Abstract
Language acquisition is characterized by progressive use of inflectional morphology marking verb tense and agreement. Linguistic milestones are also linked to left-brain lateralization for language specialization. We used neuroimaging (fNIRS) to investigate how bilingual exposure influences children’s cortical organization for processing morpho-syntax. In Study 1, monolinguals and bilinguals (n=39) completed a grammaticality judgment task that included English sentences with violations in earlier- (verb agreement) and later-acquired (verb tense/agreement) structures. Groups showed similar performance and greater activation in left inferior frontal region (IFG) for later- than earlier-acquired conditions. Bilinguals showed stronger and more restricted left IFG activation. In Study 2, bilinguals completed a comparable Spanish task revealing patterns of left IFG activation similar to English. Taken together, the findings suggest that bilinguals with linguistic competence at parity with monolingual counterparts have a higher degree of cortical specialization for language, likely a result of enriched linguistic experiences.
Keywords: children, bilingualism, fNIRS, language, proficiency, grammaticality, morphology, syntax, inferior frontal gyrus, neuroimaging
1. Introduction
A fundamental question in child brain research is how different cortical regions develop their functional specificity for higher cognitive functions (Johnson, 2011). In the early stages of development, infants show left-lateralized brain activity for language processing (Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2006; Peña et al., 2003), although some work suggests both infants and young children show a more widespread, and at times bilateral, response to language in comparison to adults (Holland et al., 2007; Imada et al., 2006; Nuñez et al., 2011). It is generally accepted that such developmental differences are related to linguistic experiences, as well as to neural maturational processes (Garcia-Sierra et al., 2011; Werker & Hensch, 2015); however, the precise nature of these dynamics is still not well understood. The present study examines the degree to which neural specialization for language, characterized by increasingly focal left-lateralized activation as monolinguals (e.g., Knoll et al., 2012), functions similarly in early Spanish-English bilinguals, and across both of their languages when receiving early and systematic dual-language exposure.
The Dual Language System Hypothesis of early bilingual acquisition holds that children exposed to two languages from birth undergo parallel and language-specific courses for each of their languages in the first years of life (Genesee, 1989; DeHouwer, 1995; Du, 2010). Early language acquisition in many of the world’s languages (e.g., English, German, Russian, Hebrew) is marked by an extended period of mastering verb finiteness, stemming from young children’s inability to compute tense and agreement simultaneously (Wexler, 1998; Schütze & Wexler, 1996), or from the ambiguity of the finite forms (Tomasello, 2000). Of particular relevance to the present study are Spanish-English bilinguals: Children acquiring English may produce ‘bare’ verb errors, such as “He bake cookies” instead of either “He bakes cookies” or “He baked cookies” (3rd person singular in present/past tenses). While children acquiring a Romance language (e.g., Spanish, Italian) may produce overgeneralizations of 3rd person singular (Grinstead et al., 2013, 2014), omissions of noun plural inflections, and errors on gender markings, especially for direct object clitics (Bedore & Leonard, 2001, 2005; Dominguez, 2003; Guasti, 1993, 2002; Pratt & Grinstead, 2007; Restrepo & Gutiérrez-Clellen, 2001). The acquisition of inflectional morphology in Spanish-English bilinguals is therefore language-specific and dependent on children’s experiences in each of their languages (Peña, Bedore & Kester, 2015).
Neurolinguistic Perspectives on (Dual) Language Processing
Neurolinguistic theoretical perspectives suggest that language processing is supported by a dual-stream model: a ventral stream supporting sound to meaning mapping and a dorsal stream supporting auditory perception to motor production (Hickok & Poeppel, 2007; Friederici, 2011, 2012; Saur et al., 2008). The dorsal stream is involved in morpho-syntactic processes during sentence comprehension, where the arcuate fascile (AF) and the superior longitudinal fascicle (SLF) transfer syntactic information between the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and left temporal regions (Friederici, 2011, 2012; Skeide, Brauer, & Friederici, 2016; Skeide & Friederici, 2016). Specifically, left IFG supports both semantic and morpho-syntactic computations (BA 44 and BA 45 respectively), posterior left superior temporal gyrus (STG) supports access to morpho-phonemic forms, and the anterior left STG supports automated phrase structure computations (Friederici, 2011, 2012; Skeide & Friederici, 2016). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies reveal that adult-like cortical organization for morpho-syntactic processing involves robust brain activity in left IFG for complex sentences that include morphemic features acquired later in language development and are less frequent in language structures (such as case assignment), in comparison to morphemic features acquired earlier or structures that occur more frequently in speech (such as canonical word order) (Luke et al., 2002; Knoll et al., 2012; Skeide et al., 2016; Skeide & Friederici, 2016).
Young children (as early as 24-months) can detect morpho-syntactic violations such as–ing omission in English (e.g., He is go(ing) home) by showing a P600 event related potential (ERP) response within milliseconds of the errors (Oberecker & Friederici, 2006; see also Silva-Pereyra et al., 2005). Nevertheless, their cortical response to language remains unadult-like until 9–10 years old (Skeide & Friederici, 2016; see also Nuñez et al., 2011). Research with monolingual children’s pattern of cortical brain activity for morpho-syntactic structures suggests a protracted trajectory that initially involves both hemispheres as well as widely spread within the left hemisphere. As children’s linguistic competence improves, their neural response to morpho-syntactic constructions becomes more specialized and focal within left-hemisphere regions (German-speaking monolingual children: Skeide & Friederici, 2016).
Critically, multiple factors must be taken into account when considering potential sources of variability in children’s neural specialization for language, including learning aptitude, language experience, and neuro-physiological brain maturation (particularly white matter tracts AF and SLF) (Nuñez et al., 2011; Silva-Pereyra et al., 2005; Skeide et al., 2016; Skeide & Friederici, 2016). For instance, young monolingual German-speaking children with greater linguistic competence demonstrated stronger (more adult-like) left IFG brain activation for complex sentences with errors on morphemic structures than their lower-performing peers. In contrast, lower-performing peers showed left IFG brain activity for simpler sentences with errors on canonical sentence structures (Knoll et al., 2012). This developmental shift in the left IFG activation pattern reflects the regions’ processing effort allocated to acquiring and developing automaticity for high-frequency regularities of the given language (Knoll et al., 2012). Given these monolingual outcomes, one could therefore expect that early Spanish-English dual-language learners of similar age and language proficiency as their monolingual counterparts should show similar patterns of left IFG activation when processing later- versus earlier-acquired grammatical structures, and do so for each of their languages.
A more comprehensive representation of dual linguistic systems, suggesting their independency and interdependency, has been extensively outlined by the bilingual field (Cook, 1991, 2003; Roeper 1999; Satterfield 1999; Vihman, 2002). Most recently, the ‘Integrated Multilingual Model’ of bilingualism offered an advancement by proposing that bilinguals can indeed become competent in each of their languages, while also recognizing that the two linguistic systems interact (MacSwan, 2017). Of the many implications of this interaction, the essential claim is that as Grosjean (1989) suggests, bilinguals are “not two monolinguals in one brain.” Rather, features of bilinguals’ both languages interact and influence each other (phonologically, lexically, syntactically), thereby shaping the bilinguals’ mind and brain differently from those of monolinguals’ (Grosjean, 1989; Kroll, Dussias, Bice, & Perrotti, 2015). This view gains support from behavioral research showing that in comparison to monolinguals, bilingual adults with childhood exposure to Spanish and English were more likely to attend to word order cues (a salient feature of English) when processing Spanish, but agreement cues (a salient feature of Spanish) figured prominently when processing English (Hernandez, Bates & Avila, 1994). Building on this evidence, a study by Kovelman, Baker and Petitto (2008) asked early and proficient Spanish-English bilingual adults to make plausibility judgments for sentences with varied syntactic complexity in English and Spanish, and found that bilinguals showed greater left IFG activation than monolinguals across all sentence types in English, suggesting a neurodevelopmental adaptation for acquiring and processing two linguistic systems (Kovelman et al., 2008). Thus, child bilinguals might also show evidence of greater left IFG recruitment, or other neuro-developmental differences in cortical specialization for language that reflect differences in morpho-syntactic processing between bilinguals and monolinguals (see also Parker-Jones et al., 2012; Román et al., 2015).
The largest portion of knowledge on bilingual brain organization comes from research on adults presenting uniform effects of age-of-acquisition and proficiency (Costa & Sebastián-Gallés, 2014; Felton et al., 2017; Garcia-Pentón et al., 2014; Garcia-Sierra et al., 2011; Klein et al., 2014; Li et al., 2014; Mechelli et al., 2004; Petitto et al., 2012). Studies on age of acquisition (AoA) suggest that learning a second language (L2) after the age of 5 results in reduced language proficiency and non-native patterns of second language organization in the brain (Berken et al., 2015; Perani et al., 1998; Hernandez & Li, 2007; Jasinska & Petitto, 2013; see also Archila-Suerte, Zevin, & Hernandez, 2015). While there is some evidence to suggest that high proficiency bilinguals with a later age of acquisition can show native-like response to their second language, the studies that demonstrate this effect typically present speakers with single word recognition tasks, semantic judgment tasks, or whole text comprehension tasks (Chee et al., 1999; Hasegawa et al., 2002; Vingerhoets et al., 2003; Perani et al., 1998; Roncaglia-Denissen & Kotz, 2016). In contrast, studies that specifically target syntactic processes typically find evidence of AoA even in highly proficient late speakers of the language. For instance, Wartenburget et al. (2003) found reduced parietal activation in highly proficient late learners in their L2 (German), relative to their L1 (Spanish). At the same time, the majority of these studies was conducted with adult bilinguals and offers a retrospective view on the effects of bilingualism on adults’ neural organization for language. Thus, the question remains open as to the developmental processes underlying the effects of bilingualism on the neural organization for language in children who are in the process of developing syntax competence and automaticity as well as the neural specificity for syntactic processing.
The Present Studies
The present work examines the influence of early dual-language exposure on children’s brain organization for processing inflectional morphology. To test this, we chose to use grammaticality judgments as the experimental method since it fits several key criteria. First, grammaticality judgment tasks can uncover general patterns of language, whereas language production is coupled with environmental constraints (Phillips, 2009). For instance, Bley-Vroman & Kweon (2011) show that adults who produced ‘wanna’ violations in an elicited production task were actually sensitive to its constraints in parallel grammaticality judgment tasks. Second, while some work has used canonical sentence structures when testing children’s judgments in grammaticality tasks (e.g., Jasinksa & Petitto, 2013), we chose to test finiteness structures since morpho-syntactic features constitute a critical component of early language acquisition (Rice, Wexler, & Hershberger, 1998). Third, children’s proficiency in finiteness is typically operationalized through measures that include both elicitation and judgment procedures–this research finds that children’s production of finiteness in obligatory contexts parallels their comprehension accuracy (Rice, Wexler, & Redmond, 1999). Critically, while typically-developing monolingual children reach adult-like accuracy in their production and judgment of finiteness, or simultaneous tense/agreement, by around age 6 (Rice, Wexler, & Cleave, 1995), the automaticity of these processes and their cortical organization continue to develop during the ages of 6–12, and possibly beyond (Skeide & Friederici, 2016). Thus, grammaticality judgments of finiteness effectively tap into children’s early language acquisition, and will allow us to situate the findings within the rich body of monolingual language acquisition and neurolinguistic literature that commonly grammaticality judgments tasks to inform our understanding of the impact of dual-language experiences on language acquisition.
In the present set of experiments, child participants underwent functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) neuroimaging covering bilateral frontal lobes, including inferior, middle and superior frontal regions (IFG, MFG, SFG). In Study 1, Spanish-English bilinguals and English monolinguals (ages 6–12) completed an English grammaticality judgment task, in which they heard well-structured sentences, sentences that violated the structure of relatively earlier-acquired elements of morpho-syntax in English (progressive -ing and “to be” agreement, as in “He was go to the store”), and later-acquired functional features of morpho-syntax (finiteness, or simultaneous tense/agreement as in “Yesterday he go to the store”). In Study 2, the same bilingual participants completed a comparable Spanish grammaticality judgment task, including sentences that violated relatively earlier-acquired elements of morpho-syntax in Spanish (subject-adjective gender disagreements across singular and plural subjects), and later-acquired functional features of morpho-syntax (finiteness, or simultaneous tense/agreement).
The present study contrasts two morpho-syntactic violations acquired at different times in language development. Children as young as 24-months elicit a P600 ERP response when hearing present participle–ing violations in sentences (Oberecker & Friederici, 2006). Yet English-speaking children still produce syntactic errors until age 5; specifically omissions in copular, present-tense 3rd person singular (–s) and past tense (–ed), likely stemming from children’s inability to compute tense and agreement at the same time (Wexler, 1998). As a result, the acquisition of temporal syntactic markers is delayed and otherwise perceived as more “complex” than that of the present participle–ing morpheme. Thus by setting up the earlier (easier) and later (harder) contrast as represented by–ing and–s/-ed markers, we evaluate both the emerging complexity of syntactic processing, through expressions of agreement and tense, as well as the bilingual child’s maturing linguistic competence that gradually comes to rely upon the syntactic system. By manipulating the developmental time course of morphemic features into the experimental conditions, it allows us to control for some “violation-general” aspects of cognition and to better isolate key aspects of morpho-syntactic processing.
To advance our understanding of how early bilingual experience influences children’s neural specialization for language function, we have built our hypotheses upon the Integrated Multilingual Model (MacSwan, 2017), leading to the prediction that early dual-language learners will show greater left IFG response to later- versus earlier-acquired morpho-syntactic features of English, as compared to English monolinguals (English: Study 1), and in both of their languages (Spanish: Study 2). We claim that these outcomes will demonstrate that Spanish-English bilinguals form neural indexes of morpho-syntactic competence specific to each of their languages, and do so in a way that is similar to monolinguals. We further predict that young bilinguals also present a “neural signature” of early dual language processing, such as greater left IFG activation relative to monolinguals, as previously reported for adult studies (Kovelman et al., 2008)–suggesting that dual language experience is associated with neurodevelopmental adjustments for accommodating the learning of the two language systems.
2. Study 1: English Morpho-syntactic Processing in Monolingual and Bilingual Children
2.1 Methods
Participants
Thirty-nine neurotypical children, with no history of developmental, learning, or hearing deficits took part in the study: 21 Spanish-English speaking bilinguals (8 females, 13 males; age range = 6.8–12.4 years, mean age [Mage] = 10.09, standard deviation [SD] = 1.50) and 18 English speaking monolinguals (9 females, 9 males; age range = 6.6–13.6 years, Mage = 9.64, SD = 1.75). Selection criteria for bilingual participants were as follows: Spanish exposure from birth, English exposure prior to age 5, which is the period demonstrating that children can be classified as ‘bilingual L1s’ where they are simultaneously acquiring two first languages; a 3 years minimum of English exposure prior to testing, adequate dual-language competence including morpho-syntactic abilities (at least 60% accuracy) as assessed by the Word Structure subtest in the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-4; Semel, Wiig & Secord, 2003, 2006) and a standard score above 85 in English and Spanish receptive vocabulary abilities as assessed by the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (KBIT-2) Verbal Knowledge subtest (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004) and Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test Spanish Bilingual Edition (ROWPVT-4; Brownell, 2000).
Bilingual families were invited to two experimental sessions within a 5 week interval: one in English (Study 1) and one in Spanish (Study 2), with language order counterbalanced across participants. Of the total sample, three bilinguals completed only the Spanish language visit (Study 2), and 3 bilinguals’ English data exceeded signal-to-noise ration criterion (see fNIRS methods below). The final sample included 15 bilinguals (6 females, 9 males; age range = 6.9–12.4 years, M=10.7, SD=1.3) and all 18 monolingual participants (see above for sample details).
Parents of monolingual children reported their child’s use of English as the primary language at home and at school. Parents of bilingual children reported their child’s continued and systematic daily use of both of languages, with Spanish in the home and English at school and in the broader community. Six (40%) bilingual children were born outside of the U.S. in a Spanish-speaking country. All parents, with the exception of one father, were native Spanish speakers and reported consistent use of Spanish with their children. All bilingual children were first exposed to Spanish at birth and to English between birth and age 4 (M = 2.11, SD = 1.84), and had obtained a minimum of 3 years of English exposure at the time of testing (M = 8.22, SD = 2.70). Eleven (73%) bilinguals were also attending a local Spanish heritage language school once a week and completed Spanish language homework assignments for the school during the week.
Participating families, bilingual and monolingual, were recruited from the same neighborhoods in southeast Michigan (United States, U.S.), covering the same school districts with English-only schools. According to data from the World Bank (2017), both groups of families would be classified as middle-income, yet monolingual families had a higher socioeconomic background than the bilingual families (see Table 1 fore more details). Children were right-handed (except one bilingual child who was left-handed, yet their imaging data did not differ from other children in the bilingual group and was thus included in analyses). The studies were approved by institutional review boards; parents and children completed respective informed consent and assent forms. Families received monetary compensation and children received a small toy (e.g., Frisbee) as a thank you gift.
Table 1.
Monolingual and Bilingual participants’ Mean (and Standard Deviation) Scores.
Measures | Bilinguals | Monolinguals |
T-values Equal variances not assumed (df) |
---|---|---|---|
Age | 10.19 (1.43) | 9.53 (1.79) | 1.30 (31.78) |
IQ | 113.48 (12.00) | 114.67 (14.25) | 0.28 (33.43) |
Demographics | |||
Income a | 7.22 (2.62) | 8.56 (0.81) | 2.06 (20.60)* |
Mother’s education b | 5.05 (2.70) | 6.94 (1.52) | 2.52 (34.23)* |
Father’s education b | 5.45 (2.28) | 6.76 (1.52) | 2.44 (36.32)* |
English Behavioral Measures | |||
Phonological Awareness (%) | 80.71 (19.51) | 79.44 (16.17) | 0.22 (36.97) |
Phonological Memory (%) | 70.28 (11.44) | 65.12 (13.31) | 1.27 (33.76) |
Vocabulary | 111.79 (18.70) | 117.22 (11.66) | 1.11 (34.02) |
Morpho-syntax (%) | 88.84 (13.57) | 91.49 (8.70) | 0.74 (34.45) |
Spanish Behavioral Measures | |||
Phonological Awareness (%) | 84.52 (20.06) | -- | 1.82 (20) |
Phonological Memory (%) | 67.78 (10.60) | -- | 0.90 (20) |
Vocabulary | 110.14 (22.23) | -- | 0.39 (20) |
Morpho-syntax (%) | 73.40 (21.21) | -- | 3.64 (20)** |
English Grammaticality Judgment Task | |||
Accuracy (%) | |||
Overall | 92.82 (8.88) | 94.81 (2.57) | 0.89 (18.53) |
Corrects | 95.14 (9.00) | 96.26 (4.27) | 0.47 (22.59) |
Errors for Early-acquired | 96.69 (6.64) | 98.33 (2.97) | 0.94 (21.88) |
Errors for Later-acquired | 86.69 (12.98) | 89.85 (6.38) | 0.91 (23.00) |
Reaction Time (ms) | |||
Overall | 2294.94 (253.98) | 2501.99 (393.58) | 1.86 (29.25) |
Corrects | 2357.94 (270.78) | 2617.23 (436.22) | 2.13* (28.64) |
Errors for Early-acquired | 2102.40 (330.70) | 2269.69 (458.22) | 1.24 (30.93) |
Errors for Later-acquired | 2445.62 (284.79) | 2645.69 (346.64) | 1.87 (32.40) |
Spanish Grammaticality Judgment Task | |||
Accuracy (%) | |||
Overall | 85.29 (8.53) | -- | -- |
Corrects | 87.79 (12.38) | -- | -- |
Errors for Early-acquired | 78.33 (22.18) | -- | -- |
Errors for Later-acquired | 89.78 (9.22) | -- | -- |
Reaction Time (ms) | |||
Overall | 3144.09 (787.08) | -- | -- |
Corrects | 3131.12 (754.87) | -- | -- |
Errors for Early-acquired | 3191.29 (862.37) | -- | -- |
Errors for Later-acquired | 3181.89 (862.37) | -- | -- |
Notes.
p < 0.05
p < 0.01
Scores for IQ and vocabulary are standardized at a mean of 100 (typical average scores range between 85 and 115); Scores for phonological awareness and memory are reported as percentage scores (%).
Options for demographic responses on yearly household income were the following: (1) less than $5,000; (2) $5,000–$11,999; (3) $12,000–$15,999; (4) $16,000–$24,999; (5) $25,000–$34,999; (6) $35,000–$49,999; (7) 50,000–$74,999; (8) $75,000–$99,999; (9) $100,000 and greater. Three sets of parents (2 in the monolingual group and 1 in the bilingual group) did not respond this question.
Options for responses on education were the following: (1) primary school, (2) some secondary school, (3) High school diploma or equivalent (GED), (4) some college, (5) Associate’s degree, (6) Bachelor’s degree, (7) Master’s degree, (8) Doctorate degree [Ph.D], (9) Professional degree [MD, DD, DDS, etc]. One set of parents in the monolingual group did not respond to this question.
Measures of Language, Literacy, and Cognitive Development
Parents completed a detailed Language Background and Use questionnaire (LBU; Kovelman et al., 2008) about their child’s cognitive, language and motor development, plus any family history of learning impairments. Parents also completed questions on their educational level and household income from the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Socioeconomic Status and Health questionnaire (retrieved from: www.macses.ucsf.edu).
Phonological Awareness
In English, children completed the Elision subtest from the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP; Wagner, Torgesen & Rashotte, 1999). During testing, the experimenter asked the child to say a word, and then to repeat it without saying a portion of it. For example, “Say winter, now say winter without saying /t/,” the correct response is “winner.” Similarly, in Spanish, the children completed Elision subtest from the Test of Phonological Processing in Spanish (TOPPS) (Francis et al., 2001). Participants earned 1 point for correct items; the task included 6 practice items and 20 testing items. Testing began on the first item and stopped when the ceiling item was reached (or 3 consecutive errors). Percentage scores (out of a total of 20 items) are reported.
Phonological Short-Term Memory
In English, children completed the CTOPP Nonword subtest (Wagner et al., 1999). The experimenter played recordings of made-up words and asked the participant to repeat it as he/she heard it, for example, “nigong;” non-words ranged in length from 3 to 15 phonemic sounds, 3 practice items and 18 testing items were presented. Similarly, in Spanish, children completed the TOPPS Nonword subtest (Francis et al., 2001). Testing stopped when ceiling level was reached (3 consecutive errors); percentage scores (out of a total of 18 items) are reported.
Vocabulary
In English, children completed the KBIT-2 Verbal Knowledge subtest (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004). During testing, the experimenter presented the child with a matrix of 6 images and a question or a word, the participant pointed to the best representing picture. In Spanish, the children completed the ROWPVT-4 Spanish Bilingual Edition (Brownell, 2000), which is a standardized assessment normed with Spanish-English bilinguals. Similar to the English Vocabulary assessment, the experimenter presented the child with a matrix of 4 images and a word. Basal and ceiling levels were established for both measures; standard scores (M=100, SD=15) are reported.
Morphosyntax in both languages was assessed using CELF-4 Word Structure subtest (Semel et al., 2003, 2006). The assessments measure participants’ ability to apply morphology rules and appropriate pronouns. The English measure included 32 items and the Spanish measure included 29 items; percentage scores are reported.
Non-verbal Intelligence was assessed using the KBIT-2 Matrices subtest (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004), which measures the ability to find spatial and abstract relationships among a set of images and patterns. During testing, children selected the missing piece in a ‘puzzle’ (out of 4 options). Basal and ceiling levels were established; standard scores (M=100, SD=15) are reported.
English Neuroimaging Measure: Auditory Grammaticality Judgment Task
The task was modeled after the comprehension portion of the Test of Early Grammatical Impairment (TEGI; Rice & Wexler, 2001; Rice et al., 1999). Children heard correctly structured sentences, sentences with errors on early-acquired morphemes, and sentences with errors on later-acquired morphemes. During sentences with errors on Later acquired morphemic features, children heard sentences with 3 types of errors: past-tense omission (regular or irregular verbs; e.g. “Yesterday, he try to win” or “Yesterday, he eat supper”), present-tense 3rd person singular omission (e.g. “She always copy her brother”), and copular omissions before adjectival and prepositional phrases (e.g. “We very wet and cold” or “She behind the yellow door”). During sentences with errors on Early-acquired morphemic features, children heard sentences that are generally judged ungrammatical by children (Rice et al., 1999), including: present participle “-ing” omission (e.g. “He is wash the car”) and subject/verb agreement errors (e.g. “Dad are washing the car”). During Correct sentences, children heard well-structured sentences that were similar to the sentences in the Early and Later acquired morpheme conditions, including: past tense (“Last week, I saw Dad”), 3rd person singular (“She always copies my answers”), copular (“We are outside the tent”), use of -ing (“He is helping his brother”), and use of agreement (“Dad is washing the car”).
Stimuli consisted of five-word sentences matched across conditions for verb and noun, age of acquisition, written frequency, concreteness, imageability, and familiarity (data from MRC Psycholinguistic database, Coltheart, 1981; one-way ANOVA, ps > 0.05 within each condition; ad-hoc t-tests comparing the conditions were also non-significant, ps > 0.05). The sentences were all recorded by the same adult female native speaker of American English and were equalized for RMS amplitude using Praat (Boersma & Weenink, 2008). Average sentence duration was 1.61s (SD = 0.16).
The task was an event-related design with a total of 20 trials per condition (randomized using OptSeq2; Dale, 1999). During each trial, children saw a cartoon image of an alien for 4-seconds at the onset of the sentence, followed by a 2-second display of a question mark in the center of the screen that allowed additional time for the participant to respond. The task contained 25% Correct sentences, 25% Early morpheme errors, 25% Later morpheme errors, and 25% jittered Rest periods. During the jittered Rest periods, participants saw a fixation cross in the center of the screen. The task lasted ~7.5 minutes and was presented using E-Prime 2 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.) on a 23-inch Philips 230E Wide LCD screen connected to a Dell Optiplex 780 desktop computer; sound played via two Creative Inspire T12 2.0 multimedia speakers. A two-button response box (Current Designs, Inc.) was connected to the desktop computer to record participants’ responses. Trials were deemed incorrect if the participant pressed the incorrect button, or did not respond. Performance was assessed by accuracy and response time.
Procedure
Following an initial phone screening, participants were invited for a testing session to a child-friendly fNIRS brain-imaging laboratory. During the present study, children interacted solely with English-speaking experimenters. Following the consenting procedure, participants first underwent fNIRS brain imaging and then completed assessments of English language and cognition. During this fNIRS session, participants also completed two unrelated measures of cognition. The session lasted up to 2 hours with short breaks in-between.
Experimenters first set the fNIRS cap and optodes in place, and pictures of the probe placement were taken. Prior to completing the neuroimaging task, children were told a story about an alien that was learning to speak English and needed help learning when he was making a mistake. Children were instructed to help the alien by pressing buttons in a button-box: if a sentence was correct and did not have any mistakes, children used their right hand to press the right button, and if the sentence had mistakes, children used their left hand to press the left button. Children were instructed to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible, without waiting for the question mark to appear if they knew the answer before then. A brief practice consisting of six sentences (which were not used during testing) was administered. During practice trials, the experimenter provided feedback, however no feedback was provided during testing.
Functional NIRS Data Acquisition, Processing, and Analyses
The study used a TechEN-CW6 system with 690 and 830 nm wavelengths. The set-up included 4 emitters of near-infrared light and 12 detectors spaced ~2.7 cm apart, yielding 16 data channels sampled at 10 Hz (8 channels per hemisphere; see Figure 1). Sensors were mounted into a custom-built head cap constructed from polyester cloth, with attached grommets to hold the emitters and detectors during data collection. The probes covered bilateral prefrontal cortex including IFG, MFG and SFG. The probe localization was established and applied consistently for each participant using the international 10-10 transcranial system positioning (Jurcak, Tsuzuki & Dan, 2007); Inion, Nasion, Fz, FpZ, Cz, auricular left and right, and F7/8 were measured for each participant, and the two lower bilateral sources were anchored at F7 and F8.
Figure 1.
Functional NIRS probe configuration. In order to visualize and estimate the brain regions maximally covered by the channels, we estimated approximate MNI coordinates using the geometric structure of our measurement setting for the 16 optodes (emitters and detectors). We used reference points to equally distribute 1,000 points along the distance of each channel (between each source and detector pair). The voxel points were the distance partitioned to 1,000 sections for a distance of ~2.7cm on a 3D image brain template provided by https://irc.cchmc.org/software/pedbrain.php. The corresponding brain regions (Brodmann areas, BA) were then estimated using xjView database (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview) in MATLAB. The brain areas covered by the 1000 points distributed along each channel are recognized as the brain areas covered by that channel. If a channel covered more than one area, the area indices were arranged in sequence according to the proportion of the 1000 points falling within the given areas. We used this brain template with interpolated optodes and the “patch” function in MATLAB to generate 3D images to display the imaging results. (A) Dots correspond to optode placements at a distance of ~2.7 cm, over an average brain template (blue = sources of light; green = detectors; black = approximate area of the brain covered by the fNIRS measurement). (B) Probe-set and channel configuration for right and left hemispheres, respectively. (C) Brain regions maximally overlaid by the probe arrangement in the order of greatest probability for each channel (BA = Brodmann Area).
Data preprocessing was completed using MATLAB-based software, including Homer2 software retrieved from the NITRC database (Huppert et al., 2009), NIRS Toolbox (Huppert & Barker, 2015), and several customized scripts (Hu et al., 2010). First, data visualization was done using Homer2, in which the 690 and 830nm wavelengths timeseries data were examined to exclude participants whose signal quality was below 3 molar units and did not reveal cardiac signal for over 50% of 690 data channels, likely due to a large amount of motion artifacts and/or hair obstruction (3 bilinguals).
Using NIRS Toolbox, raw time course data was converted into units of optical density change (ΔOD). Using the modified Beer-Lambert law, we converted the data to hemoglobin concentration signal change yielding oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (HbR) values. In order to correct for motion artifacts and serial correlations, we carried out a pre-whitening filter on participants’ hemoglobin data time series. At the first-level, each participant’s hemoglobin concentration data was analyzed using an autoregressive general linear model that assumed the dual-gamma canonical hemodynamic response function (Friston et al., 2006). The first-level GLM analysis estimated beta values, which are indices of percent signal change, for each condition and each participant: errors of Early morphemic features, errors of Later morphemic features, and Correct sentences. Second-level group analyses were conducted using a second-level GLM that included conditions and groups (monolingual, bilingual) as fixed effects, participants were treated as a random effect variable, and hemoglobin beta values (HbO and HbR) as the predicting dependent variables. HbR analyses are reported in Table S1. The regressions estimated betas for each condition (Later, Early, and Corrects), as well as the following contrasts: Later > Early, Early > Later, Later > Corrects, Corrects > Later, Early > Corrects, and Corrects > Early. Within-group condition comparisons aimed to identify positive activations for each of the groups, and between-group comparisons aimed to identify group differences; both types of analyses were carried out using two-tailed t-tests with an alpha-threshold of p < 0.05.
2.2 Results
Task performance and language abilities are reported in Table 1 for the entire group of bilinguals and monolinguals, including participants whose neuroimaging data did not pass the criterion thresholds. The children did not differ in age, IQ, English language abilities or cognitive abilities (p > .05; see Table 1). Families from monolingual children were from significantly higher socio-economic backgrounds than bilinguals’, as measured by household income and parents’ education. Comparisons between bilingual children’s English and Spanish language abilities revealed that most language scores were comparable, however bilingual children performed significantly better during the English than Spanish morpho-syntax assessment, as would be expected of children in English-dominant educational environments (see Table 1).
English Grammaticality Judgment Task Performance
A linear mixed-effects model in IBM SPSS Statistics 24 was performed to examine accuracy task performance, group and condition factors were treated as fixed effects and participants were specified as a random factor. The random intercept model was centered at the bilingual group and Correct sentences condition. The model of best fit was selected using Akaike’s information criterion (AIC) goodness-of-fit index where the smallest AIC value was preferred (−286.09). The model revealed a significant effect of condition in sentence type (F(2, 46.52) = 20.16, p < .001) that stemmed from participants’ better performance in Corrects than sentences with errors of Later-acquired morphemic features (β= −8.37%, SE= 2.20, t= −3.80, p < .001), but not sentences with errors of Early-acquired morphemic features (β= 1.05%, SE= 1.17, t= 0.90, p= .373); see Table 1. We did not find a significant effect of language group (F(1, 42.96) = 0.68, p= .796), or a language group by sentence condition-type interaction (F(2, 46.52) = 0.31, p= .734).
A similar linear mixed-effects model was performed to examine response time task performance, group and condition factors were treated as fixed effects and participants were specified as a random factor. The random intercept model was centered at the bilingual group and Correct sentences condition. The model of best fit was selected using Akaike’s information criterion (AIC) goodness-of-fit index (1320.67). The model revealed a significant effect of condition in sentence type (F(2, 31.53) = 39.16, p < .001) that stemmed from participants’ faster performance in sentences with errors of Earlier-acquired morphemic features than Corrects (β= −293.15 ms, SE= 66.79, t= −4.39, p < .001), but not sentences with errors of Early-acquired morphemic features ( β= 47.40 ms, SE= 59.74, t= 0.79, p= .433); see Table 1. We did not find a significant effect of language group (F(1, 30.37) = 2.87, p= .10), or a language group by sentence condition-type interaction (F(2, 31.53) = 0.18, p= .83).
Functional Neuroimaging Data Results
Within-Group Comparisons
During the Later > Early contrast (see Figure 2A and 4A), monolinguals activated left channel 4, right channels 1 and 4–6, while bilinguals activated left channel 1. The reverse contrast Early > Later did not yield significant activity for either group. Within-group contrasts comparing Later-acquired errors and correct sentences (see Figure 2B; Later > Corrects and Corrects > Later) also did not reveal significant activity for either monolingual or bilingual groups. During Corrects > Early (see Figure 2C), monolinguals activated right channels 1–2 and 4–6, while bilinguals activated right channels 4 and 7. The reverse contrast Early > Corrects did not reveal significant activity for either group.
Figure 2.
Oxygenated hemoglobin beta values (as percent of signal change for contrasts) during the English grammaticality judgment task. Within-group brain activation for bilinguals and monolinguals during sentences with (A) errors on Later-acquired morpho-syntactic features relative to Early-acquired, (B) errors on Later-acquired relative to Correct sentences, and (C) Correct sentences relative to Early-acquired morpho-syntactic features.
Figure 4.
3-D brain images. (A) T-values mapped for comparison of brain activation for monolinguals (top row) and bilinguals (bottom row). Higher values on the scale indicate greater brain activity during the sentences with errors on Later-acquired morphosyntax, relative to errors on Early-acquired. (B) T-value map for comparison of brain activation between bilinguals and monolinguals: top figure shows greater activity for monolinguals, bottom figure shows greater activity for bilinguals. The color bar reflects significant t-values.
Between-Group Comparisons
See Figure 3. The between-group comparison for Later > Early revealed that monolinguals had greater activation in left channel 4 relative to bilinguals, while bilinguals showed greater activation in left channels 1–3 relative to monolinguals (see Figure 3A and 4B). As shown in Figure S1, the double-subtraction group differences for the Later > Earlier errors contrast stemmed from both conditions, the bilinguals had more activation during the Later condition and less activation during the Early condition. The between-group comparison for Later > Corrects (Figure 3B) revealed that monolinguals had greater activation in right channel 4 relative to bilinguals. While bilinguals showed greater activation in left channels 1–2, as well as right channels 2–3 and 5, relative to monolinguals. As shown in Figure S1, group differences in channels 1–2 stem from bilinguals’ stronger activation during the Later-acquired morphemic features condition than Correct sentences, while monolinguals had similar levels of activation for the two. The between-group comparison for remaining contrasts including Early > Later, Corrects > Later, Early > Corrects, and Corrects > Early did not reveal significant differences between monolinguals and bilinguals in brain activity.
Figure 3.
Between-group comparison for the English grammaticality judgment task among bilinguals’ and monolinguals’ brain activation during (A) errors on Later-acquired morpho-syntactic features relative to Early-acquired, and (B) Later-acquired relative to Correct sentences.
3. Study 2: Spanish Morpho-syntactic Processing in Bilingual Children
Study 2 was designed as a companion to Study 1, to investigate whether the impact of early bilingualism on neural specialization for language processing was specific to morphosyntactic error detection in one or both languages. Since bilinguals in the present sample were more competent in English than Spanish, it also allows the examination of whether any differences in brain activity are due to language abilities. Importantly, this notion is uniquely tested within the bilingual group rather than contrasting to monolinguals whose linguistic and socio-cultural experiences vary from bilinguals growing up in the United States. Study 2 tested the same group of Spanish-English bilinguals in a Spanish grammaticality judgment task. If bilingual children show a pattern of brain activity that is more widespread (less specialized, e.g., more bilateral) for their Spanish than English, then this would suggest that children’s brain activity is likely driven by language abilities. In contrast, if bilingual children show similar levels of specialization for the two languages, then this would suggest that children’s neural specialization for language is also driven by the combined dual-language experiences. In such a case, evidence for the latter hypothesis would support the notion that the “neural signature” of bilingual language experiences can modulate the neurodevelopmental course of syntactic processing and organization in the young brain, and it is quantitatively different than that of monolinguals.
3.1 Methods
Participants
The present study includes data from 18 Spanish-English bilinguals who fit the same selection criteria as the children in Study 1, (8 females, 10 males; age range = 8.4–11.8 years, mean age [M]=10, standard deviation [SD]=1.13). The children completed the language, literacy, and cognitive assessments listed in Study 1.
Spanish Auditory Grammaticality Judgment Imaging Task
Similar to the English task in Study 1, the children completed a Spanish grammaticality judgment task with the same number of sentences (items) containing violations of Early- and Later-acquired inflectional morphemes as well as correct sentences. Later-acquired condition included erroneous use of 3rd person singular instead of obligatory 1st or 2nd person singular past and present forms (e.g., 1st person present tense “Yo junta monedas amarillas [I collects yellow coins],” 2nd person past tense “Ayer, tú peino las muñecas [Yesterday, you comb (hair of) the dolls],” 3rd person present tense “El niño dibujo una casa [The boy draw a house]”). The Early-acquired condition included errors of subject-adjective gender agreement for singular and plural subject forms (e.g., female and male singular “La flor blanco es para mamá [The white flower is for mom]” “El perro cansadas tiene sed [The tired dog was thirsty]”, female and male plural “Los gatos pequeñas toman leche [The small cats drink milk]” “La taza llenos tiene café [The full cup has coffee]”). During Correct sentences, children heard well-structured sentences, including correct 1st and 2nd person assignment (“A diario, yo toco el piano [Everyday, I play the piano],” “Ayer, tú sacaste la basura [Yesterday, you took out the trash]”), as well as subject-adjective gender agreements (“La gata negra asustó a mamá [the black cat scared mom],” “Los lápices nuevos están en casa [the new pencils are at home]”).
Procedure, Data Collection and Analyses
The testing protocol and procedure were similar to Study 1, with the exception that during the Spanish/Study 2 visit, the experimenters only used Spanish to interact with the children. Data collection and analyses protocols were all also similar to Study. Similar to Study 1, the analyses were carried out using two-tailed t-tests with an alpha-threshold of p < 0.05.
3.2 Results
Spanish Grammaticality Judgment Task Performance
A linear mixed-effects model in IBM SPSS Statistics 24 was performed to examine accuracy task performance, condition sentence types were treated as a fixed effect and participants were specified as a random factor. The random intercept model was centered at the Correct sentences condition. The model of best fit was selected using Akaike’s information criterion (AIC) goodness-of-fit index where the smallest AIC value was preferred (−24.44). The model did not reveal a significant effect of condition in sentence type (F(2, 39.51) = 2.56, p = .09). A similar linear mixed-effects model was performed to examine response time task performance; the model of best fit was selected using Akaike’s information criterion (AIC) goodness-of-fit index (842.60). The model did not reveal a significant effect of condition in sentence type (F(2, 35.77) = .04, p = .96). See Table 1 for task performance.
Functional Neuroimaging Data Results
See Figure S2 for estimated betas for each condition. During the Later condition, children activated left channels 1, 4–8, and right channels 4–8. During the Early condition, children activated left channels 7–8 and right channels 1–3 and 8. During Corrects, children activated right channels 1–2 and 6.
During the Later > Early contrast, children activated left channels 1 and 4 (Figure 5A and Figure 6). The reverse contrast Early > Later did not yield significant activity. During the Later > Corrects contrast, children activated left channel 1 and 4, as well as right channels 4 and 8 (Figure 5B). The reverse contrast Corrects > Later revealed children activated right channel 2 (Figure 5C). The Early > Corrects and Corrects > Early contrasts did not yield significant activity.
Figure 5.
Bilinguals’ oxygenated hemoglobin beta values (as percent of signal change for contrasts) during the Spanish grammaticality judgment task. Within-group brain activation during sentences with (A) errors on Later-acquired morpho-syntactic features relative to Early-acquired, (B) errors on Later-acquired relative to Correct sentences, and (C) Correct sentences relative to Early-acquired morpho-syntactic features.
Figure 6.
3-D brain image. Significant t-values mapped for comparison of brain activation during the Spanish grammaticality judgment task. Presented here is the contrast between errors on Later-acquired morpho-syntactic features relative to errors on Early-acquired. Higher values on the scale indicate greater brain activity.
4. Discussion
The study investigated how early and systematic bilingual exposure influences children’s functional organization for syntactic processing. The Integrated Multilingual Perspective (MacSwan, 2017) on bilingual processing advances the notion that young bilinguals learn each of their languages in a monolingual-like manner, yet both languages interact, and develop neurocognitive mechanisms to support their interactive dual-language processes (see also Grosjean, 1989; Cook, 1991, 2003). The left IFG region is critical to the development of inflectional morphology processing, where complex morpho-syntactic features recruit greater activity (Friederici, 2011, 2012). Thus, we predicted greater left IFG activation for later- versus earlier-acquired aspects of morpho-syntax. We hypothesized that bilinguals’ frontal lobe cortical specialization for language should correspond directly to their language abilities, predicting that since we tested highly competent bilinguals, their brain activation patterns for both languages should not differ substantially from monolinguals. The findings confirm this prediction: Spanish-English bilinguals and English monolinguals in the present study had similar English language abilities, experimental task accuracy, and greater left IFG activation for later- versus earlier-acquired aspects of morpho-syntax.
Furthermore, while bilingual children demonstrated greater competence in English than Spanish, they also showed greater left frontal activation for later- versus earlier acquired aspects of morpho-syntax in Spanish, in regions similar to those of English activation where bilinguals showed greater activity than monolinguals. These results are consistent with our second exploratory hypothesis that young bilinguals present evidence of early neurodevelopmental adaptation to bilingualism by showing overall greater left IFG activation than monolinguals, and to both of their languages although their language abilities were greater for English than Spanish. Taken together, the findings suggest that early bilingual learners can achieve language competence comparable to monolinguals, neural response specificity in left IFG regions is shown to be similar across their two languages, and stronger in bilinguals relative to monolinguals. The findings advance the proposed theoretical perspectives of bilingual acquisition by revealing fundamental similarities between bilingual and monolingual children’s neurocognitive bases for inflectional morphology at the core of their language systems. The findings also advance theoretical perspectives that link enriched linguistic experiences with more advanced neural specialization for language processing (Raizada, Richards, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2008).
Bilinguals demonstrated task performance equivalent to monolinguals as well as greater activation for the later- than earlier-acquired morpho-syntactic structures of tense and agreement. Notably, the participant groups were comparable in age, English language abilities, and IQ. Nevertheless, the differences between the two syntactic conditions were more pronounced in bilinguals’ left IFG region (Figure 3), while monolinguals exhibited a more distributed and bilateral response. Prior work has shown that as sentence complexity increases, the engagement of right frontal regions also increases–which is often attributed to the increase in generalized working memory and attention demands (Just et al., 1996; Caplan, Alpert, & Waters, 1998; Keller, Carpenter, & Just, 2001). Individuals with greater language abilities (e.g., larger vocabulary) often show less distributed neural activity in comparison to those with lower language abilities (Knoll et al., 2012; Raizada et al., 2008; see also Prat, Keller, & Just, 2007), suggesting more efficient use of language-specific neural resources in those with better linguistic competence (Prat & Just, 2010). Yet functionality for morpho-syntactic features develop slowly with age, even 9-year-old children recruit broad left frontal activity, as compared to adults, even when highly accurate in detecting morpho-syntactic errors (Skeide et al., 2016). Monolingual children in the present results support previous findings showing monolingual adults activate bilateral frontal regions when listening to complex sentences (Just et al., 1996; Caplan et al., 1998; Keller et al., 2001). As can be seen in Figure 5, bilinguals’ greater activation for later-acquired structures and lower activation to earlier-acquired structures, is generally limited to left IFG activation–in English (Study 1) and in Spanish (Study 2), and support the notion that early bilingual exposure paves the way for language-specific acquisition and high-level language competency. In sum, bilinguals show a reduced spread of activation as compared to monolinguals, suggestive of better specialization of neural response to syntax in left IFG—the key region for syntax acquisition and processing (Knoll et al., 2012).
Two interrelated explanations for these effects surface: First, this specialization may be the advantageous result of an overall linguistic enrichment provided by early dual-language experiences. For instance, research finds greater left IFG activation during a rhyme versus tone judgment task in children from higher-income families, which is typically associated with more enriched linguistic input from caregivers (Raizada et al., 2008). Moreover, research often finds that bilingual children have higher sensitivity to linguistic structures, as reflected in being better learners of new artificial languages as compared to monolinguals (Kuo & Anderson, 2016). This result likely stems from the need to consider a greater number of possible interpretations for sounds, meanings, and structures across two linguistic systems from early life, thereby heightening children’s structural sensitivity to language (Kuo & Anderson, 2010). Left frontal lobe regions are known to support the computation of linguistic structures as well as children’s ability to alternate between their two languages, the mechanism that likely builds upon any language users’ ability to alternate between language registers (home language, school language etc.). Therefore, if one were to consider bilingual and monolingual states along a continuum of enriched linguistic experiences in which children consider more diverse language and language register usage (MacSwan, 2017; Grosjean, 1989; Cook, 1991, 2003), the present findings further support the idea that cortical specialization for language varies as a function of children’s overall linguistic experiences.
The results did not shed light on significant differences between children’s language competence (vocabulary, syntax, or grammaticality judgment task performance), linguistic input (as measured through questionnaires), and brain function. The findings are nonetheless commensurate with previous research showing that while AoA and length of exposure play a significant role in language competence as well as bilinguals’ brain organization in general, these effects are smaller for those with early and systematic bilingual exposure to English within the United States immigration context (prior to age 9: Archila-Suerte et al., 2016; prior to age 4: Bedore, Peña, Griffin, & Hixon, 2016; see also Consonni et al., 2013; Wartenburger et al., 2003). Similar to some of the previous work (e.g., Roncaglia-Denissen & Kotz, 2016), the present study suggests that high dual language competence, regardless of AoA, bilinguals can activate similar brain areas when processing morpho-syntax in both languages (see also Chee et al., 1999; Hasegawa et al., 2002; Perani et al., 1998; Vingerhoets et al., 2003). A limitation of this work is the inclusion of only one bilingual group with early, systematic and high dual-language proficiency. The present findings nevertheless provide a set of principled findings that can now be used for future works with bilinguals that may have a wider range of dual-language proficiency abilities, language typologies, and contexts of acquisition. Although interpretations for the present data represent global bilingual versus monolingual distinctions on a small scale, they support the idea that for children who are exposed earlier to a new language, as well as growing up in environments where both languages are encouraged, may not only yield normative development (Bedore et al., 2016), but also high levels of language/linguistic competence, which may advance children’s overall language faculty (Kuo & Anderson, 2010).
Second, the findings may stem from language-specific experiences with Spanish and English. Morpho-syntactic inflectional variation in English is almost entirely limited to -ing, -ed and -s morphemes. In contrast, Spanish provides a rich variety of morpho-syntactic agreement contexts that include gender and number agreement for nouns, as well as a rich variation for tense and number agreement for verbs. Spanish-English bilinguals attend to morpho-syntactic structures of English sentences at a greater rate than monolingual English speakers (Hernandez et al., 1994). Although both language tasks were completed in different sessions and fNIRS methodology is limited in its spatial resolution and cap positioning, children show an increased neural response to sentence processing in similar and predicted left IFG regions across their two languages. These results suggest that left frontal cortical regions support bilinguals’ ability to compute inflectional morphology for their two languages. Nevertheless, the current data identify the high specificity and selectivity of left IFG response in Spanish-English bilinguals, and provides a principled explanation for the heightened neural response to morpho-syntactic variations in English. This interpretation is consistent with the interrelated and bilingual language transfer hypotheses of bilingualism (Abutalebi & Green, 2016; Cummins, 2001; Green & Abutalebi, 2013; Kuo & Anderson, 2010), but requires further adjudication with studies that consider other bilingual language configurations.
The findings are generally consistent with the Integrated Multilingual Model (MacSwan, 2017) that integrates the idea that bilinguals can have language-specific development as well as shared cross-linguistic interaction that yields bilingual-specific signature processing. As is consistent with behavioral data for children acquiring “two first languages” (e.g., Petitto & Kovelman, 2003), bilinguals and monolinguals showed similar levels of language proficiency and higher left IFG activation for later- versus earlier- acquired inflectional morphemes. Second, this increase in left IFG activation was observed in similar left IFG regions across the bilinguals’ two languages, suggesting a possible interaction in bilinguals’ neural representation for their two languages. Finally, the Adaptive Control model (Abutalebi & Green, 2016; Green & Abutalebi, 2013) suggests that daily demands of dual-language experiences impact linguistic and cognitive processes; bilinguals’ two languages are assumed to be co-active even when only one language is in use (Kroll et al., 2015). Another possible explanation is that the increased attentional and mnemonic demand for processing two often co-active languages might alter the functionality of left IFG regions that specifically support the linguistic, mnemonic, and attentional demands of language processing. These same child participants also completed a visuo-spatial attention task during which the bilinguals showed greater left IFG activation than the monolinguals (Arredondo, Hu, Satterfield, & Kovelman, 2017). Taken together, the systematic use of two languages during the key years of brain plasticity for language organization may enhance the computational capabilities of the left IFG region and its engagement in dual-language development in young learners.
The present study’s method is limited by the inclusion of ungrammatical sentences that the children are unlikely to hear from adult native speakers English in English or adult native speakers of Spanish in Spanish. However, we note that errors of morpheme omission are typical of child language until about age 6 (and sometimes beyond as is the case for individuals with language impairments). Yet children within the age range of our participants (6–11 years) often interact with slightly younger children at school, home, play dates (or L2 speakers such as bilingual children’s parents) and may hear such morpheme omissions in their regular environment. Lastly, one of the potential caveats is the number of experimental items of 20 sentences per condition. This number of experimental items (and thus the duration of testing) was based on attention span for young children, as well as prior neuroimaging work suggesting that it is possible to obtain significant findings with children using around 16 experimental sentences per condition (e.g., Nuñez et al., 2011).
The use of a grammatical judgment task in the present study allows us to not only build on the findings within the literature on typical language development, but also to use neuroimaging to understand the neurobiological mechanisms of language processing. Several prior and current studies show that children and adolescents still produce a protracted P600 ERP response on syntactic violations in contrast to adults. This outcome is identified as a possible indicator of syntactic re-analysis, or increased memory load associated with holding incomplete syntactic dependencies and derivations in memory (Osterhout et al. 1992; Gouvea et al., 2010). The present findings critically diverge from traditional theories of language processing that presume that bilinguals have reduced sensitivity to syntactic information and might compensate (i.e., achieve monolingual-like accuracy) by placing greater reliance on semantic and pragmatic cues in the interpretation of linguistic information (as evidenced through the inconsistency of ELAN response found across bilingual studies of morpho-syntactic processing, e.g., Frenck-Mestre et al., 2010). In conclusion, these results uncover important data on young bilinguals’ strong reliance on their morpho-syntactic system.
5. Conclusions
The present study aimed to shed light on whether early bilingual exposure influences children’s cortical organization for processing features of morpho-syntax. In our increasingly multilingual and multicultural society, it is important to consider the impact of bilingualism on the development of language, and how exposure to more than one language from a young age might shape an individual’s brain both structurally and functionally. In the recent past, bilingualism was thought to impede cognitive and linguistic development, effectively “confusing” the child’s brain with multiple languages. We now know that this is not the case. The present findings add to the growing corpus of evidence that supports a “neural signature” of bilingualism, quantitatively different from that of monolinguals, and carries implications for the future of educational policy in a multilingual world.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Child language study of cortical specialization for language in bilingualism
Bilinguals and monolinguals judged sentences of varied morpho-syntatic complexity
fNIRS activation patterns were similar across bilinguals’ two languages
Bilinguals showed stronger and more focal left IFG activation than monolinguals
Bilingualism may yield a higher degree of cortical specialization in children
Footnotes
Acknowledgements: The authors thank the University of Michigan Departments of Psychology, Romance Languages and Literatures, and Center for Human Growth and Development. The authors also thank the ‘En Nuestra Lengua’ Literacy and Culture Program in Ann Arbor, Michigan, participating families, Lourdes M. Delgado Reyes, Ka I Ip, Jaime Muñoz Velazquez, Paola Velosa, Stefanie Younce, and Melanie Armstrong for their assistance with data collection. Maria Arredondo thanks the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (NSF GRFP, Grant No. DGE 1256260). Ioulia Kovelman thanks the National Institutes of Health (R01HD078351 PI: Hoeft). Any opinions, conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of NSF or NIH.
References
- Abutalebi J, Green DW. Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2016;19:689–698. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000225. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Archila-Suerte P, Zevin J, Hernandez AE. The effect of age of acquisition, socioeducational status, and proficiency on the neural processing of second language speech sounds. Brain and Language. 2015;141:35–49. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arredondo MM, Hu X, Satterfield T, Kovelman I. Bilingualism alters children’s frontal lobe functioning for attentional control. Developmental Science. 2016 doi: 10.1111/desc.12377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bedore LM, Leonard LB. Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2001;44:905–924. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/072). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bedore LM, Leonard LB. Verb inflections and noun phrase morphology in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2005;26:195–225. doi: 10.1017/S0142716405050149. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bedore LM, Peña ED, Griffin ZM, Hixon JG. Effects of age of English exposure, current input/output, and grade on bilingual language performance. Journal of Child Language. 2016;43:687–706. doi: 10.1017/S0305000915000811. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berken JA, Gracco VL, Chen JK, Watkins KE, Baum S, … Klein D. Neural activation in speech production and reading aloud in native and non-native languages. NeuroImage. 2015;112:208–217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boersma P, Weenink D. Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.0.21) [Computer program] 2008 http://www.praat.org/
- Brownell R. Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (ROWPVT) Spanish- Bilingual Edition. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Caplan D, Alpert N, Waters G. Effects of syntactic structure and propositional number on patterns of regional cerebral blood flow. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1998;10:541–552. doi: 10.1162/089892998562843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chee MW, Tan EW, Thiel T. Mandarin and English single word processing studied with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:3050–3056. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-08-03050.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coltheart M. The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1981;33A:497–505. [Google Scholar]
- Consonni M, Cafiero R, Marin D, Tettamanti M, ladanza A, … Perani D. Neural convergence for language comprehension and grammatical class production in highly proficient bilinguals is independent of age of acquisition. Cortex. 2013;49:1252–1258. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook VJ. The poverty-of-the-stimulus argument and multicompetence. Second Language Research. 1991;7:103–117. [Google Scholar]
- Cook V. Effects of the Second Language on the First. Multilingual Matters Ltd; Tonawanda, NY: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Costa A, Sebastián-Gallés N. How does the bilingual experience sculpt the brain? Nature Reviews: Neuroscience. 2014;15:336–345. doi: 10.1038/nrn3709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cummins J. Bilingual children’s mother tongue: why is it important for education. Sprogforum. 2001;19:15–20. [Google Scholar]
- Dale AM. Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI. Human Brain Mapping. 1999;8:109–114. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:2/3<109::AID-HBM7>3.0.CO;2-W. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene-Lambertz G, Hertz-Pannier L, Dubois J, Mériaux S, Roche A, … Dehaene S. Functional organization of perisylvian activation during presentation of sentences in preverbal infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS) 2006;103:14210–14245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0606302103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Houwer A. Bilingual language acquisition. In: Fletcher P, MacWhinney B, editors. The Handbook of Child Language. Oxford, UK: Blackwell; 1995. pp. 219–250. [Google Scholar]
- Dominguez L. Interpreting reference in the early acquisition of Spanish clitic. In: Montrul S, Ordoñez F, editors. Linguistic Theory and Language Development in Hispanic Languages. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Johns Benjamins; 2003. pp. 212–228. [Google Scholar]
- Du L. Initial Bilingual Development: One Language or Two? Asian Social Science. 2010;6(5):132–139. doi: 10.5539/ass.v6n5p132. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Francis D, Carlo M, August D, Kenyon D, Malabonga V, … Louguit M. Test of Phonological Processing in Spanish (TOPPS) Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Felton A, Vazquez D, Ramos-Nunez AI, Greene MR, Macbeth A, Hernandez AE, Chiarello C. Bilingualism influences structural indices of interhemispheric organization. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2017;42:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frenck-Mestre C, Carrasco-Ortiz H, McLaughlin J, Osterhout L, Foucart A. Linguistic input factors in native and L2 processing of inflectional morphology: Evidence from ERPs and behavioral studies. Language, Interaction and Acquisition. 2010;1:206–228. doi: 10.1075/lia.1.2.04fre. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Friederici AD. The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiological Reviews. 2011;91:1357–1392. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00006.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friederici AD. The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16:262–268. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Ashburner JT, Kiebel SJ, Nichols TE, Penny WD. Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images. London: Academic Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- García-Pentón L, Fernández AP, Iturria-Medina Y, Gillon-Dowens M, Carreiras M. Anatomical connectivity changes in the bilingual brain. NeuroImage. 2014;84:495–504. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Sierra A, Rivera-Gaxiola M, Percaccio CR, Conboy BT, Romo H, … Kuhl PK. Bilingual language learning: an ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word production. Journal of Phonetics. 2011;39:546–557. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2011.07.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Genesee F. Early bilingual development: one language or two? Journal of Child Language. 1989;16:161–179. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900013490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gouvea AC, Phillips C, Kazanina N, Poeppel D. The linguistic processes underlying the P600. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2010;25:149–188. doi: 10.1080/01690960902965951. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Green DW, Abutalebi J. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 2013;25:515–530. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.796377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grinstead J, Baron A, Vega-Mendoza M, De la Mora J, Cantú-Sánchez M, Flores B. Tense marking and spontaneous speech measures in Spanish specific language impairment: a discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2013;56:352–363. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0289). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grinstead J, Lintz P, Vega-Mendoza M, De la Mora J, Cantú-Sánchez M, Flores-Avalos B. Evidence of optional infinitive verbs in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with SLI. Lingua. 2014;140:52–66. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2013.11.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Grosjean F. Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language. 1989;36:3–15. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(89)90048-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guasti MT. Verb syntax in Italian child grammar: Finite and nonfinite verbs. Language acquisition. 1993;3:1–40. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0301_1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Guasti MT. Language Acquisition: The Growth of Grammar. MIT Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Hasegawa M, Carpenter PA, Just MA. An fMRI study of bilingual sentence comprehension and workload. NeuroImage. 2002;15:647–660. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez AE, Bates EA, Avila LX. On-line sentence interpretation in Spanish-English bilinguals: what does it mean to be “in between”? Applied Psycholinguistics. 1994;15:417–446. doi: 10.1017/S014271640000686X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez AE, Li P. Age of acquisition: its neural and computational mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:638–650. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hickok G, Poeppel D. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007;8:393. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland SK, Vannest J, Mecoli M, Jacola LM, Tillema JM, … Byars AW. Functional MRI of language lateralization during development in children. International Journal of Audiology. 2007;46:533–551. doi: 10.1080/14992020701448994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu XS, Hong KS, Ge SS, Jeong MY. Kalman estimator- and general linear model-based on-line brain activation mapping by near-infrared spectroscopy. Biomedical Engineering Online. 2010;9 doi: 10.1186/1475-925X-9-82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huppert TJ, Barker JW. +NIRS Toolbox. 2015 Retrieved from https://bitbucket.org/huppertt/nirs-toolbox/downloads.
- Huppert TJ, Diamond SG, Franceschini MA, Boas DA. HomER: A review of times-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Applied Optics. 2009;48:D280–298. doi: 10.1364/AO.48.00D280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imada T, Zhang Y, Cheour M, Taulu S, Ahonen A, Kuhl PK. Infant speech perception activates Broca’s area: a developmental magnetoencephalography study. NeuroReport. 2006;17:957–962. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000223387.51704.89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jasinska KK, Petitto LA. How age of bilingual exposure can change the neural systems for language in the developing brain: A functional near infrared spectroscopy investigation of syntactic processing in monolingual and bilingual children. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2013;6:87–101. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson MH. Interactive specialization: a domain-general framework for human functional brain development? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;1:7–21. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jurcak V, Tsuzuki D, Dan I. 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems. NeuroImage. 2007;34:1600–1611. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Just MA, Carpenter PA, Keller TA, Eddy WF, Thulborn KR. Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science. 1996;274:114–116. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5284.114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman A, Kaufman N. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test Second Edition (KBIT- 2) Minneapolis, MN: Pearson; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Keller TA, Carpenter PA, Just MA. The neural bases of sentence comprehension: a fMRI examination of syntactic and lexical processing. Cerebral Cortex. 2001;11:223–237. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.3.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein D, Mok K, Chen JK, Watkins KE. Age of language learning shapes brain structure: a cortical thickness study of bilingual and monolingual individuals. Brain and Language. 2014;131:20–24. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knoll LJ, Obleser J, Schipke CS, Friederici AD, Brauer J. Left prefrontal cortex activation during sentence comprehension covaries with grammatical knowledge in children. NeuroImage. 2012;62:207–216. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovelman I, Baker SA, Petitto LA. Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of syntactic processing and a possible “neural signature” of bilingualism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20:153–160. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroll JF, Dussias PE, Bice K, Perrotti L. Bilingualism, mind, and brain. Annual Review of Linguistics. 2015;1:377–394. doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-124937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuo LJ, Anderson RC. Beyond cross-language transfer: reconceptualizing the impact of early bilingualism on phonological awareness. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2010;14:365–385. doi: 10.1080/10888431003623470. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kweon SO, Bley-Vroman R. Acquisition of the constraints on wanna contraction by advanced second language learners: Universal Grammar and imperfect knowledge. Second Language Research. 2011;27:207–228. doi: 10.1177/0267658310375756. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Li P, Legault J, Litcofsky KA. Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex. 2014;58:301–324. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luke KK, Liu HL, Wai YY, Wan YL, Tan LH. Functional anatomy of syntactic and semantic processing in language comprehension. Human Brain Mapping. 2002;16:133–145. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacSwan J. A multilingual perspective on translanguaging. American Educational Research Journal. 2017;54:167–201. doi: 10.3102/0002831216683935. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mechelli A, Crinion JT, Noppeney U, O’doherty J, Ashburner J, … Price CJ. Neurolinguistics: structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature. 2004;431:757. doi: 10.1038/431757a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nuñez SC, Dapretto M, Katzir T, Starr A, Bramen J, Kan E, … Sowell ER. fMRI of syntactic processing in typically developing children: structural correlates in the inferior frontal gyrus. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;1:313–323. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberecker R, Friederici AD. Syntactic event-related potential components in 24-month-olds’ sentence comprehension. NeuroReport. 2006;17:1017–1021. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000223397.12694.9a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osterhout L, Holcomb PJ. Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language. 1992;31:785–806. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(92)90039-Z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Parker-Jones O, Green DW, Grogan A, Pliatsikas C, Filippopolitis K, … Price CJ. Where, when and why brain activation differs for bilinguals and monolinguals during picture naming and reading aloud. Cerebral Cortex. 2012;22:892–902. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peña ED, Bedore LM, Kester ES. Assessment of language impairment in bilingual children using semantic tasks: two languages classify better than one. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 2015;51:192–202. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peña M, Maki A, Kovačić D, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Koizumi H, Bouquet F, Mehler J. Sounds and silence: an optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2003;100:11702–11705. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1934290100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perani D, Paulesu E, Galles NS, Dupoux E, Dehaene S, Bettinardi V, … Mehler J. The bilingual brain. Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 1998;121:1841–1852. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.10.1841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petitto LA, Berens MS, Kovelman I, Dubins MH, Jasinska K, Shalinsky M. The “perceptual wedge hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: new insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language. 2012;121:130–143. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petitto LA, Kovelman I. The bilingual paradox: How signing-speaking bilingual children help us to resolve it and teach us about the brain’s mechanisms underlying all language acquisition. Learning Languages. 2003;8:5–18. [Google Scholar]
- Phillips C. Should we impeach armchair linguists. Japanese/Korean Linguistics. 2009;17:49–64. [Google Scholar]
- Prat CS, Just MA. Exploring the neural dynamics underpinning individual differences in sentence comprehension. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;21:1747–1760. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prat CS, Keller TA, Just MA. Individual differences in sentence comprehension: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of syntactic and lexical processing demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2007;19:1950–1963. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.1950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pratt A, Grinstead J. Optional infinitives in child Spanish. Proceedings of the 2nd Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (GALANA); Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project; 2007. pp. 351–362. [Google Scholar]
- Raizada RD, Richards TL, Meltzoff A, Kuhl PK. Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialization of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. NeuroImage. 2008;40:1392–1401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Restrepo MA, Gutiérrez-Clellen VF. Article use in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language. 2001;28:433–452. doi: 10.1017/S0305000901004706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Wexler K. Rice/Wexler Test of Early Grammatical Impairment. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Wexler K, Cleave PL. Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1995;38:850–863. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3804.850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Wexler K, Hershberger S. Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:1412–1431. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4106.1412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Wexler K, Redmond SM. Grammaticality judgments of an extended optional infinitive grammar: evidence from English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999;42:943–961. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4204.943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roeper T. Universal bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 1999;2:169–186. [Google Scholar]
- Román P, González J, Ventura-Campos N, Rodríguez-Pujadas A, Sanjuán A, Avila C. Neural differences between monolinguals and early bilinguals in their native language during comprehension. Brain & Language. 2015;150:80–89. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roncaglia-Denissen MP, Kotz SA. What does neuroimaging tell us about morphosyntactic processing in the brain of second language learners? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2016;19:665–673. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000413. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Satterfield T. Bilingual Selection of Syntactic Knowledge. Dordrecht: Kluwer Publishers; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Saur D, Kreher BW, Schnell S, Kümmerer D, Kellmeyer P, … Huber W. Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008;105:18035–18040. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805234105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schütze C, Wexler K. Subject case licensing and English root infinitives. Proceedings of the 20th annual Boston University conference on language development; Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project; 1996. pp. 670–681. [Google Scholar]
- Semel E, Wiig EH, Secord WA. Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Fourth Edition (CELF-4) Bloomington, MN: Pearson; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Semel E, Wiig EH, Secord WA. Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Fourth Spanish Edition (CELF-4) Bloomington, MN: Pearson; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Silva-Pereyra J, Rivera-Gaxiola M, Kuhl PK. An event-related brain potential study of sentence comprehension in preschoolers: semantic and morphosyntactic processing. Cognitive Brain Research. 2005;23:247–258. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skeide MA, Brauer J, Friederici AD. Brain functional and structural predictors of language performance. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;26:2127–2139. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skeide MA, Friederici AD. The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2016;17:323–332. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M. Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition. 2000;74:209–253. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00069-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vihman MM. Getting started without a system: From phonetics to phonology in bilingual development. International Journal of Bilingualism. 2002;6:239–254. [Google Scholar]
- Vingerhoets G, Van Borsel J, Tesink C, van den Noort M, Deblaere K, … Achten E. Multilingualism: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2003;20:2181–2196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP) Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Wartenburger I, Heekeren HR, Abutalebi J, Cappa SF, Villringer A, Perani D. Early setting of grammatical processing in the bilingual brain. Neuron. 2003;37:159–170. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01150-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werker JF, Hensch TK. Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. Annual Review of Psychology. 2015;66:173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wexler K. Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua. 1998;106:23–79. doi: 10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00029-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.