Skip to main content
PeerJ logoLink to PeerJ
. 2019 Aug 1;7:e7432. doi: 10.7717/peerj.7432

Defining cognitive profiles of depressive patients using the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Affective Disorders

Ruei-An Chen 1, Chun-Yi Lee 1, Yu Lee 1, Chi-Fa Hung 1, Yu-Chi Huang 1, Pao-Yen Lin 1, Sheng-Yu Lee 2,3, Liang-Jen Wang 4,
Editor: Adrian Preda
PMCID: PMC6679906  PMID: 31396459

Abstract

Background

Cognitive impairments in patients with depressive disorders have a negative impact on their daily skill functioning and quality of life. In this study, we evaluated the cognitive profiles and associated factors of patients with depressive disorders with the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Affective Disorders (BAC-A).

Methods

This cross-sectional study consisted of 75 patients with depressive disorders (56 patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) and 19 patients with depressive disorder NOS or dysthymic disorder (non-MDD)). We evaluated the participants’ cognitive functions at euthymic status using the BAC-A. The BAC-A includes six subtests derived from the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BAC-S) and Affective Processing Tests. The current severity of depressive symptoms was assessed with the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD-17), and we recorded any psychotropic drugs being used by the patients.

Results

We observed no differences in cognitive profiles in the MDD group and non-MDD group after adjusting for educational levels, severity of depression, and psychotropic drugs. Instead, the HAMD-17 scores were negatively correlated to cognitive performance in working memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, attention and processing speed, executive function, composite score, and the six indexes of the Affective Processing Test measured by the BAC-A. A longer illness duration was associated with worse performance of four indexes of the Affective Processing Test. Furthermore, benzodiazepine use was associated with a worse performance of verbal memory, and antidepressant use was associated with better motor speed performance.

Conclusion

The current severity of depressive symptoms and psychotropic drugs being taken, not the diagnosis category, are associated with cognitive impairments in patients with depressive disorders. Clinicians should pay particular attention to managing residual depressive symptoms and prescribing adequate psychotropic drugs in order to eliminate depressive patients’ cognitive deficits.

Keywords: Neurocognitive tests, Emotion, Depression, Attention, BACA

Introduction

Depressive disorder, including major depressive disorder (MDD), depressive disorder NOS, or dysthymic disorder, is among the leading causes of burden within the general population (Ferrari et al., 2013). Impaired cognitive function has been widely reported in patients with depressive disorder  (Burt, Zembar & Niederehe, 1995) and affects nearly two-thirds of depressed patients (Afridi et al., 2011; Butters et al., 2004), which subsequently causes significant disabilities in people’s lives. Previous evidence has indicated that reduced cognition in patients with MDD is closely related to an impaired quality of life and diminished personal function, while also contributing to disability (Jaeger et al., 2006; Naismith et al., 2007).Therefore, an adequate assessment for cognition decline in patients with depressive disorders has become a matter of great concern.

Cognitive impairment in such domains as complex attention, executive function, learning and memory, psychomotor skills, and processing speed has been proved in both acute episodes of depressive disorder and the euthymic mood state (Marazziti et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2012; Bora et al., 2013; Bortolato, Carvalho & McIntyre, 2014). However, the relationship between medication and cognitive function in patients with MDD and dysthymic patients is still poorly understood, and whether cognitive function may improve after treatment remains unknown.

Cognitive function in patients with MDD is usually assessed by using a well-developed cognitive assessment tool, such as the Screen for Cognitive Impairment in Psychiatry or the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery . Compared to the aforementioned batteries, the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Affective Disorders (BAC-A) was developed to evaluate cognitive deficits specifically in patients with affective disorder (Bauer et al., 2015). BAC-A includes six subtests derived from the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BAC-S), including the Controlled Oral Word Association, Tower of London, Token Motor Task, Digit Sequencing Task, Category Instances (Animals), Symbol Coding, and List Learning tests (Keefe et al., 2014). Such Affective Processing Tests as the Emotional Inhibition Test, Delayed Recognition Test, and Affective Interference Test were also included in the BAC-A (Bauer et al., 2015). BAC-A has been proven to be a reliable battery compared to traditional neuropsychological exams (Keefe et al., 2014) and has been validated for identifying cognitive impairment in patients with bipolar disorder (Bauer et al., 2015). However, the BACA has not yet been used in MDD for further profiling, and whether BACA is able to detect cognitive deficits in depressive patients even with residual symptoms remains unclear.

Aims of the study

Previous studies have demonstrated the potential benefit of differentiating patients with bipolar disorder from those with unipolar depression at euthymic status through BAC-A (Lee et al., 2018b). Nevertheless, the effects of the different characteristics of depressive disorder and psychotropic medications on the performance of BAC-A have not yet been clearly identified. We proposed that patients with MDD diagnosis, those with residual depressive symptoms had poorer BACA performance than their counterparts. Therefore, this study aimed to investigate the cognitive profiles of patients with depressive disorders by using the BAC-A for neuropsychological assessment and to explore the effects of depression characteristics on the performance of each BAC-A index.

Material and Methods

Study participants

This cross-sectional study at Chang Gung Memorial Hospital was approved by the hospital’s Institutional Review Board (IRB No: 104-7324B). We carried out the procedures of this study in accordance with the Helsinki declaration and the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee. Informed consent was obtained from all the participants in this study. All participants were paid 250 New Taiwan Dollars (approximately eight US dollars) for participating in this study.

Patients with a depressive disorder were recruited from the outpatient department of Kaohsiung Chang Gung Memorial Hospital and Kaohsiung Veterans General Hospital. The eligibility criteria for patients included the following: (a) diagnosis of a depressive disorder according to the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV-TR); (b) no known neurological or systemic diseases affecting cognitive performance; (c) age ≥ 18 years; and (d) able to speak and read Mandarin Chinese and provide informed consent. We recruited 75 patients with major depressive disorder (MDD), depressive disorder NOS or dysthymic disorder and interviewed them, while neuropsychological tests were performed in a euthymic state.

We previously recruited 220 healthy subjects to develop the norms of the Chinese BAC-A, and the finding had been published elsewhere (Lee et al., 2018a). In brief, the healthy subjects consisted of healthy individuals recruited from the employees of Kaohsiung Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Kaohsiung Veterans General Hospital, and Keelung Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, as well as from community volunteers in Kaohsiung City and Keelung City, Taiwan. We herein selected 75 healthy subjects who were age- and sex-matched to our depression patients for making neurocognitive comparisons.

Cognitive assessment

The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) was administered to evaluate the cognitive functions of all participants  (Keefe et al., 2004). The BACS consists of tests with high test–retest reliability for measuring the cognitive deficits in schizophrenic patients (Keefe et al., 2006). The BAC-A contains all six subtests of the BACS and adds the Affective Processing Test. The Chinese version of the BAC-A has also been adapted and established by the original authors. Our research team established the normative data of the Chinese version of the BACS (Wang et al., 2017), which provides satisfactory psychometric properties (Wang et al., 2016). Furthermore, a previous study suggested that the Chinese BAC-A may contribute to differentiating unipolar depressive disorder from bipolar disorder patients in clinical settings (Lee et al., 2018b).

BACS subtests

Traditional neurocognitive domains such as working memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, attention and processing speed, verbal memory, and executive function were assessed using the following six subtests: the Digit Sequencing Task, List Learning Test, Token Motor Task, Controlled Oral Word Association Test, Category Instances Test, and Symbol Coding and Tower of London Test, respectively. Each patient’s performance in the individual tests is compared with a healthy comparison group to calculate the T- or Z-score of that sum (Keefe et al., 2008). We applied and computed statistical analysis using T-scores based on Taiwanese norms (Wang et al., 2017).

Affective processing test

The Affective Processing Tests are applied in addition to the six BAC-A subtests, and include the Affective Interference Test (AIT), Emotion Inhibition Test (EIT), and Delayed Recognition. A list of 20 words is given to the subjects in the Affective Interference Test, with half of the words featuring emotional content (affective words like “intimate” and “killer”) and the other half being fruits or vegetables (non-affective words like “zucchini” and “apple”). Three learning trials were given to the subjects, and each trial required them to recall as many of these terms as possible. The subjects were asked to freely recall the non-affective words (fruits and vegetables) and then the “other words”. We calculated the analyses using the following four indexes: (a) total non-affective words; (b) total affective words; (c) cued non-affective words, and (d) cued affective words.

Recognition memory is tested after a delay of 15–20 min by presenting the initial 20 words (10 fruits and vegetables and 10 emotional) with 20 foil words that had not already been presented. The subjects were asked if certain non-affective and affective words were previously included in the word list. Four indexes were assessed to analyze the AIT-Delayed Recognition, including (a) number of correct non-affective words; (b) number of correct affective words; (c) non-affective false alarms, and (d) affective false alarms.

Sheets of papers containing four columns of words of either affective polarity or neutral in colored (red, blue, green, and yellow) or black ink were presented to the subjects in the EIT. The subjects are then instructed to either read the color of the words (color naming) or the words (word naming) going down the columns. They are given 30 s to read as many words as they can. The Emotion Inhibition index is identified by subtracting: (a) the number of correct responses to the neutral color words; (b) the number of correct responses to the color naming; (c) the number of correct responses to neutral words, and (d) the number of correct responses to the affective color words;

Psychopathological assessment

The diagnosis of depression was made/confirmed through face-to-face interview and chart review according to the criteria of DSM-IV-TR by psychiatrists. The clinical psychopathology of patients was evaluated using the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (17-item HAM-D). The 17-item HAM-D consists of 17 items and is a clinician-rated scale for depressive symptoms (Ramos-Brieva & Cordero-Villafafila, 1988). A higher total score indicates a greater severity of depressive symptoms (Zheng et al., 1988). Furthermore, we recorded the duration of illness and psychotropic drugs in use (antipsychotics, antidepressants, mood stabilizers, and benzodiazepines) during the interview and by reviewing the patients’ medical records.

Statistical analyses

The statistical software package SPSS (Version 21.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used to analyze the data. The variables were presented as either frequency (%) or mean (±SD). We categorized the depressive patients into “those without residual symptoms” and “those with residual symptoms” based on a HAMD score of 7. Residual symptoms are classified by those symptoms that remained after treatment in either group of remission (defined as those patients who scores ≤7 in HAMD17) (Hiranyatheb et al., 2016). We used the chi-square test to compare the categorical variables among participant groups and the variables between depressive patients and controls, patients with depressive disorder NOS or dysthymic disorder (non-MDD group) and major depressive disorder (MDD group), as well as patients with and without residual symptoms. We performed group comparisons using t-tests for continuous variables, and Mann–Whitney U test was applied for group comparison when the continuous variables violated normal distribution. The gender- and age- adjusted T-scores were set as dependent variables in each BAC-A subtest.

Multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was used for detecting the differences in BACA across groups (controlling for age, sex and education levels), with LSD post-hoc test. We also adopted multiple linear regression models to explore the effects of depression characteristics on the performance of each BAC-A subtest. The subtest-scores of the BAC-A were set as dependent variables, while we viewed depression characteristics (age, sex, education levels, HAMD-17 scores, duration of illness, antidepressant use and benzodiazepine use) as independent variables. Values were considered statistically significant if a two-tailed test <0.05. We used Bonferroni correction (0.05/19 = 0.0026) to adjust for multiple testing in BACA performance (a total of 19 indices).

Results

The performance in the BAC-A and characteristics of the 75 patients with depression (mean age: 45.4 years, 36.0% males) and 75 age- and sex-matched controls (mean age: 46.1 years, 36.0% males) are shown in Table 1. The control group had a greater level of education than the patient group (p = 0.025). Regarding BAC-A performance, patients with depression had significantly worse performance in all indices, except executive function, cued affective words of the AIT, and four indexes of the AIT-Delayed Recognition.

Table 1. Characteristics and cognitive function of patients with depressive disorders and healthy controls.

Depression (n = 75) Healthy controls (n = 75) Statistical value p-value
Gender, n (%) 0.000 1.000
Male 27 (36.0) 27 (36.0)
Female 48 (64.0) 48 (64.0)
Age (years) 45.4  ± 12.1 46.1  ± 11.2 −0.337 0.737
Years of education 13.6  ± 3.1 14.7  ± 2.7 −2.270 0.025
Age of onset (years) 36.9  ± 12.3
Duration of illness (years) 8.5  ± 8.0
Mood episode (times) 1.8  ± 1.7
HAMD-17 items total scores 7.1  ± 4.4
Antidepressant use, n (%) 56 (74.7)
Duration of use (months) 28.3  ± 37.9
Defined daily dose 1.1  ± 0.7
Antipsychotics use, n (%) 21 (28.0)
Duration of use (months) 28.0  ± 34.0
Defined daily dose 0.3  ± 0.3
Benzodiazepine use, n (%) 60 (80.0)
Duration of use (months) 31.0  ± 35.1
Defined daily dose 1.3  ± 1.2
Cognitive assessment (BACA)
BACS
Verbal memory 38.7  ± 10.7 50.7  ± 10.2 38.957 <0.001*
Working memory 44.2  ± 9.7 50.4  ± 11.9 6.771 0.010
Motor speed 42.5  ± 10.3 51.0  ± 11.5 18.047 <0.001*
Verbal fluency 42.0  ± 10.7 52.1  ± 9.3 27.204 <0.001*
Attention and processing speed 37.8  ± 13.5 50.7  ± 10.8 31.663 <0.001*
Executive function 44.8  ± 15.8 48.9  ± 10.5 2.170 0.143
BACS Composite score 35.6  ± 15.8 50.7  ± 10.7 36.716 <0.001*
Affective Processing Test
AIT: total affective words 41.6  ± 9.1 50.8  ± 10.4 24.865 <0.001*
AIT: total non-affective words 39.7  ± 13.4 50.3  ± 10.3 21.077 <0.001*
AIT: cued affective words 47.0  ± 10.1 50.6  ± 10.7 2.891 0.091
AIT: cued non-affective words 44.5  ± 9.8 50.4  ± 10.7 7.759 0.006
DR: correct affective words 51.5  ± 6.6 49.7  ± 10.7 2.256 0.135
DR: non-affective correct words 50.3  ± 6.2 49.8  ± 10.1 0.813 0.369
DR: affective false alarms 51.9  ± 9.5 50.7  ± 10.4 0.202 0.653
DR: non-affective false alarms 55.4  ± 13.8 50.3  ± 10.2 3.882 0.051
EIT: color naming score 45.0  ± 12.7 51.7  ± 10.0 7.224 0.008
EIT: neutral color word score 42.2  ± 13.8 51.0  ± 10.1 13.049 <0.001*
EIT: affective color word score 42.0  ± 12.4 51.0  ± 10.2 16.979 <0.001*
EIT: neutral word score 43.2  ± 12.5 49.7  ± 9.8 8.936 0.003

Notes.

Data are expressed as mean  ± SD or n (%). Bonferroni correction was used to adjust for multiple testing in the correlation matrix (p-value = 0.05/19 = 0.0026).

*

Significant correlation remains after Bonferroni correction.

We also compared the performance in the BAC-A and characteristics of the 56 patients in the MDD group (mean age: 46.6 years, 32.1% males) and 19 patients in the non-MDD group (mean age: 42.0 years, 47.4% males) (Table 2). After adjustment for education levels, no significant difference in any indexes of the BACA was found between the MDD group and non-MDD group.

Table 2. Characteristics and cognitive function of patients with major depressive disorder (MDD group) and patients with dysthymic disorder or depressive disorder NOS (non-MDD group).

MDD (n = 56) Non-MDD (n = 19) Statistical value p-value
Gender, n (%) 1.427 0.232
Male 18 (32.1) 9 (47.4)
Female 38 (67.9) 10 (52.6)
Age (years) 46.6  ± 12.5 42.0 ± 10.0 1.780 0.075
Years of education 13.0  ± 3.0 15.3  ± 2.8 2.668 0.008
Age of onset (years) 37.7 ± 12.4 34.5  ± 11.9 1.042 0.297
Duration of illness (years) 8.9  ± 7.8 7.5  ± 8.8 1.143 0.253
Mood episode (times) 2.0  ± 1.9 1.2  ± 0.9 2.608 0.009
HAMD-17 items total scores 7.4  ± 4.5 6.1  ± 4.0 0.984 0.325
Antidepressant use, n (%) 42 (75.0) 14 (73.7) 0.013 0.909
Duration of use (months) 30.5  ± 36.0 21.6  ± 44.0 1.629 0.103
Defined daily dose 1.2  ± 0.7 0.9  ± 0.5 1.816 0.069
Antipsychotics use, n (%) 18 (32.1) 3 (15.8) 1.882 0.170
Duration of use (months) 32.17  ± 35.0 2.7 ± 2.9 2.514 0.006
Defined daily dose 0.3  ± 0.4 0.1  ± 0.1 0.682 0.523
Benzodiazepine use, n (%) 47 (83.9) 13 (68.4) 2.132 0.144
Duration of use (months) 36.8 ± 37.5 9.9  ± 13.5 3.072 0.002
Defined daily dose 1.4  ± 1.3 0.9  ± 0.5 1.368 0.171
Cognitive assessment (BACA)
BACS
Verbal memory 37.8 ± 11.7 41.1  ± 7.2 0.037 0.848
Working memory 43.4  ± 9.3 46.6  ± 10.7 0.055 0.815
Motor speed 41.7  ± 11.2 44.9  ± 7.2 0.007 0.935
Verbal fluency 40.3  ± 10.3 46.8  ± 10.7 0.836 0.364
Attention and processing speed 35.8  ± 13.7 43.2  ± 11.5 1.250 0.268
Executive function 43.3  ± 17.5 48.8  ± 9.0 0.853 0.359
BACS Composite score 33.4  ± 16.9 41.7 ± 10.5 1.074 0.304
Affective Processing Test
AIT: total affective words 42.0  ± 9.9 40.6  ± 6.7 2.620 0.110
AIT: total non-affective words 37.4  ± 13.1 46.3 ± 12.2 2.069 0.155
AIT: cued affective words 46.7 ± 11.2 47.5  ± 6.2 0.467 0.497
AIT: cued non-affective words 43.5  ± 10.4 47.2  ± 7.5 0.029 0.866
DR: correct affective words 50.9  ± 7.2 53.4 ± 4.3 0.349 0.557
DR: non-affective correct words 49.2 ± 6.7 53.3  ± 3.1 2.351 0.130
DR: affective false alarms 52.7  ± 10.1 49.6  ± 7.3 0.402 0.528
DR: non-affective false alarms 57.1  ± 14.7 50.6  ± 9.5 1.140 0.290
EIT: color naming score 43.5  ± 12.7 49.1 ± 11.9 0.264 0.609
EIT: neutral color word score 40.9 ± 14.4 45.6  ± 11.6 0.006 0.939
EIT: affective color word score 41.3  ± 12.9 44.1  ± 11.0 0.048 0.827
EIT: neutral word score 43.3  ± 13.1 42.7 ± 11.0 0.793 0.376

Notes.

Data are expressed as mean  ± SD or n (%).

MDD
major depressive disorder

Bonferroni correction was used to adjust for multiple testing in the correlation matrix (p-value = 0.05/19 = 0.0026).

We further compared the performance in the BAC-A of the 31 patients with residual symptoms (mean age: 46.1 years, 41.9% males), 44 patients without residual symptoms (mean age: 45.0 years, 31.8% males) and healthy controls (Table 3). We found that patients with residual symptoms had worst performance in working memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, attention, executive function, composite BACS score, three indexes of the AIT and all four indexes of the EIT. Additionally, patients without residual symptoms exhibited intermediate performance (between those with residual symptoms and healthy controls) in motor speed, verbal fluency, attention, composite BACS score, total non-affective words of the AIT and affective color word scores of the EIT.

Table 3. Characteristics and cognitive function of depression patients with residual symptoms, patients without residual symptoms and healthy controls.

Residual (A) (n = 31) No-residual (B) (n = 44) Healthy controls (C) (n = 75) χ2 or F p-value Post-hoc test
Gender, n (%) 0.808 0.668
Male 13 (41.9) 14 (31.8) 27 (36.0)
Female 18 (58.1) 30 (68.2) 48 (64.0)
Age (years) 46.1  ± 12.4 45.0  ± 12.0 46.1  ± 11.2 0.136 0.873
Years of education 13.0  ± 3.3 14.1  ± 2.9 14.7  ± 2.7 3.860 0.023 C >A
Cognitive assessment (BACA)
BACS
Verbal memory 36.6  ± 10.3 40.1  ± 10.9 50.7  ± 10.2 20.059 <0.001* C >A, C >B
Working memory 39.8  ± 9.6 47.1  ± 8.8 50.4  ± 11.9 6.418 0.002* C >A, B >A
Motor speed 37.9  ± 9.7 45.6  ± 9.7 51.0  ± 11.5 13.335 <0.001* C >B >A
Verbal fluency 37.9  ± 12.4 44.7  ± 8.6 52.1  ± 9.3 16.907 <0.001* C >B >A
Attention and processing speed 32.2  ± 11.8 41.5  ± 13.4 50.7  ± 10.8 20.656 <0.001* C >B >A
Executive function 40.1  ± 21.6 47.9  ± 9.5 48.9  ± 10.5 3.614 0.030 C >A, B >A
BACS Composite score 28.2  ± 17.7 40.4  ± 12.4 50.7  ± 10.7 26.133 <0.001* C >B >A
Affective Processing Test
AIT: total affective words 40.1  ± 8.7 42.6  ± 9.4 50.8  ± 10.4 12.702 <0.001* C >A, C >B
AIT: total non-affective words 34.2  ± 11.1 43.4  ± 13.6 50.3  ± 10.3 15.229 <0.001* C >B >A
AIT: cued affective words 42.6  ± 8.9 49.8  ± 10.0 50.6  ± 10.7 5.018 0.008 C >A, B >A
AIT: cued non-affective words 40.2  ± 9.6 47.3  ± 9.0 50.4  ± 10.7 7.360 0.001* C >A, B >A
DR: correct affective words 48.9  ± 8.1 53.3  ± 4.8 49.7  ± 10.7 2.931 0.057
DR: non-affective correct words 49.6  ± 5.8 50.7  ± 6.4 49.8  ± 10.1 .430 0.652
DR: affective false alarms 54.7  ± 10.2 50.0  ± 8.7 50.7  ± 10.4 1.736 0.180
DR: non-affective false alarms 55.4  ± 11.0 55.3  ± 15.5 50.3  ± 10.2 1.970 0.143
EIT: color naming score 39.6  ± 10.6 48.6  ± 12.8 51.7  ± 10.0 8.208 <0.001* C >A, B >A
EIT: neutral color word score 36.5  ± 10.9 45.9  ± 14.3 51.0  ± 10.1 11.164 <0.001* C >A, B >A
EIT: affective color word score 37.0  ± 9.4 45.3  ± 13.2 51.0  ± 10.2 12.746 <0.001* C >B >A
EIT: neutral word score 38.9  ± 10.3 46.0  ± 13.2 49.7  ± 9.8 7.483 0.001* C >A, B >A

Notes.

Data are expressed as mean  ± SD or n (%). Statistical Value are estimated using MANCOVA (controlling for age, sex and education levels).

Residual
the HAMD scores >7
No-residual
HAMD scores ≤7
A
patients with residual symptoms
B
patients without residual symptoms
C
healthy controls

Bonferroni correction was used to adjust for multiple testing in the correlation matrix (p-value = 0.05/19 = 0.0026).

*

Significant correlation remains after Bonferroni correction.

We further used multiple linear regression to explore the effects of depression characteristics on the performance of each BAC-A index (Table 4). After controlling for patients’ age, sex and educational levels, we found that the past MDD diagnoses were not significantly associated with worse cognitive function than non-MDD patients. The HAMD-17 scores were negatively correlated to working memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, attention and processing speed, executive function, the BACS composite score, cued affective words and cued non-affective words of the AIT, and all four indexes of the EIT. Furthermore, benzodiazepine use was associated with worse performance of verbal memory; antidepressant use was correlated with better performance of motor speed; and a longer illness duration was associated with worse performance of total non-affective words of the AIT and three indexes of the EIT.

Table 4. Effects of illness characteristics and psychotropic drugs on the performance of each index of the BACA.

BACA index Associated factors B (95% CI) Std. error t p-value Observed power
Verbal memory Benzodiazepine use −9.82 (−17.00, −2.63) 3.589 −2.735 0.008 0.767
Working memory HAMD-17 scores −0.77 (−1.38, −0.17) 0.303 −2.554 0.013 0.709
Motor speed HAMD-17 scores −1.24 (−1.89, −0.59) 0.326 −3.804 <0.001 0.963
Antidepressant use 8.02 (2.01, 14.04) 3.004 2.670 0.010 0.747
Verbal fluency HAMD-17 scores −0.82 (−1.47, −0.16) 0.327 −2.495 0.015 0.689
Attention and processing speed HAMD-17 scores −1.24 (−2.10, −0.38) 0.429 −2.887 0.005 0.810
Executive function HAMD-17 scores −1.19 (−2.31, −0.07) 0.560 −2.119 0.038 0.549
BACS Composite score HAMD-17 scores −1.86 (−2.83, −0.89) 0.484 −3.842 <0.001 0.965
AIT: total affective words NS
AIT: total non-affective words Duration of illness −0.53 (−0.92, −0.15) 0.193 −2.762 0.008 0.775
AIT: cued affective words HAMD-17 scores −0.83 (−1.49, −0.18) 0.325 −2.565 0.013 0.713
AIT: cued non-affective words HAMD-17 scores −0.83 (−1.41, −0.26) 0.287 −2.908 0.005 0.816
DR: correct affective words NS
DR: non-affective correct words NS
DR: affective false alarms NS
DR: non-affective false alarms NS
EIT: color naming score HAMD-17 scores −1.16 (−1.92, −0.39) 0.381 −3.035 0.004 0.847
Duration of illness −0.45 (−0.84, −0.06) 0.195 −2.328 0.023 0.629
EIT: neutral color word score HAMD-17 scores −1.34 (−2.17, −0.52) 0.413 −3.250 0.002 0.892
EIT: affective color word score Duration of illness −0.37 (−0.73, −0.01) 0.181 −2.045 0.045 0.520
HAMD-17 scores −1.13 (−1.90, −0.36) 0.383 −2.946 0.005 0.826
EIT: neutral word score Duration of illness −0.51 (−0.90, −0.12) 0.194 −2.645 0.011 0.739
HAMD-17 scores −1.36 (−2.18, −0.54) 0.410 −3.323 0.002 0.904

Notes.

All models were controlled for age, sex and education levels. Only the factors with significant effect were shown in the Table.

AIT
Affective Interference Test
DR
Affective Interference Test: Delayed Recognition
EIT
the Emotion Inhibition Test
MDD
major depressive disorder
NS
none of the factors showed significant effect

Discussion

In our study, depressive patients showed impairments in many domains of BACA, including verbal memory, working memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, attention and speed of information processing. The magnitude of cognitive dysfunction was compatible to which reported in a previous study (using BACS to assess cognitive function among MDD patients, Fig. 1A) (Terachi et al., 2017). We also compared our results with previous studies with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia patients conducting BACS tests (Fig. 1B). Similar degrees of cognitive impairments were observed in patients with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia patients, while patients with MDD may exhibited smaller impairments in several neurocognitive domains compared to patients with either of the other two disorders (Terachi et al., 2017).

Figure 1. Comparisons of cognitive performance of our patients with depressive disorders with previous studies using BACS as an assessment tool.

Figure 1

(A) Comparison of performance of each BACS subtest in patients with depressive disorder (Compared to Terachi et al. (2017)). (B) Comparison of BACS composite score in patients with depressive disorder, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia (Compared to Terachi et al. (2017) and Keefe et al. (2014)).

In this study, we aimed to define the cognitive profiles of patients with depressive disorders using the BAC-A and found that HAMD-17 scores were positively correlated with cognitive dysfunction in motor speed, verbal fluency, attention and processing speed, and executive function domains measured using the BAC-A. A previous study suggested that cognitive dysfunction may remain in the remitted state of depression, which gives rise to whether the severity of depression is the only component affecting cognitive function. In line with our findings, another study indicated that cognitive performance was significantly associated with depression severity. Notably, we found no differences in cognitive profiles in the MDD group and non-MDD group after adjusting for potential confounding factors. This result implies that the current residual depressive symptom severity, not the diagnosis categories, was most closely related to cognitive deficits among patients with depressive disorders.

Symptoms of depressive disorder, such as lack of interest, impaired processing initiative, lack of motivation, and attention deficit, impact the performance of memory function (Marazziti et al., 2010; Ellis, 1990) and are closely related to the depression course regarding current mood states (Bartfai et al., 1991; MacQueen et al., 2002). In a previous study, perceived cognitive performance and residual depressive symptoms were found to be significantly correlated to psychosocial functioning in patients with affective disorder (Samalin et al., 2017). Our studies further identified that residual depressive symptoms was a significant indicator for cognitive impairments in many domains of the BACA. Affective performance biases such as problems with pessimistic attributional style, automatic thoughts, levels of neuroticism and attentional control problems in MDD patients during remission had been discussed before (Cerny et al., 2019). As for delayed recognition, it is worth noting that unipolar depression patients committed more non-affective false alarms during delayed recognition compared to patients with bipolar disorder and healthy controls (Lee et al., 2018b). This result may be due to the fact that depressed patients tend to process negative information preferably  (Siegle & Hasselmo, 2002).

With regard to the relationship between psychotropic drugs and cognitive function, we found that benzodiazepine use was associated with worse verbal memory performance. Previous studies have offered support indicating consistent impairment in multiple cognitive domains, including verbal memory, in long-term benzodiazepine users regarding psychiatric illness and in people under withdrawal and those who had abstained. However, we did not discuss the duration of benzodiazepine usage or withdrawal status in this study. Although the exact mechanism of how benzodiazepine affects cognitive function has remained unclear, the alternation of the α1-GABAA receptor subtype by benzodiazepine may play a role in cognitive function performance (Makaron et al., 2013).

We found that antidepressant use was associated with better performance of motor speed. In line with our study’s finding, a recent meta-analysis also revealed a positive effect of antidepressants on delayed recall and psychomotor speed (Rosenblat, Kakar & McIntyre, 2015). One previous study suggested that verbal learning and memory were particularly impaired in patients with depression and were sensitive to the effects of certain selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) (Schmitt, Kruizinga & Riedel, 2001). As Schmitt reported, delayed recall in word learning had been significantly impaired by paroxetine at the doses of 20 mg and 40 mg, while sertraline has been reported to impact word learning but improve verbal fluency. The enhancement of monoamines (dopamine, norepinephrine and serotonin) by antidepressants may be related to better cognitive function (Trivedi et al., 2008). However, our studies demonstrated that neither antidepressant nor antipsychotics usage was correlated with verbal memory or working memory function in depressed patients. A randomized longitudinal study recently demonstrated that no significant improvement was shown in several cognitive domains in depressed patients following medication treatment (Shilyansky et al., 2016). Therefore, a longitudinal follow-up is needed to clarify whether antidepressant treatment actually improves the performance of motor speed among depressive patients.

We have found that duration of illness is significantly correlated to total non-affective words and three subtest of the Emotion Inhibition Test. Which indicate that valence of past period might play roles by which depressed patients might have difficulty processing positive stimuli (Gotlib et al., 2011). Verbal memory is found to be correlated to benzodiazepine use and education level in our study. The result is similar to previous research conducted by Bas and Poyraz (Bas et al., 2015) that indicate low functioning bipolar patients perform significantly worse in verbal memory in terms that low functioning patients tends to receive lower level of education and greater amount of medication for their symptoms. However, the high risk of developing major depressive disorder in low education group had been identified in some previous studies indicating that environmental or genetic factors may play roles (Lorant et al., 2003; Peyrot et al., 2015) which is compatible to our data indicating non-MDD group tends to receive longer period of education.

This study has several limitations that should be noted. First, the sample size of this study was small, particularly the non-MDD group. Therefore, negative findings in MDD-related cognitive decline may be due to the small sample size. Besides, homogeneity of variance between the MDD and non-MDD groups was lack due to limited sample size. Second, the age and gender were not perfectly matched between the MDD group and the non-MDD group, and the case numbers were unequal. Differences in educational level and psychotropic drugs in use across groups also played confounding factors in the investigation for cognitive performance. Third, several crucial factors potentially associated with cognitive function (e.g., premorbid function, smoking, alcohol use, comorbidities and treatment response to medication) were not addressed in this study. Additionally, we did not collected the information about socioeconomic status, average/quality of sleep prior to testing, use of coffee or nicotine during the day of testing and immediately prior to testing. Future studies with larger sample sizes and comprehensive psychopathological assessments are necessary to understand the influence that these aforementioned factors may have on between-group neurocognitive differences.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we found that the current severity of depressive symptoms and psychotropic in use, not the diagnosis category, were associated with cognitive impairments in patients with depressive disorders. Clinicians should make sure to manage residual depressive symptoms and prescribe adequate psychotropic drugs in order to eliminate the cognitive deficits of depressive patients. BACA is a reliable instrument to detect cognitive deficits in depressive patients even with minimal residual symptoms. However, future studies with adequate statistic power and comprehensive psychopathological assessments are necessary to understand the neurocognition-related factors among depressive patients.

Supplemental Information

Table S1. Psychotropic drugs in use and mean daily dose among patients with depressive disorders.
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.7432/supp-1
Dataset S1. Raw data of BACA performance in patients with depressive disorders.
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.7432/supp-2

Acknowledgments

The authors express their deepest gratitude to YF Lo and SM Wang for helping participant recruitment, and thank all of the individuals who participated in this study.

Funding Statement

This study was funded by Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taiwan (CMRPG8C1051, CMRPG8C1291 and CMRPG8E1351). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Additional Information and Declarations

Competing Interests

The authors declare there are no competing interests.

Author Contributions

Ruei-An Chen analyzed the data, prepared figures and/or tables, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper.

Chun-Yi Lee, Chi-Fa Hung and Yu-Chi Huang performed the experiments.

Yu Lee conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments.

Pao-Yen Lin analyzed the data.

Sheng-Yu Lee conceived and designed the experiments, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools.

Liang-Jen Wang conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, prepared figures and/or tables, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, approved the final draft.

Human Ethics

The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):

This cross-sectional study at Chang Gung Memorial Hospital was approved by the hospital’s Institutional Review Board (104-7324B).

Data Availability

The following information was supplied regarding data availability:

The raw measurements are available as Supplemental File.

References

  • Afridi et al. (2011).Afridi MI, Hina M, Qureshi IS, Hussain M. Cognitive disturbance comparison among drug-naive depressed cases and healthy controls. Journal of the College of Physicians and Surgeons–Pakistan. 2011;21(6):351–355. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bartfai et al. (1991).Bartfai A, Asberg M, Martensson B, Gustavsson P. Memory effects of clomipramine treatment: relationship to CSF monoamine metabolites and drug concentrations in plasma. Biological Psychiatry. 1991;30(11):1075–1092. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(91)90179-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bas et al. (2015).Bas TO, Poyraz CA, Bas A, Poyraz BC, Tosun M. The impact of cognitive impairment, neurological soft signs and subdepressive symptoms on functional outcome in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2015;174:336–341. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bauer et al. (2015).Bauer IE, Keefe RS, Sanches M, Suchting R, Green CE, Soares JC. Evaluation of cognitive function in bipolar disorder using the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Affective Disorders (BAC-A) Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2015;60:81–86. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bora et al. (2013).Bora E, Harrison BJ, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive impairment in euthymic major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2013;43(10):2017–2026. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bortolato, Carvalho & McIntyre (2014).Bortolato B, Carvalho AF, McIntyre RS. Cognitive dysfunction in major depressive disorder: a state-of-the-art clinical review. CNS & Neurological Disorders-Drug Targets. 2014;13(10):1804–1818. doi: 10.2174/1871527313666141130203823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Burt, Zembar & Niederehe (1995).Burt DB, Zembar MJ, Niederehe G. Depression and memory impairment: a meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117(2):285–305. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Butters et al. (2004).Butters MA, Whyte EM, Nebes RD, Begley AE, Dew MA, Mulsant BH, Zmuda MD, Bhalla R, Meltzer CC, Pollock BG, Reynolds 3rd CR, Becker JT. The nature and determinants of neuropsychological functioning in late-life depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61(6):587–595. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.6.587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cerny et al. (2019).Cerny BM, Stange JP, Kling LR, Hamlat EJ, O’Donnell LA, Deveney C, Langenecker SA. Self-reported affective biases, but not all affective performance biases, are present in depression remission. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2019 doi: 10.1111/bjc.12217. Epub ahead of print Mar 10 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ellis (1990).Ellis HC. Depressive deficits in memory: processing initiative and resource allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1990;119(1):60–62. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.119.1.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ferrari et al. (2013).Ferrari AJ, Charlson FJ, Norman RE, Patten SB, Freedman G, Murray CJ, Vos T, Whiteford HA. Burden of depressive disorders by country, sex, age, and year: findings from the global burden of disease study 2010. PLOS Medicine. 2013;10(11):e1001547. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gotlib et al. (2011).Gotlib IH, Jonides J, Buschkuehl M, Joormann J. Memory for affectively valenced and neutral stimuli in depression: evidence from a novel matching task. Cognition & Emotion. 2011;25(7):1246–1254. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.538374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hiranyatheb et al. (2016).Hiranyatheb T, Nakawiro D, Wongpakaran T, Wongpakaran N, Bookkamana P, Pinyopornpanish M, Saisavoey N, Wannarit K, Satthapisit S, Tanchakvaranont S. The impact of residual symptoms on relapse and quality of life among Thai depressive patients. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment. 2016;12:3175–3181. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S124277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Jaeger et al. (2006).Jaeger J, Berns S, Uzelac S, Davis-Conway S. Neurocognitive deficits and disability in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research. 2006;145(1):39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Keefe et al. (2014).Keefe RS, Fox KH, Davis VG, Kennel C, Walker TM, Burdick KE, Harvey PD. The brief assessment of cognition in affective disorders (BAC-A): performance of patients with bipolar depression and healthy controls. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2014;166:86–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Keefe et al. (2004).Keefe RS, Goldberg TE, Harvey PD, Gold JM, Poe MP, Coughenour L. The brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophrenia Research. 2004;68(2–3):283–297. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Keefe et al. (2008).Keefe RS, Harvey PD, Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Walker TM, Kennel C, Hawkins K. Norms and standardization of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) Schizophrenia Research. 2008;102(1–3):108–115. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Keefe et al. (2006).Keefe RS, Poe M, Walker TM, Kang JW, Harvey PD. The schizophrenia cognition rating scale: an interview-based assessment and its relationship to cognition, real-world functioning, and functional capacity. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163(3):426–432. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lee et al. (2012).Lee RS, Hermens DF, Porter MA, Redoblado-Hodge MA. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2012;140(2):113–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.10.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lee et al. (2018a).Lee CY, Lee SY, Huang YC, Hung CF, Lee Y, Lee MI, Wang LJ. The Chinese version of the Brief assessment of cognition in affective disorders: normative data of a Mandarin-speaking population. Clin Neuropsychol. 2018a;32(supp 1):1–14. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2017.1400108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lee et al. (2018b).Lee CY, Wang LJ, Lee Y, Hung CF, Huang YC, Lee MI, Lee SY. Differentiating bipolar disorders from unipolar depression by applying the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Affective Disorders. Psychological Medicine. 2018b;48(6):929–938. doi: 10.1017/S003329171700229X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lorant et al. (2003).Lorant V, Deliege D, Eaton W, Robert A, Philippot P, Ansseau M. Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2003;157(2):98–112. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • MacQueen et al. (2002).MacQueen GM, Galway TM, Hay J, Young LT, Joffe RT. Recollection memory deficits in patients with major depressive disorder predicted by past depressions but not current mood state or treatment status. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32(2):251–258. doi: 10.1017/S0033291701004834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Makaron et al. (2013).Makaron L, Moran CA, Namjoshi O, Rallapalli S, Cook JM, Rowlett JK. Cognition-impairing effects of benzodiazepine-type drugs: role of GABAA receptor subtypes in an executive function task in rhesus monkeys. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior. 2013;104:62–68. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2012.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Marazziti et al. (2010).Marazziti D, Consoli G, Picchetti M, Carlini M, Faravelli L. Cognitive impairment in major depression. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2010;626(1):83–86. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.08.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Naismith et al. (2007).Naismith SL, Longley WA, Scott EM, Hickie IB. Disability in major depression related to self-rated and objectively-measured cognitive deficits: a preliminary study. BMC Psychiatry. 2007;7:32. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-7-32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Peyrot et al. (2015).Peyrot WJ, Lee SH, Milaneschi Y, Abdellaoui A, Byrne EM, Esko T, De Geus EJ, Hemani G, Hottenga JJ, Kloiber S, Levinson DF, Lucae S, Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric GWAS Consortium (Corporate Collaborator) Martin NG, Medland SE, Metspalu A, Milani L, Noethen MM, Potash JB, Rietschel M, Rietveld CA, Ripke S, Shi J, Social Science Genetic Association Consortium Corporate Collaborator. Willemsen G, Zhu Z, Boomsma DI, Wray NR, Penninx BW, Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric GWAS Consortium Corporate Collaborator; Social Science Genetic Association Consortium Corporate Collaborator The association between lower educational attainment and depression owing to shared genetic effects? Results in ∼25,000 subjects. Molecular Psychiatry. 2015;20(6):735–743. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ramos-Brieva & Cordero-Villafafila (1988).Ramos-Brieva JA, Cordero-Villafafila A. A new validation of the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1988;22(1):21–28. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(88)90024-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rosenblat, Kakar & McIntyre (2015).Rosenblat JD, Kakar R, McIntyre RS. The cognitive effects of antidepressants in major depressive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;19(2):pyv082. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyv082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Samalin et al. (2017).Samalin L, Boyer L, Murru A, Pacchiarotti I, Reinares M, Bonnin CM, Torrent C, Verdolini N, Pancheri C, De Chazeron I, Boucekine M, Geoffroy PA, Bellivier F, Llorca PM, Vieta E. Residual depressive symptoms, sleep disturbance and perceived cognitive impairment as determinants of functioning in patients with bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2017;210:280–286. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schmitt, Kruizinga & Riedel (2001).Schmitt JA, Kruizinga MJ, Riedel WJ. Non-serotonergic pharmacological profiles and associated cognitive effects of serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2001;15(3):173–179. doi: 10.1177/026988110101500304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Shilyansky et al. (2016).Shilyansky C, Williams LM, Gyurak A, Harris A, Usherwood T, Etkin A. Effect of antidepressant treatment on cognitive impairments associated with depression: a randomised longitudinal study. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(5):425–435. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00012-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Siegle & Hasselmo (2002).Siegle GJ, Hasselmo ME. Using connectionist models to guide assessment of psychological disorder. Psychological Assessment. 2002;14(3):263–278. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.14.3.263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Terachi et al. (2017).Terachi S, Yamada T, Pu S, Yokoyama K, Matsumura H, Kaneko K. Comparison of neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia in later life: a cross-sectional study of euthymic or remitted, non-demented patients using the Japanese version of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS-J) Psychiatry Research. 2017;254:205–210. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.04.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Trivedi et al. (2008).Trivedi MH, Hollander E, Nutt D, Blier P. Clinical evidence and potential neurobiological underpinnings of unresolved symptoms of depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2008;69(2):246–258. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n0211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wang et al. (2017).Wang LJ, Huang YC, Hung CF, Chen CK, Chen YC, Lee PY, Wang SM, Liu MH, Lin CJ, Hsu ST. The Chinese version of the brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: data of a large-scale mandarin-speaking population. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: the Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists. 2017;32(3):289–296. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acw100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wang et al. (2016).Wang LJ, Lin PY, Lee Y, Huang YC, Hsu ST, Hung CF, Chen CK, Chen YC, Wang YL, Tsai MC. Validation of the Chinese version of Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment. 2016;12:2819–2826. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S118110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zheng et al. (1988).Zheng YP, Zhao JP, Phillips M, Liu JB, Cai MF, Sun SQ, Huang MF. Validity and reliability of the Chinese Hamilton Depression Rating Scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry: the Journal of Mental Science. 1988;152:660–664. doi: 10.1192/bjp.152.5.660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Table S1. Psychotropic drugs in use and mean daily dose among patients with depressive disorders.
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.7432/supp-1
Dataset S1. Raw data of BACA performance in patients with depressive disorders.
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.7432/supp-2

Data Availability Statement

The following information was supplied regarding data availability:

The raw measurements are available as Supplemental File.


Articles from PeerJ are provided here courtesy of PeerJ, Inc

RESOURCES