Abstract
Alterations in serotonin (5-HT) function have been hypothesized to underlie a range of physiological, emotional, and cognitive changes in older age. Here, we conducted a quantitative synthesis and comparison of the effects of age on 5-HT receptors and transporters from cross-sectional PET and SPECT imaging studies. Random-effects meta-analyses of 31 studies including 1087 healthy adults yielded large negative effects of age in 5-HT-2A receptors (largest in global cortex), moderate negative effects of age in 5-HT transporters (largest in thalamus), and small negative effects of age in 5-HT-1A receptors (largest in parietal cortex). Presynaptic 5-HT-1A autoreceptors in raphe/midbrain, however, were preserved across adulthood. Adult age differences were significantly larger in 5-HT-2A receptors compared to 5-HT-1A receptors. A meta-regression showed that 5-HT target, radionuclide, and publication year significantly moderated the age effects. The findings overall identify reduced serotonergic signal transmission in healthy aging. The evidence for the relative preservation of 5-HT-1A compared to 5-HT-2A receptors may partially explain psychological age differences, such as why older adults use more emotion-focused rather than problem-focused coping strategies.
Keywords: Meta-analysis, Serotonin, Healthy aging, Receptors, Transporters
1. Introduction
Serotonin (5-HT) is involved in a wide range of physiological, emotional, and cognitive functions (Lucki, 1998). The existence of at least 14 distinct receptor types that differ in their distribution across the brain might explain the functional diversity of the 5-HT system (Barnes and Sharp, 1999). Although the majority of studies of the functional consequences of alterations in the 5-HT system have focused on the role of 5-HT transporters in depression (Gryglewski et al., 2014; Kambeitz and Howes, 2015; Ogilvie et al., 1996), 5-HT receptors have been specifically associated with several functions. For example, the inhibitory 5-HT-1A receptor has been linked to the sleep-wake cycle, the modulation of anxiety, and long-term memory, whereas the excitatory 5-HT-2A receptor has been associated with the modulation of hallucinogenic effects and both short and long-term memory (Gaspar et al., 2003; Hannon and Hoyer, 2008). Moreover, 5-HT dysfunctions in general have been implicated in several psychiatric and neurological diseases across the life span (Lucki, 1998) including age-related diseases like Alzheimer’s disease (Blin et al 1993).
In contrast to candidate gene studies which make up the majority of individual difference studies of 5-HT in humans (Caspi et al., 2010), positron emission tomography (PET) and single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) are brain imaging techniques that provide direct measures of 5-HT system function in-vivo. Over the last several decades, one of the most consistently observed findings in molecular imaging studies of human brain 5-HT targets is a decline with healthy aging. Studies of different receptor types and transporters have revealed small to moderate negative effects of age on 5-HT-1A receptors (Rabiner et al., 2002; Tauscher et al., 2001), moderate to large effects of age on 5-HT-2A receptors (Sheline et al., 2002; Uchida et al., 2011), and moderate to large effects of age on 5-HT transporters (Fazio et al., 2016; Yamamoto et al., 2002) across adulthood. The only prior summary of the literature on 5-HT in aging, based primarily on post-mortem studies, concluded that 5-HT-1A and 5-HT-2A receptors decline with age (Meltzer et al., 1998). The evidence for adult age differences in 5-HT transporters was mixed, including findings of reduced, increased, and stable 5-HT transporter levels with age (Meltzer et al., 1998).
From neuroimaging studies, the previously reported sizes of adult age effects varied across 5-HT targets and cortical and subcortical brain regions. Some of the observed heterogeneity might be due also to sample variation in other variables of influence such as sex (Cidis Meltzer et al., 2001; Costes et al., 2005), body mass index (Hesse et al., 2014), hormone levels (Moses-Kolko et al., 2011), and genes (David et al., 2005). It is also possible that different 5-HT targets do not change uniformly across the life span but instead develop and age in a non-linear way. This might produce different estimates of age effects depending on the specific age ranges within each study. Furthermore, many PET and SPECT imaging studies lack appropriate sample sizes due to the high costs and safety concerns of nuclear imaging technology. Thus, it is difficult to assess the representativeness or accuracy of a single effect from most PET and SPECT studies, as they usually include less than 25 participants per group.
Recent meta-analytic approaches have been used to aggregate and compare individual effects across studies. Although decades of research have documented age-related declines in neuromodulatory function in general, recent meta-analytic approaches have identified differential rates of decline across subcomponents of neuromodulatory systems. For example, a quantitative synthesis of adult age effects on the dopamine system in over 2000 human participants revealed moderate to large negative effects of age on D1- and D2-like receptors and dopamine transporters (Karrer et al., 2017). Critically, the age effect in D1-like receptors was significantly larger than in D2-like receptors, whereas presynaptic synthesis capacity was relatively preserved across adulthood (Karrer et al., 2017). Although a qualitative summary of the existing neuroimaging literature suggests moderate to strong negative effects of age across the 5-HT system, human neuroimaging studies have not systematically and directly compared effects of age across different 5-HT targets. Here, we used a meta-analytic approach to investigate variation in the effects of aging across the 5-HT system.
To quantitatively synthesize the existing literature on adult age differences in several targets of the 5-HT system, we conducted a comprehensive meta-analysis of cross-sectional PET and SPECT imaging studies. In light of previous individual study findings, we expected small to moderate negative effects of age on 5-HT-1A receptors and moderate to large negative effects of age on 5-HT-2A receptors and transporters. To investigate which factors might explain the heterogeneity of findings across previous studies, we examined the influence of potential moderating variables (e.g., age range, sex, imaging method, 5-HT target, brain region) in a meta-regression. In additional more exploratory analyses, we aggregated individual participant data across studies (where available) to examine the linearity of age effects across 5-HT targets. We did not make specific predictions for these exploratory analyses given the lack of prior reporting and low power of individual studies to examine these effects.
2. Methods
We conducted and reported this meta-analysis adhering to the guidelines of the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) statement (Liberarti et al., 2009). Note that we did not pre-specify our approach in a review protocol. Instead, we closely followed the methods described in an earlier meta-analysis of our group on age differences in the dopamine system (Karrer et al., 2017). In addition, this facilitates the comparison of age effects across both neurochemical systems.
2.1. Literature search
Potentially relevant studies from the personal library of G.R.S.-L. helped to define the keywords for the systematic literature search. We capitalized on the hierarchically organized vocabulary of Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) terms that enables a search for specific topics independent of the used words or spellings (Lipscomp, 2000). The MeSH terms “aging”, “emission computed tomography”, “humans”, “serotonin receptors”, “serotonin plasma membrane transport proteins”, “serotonin” and “synthesis” were combined to search the largest biomedical literature database PubMed (cf. Supplementary Materials for exact literature search terms). The last search was performed on 01/16/2019. To further complement the study corpus, we carefully checked the references of all relevant studies to search for additional includable studies.
We included all accessible PET and SPECT imaging studies of the 5-HT system written in English or German that (i) reported original results, (ii) contained a sample of healthy adults (> 18 years) with a minimum age range of 25 years to representatively measure the age effect, and (iii) reported or allowed for the computation of the Pearson correlation coefficient r between age and a 5-HT target. We included different tracer kinetic measures that quantify the functioning of 5-HT targets such as non-displaceable binding potential and binding potential based on an arterial input function (Innis et al., 2007). We excluded studies (i) if their sample primarily consisted of relatives of depressive patients because serotonergic alterations have been observed in this subgroup (Leboyer et al., 1999), (ii) if we could not extract a standardized main effect of age comparable to Pearson’s r (e.g., in multiple regressions), and (iii) if their sample could be identified as subsample of another included study. To avoid duplicates in the included participants, we thoroughly inspected sample descriptions (e.g., age range and female percent) and compared experimental characteristics (e.g., scanner model and radiotracer) across studies.
2.2. Data extraction
Given that many studies reported the Pearson correlation coefficient r between age and individual targets of the 5-HT system (receptors, transporters, synthesis capacity), we used this standardized statistic in all meta-analytic analyses. The included studies either directly reported or enabled the computation of the Pearson correlation coefficient r from (i) a table, (ii) a figure (using PlotDigitizer version 2.6.8), or (iii) the group effect of age in older versus younger adults. We summarized the age effects for each of the following brain regions: raphe/midbrain, thalamus, medial temporal lobe (amygdala, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, entorhinal cortex, and perirhinal cortex), cingulate, insula, occipital, parietal, temporal, frontal and global cortex. Note that in case of 5-HT-1A receptors, raphe/midbrain regions predominantly contain presynaptic autoreceptors as opposed to the postsynaptic receptors that are in terminal regions (Barnes and Sharp, 1999). If a study reported the age effect separately for males and females, right and left hemisphere, or several sub-regions of the brain, we averaged the correlation coefficients using Fisher’s z transformation. Particularly in small samples, this conversion to an approximately normally distributed measure introduces less bias in the computation of the mean (Silver and Dunlap, 1987). If a study computed the age effects using several nuclear imaging analysis methods, we chose the method which the authors referred to as gold-standard or which was most commonly used in the literature.
In addition to the effect sizes of primary interest (r between 5-HT target and age), we extracted the following data: sample size, age range, female percent, imaging method, scanner model, 5-HT target, brain region, radiotracer, tracer kinetic measure, reference region, and partial volume correction. To examine the nature of the age effects in more detail, we extracted age and 5-HT target data of individual participants from tables or figures reported in a majority of the included studies. All extracted data are available at osf.io/3897j.
2.3. Data analysis
First, we examined the possibility that studies reporting stronger effects were more likely to get published and thus, included in the meta-analyses. To qualitatively assess publication bias, we used contour-enhanced funnel plots. In contrast to the traditional version, this visual tool indicates areas of statistical significance. This approach distinguishes publication bias from other types of biases such as study quality (Peters et al., 2008). We additionally applied the rank correlation test (Begg and Mazumdar, 1994) to quantitatively test for publication bias.
The primary goal was to meta-analytically summarize the correlations between age and different 5-HT targets across studies. We estimated one meta-analysis for each 5-HT target split into several target regions. Because the studies were based on different populations and varied in many aspects, we chose random-effects models to account for the heterogeneity between studies (Borenstein et al., 2010). In case the meta-analyses relied on too few studies to robustly estimate the heterogeneity between studies, we additionally compared the results to fixed-effect models, which only assume variance within the studies (Borenstein et al., 2010). If a study provided age correlations for more than one region, we used a multi-variate meta-analysis model to account for the statistical dependence of the effect sizes (Jackson et al., 2011). To stabilize the variances of the effect sizes, Pearson’s correlation coefficients r were converted to Fisher’s z before analysis (e.g., Viechtbauer, 2010).
In the next step, we examined how much of the overall study heterogeneity could be attributed to differences between studies rather than errors within studies using Q and I2 statistics (Higgins et al., 2003). To investigate which variables might explain the observed heterogeneity between the studies, we used a mixed-effects meta-regression. We included the following potential moderator variables in the meta-regression: age range and female percent of the sample, publication year, imaging method, radionuclide, tracer kinetic measure, reference region, partial volume correction, 5-HT target, and brain region (Table S1). We centered continuous variables first to facilitate interpretation. Since some studies contributed several effect sizes to the meta-regression, we again used a multivariate model that accounts for the statistical dependence of the effect sizes.
Because the study-level meta-analyses of linear effects of age were limited in providing insight into potential nonlinear associations between age and 5-HT targets, we additionally analyzed individual participant data. We estimated the type of relationship between age and different 5-HT targets using raw data extracted from a subset of the studies. If a study reported the tracer kinetic measure values for several brain regions instead of a global aggregation, we averaged the tracer kinetic measure values in each participant first. Then, we z-scored the individual tracer kinetic measure values in each study to allow for a comparison across different tracer kinetic measures. Finally, we fit linear, logarithmic, and quadratic functions to the individual data points to investigate which model best described the relationship between age and 5-HT targets. Mechanistically, linear models imply zero-order processes (e.g., constant decline per time unit), whereas logarithmic models suggest first-order processes (e.g., amount of decline dependent on 5-HT target concentration). Quadratic models, however, might indicate several competing processes. The extracted raw data additionally enabled us to estimate the percent difference per decade for different 5-HT targets. For this purpose, we divided the slope of the linear function by the range of the tracer kinetic measure values and multiplied the proportion by 10.
Finally, the meta-analytically-derived summary effect sizes were used to conduct statistical power analyses. Based on a two-sided significance level of alpha = 0.05, we determined the percentage of included studies with at least 80% statistical power. In addition, we computed the necessary sample sizes to detect effects of age in different 5-HT targets, which could be useful for future studies.
Analyses were conducted in R (version 3.4.0) primarily using the metafor package (Viechtbauer, 2010). All data analysis scripts are publicly available at osf.io/ctn83 for transparency and reuse.
3. Results
3.1. Literature search
The literature search yielded 299 potentially relevant studies. The study selection process is detailed in a PRISMA flowchart (Figure S1). Studies reporting age effects in 5-HT-1B receptors (Matuskey et al., 2012; Nord et al., 2014), 5-HT-4 receptors (Madsen et al., 2011; Marner et al., 2010), and 5-HT synthesis capacity (Rosa-Neto et al., 2007) were excluded due to an insufficient number to conduct a meta-analysis. A total of 31 studies reporting age effects in 5-HT-1A receptors (n=8), 5-HT-2A receptors (n=18), and 5-HT transporters (n=6) were included in the meta-analyses (cf. Supplementary Materials for the reference list; one study examined age effects in two 5-HT targets (Moses-Kolko et al., 2011)). The study corpus included 83.9% PET studies, 35.1% females, and a mean age range of 49 years in 1087 healthy adults (Table S2).
3.2. Publication bias and meta-analysis
The funnel plots contained few non-significant studies in 5-HT-2A receptors and 5-HT transporters, which could indicate publication bias (Figure S2). However, the applied rank correlation tests did not detect any significant relationship between effect sizes and sampling variance in 5-HT-2A receptors (Kendall’s t = −0.18; p = 0.149) and in 5-HT transporters (Kendall’s t = −0.15; p = 0.615). In 5-HT-1A receptors, however, the rank correlation test yielded a significant funnel plot asymmetry (Kendall’s t = −0.30; p = 0.010).
The random-effects meta-analyses estimated the summary correlation coefficients and their 95% confidence intervals. The average age effect was r = −0.23 [−0.37; −0.08] in 5-HT-1A receptors (Figure 1), r = −0.71 [−0.77; −0.63] in 5-HT-2A receptors (Figure 2), and r = −0.49 [−0.65; −0.28] in 5-HT transporters (Figure 3). Different variants of univariate and fixed-effect meta-analytic models yielded comparable results (Table S3). In addition, we computed the summary effect sizes separately for different brain regions (Table 1). We consistently observed negative effects of age across 5-HT target and brain region with the exception of 5-HT-1A receptors in raphe/midbrain and cingulate, which did not show significant age-related differences. The comparison of the 95% confidence intervals revealed significantly larger age differences in 5-HT-2A receptors compared to 5-HT-1A receptors (Figure 4).
Fig. 1.
Forest plot of age effects in 5-HT-1A receptors. Depicts reported effect size (diamond) and 95% confidence interval (line) for each included study. Weight of each study (size of diamond) depends on sample size and between-study variance. Summary effects sizes (polygon) and 95% confidence interval (width of polygon) for separate brain regions and in total. N represents the number of individual participants included in each study and in the computation of the summary effect sizes.
Fig. 2.
Forest plot of age effects in 5-HT-2A receptors. Depicts reported effect size (diamond) and 95% confidence interval (line) for each included study. Weight of each study (size of diamond) depends on sample size and between-study variance. Summary effects sizes (polygon) and 95% confidence interval (width of polygon) for separate brain regions and in total. N represents the number of individual participants included in each study and in the computation of the summary effect sizes.
Fig. 3.
Forest plot of age effects in 5-HT transporters. Depicts reported effect size (diamond) and 95% confidence interval (line) for each included study. Weight of each study (size of diamond) depends on sample size and between-study variance. Summary effects sizes (polygon) and 95% confidence interval (width of polygon) for separate brain regions and in total. N represents the number of individual participants included in each study and in the computation of the summary effect sizes. Note that in one study (Hesse et al., 2003) the region of interest for thalamus also included hypothalamus.
Table 1.
Results of meta-analyses of adult age differences in 5-HT-1A receptors, 5-HT-2A receptors, and 5-HT transporters.
Target | r | 95% CI | # of studies | # of participants | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
5-HT-1A receptor | −0.23 | −0.37 | −0.08 | 8 | 277 |
Frontal cortex | −0.21 | −0.38 | −0.02 | 7 | 269 |
Temporal cortex | −0.23 | −0.38 | −0.07 | 5 | 162 |
Parietal cortex | −0.42 | −0.63 | −0.14 | 3 | 133 |
Occipital cortex | −0.35 | −0.49 | −0.2 | 4 | 154 |
Insula | −0.33 | −0.51 | −0.12 | 3 | 133 |
Medial temporal lobe | −0.18 | −0.33 | −0.03 | 5 | 179 |
Cingulate | −0.18 | −0.35 | 0 | 5 | 179 |
Raphe/midbrain | −0.15 | −0.32 | 0.03 | 7 | 206 |
5-HT-2A receptor | −0.71 | −0.77 | −0.63 | 17 | 472 |
Global cortex | −0.81 | −0.9 | −0.66 | 5 | 184 |
Frontal cortex | −0.7 | −0.78 | −0.59 | 13 | 335 |
Temporal cortex | −0.67 | −0.82 | −0.45 | 5 | 125 |
Parietal cortex | −0.7 | −0.82 | −0.54 | 4 | 97 |
Occipital cortex | −0.66 | −0.75 | −0.53 | 5 | 118 |
5-HT transporter | −0.49 | −0.65 | −0.28 | 6 | 338 |
Thalamus | −0.58 | −0.83 | −0.14 | 3 | 177 |
Raphe/midbrain | −0.5 | −0.6 | −0.39 | 5 | 211 |
Fig. 4.
Comparison of overall age effects in 5-HT targets. Effects compared here are based on the lowermost meta-analytically-derived summary effect size polygons from Figures 1–3 in 5-HT-1A receptors (across frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortex, insula, medial temporal lobe, cingulate, and raphe/midbrain), 5-HT-2A receptors (across global, frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortex), and 5-HT transporters (across thalamus and raphe/midbrain). Black line indicates summary effect size and height of polygon represents 95% confidence interval. Dotted line marks an age effect of zero. Age effects on 5-HT-1A receptors and 5-HT-2A receptors were significantly different at *p < 0.01 (Cumming, 2009).
3.3. Heterogeneity and meta-regression
Significant Q-statistics in 5-HT-1A receptors (Q = 58.0%; p = 0.026), 5-HT-2A receptors (Q = 61.6%; p = 0.003), and 5-HT transporters (Q = 23.2%; p = 0.002) indicated that the included studies did not share a common effect size. Similarly, the I2-statistics revealed moderate amounts of heterogeneity in 5-HT-1A receptors (I2 = 55.1%), 5-HT-2A receptors (I2 = 55.9%), and 5-HT transporters (I2 = 72.5%). The mixed-effects meta-regression showed that the included variables explained a significant amount of the observed heterogeneity in the age effect (p < 0.001). Publication year (p = 0.005) and radionuclide (p = 0.008) emerged as significant moderators of the age effect (Table 2). Specifically, smaller effects of age were reported in more recent publications and in studies using the radiotracer 11C instead of 18F. Post-hoc tests, which evaluated the influence of a variable across several factor levels, additionally showed a significant moderating effects of 5-HT target (p = 0.001). Age range, female percent, imaging method, tracer kinetic measure, reference region, partial volume correction, and brain region did not significantly moderate the age effect. A test for residual heterogeneity indicated that other variables not considered in the meta-regression did not significantly influence the age effect (p = 0.226).
Table 2.
Influence of potential moderators on the correlation between age and 5-HT targets in a multi-variate mixed-effects meta-regression.
Moderator variable (factor level) |
r | SE | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | −0.13 | 0.12 | −0.35 | 0.11 |
Age range | 0 | 0.01 | −0.01 | 0.01 |
Female percent | −0.25 | 0.31 | −0.69 | 0.33 |
Publication year | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.04 |
Imaging method (SPECT) | 0.18 | 0.23 | −0.27 | 0.56 |
Tracer kinetic measure (BPai) | 0.24 | 0.13 | −0.01 | 0.45 |
Radionuclide (18F) | −0.39 | 0.16 | −0.62 | −0.11 |
Reference region (occipital) | 0.24 | 0.25 | −0.24 | 0.62 |
Partial volume correction (Applied) | −0.01 | 0.14 | −0.28 | 0.25 |
Target (5-HT transporter) | −0.66 | 0.22 | −0.84 | −0.35 |
Target (5-HT-2A receptor) | −0.3 | 0.15 | −0.55 | −0.01 |
Region (Cingulate) | −0.12 | 0.1 | −0.31 | 0.09 |
Region (Frontal cortex) | −0.19 | 0.09 | −0.36 | 0 |
Region (Global cortex) | −0.54 | 0.21 | −0.77 | −0.2 |
Region (Insula) | −0.24 | 0.11 | −0.43 | −0.02 |
Region (Medial temporal lobe) | −0.09 | 0.1 | −0.29 | 0.11 |
Region (Occipital cortex) | −0.23 | 0.1 | −0.41 | −0.02 |
Region (Parietal cortex) | −0.25 | 0.11 | −0.44 | −0.04 |
Region (Temporal cortex) | −0.09 | 0.1 | −0.28 | 0.11 |
Region (Thalamus) | 0 | 0.14 | −0.27 | 0.27 |
Note: Three studies missing information were automatically dropped from the meta-regression. The factor level “123I” of the radionuclide variable was automatically dropped from the model due to a complete redundancy with the factor level “SPECT” of the imaging method variable.
3.4. Linearity/non-linearity of age effects and percent difference per decade
Individual participant data for at least a subset of brain regions were available for nearly all of the studies (n=29 out of 31). This allowed us to test linear, logarithmic, and quadratic effects of age across 5-HT targets (Figure 5). Linear age effects explained 2% of the variance in 5-HT-1A receptors (p < 0.05), 41.7% of variance in 5-HT-2A receptors (p < 0.001) and 11.7% of variance in 5-HT transporters (p < 0.001). The fits of the logarithmic and quadratic functions were comparable to those of the linear function while adjusting for model complexity (Table S4). The quadratic term, however, did not add a significant amount of explained variance in any of the three 5-HT targets (p5-HT-1A = 0.976; p5-HT-2A = 0.727; p5-HT = 0.934).
Fig. 5.
Scatter plots depicting the relationship between age and 5-HT targets based on a subset of studies (29 out of 31 studies) for which raw data were available. Data points represent individual age effects averaged across brain regions for 5-HT-1A receptors (across frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortex, insula, medial temporal lobe, cingulate, and raphe/midbrain), 5-HT-2A receptors (across global, frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortex), and 5-HT transporters (across thalamus and raphe/midbrain). Linear, quadratic, and logarithmic model were fit to individual age and standardized tracer kinetic measure data in 5-HT targets. N indicates the number of individual participants included in the analysis. R2 values are adjusted R2.
Using linear models, the observed percent differences per decade were 1.5% in 5-HT-1A receptors, 7.0% in 5-HT-2A receptors, and 3.0% in 5-HT transporters. The rank order of these effects is consistent with the average age correlations yielded from the meta-analyses. These measures are provided as a more concrete quantification of age-related differences relative to 5-HT functioning in younger adulthood.
3.5. Statistical power analyses
The average sample sizes of the included studies were about 35 participants (median = 22), ranging from 7 to 132 participants. Assuming that the meta-analytic effect sizes closely resembled the true effects of age, we found an average power of 71.0% percent across studies. Half of the studies reached the commonly recommended statistical power of at least 80%. The power analyses revealed the minimum sample sizes required for a minimum of 80% of statistical power in detecting linear effects of age at 146 participants for 5-HT-1A receptors, 13 participants for 5-HT-2A receptors, and 30 participants for 5-HT transporters.
4. Discussion
The results of the present meta-analysis of cross-sectional PET and SPECT imaging studies in healthy adults revealed several negative effects of age on serotonergic functioning. We found large negative effects of age in 5-HT-2A receptors (largest in global cortex), moderate negative effects of age in 5-HT transporters (largest in thalamus), and small negative effects of age in 5-HT-1A receptors (largest in parietal cortex). Presynaptic 5-HT-1A autoreceptors in raphe/midbrain, however, were relatively preserved in older age. Overall, the estimated age-related reductions across 5-HT targets ranged between 1.5% and 7.0% per decade. In addition to variation in age effects across 5-HT targets, we identify further variables that moderated age effects. Overall, the study provides the first quantitative synthesis of adult age differences in the 5-HT system.
The meta-analytic evidence for fewer postsynaptic receptors in terminal regions implies a restricted capacity to transmit 5-HT mediated signals in older age. This alteration in signal transduction might be compensated by a mechanism whereby 5-HT could be active in the synaptic cleft for a longer time interval. The activity of synaptic 5-HT could be prolonged due to the lower number of 5-HT transporters with age. The presynaptic transporter proteins may limit clearance of 5-HT from the synaptic cleft via re-uptake into the presynaptic neuron. In contrast to the age effects on post-synaptic receptors in terminal regions, when examining effects within individual brain regions only the negative feedback process that enables the inhibition of 5-HT release into the synaptic cleft seemed to be intact with age. Specifically, there was a non-significant effect of age on 5-HT-1A receptors in raphe/midbrain which are presynaptic somatodendritic autoreceptors. Note that it remains unclear which of these age effects (i.e., pre- versus post-synaptic) appear first and which effects represent only a subsequent auto-regulatory reaction such as triggered by synaptic remodeling (Kitahara et al., 2016; Pineyro and Blier, 1999). Future studies could investigate how age affects other receptor types and presynaptic mechanisms such as synthesis capacity. Unfortunately, there were not enough studies of age differences in synthesis capacity to meta-analyze. One of the few studies on 5-HT synthesis capacity reported stable levels of this presynaptic 5-HT target across the adult life span (Rosa-Neto et al., 2007).
5-HT-1A and 5-HT-2A receptors differ in their distribution across the brain and signaling cascades (Hannon and Hoyer, 2008). For instance, electrophysiological experiments have shown that 5-HT-1A receptor activation induces a hyperpolarization (Nicoll et al., 1990), whereas 5-HT-2A receptor activation primarily leads to a depolarization of the postsynaptic neuron (Aghajanian, 1995). Given the localization of 5-HT-1A and 5-HT-2A on cortical glutamatergic neurons, the relative preservation of 1A compared to 2A receptors might suggest an age-related increase in hyperpolarization of glutamatergic cells. Relatedly, age-related reductions of 5-HT transporters and preservation of somatodendritic 5-HT-1A autoreceptors might predict an age-related increase in 5-HT levels. For a deeper understanding of the functional implications of the present work, the interactions of 5-HT with other neurochemical systems such as glutamate and GABA should be taken into account (Ciranna, 2006).
In general, 5-HT-1A and 5-HT-2A receptors likely mediate distinct neural and behavioral functions (Barnes and Sharp, 1999). A recent theory suggests that 5-HT-1A and 5-HT-2A receptors enable humans to adapt flexibly to adverse events in the environment (Carhart-Harris and Nutt, 2017). Concretely, the mostly inhibitory 5-HT-1A receptor was assumed to mediate passive coping responses such as internal reappraisal of a stressful situation (Carhart-Harris and Nutt, 2017). The mainly excitatory 5-HT-2A receptor however, was suggested to mediate active coping strategies such as engaging in an interpersonal confrontation (Carhart-Harris and Nutt, 2017). In light of this hypothesis, the finding of stronger age-related differences in 5-HT-2A receptors compared to 5-HT-1A receptors might be viewed as consistent with the evidence that older adults use more passive, emotion-focused than active, problem-focused coping strategies (Folkman et al., 1987). The potential mediation of these behavioral age differences by serotonergic function has not been directly tested. Surprisingly, few studies (n=6) examined associations between 5HT receptors and transporters and physiological, emotional, and cognitive variables, primarily focusing on personality traits (Frokjaer et al., 2009; Parsey et al., 2002; Rosell et al., 2010; Zientek et al., 2016) and hormone levels (Frokjaer et al., 2009; Moses-Kolko et al., 2011). Thus, additional research is needed to examine the functional consequences of these neurobiological age differences.
Besides variation across 5-HT targets, we observed that the age effects were moderated by publication year and radionuclide. First, the evidence that smaller effects of age were reported in more recently published studies might be explained by higher effective camera resolutions due to improved technologies and more advanced quantification methods. These newer cameras may better assess individual subregions of the brain and minimize signal mixing with nearby white matter or cerebrospinal fluid (see below for additional discussion of partial volume effects). Unfortunately, we were not able to further investigate this hypothesis since effective scanner resolution is only rarely reported in PET and SPECT imaging studies. Second, the moderating effect of radionuclide might be accounted for by the more reliable and accurate signal of 18F because of its longer half-life and shorter positron range compared to 11C (Morris et al., 2013). An important caveat here is not to over-interpret the results of the meta-regression. The moderating effects could also be explained by shared confounds between the studies. For instance, we were not able to consider previously reported moderators of the age effect such as body mass index (Hesse et al., 2014), sex hormones (Moses-Kolko et al., 2011), and genes (David et al., 2005). Furthermore, we did not find any evidence for a moderating effect of sex as some studies have reported previously (e.g., Baeken et al., 1998; Cidis Meltzer et al., 2001). Yet, this finding is in line with studies based on larger samples that did also not find an age by sex interaction (e.g., Frokjaer et al., 2009; van Dyck et al., 2000).
A further limitation of the meta-analysis is the potential overestimation of the effects of age. One reason might be publication bias as quantitatively indicated in 5-HT-1A receptors and informally observed during the literature search. A few studies reporting a non-significant age effect did not state the exact size of the age effect and could hence not be included in the meta-analyses. This is a major limitation of the study. Partial volume effects could have additionally contributed to an overestimation of the effects of age in the 5-HT system. There is considerable evidence for grey matter density reductions with age (Sowell et al., 2003). PET and SPECT signals in atrophy-affected, smaller brain regions are more susceptible to blurring by surrounding tissue (Hoffman et al., 1979; Hoffman et al., 1982). Such partial volume effects can lead to an underestimation of the 5-HT signal in age, as has been reported in the dopamine system (Morris et al., 1999; Smith et al., 2017). Several established techniques are available to account for partial volume effects (e.g., Meltzer et al., 1990; Muller-Gartner et al., 1992; Rousset et al., 1998), but only 7 out of 31 included studies applied such corrections. These potential measurement confounds in a majority of the included studies might have led to an overestimation of the age effect in the meta-analyses. Yet, we did not find any evidence for a systematic influence of partial volume corrections on the reported effects of age. To differentiate age-related receptor loss from age-related neuronal atrophy, it will be important for future studies to directly compare age effects in uncorrected data and after correcting for partial volume effects. Additionally, it is important to note that mean levels of binding vary across brain regions, which may bias estimates of age effects. Specifically, age effects may be underestimated due to floor effects in regions that show low levels of binding.
The present study has several additional limitations, both at the level of the meta-analyses and the level of the included studies. First, the literature search was only based on one database as opposed to the recommendations of the PRISMA statement. Yet, we inspected a large number of potentially relevant studies and conducted a thorough reference search. Second, the individual studies seemed to involve on average a lower number of older adults than younger adults as indicated by the scatter plots. This imbalance might have limited the ability to detect nonlinear effects of age on the 5-HT system across adulthood. Third, the individual studies used radionuclides of varying quality. We attempted to include only studies using radionuclides of sufficient affinity and selectivity for 5-HT (Paterson et al., 2013). Finally, our meta-analyses were based on cross-sectional studies which measure age-related differences as opposed to age-related changes and these age differences may be related to cohort differences rather than developmental change. Both limitations can be addressed with longitudinal study designs. The only longitudinal study so far showed stable 5-HT-2A receptor levels after two years (Marner et al., 2009) but suffered from a small sample size, a limited age range, and a relatively short longitudinal study interval given the estimated rate of decline inferred from the results of cross-sectional studies. Hence, longitudinal studies with sufficient power, a broad age range, and a longer follow-up period will be necessary to disentangle age and cohort effects. Longitudinal studies could further provide insight into the nature of the underlying degradation processes (e.g. zero-order or first-order process), for which the present dataset lacked sufficient granularity.
Here we conducted the first meta-analysis that precisely quantifies adult age differences in several 5-HT targets. The finding of an age-related reduction in 5-HT receptors reveals consistent evidence for limitations in serotonergic signal transmission in older age. Age-related losses in the 5HT system might be compensated by fewer 5-HT transporters, which might lead to a prolonged activity of 5-HT in the synaptic cleft. The regulation of 5-HT release into the synaptic cleft via presynaptic 5-HT-1A autoreceptors may remain intact in older age. The evidence for a relative preservation of 5-HT-1A compared to 5-HT-2A receptors is consistent with a recent theory linking 5-HT functioning to behavioral evidence for age differences in the use of coping strategies. Finally, the present meta-analytic results may also inform clinical studies examining the role of 5-HT in neurological and psychiatric disorders. The results provide an informative baseline for healthy aging to which clinical groups could be compared (e.g., late life depression, Parkinson’s Disease, Alzheimer’s’ Disease). Related to this, the meta-analytic effects were used to estimate required minimum sample sizes in future studies of age differences in 5HT function in healthy adults (5-HT-1A receptors, N=146; 5-HT-2A receptors, N=13; 5-HT transporters, N=30).
Supplementary Material
Highlights:
Meta-analytic estimates of adult age effects on 5-HT transporters and receptors.
Larger negative effects of age on 5-HT-2A compared to 5-HT-1A receptors.
5-HT target, publication year, and radionuclide moderated the age effects.
Observed decline per decade ranged between 1.5% and 7.0% across 5-HT targets.
Acknowledgements
TMK was supported by the International Research Training Group (IRTG 2150) of the German Research Foundation and by a full doctoral scholarship of the German National Academic foundation. GRSL was supported by US National Institute on Aging Pathway to Independence Award R00-AG042596.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Aghajanian GK, 1995. Electrophysiology of serotonin receptor subtypes and signal transduction pathways., in: Bloom FR, Kupfer DJ (Eds.), Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress. Raven, New York, pp. 1451–1459. [Google Scholar]
- Baeken C, D’Haenen H, Flamen P, Mertens J, Terriere D, Chavatte K, Boumon R, Bossuyt A, 1998. 123I-5-I-R91150, a new single-photon emission tomography ligand for 5-HT2A receptors: influence of age and gender in healthy subjects. Eur. J. Nucl. Med 25(12), 1617–1622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes NM, Sharp T, 1999. A review of central 5-HT receptors and their function. Neuropharmacology 38(8), 1083–1152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Begg CB, Mazumdar M, 1994. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics 50(4), 1088–1101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR, 2010. A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Research synthesis methods 1(2), 97–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carhart-Harris RL, Nutt DJ, 2017. Serotonin and brain function: a tale of two receptors. Journal of psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 31(9), 1091–1120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi A, Hariri AR, Holmes A, Uher R, Moffitt TE, 2010. Genetic sensitivity to the environment: the case of the serotonin transporter gene and its implications for studying complex diseases and traits. Am. J. Psychiatry 167(5), 509–527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cidis Meltzer C, Drevets WC, Price JC, Mathis CA, Lopresti B, Greer PJ, Villemagne VL, Holt D, Mason NS, Houck PR, Reynolds CF 3rd, DeKosky ST, 2001. Gender-specific aging effects on the serotonin 1A receptor. Brain Res. 895(1–2), 9–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciranna L, 2006. Serotonin as a Modulator of Glutamate- and GABA-Mediated Neurotransmission: Implications in Physiological Functions and in Pathology. Curr. Neuropharmacol 4(2), 101–114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costes N, Merlet I, Ostrowsky K, Faillenot I, Lavenne F, Zimmer L, Ryvlin P, Le Bars D, 2005. A 18F-MPPF PET normative database of 5-HT1A receptor binding in men and women over aging. J. Nucl. Med 46(12), 1980–1989. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cumming G, 2009. Inference by eye: reading the overlap of independent confidence intervals. Stat. Med 28, 205–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- David SP, Murthy NV, Rabiner EA, Munafo MR, Johnstone EC, Jacob R, Walton RT, Grasby PM, 2005. A functional genetic variation of the serotonin (5-HT) transporter affects 5-HT1A receptor binding in humans. J. Neurosci 25(10), 2586–2590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fazio P, Schain M, Varnas K, Halldin C, Farde L, Varrone A, 2016. Mapping the distribution of serotonin transporter in the human brainstem with high-resolution PET: Validation using postmortem autoradiography data. Neuroimage 133, 313–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folkman S, Lazarus RS, Pimley S, Novacek J, 1987. Age differences in stress and coping processes. Psychol. Aging 2(2), 171–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frokjaer VG, Erritzoe D, Madsen J, Paulson OB, Knudsen GM, 2009. Gender and the use of hormonal contraception in women are not associated with cerebral cortical 5-HT 2A receptor binding. Neuroscience 163(2), 640–645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaspar P, Cases O, Maroteaux L, 2003. The developmental role of serotonin: news from mouse molecular genetics. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 4(12), 1002–1012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gryglewski G, Lanzenberger R, Kranz GS, Cumming P, 2014. Meta-analysis of molecular imaging of serotonin transporters in major depression. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab 34(7), 1096–1103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hannon J, Hoyer D, 2008. Molecular biology of 5-HT receptors. Behav. Brain Res 195(1), 198–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hesse S, Barthel H, Murai T, Muller U, Muller D, Seese A, Kluge R, Sabri O, 2003. Is correction for age necessary in neuroimaging studies of the central serotonin transporter? Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 30(3), 427–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hesse S, van de Giessen E, Zientek F, Petroff D, Winter K, Dickson JC, Tossici-Bolt L, Sera T, Asenbaum S, Darcourt J, Akdemir UO, Knudsen GM, Nobili F, Pagani M, Vander Borght T, Van Laere K, Varrone A, Tatsch K, Sabri O, Booij J, 2014. Association of central serotonin transporter availability and body mass index in healthy Europeans. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol 24(8), 1240–1247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG, 2003. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327(7414), 557–560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman EJ, Huang SC, Phelps ME, 1979. Quantitation in positron emission computed tomography: 1. Effect of object size. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr 3(3), 299–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman EJ, Huang SC, Plummer D, Phelps ME, 1982. Quantitation in positron emission computed tomography: 6. effect of nonuniform resolution. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr 6(5), 987–999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Innis RB, Cunningham VJ, Delforge J, Fujita M, Gjedde A, Gunn RN, Holden J, Houle S, Huang S-C, Ichise M, Iida H, Ito H, Kimura Y, Koeppe RA, Knudsen GM, Knuuti J, Lammertsma AA, Laruelle M, Logan J, Maguire RP, Mintun MA, Morris EDW, Dean F, Parsey R, Price JC, Slifstein M, Sossi V, Suhara T, Votaw JR, Wong DF, Carson RE, 2007. Consensus nomenclature for in vivo imaging of reversibly binding radioligands. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab 27, 1533–1539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson D, Riley R, White IR, 2011. Multivariate meta-analysis: potential and promise. Stat. Med 30(20), 2481–2498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kambeitz JP, Howes OD, 2015. The serotonin transporter in depression: Meta-analysis of in vivo and post mortem findings and implications for understanding and treating depression. J. Affect. Disord 186, 358–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karrer TM, Josef AK, Mata R, Morris ED, Samanez-Larkin GR, 2017. Reduced dopamine receptors and transporters but not synthesis capacity in normal aging adults: a meta-analysis. Neurobiol. Aging 57, 36–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kitahara Y, Ohta K, Hasuo H, Shuto T, Kuroiwa M, Sotogaku N, Togo A, Nakamura K, Nishi A, 2016. Chronic Fluoxetine Induces the Enlargement of Perforant Path-Granule Cell Synapses in the Mouse Dentate Gyrus. PLoS One 11(1), e0147307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leboyer M, Quintin P, Manivet P, Varoquaux O, Allilaire J-F, Launay J-M, 1999. Decreased serotonin transporter binding in unaffected relatives of manic depressive patients. Biol. Psychiatry 46(12), 1703–1706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liberarti A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gotzsche PC, Ioannidis JPA, Clarke M, Devereaux PJ, Kleinjen J, Moher D, 2009. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: explanation and elaboration. The BMJ 339, 1–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipscomp CE, 2000. Medical Subject Headings (MeSH). Bull. Med. Libr. Assoc 88, 265–266. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucki I, 1998. The spectrum of behaviors influenced by serotonin. Biol. Psychiatry 44(3), 151–162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madsen K, Haahr MT, Marner L, Keller SH, Baare WF, Svarer C, Hasselbalch SG, Knudsen GM, 2011. Age and sex effects on 5-HT(4) receptors in the human brain: a [(11)C]SB207145 PET study. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab 31(6), 1475–1481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marner L, Gillings N, Madsen K, Erritzoe D, Baare WF, Svarer C, Hasselbalch SG, Knudsen GM, 2010. Brain imaging of serotonin 4 receptors in humans with [11C]SB207145-PET. Neuroimage 50(3), 855–861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marner L, Knudsen GM, Haugbol S, Holm S, Baare W, Hasselbalch SG, 2009. Longitudinal assessment of cerebral 5-HT2A receptors in healthy elderly volunteers: an [18F]-altanserin PET study. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 36(2), 287–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matuskey D, Pittman B, Planeta-Wilson B, Walderhaug E, Henry S, Gallezot JD, Nabulsi N, Ding YS, Bhagwagar Z, Malison R, Carson RE, Neumeister A, 2012. Age effects on serotonin receptor 1B as assessed by PET. J. Nucl. Med 53(9), 1411–1414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer CC, Leal JP, Mayberg HS, Wagner HN Jr., Frost JJ, 1990. Correction of PET data for partial volume effects in human cerebral cortex by MR imaging. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr 14(4), 561–570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer CC, Smith G, DeKosky ST, Pollock BG, Mathis CA, Moore RY, Kupfer DJ, Reynolds CF 3rd, 1998. Serotonin in aging, late-life depression, and Alzheimer’s disease: the emerging role of functional imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 18(6), 407–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris ED, Chefer SI, Lane MA, Muzic RF Jr., Wong DF, Dannals RF, Matochik JA, Bonab AA, Villemagne VL, Grant SJ, Ingram DK, Roth GS, London ED, 1999. Loss of D2 receptor binding with age in rhesus monkeys: importance of correction for differences in striatal size. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab 19(2), 218–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris ED, Lucas MV, Cosgrove KP, 2013. How to study smoking and drinking with PET, Positron Emission Tomography - Recent Developments in Instrumentation, Research and Clinical Oncological Practiceal. InTech, pp. 103–149. [Google Scholar]
- Moses-Kolko EL, Price JC, Shah N, Berga S, Sereika SM, Fisher PM, Coleman R, Becker C, Mason NS, Loucks T, Meltzer CC, 2011. Age, sex, and reproductive hormone effects on brain serotonin-1A and serotonin-2A receptor binding in a healthy population. Neuropsychopharmacology 36(13), 2729–2740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller-Gartner HW, Links JM, Prince JL, Bryan RN, McVeigh E, Leal JP, Davatzikos C, Frost JJ, 1992. Measurement of radiotracer concentration in brain gray matter using positron emission tomography: MRI-based correction for partial volume effects. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab 12(4), 571–583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicoll RA, Malenka RC, Kauer JA, 1990. Functional comparison of neurotransmitter receptor subtypes in mammalian central nervous system. Physiol. Rev 70(2), 513–565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nord M, Cselenyi Z, Forsberg A, Rosenqvist G, Tiger M, Lundberg J, Varrone A, Farde L, 2014. Distinct regional age effects on [11C]AZ10419369 binding to 5-HT1B receptors in the human brain. Neuroimage 103, 303–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogilvie AD, Battersby S, Bubb VJ, Fink G, Harmar AJ, Goodwim GM, Smith CA, 1996. Polymorphism in serotonin transporter gene associated with susceptibility to major depression. Lancet 347(9003), 731–733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parsey RV, Oquendo MA, Simpson NR, Ogden RT, Van Heertum R, Arango V, Mann JJ, 2002. Effects of sex, age, and aggressive traits in man on brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptor binding potential measured by PET using [C-11]WAY-100635. Brain Res 954(2), 173–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paterson LM, Kornum BR, Nutt DJ, Pike VW, Knudsen GM, 2013. 5-HT radioligands for human brain imaging with PET and SPECT. Med. Res. Rev 33(1), 54–111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters JL, Sutton AJ, Jones DR, Abrams KR, Rushton L, 2008. Contour-enhanced meta-analysis funnel plots help distinguish publication bias from other causes of asymmetry. J. Clin. Epidemiol 61(10), 991–996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pineyro G, Blier P, 1999. Autoregulation of serotonin neurons: role in antidepressant drug action. Pharmacol. Rev 51(3), 533–591. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabiner EA, Messa C, Sargent PA, Husted-Kjaer K, Montgomery A, Lawrence AD, Bench CJ, Gunn RN, Cowen P, Grasby PM, 2002. A database of [(11)C]WAY-100635 binding to 5-HT(1A) receptors in normal male volunteers: normative data and relationship to methodological, demographic, physiological, and behavioral variables. Neuroimage 15(3), 620–632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosa-Neto P, Benkelfat C, Sakai Y, Leyton M, Morais JA, Diksic M, 2007. Brain regional alpha-[11C]methyl-L-tryptophan trapping, used as an index of 5-HT synthesis, in healthy adults: absence of an age effect. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 34(8), 1254–1264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosell DR, Thompson JL, Slifstein M, Xu X, Frankle WG, New AS, Goodman M, Weinstein SR, Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A, Siever LJ, 2010. Increased serotonin 2A receptor availability in the orbitofrontal cortex of physically aggressive personality disordered patients. Biol. Psychiatry 67(12), 1154–1162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rousset OG, Ma Y, Evans AC, 1998. Correction for partial volume effects in PET: principle and validation. Journal of Nuclear Medicine Med 39, 904–911. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheline YI, Mintun MA, Moerlein SM, Snyder AZ, 2002. Greater loss of 5-HT(2A) receptors in midlife than in late life. Am. J. Psychiatry 159(3), 430–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver NC, Dunlap WP, 1987. Averaging correlation coefficients: should Fisher’s z transformation be used? J. Appl. Psychol 72, 146–148. [Google Scholar]
- Smith CT, Crawford JL, Dang LC, Seaman KL, San Juan MD, Vijay A, Katz DT, Matuskey D, Cowan RL, Morris ED, Zald DH, Samanez-Larkin GR, 2017. Partial-volume correction increases estimated dopamine D2-like receptor binding potential and reduces adult age differences. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab, 271678×17737693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW, 2003. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat. Neurosci 6(3), 309–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tauscher J, Verhoeff NP, Christensen BK, Hussey D, Meyer JH, Kecojevic A, Javanmard M, Kasper S, Kapur S, 2001. Serotonin 5-HT1A receptor binding potential declines with age as measured by [11C]WAY-100635 and PET. Neuropsychopharmacology 24(5), 522–530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uchida H, Chow TW, Mamo DC, Kapur S, Mulsant BH, Houle S, Pollock BG, Graff-Guerrero A, 2011. Effects of aging on 5-HT(2A) R binding: a HRRT PET study with and without partial volume corrections. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 26(12), 1300–1308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Dyck CH, Malison RT, Seibyl JP, Laruelle M, Klumpp H, Zoghbi SS, Baldwin RM, Innis RB, 2000. Age-related decline in central serotonin transporter availability with [(123)I]beta-CIT SPECT. Neurobiol. Aging 21(4), 497–501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viechtbauer W, 2010. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software 36, 1–48. [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto M, Suhara T, Okubo Y, Ichimiya T, Sudo Y, Inoue M, Takano A, Yasuno F, Yoshikawa K, Tanada S, 2002. Age-related decline of serotonin transporters in living human brain of healthy males. Life Sci. 71(7), 751–757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zientek F, Winter K, Muller A, Rullmann M, Luthardt J, Becker GA, Bresch A, Patt M, Sabri O, Hilbert A, Hesse S, 2016. Effortful control as a dimension of temperament is negatively associated with prefrontal serotonin transporter availability in obese and non-obese individuals. Eur. J. Neurosci 44(7), 2460–2466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.