals were digitized at 1020 Hz, high-pass filtered at 1 Hz, and low-pass filtered at 120 Hz. Neuromagnetic activity was averaged time-locked to application of tactile and painful stimuli. Vertical electrooculograms were used to reject epochs contaminated with blink artifacts.

Global stimulus-evoked neuromagnetic activity was calculated as root mean square of the signals of all 122 sensors corrected to baseline. Onsets of global stimulus-evoked responses were defined as time points at which cortical activity exceeds 2 SDs of baseline activity from −100 to −10 ms. The latency difference between response onsets and reaction times, termed central processing time, was determined as a measure of the central processing demands of painful and tactile stimuli in the simple reaction time experiment (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Paradigm and analysis. Stimuli were painful and tactile stimuli randomly applied to the right hand. Reaction times were button presses with the index finger of the left hand. Central processing time was defined as latency between earliest stimulus-evoked cortical responses and reaction times.

Additional analysis of somatosensory and pain-evoked fields was based on a spatiotemporal source model (Hämäläinen et al., 1993). Sources of evoked responses were modeled as equivalent current dipoles identified during clearly dipolar field patterns. Only sources accounting for >85% of the local field variance were accepted. Source locations, orientations, and strengths were calculated within a realistic head model (boundary-element model) of each subject's head determined from the individual magnetic resonance images acquired on a 1.5 T Siemens (Munich, Germany) Magnetom. Individual locations of responses were transformed to normalized Talairach space, and group mean Talairach coordinates were calculated. Time courses of activations were obtained from the spatiotemporal source model in which locations and orientations of sources were kept fixed and activation strengths were allowed to vary over time to provide the best fit for the recorded data. Onsets of local stimulus-evoked responses were defined as time points at which activation strength exceeds 2 SDs of baseline activity from −100 to −10 ms. Latencies between S1 and S2 responses to both stimuli and reaction times were calculated.

Reaction times and latencies of global and local stimulus-evoked cortical responses were compared by using Friedman's ANOVA and two-tailed Wilcoxon's signed-rank tests.

Control experiment.

Reaction times tend to shorten with an increase in stimulus intensity (Brebner and Welford, 1980). Thus, we verified whether an intensity increase of the electrical stimuli may yield a shortening of reaction times, which could explain our observation of an 60 ms difference in central processing time between modalities.

Six of the 10 subjects participated in the control experiment (mean age, 30 years; range, 22–36 years). In principle, the procedure of the control experiment was the same as in the main experiment. The only difference was that, instead of a laser and an electrical stimulus, two different intensities of electrical stimuli were applied to the right index finger. Stimuli were constant voltage electrical pulses of 0.3 ms duration delivered to the middle and end phalanx of the right index finger by using ring electrodes. Stimulus intensity was adjusted to twofold and fourfold detection threshold intensity, i.e., 16–20 and 32–40 V, respectively. Twofold detection threshold intensity induced clear and consistent nonpainful sensations, whereas fourfold detection threshold intensity induced clearly unpleasant, slightly painful sensations.

Results

Figure 2 shows individual (left) and group mean (right) results. The analysis reveals that reaction times to painful stimuli were 26 ms later than reaction times to tactile stimulation (387 ± 20 vs 361 ± 25 ms, mean ± SEM; p < 0.05, Wilcoxon's signed-rank test). Likewise, onsets of global stimulus-evoked cortical responses were later for painful than for tactile stimulation (128 ± 6 vs 36 ± 2 ms; p < 0.01). This delay of cortical and behavioral responses to pain is attributable to the lower conduction velocity of nociceptive peripheral and spinal pathways (10–20 m/s) compared with tactile pathways (∼50 m/s) (Vallbo et al., 1979; Ferrington et al., 1987; Kakigi and Shibasaki, 1991; Meyer et al., 2006).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Cortical responses, reaction times, and central processing times to painful and tactile stimuli. Left and right show individual and group mean results, respectively. Cortical responses are global stimulus-evoked responses detected by all 122 magnetoencephalographic sensors over the whole head. Lines in the bottom left connect data points from the same individual. Shaded areas in the top right time course panels depict ±SEM. Error bars refer to SEM. **p < 0.01, Wilcoxon's signed-rank test.

Calculation of central processing time as the latency between arrival of nociceptive information at the cortex and reaction time reveals a difference between pain processing and tactile processing. Central processing time was on average 62 ± 14 ms shorter for painful than for tactile stimuli (271 ± 19 vs 333 ± 25 ms; p < 0.01). This difference in central processing time was observed in all subjects. The magnitude of the difference indicates that the cortical organization of the perception–action sequence in our simple reaction time experiment differs substantially between modalities.

To specify further the different central processing times of painful and tactile stimuli, we localized early cortical responses to both stimuli and calculated time courses for each area and modality. The analysis confirms generation of early cortical responses to painful (Kakigi et al., 2005) and tactile (Hari and Forss, 1999) stimuli in S1 and S2 (Fig. 3). However, the temporal activation pattern of S1 and S2 differs between modalities. Painful stimuli nearly simultaneously activate S1 and contralateral S2 (S1, 125 ± 9 ms; contralateral S2, 117 ± 6 ms; ipsilateral S2, 124 ± 5 ms), whereas tactile stimuli sequentially activate these areas (S1, 32 ± 1 ms; contralateral S2, 67 ± 2 ms; ipsilateral S2, 88 ± 4 ms), which is in good accordance with previous investigations (Ploner et al., 1999; Kanda et al., 2000; Ohara et al., 2004), The intermodal difference in the temporal activation pattern of S1 and S2 most probably reflects a difference in the hierarchical organization of these areas. Pain-related information is conveyed in parallel from the thalamus directly to S1 and S2, whereas tactile information is transmitted serially from the thalamus to S1 and thence to S2 (Ploner et al., 1999; Kanda et al., 2000). Next, latencies between S1 and S2 responses to both stimuli and reaction times were calculated. Statistical analysis reveals a significant effect of modality (pain and tactile; p < 0.01, Friedman's ANOVA) and area (S1, contralateral S2, and ipsilateral S2; p < 0.01, Friedman's ANOVA) on latencies between local cortical responses and reaction times. Post hoc testing shows that latencies between S1 responses and reaction times differ significantly between modalities (pain, 274 ± 18 ms; tactile, 337 ± 26 ms; p < 0.01), whereas latencies between bilateral S2 responses and reaction times do not differ between modalities (contralateral S2: pain, 274 ± 18 ms; tactile, 302 ± 26 ms; p = 0.22; ipsilateral S2: pain, 275 ± 19 ms; tactile, 281 ± 25 ms; p = 0.76).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Local cortical responses, reaction times, and latencies between local cortical responses and reaction times. Locations of responses to painful and tactile stimulation were calculated as group mean normalized dipole locations. Mean coordinates of responses to painful stimuli are as follows: S1, −30, −34, 65; contralateral S2, −52, −4, 18; ipsilateral S2, 50, −8, 15. Mean coordinates of responses to tactile stimuli are as follows: S1, −46, −23, 58; contralateral S2, −56, −14, 21; ipsilateral S2 49, −10, 20. The latency difference between earliest local responses (S1, contralateral S2, and ipsilateral S2) and reaction times represents the local equivalent of the central processing time of Figure 2, which refers to global cortical responses. Shaded areas in the time course panels depict ±SEM. Error bars refer to SEM. cl, Contralateral; il, ipsilateral. *p < 0.05, Wilcoxon's signed-rank test.

In the control experiment, reaction times to stronger and painful electrical stimuli were 15 ms shorter than reaction times to standard electrical stimuli (strong, 265 ± 19 ms; standard, 280 ± 17 ms). Thus, an intensity difference between stimuli is unlikely to account for the 60 ms difference in central processing time between modalities.

Discussion

Here, we show that, in a simple reaction time experiment, cortical processing of pain is substantially faster than processing of tactile information. To the very best of our knowledge, no study has yet directly compared the central processing times of selective nociceptive and tactile stimuli. Reaction times of the present study are in good accordance with previous studies investigating reaction times to painful laser stimuli (Campbell and LaMotte, 1983; Pertovaara et al., 1984; Arendt-Nielsen and Bjerring, 1988). A single study directly compared reaction times to both stimuli (Pertovaara et al., 1984). In this study, reaction times to painful stimuli were ∼60 ms longer than reaction times to tactile stimulation. Considering the longer receptor activation latency of their nociceptive stimulus, these results correspond well to our findings.

The delay of cortical responses and reaction times to painful stimuli compared with tactile stimuli is attributable to the low conduction velocity of nociceptive afferents. Peripheral (Meyer et al., 2006) and spinal (Ferrington et al., 1987; Kakigi and Shibasaki, 1991) nociceptive afferents conduct at ∼10–20 m/s, whereas conduction velocities of tactile afferents amount to ∼50 m/s (Vallbo et al., 1979). Consequently, in intracranial and extracranial recordings, earliest responses to selective nociceptive stimuli from the hand are typically recorded at ∼120 ms (Ploner et al., 1999; Kanda et al., 2000; Ohara et al., 2004; Kakigi et al., 2005). Only recently, S1 responses to painful stimuli at latencies of ∼90 ms have been reported (Inui et al., 2003), but even these latencies cannot explain our present observation of a 60 ms difference in central processing time between modalities. The functional significance of the slow peripheral and spinal conduction velocities of pain has remained unknown. However, the thinly myelinated nociceptive afferents are less vulnerable to mechanical, thermal, and chemical interference than the faster conducting tactile fibers. Thus, the peripheral conduction of pain may provide a slow but particular safe pathway of nociceptive information to the brain.

Assuming a common final motor pathway of behavioral responses to painful and tactile stimuli, our results indicate that the cortical organization of sensorimotor transformation differs between modalities. A difference in intensity is unlikely to account for the intermodal difference in sensorimotor transformation. The control experiment shows that even more intense tactile stimuli do not yield a shortening of processing times in the order of 60 ms. Possibly, the difference in central processing time is caused by an intermodal difference in sensory pathways to the motor system. Anatomical data indicate three plausible candidates for conveying pain-related information to the motor system. These areas are the midcingulate cortex (Vogt et al., 2004), the posterior parietal cortex, and S2 (Kaas, 2004). Based on neither previous nor present data can we decide which area represents the crucial relay to the motor system. However, the cross-modally stable temporal relationship between S2 responses and reaction times would be well compatible with an important role of S2 for conveying somatosensory information to the motor system. Such a relay function of S2 is supported by anatomical (Krubitzer and Kaas, 1990; Disbrow et al., 2003), neurophysiological (Huttunen et al., 1996; Forss and Jousmaki, 1998), and functional imaging (Ledberg et al., 1995; Binkofski et al., 1999) evidence. Thus, in the present simple reaction time experiment, both tactile and pain-related information may be routed to the motor system via the S2 region. The parallel organization of pain processing with a direct thalamic access of pain-related information to S2 may then allow for the particular short central processing time of pain.

In conclusion, the present findings show that, in a simple reaction time experiment, cortical processing of pain is faster than processing of tactile information. The short cortical processing time of pain may be subserved by the parallel organization of somatosensory cortices in human pain processing, which relatively compensates for the slow peripheral and spinal conduction velocities of nociceptive pathways, enhances motor responses to potentially harmful stimuli, and thereby subserves the particular behavioral demands of pain.

Footnotes

This work was supported by the German Ministry of Education and Science (Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung Grant 01GW0533). We thank C. Ploner, B. Hommel and L. Plaghki for helpful comments on this manuscript.

References

  1. Arendt-Nielsen L, Bjerring P. Reaction times to painless and painful CO2 and argon laser stimulation. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol. 1988;58:266–273. doi: 10.1007/BF00417261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Binkofski F, Buccino G, Posse S, Seitz RJ, Rizzolatti G, Freund H. A fronto-parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: evidence from an fMRI-study. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:3276–3286. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00753.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Brebner JMT, Welford AT. Introduction: an historical background sketch. In: Welford AT, editor. Reaction times. London: Academic; 1980. pp. 1–23. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bromm B, Treede RD. Nerve fibre discharges, cerebral potentials and sensations induced by CO2 laser stimulation. Hum Neurobiol. 1984;3:33–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bushnell MC, Apkarian AV. Representation of pain in the brain. In: McMahon SB, Koltzenburg M, editors. Wall and Melzack's textbook of pain. Philadelphia: Elsevier; 2006. pp. 107–124. [Google Scholar]
  6. Campbell JN, LaMotte RH. Latency to detection of first pain. Brain Res. 1983;266:203–208. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90650-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Craig AD. Pain mechanisms: labeled lines versus convergence in central processing. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2003;26:1–30. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Disbrow E, Litinas E, Recanzone GH, Padberg J, Krubitzer L. Cortical connections of the second somatosensory area and the parietal ventral area in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 2003;462:382–399. doi: 10.1002/cne.10731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dostrovsky JO, Craig AD. Ascending projection systems. In: McMahon SB, Koltzenburg M, editors. Wall and Melzack's textbook of pain. Philadelphia: Elsevier; 2006. pp. 187–203. [Google Scholar]
  10. Ferrington DG, Sorkin LS, Willis WD., Jr Responses of spinothalamic tract cells in the superficial dorsal horn of the primate lumbar spinal cord. J Physiol (Lond) 1987;388:681–703. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Forss N, Jousmaki V. Sensorimotor integration in human primary and secondary somatosensory cortices. Brain Res. 1998;781:259–267. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)01240-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hämäläinen M, Hari R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Knuutila J, Lounasmaa OV. Magnetoencephalography: theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain. Rev Mod Phys. 1993;65:413–497. [Google Scholar]
  13. Hari R, Forss N. Magnetoencephalography in the study of human somatosensory cortical processing. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1999;354:1145–1154. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1999.0470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Huttunen J, Wikstrom H, Korvenoja A, Seppalainen AM, Aronen H, Ilmoniemi RJ. Significance of the second somatosensory cortex in sensorimotor integration: enhancement of sensory responses during finger movements. NeuroReport. 1996;7:1009–1012. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199604100-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Inui K, Tran TD, Qiu Y, Wang X, Hoshiyama M, Kakigi R. A comparative magnetoencephalographic study of cortical activations evoked by noxious and innocuous somatosensory stimulations. Neuroscience. 2003;120:235–248. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00261-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kaas JH. Somatosensory system. In: Paxinos G, Mai JK, editors. The human nervous system. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  17. Kakigi R, Shibasaki H. Estimation of conduction velocity of the spino-thalamic tract in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1991;80:39–45. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(91)90041-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kakigi R, Inui K, Tamura Y. Electrophysiological studies on human pain perception. Clin Neurophysiol. 2005;116:743–763. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kanda M, Nagamine T, Ikeda A, Ohara S, Kunieda T, Fujiwara N, Yazawa S, Sawamoto N, Matsumoto R, Taki W, Shibasaki H. Primary somatosensory cortex is actively involved in pain processing in human. Brain Res. 2000;853:282–289. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(99)02274-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Krubitzer LA, Kaas JH. The organization and connections of somatosensory cortex in marmosets. J Neurosci. 1990;10:952–974. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-03-00952.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ledberg A, O'Sullivan BT, Kinomura S, Roland PE. Somatosensory activations of the parietal operculum of man. A PET study. Eur J Neurosci. 1995;7:1934–1941. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1995.tb00716.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Meyer RA, Ringkamp M, Campbell JN, Raja SN. Peripheral mechanisms of cutaneous nociception. In: McMahon SB, Koltzenburg M, editors. Wall and Melzack's textbook of pain. Philadelphia: Elsevier; 2006. pp. 3–34. [Google Scholar]
  23. Ohara S, Crone NE, Weiss N, Treede RD, Lenz FA. Amplitudes of laser evoked potential recorded from primary somatosensory, parasylvian and medial frontal cortex are graded with stimulus intensity. Pain. 2004;110:318–328. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Pertovaara A, Reinikainen K, Hari R. The activation of unmyelinated or myelinated afferent fibers by brief infrared laser pulses varies with skin type. Brain Res. 1984;307:341–343. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(84)90490-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ploner M, Schmitz F, Freund HJ, Schnitzler A. Parallel activation of primary and secondary somatosensory cortices in human pain processing. J Neurophysiol. 1999;81:3100–3104. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.6.3100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Vallbo AB, Hagbarth KE, Torebjork HE, Wallin BG. Somatosensory, proprioceptive, and sympathetic activity in human peripheral nerves. Physiol Rev. 1979;59:919–957. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1979.59.4.919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Vogt BA, Hof PR, Vogt LJ. Cingulate gyrus. In: Paxinos G, Mai JK, editors. The human nervous system. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2004. pp. 915–949. [Google Scholar]
  28. Willis WD, Westlund KN. Pain system. In: Paxinos G, Mai JK, editors. The human nervous system. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2004. pp. 1125–1170. [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES