Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2015 Jan 5;78(5):561–572. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22360

To pair or not to pair: Sources of social variability with white‐faced saki monkeys (Pithecia pithecia) as a case study

Cynthia L Thompson 1,
PMCID: PMC6680232  PMID: 25561183

ABSTRACT

Intraspecific variability in social systems is gaining increased recognition in primatology. Many primate species display variability in pair‐living social organizations through incorporating extra adults into the group. While numerous models exist to explain primate pair‐living, our tools to assess how and why variation in this trait occurs are currently limited. Here I outline an approach which: (i) utilizes conceptual models to identify the selective forces driving pair‐living; (ii) outlines novel possible causes for variability in social organization; and (iii) conducts a holistic species‐level analysis of social behavior to determine the factors contributing to variation in pair‐living. A case study on white‐faced sakis (Pithecia pithecia) is used to exemplify this approach. This species lives in either male‐female pairs or groups incorporating “extra” adult males and/or females. Various conceptual models of pair‐living suggest that high same‐sex aggression toward extra‐group individuals is a key component of the white‐faced saki social system. Variable pair‐living in white‐faced sakis likely represents alternative strategies to achieve competency in this competition, in which animals experience conflicting selection pressures between achieving successful group defense and maintaining sole reproductive access to mates. Additionally, independent decisions by individuals may generate social variation by preventing other animals from adopting a social organization that maximizes fitness. White‐faced saki inter‐individual relationships and demographic patterns also lend conciliatory support to this conclusion. By utilizing both model‐level and species‐level approaches, with a consideration for potential sources of variation, researchers can gain insight into the factors generating variation in pair‐living social organizations. Am. J. Primatol. 78:561–572, 2016. © 2014 The Authors, American Journal of Primatology. published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Keywords: social variation, intraspecific variation, monogamy, socioecological models, social system

INTRODUCTION

Sociality is a pervasive feature of anthropoid primates. Primates are considered to have a relatively high number of pair‐living species, with reports of 10–29% of primate species displaying this pattern, compared to only 3–9% of all mammals [Fuentes, 1998; Lukas & Clutton‐Brock, 2013; van Schaik & Dunbar, 1990]. However, these estimates do not always take into account species that may sometimes live in pairs, but also display variation in grouping patterns. Although pair‐living has long been an area of intense investigation, recent and increasing recognition has been given to the plasticity primates can display in social organization [Chapman & Rothman, 2009; Reichard, 2009; Setchell, 2013; Strier, 2009, 2003; Struhsaker, 2000]. For pair‐living primates, plasticity in social organization entails incorporating extra adult males and/or females into the group, which could lead to greater potential for promiscuous matings, within‐group competition, or cooperative behavior [e.g., Digby, 1995; Savini et al., 2009]. While many models for the evolution of pair‐living and monogamy exist [reviewed in Brotherton & Komers, 2003; Fuentes, 2002; van Schaik & Dunbar, 1990; van Schaik & Kappeler, 2003], the conceptual framework for understanding how and when variations occur (and what factors constrain this variation) is currently lacking. Additionally, many discussions of intraspecific social variation have focused on differing demographic structures in larger groups [Chapman & Rothman, 2009; Struhsaker, 2000, 2008; Struhsaker et al., 2004; Yamagiwa & Hill, 1998], although variation in the grouping patterns of uni‐ and bi‐male harems has also been well documented [Pope, 1990; Robbins, 1995; Watts, 2000; Yamagiwa et al., 2003]. Socioecological models for large multi‐male, multi‐female groups are usually based on competitive interactions between individuals of the same sex [e.g., Isbell, 2004; Sterck et al., 1997; Wrangham, 1980; van Schaik, 1989 but see Sussman & Garber, 2011 for a discussion of the advantages of living in a larger group]. These models are difficult to apply to small groups with only 1–3 members of each sex and their accuracy has also come under scrutiny due to their lack of fit to currently available data [Koenig & Borries, 2009; Lawler, 2010; Sussman et al., 2011; Thierry, 2008]. Understanding social variation is a critical area to improve upon since many ‘pair‐living’ species have been reported to incorporate extra adults into the group [galagos (Galago, Galagoides): Harcourt & Nash, 1986; Müller & Thalmann, 2000; lemurs (Eulemur, Varecia, Hapalemur, Propithecus): Jolly, 1998; Kappeler, 1997, 2000; tarsiers (Tarsius): Gursky, 2000; MacKinnon & MacKinnon, 1980; owl monkeys (Aotus): Aquino et al., 1990; white‐faced sakis (Pithecia): Norconk, 2011; callitrichines (Saguinus, Leontopithecus): Goldizen, 2003; Digby et al., 2011; pig‐tailed langurs (Simias): Tenaza & Fuentes, 1995; gibbons and siamangs (Hylobates, Nomascus, Symphalangus): Fan & Jiang, 2010; Fan et al., 2010; Fuentes, 2000; Lappan, 2007; Reichard, 2003, 2009]. While primates will inevitably have some stochastic variation in the size and composition of groups, many of the above species have demonstrated consistent tendencies to form small multi‐adult groups. Despite this social variation, conceptual models can still provide valuable insight into pair‐living social systems by identifying the relative importance of selective pressures acting on a species.

To better assess the appearance of consistent variability in pair‐living behavior, I advocate an approach that: (i) utilizes conceptual models to identify the selective forces driving pair‐living; (ii) acknowledges possible causes for variability in social organization in light of said selective forces; and (iii) considers a holistic species‐level analysis of social behavior. These steps will allow researchers to make testable predictions and draw conclusions about how and when animals adopt pair‐living versus multi‐adult scenarios. Here I use the social system of white‐faced saki monkeys (Pithecia pithecia) as a case study to demonstrate the utility of this approach.

Describing Social Systems

The complexity of primate social behavior has often been parceled down by researchers into smaller, discrete components that can be easily analyzed. These components of sociality can be helpful in identifying interconnected themes at the species level. A variety of terminologies have been applied for these variables [Eisenberg et al., 1972; Müller & Thalmann, 2000; van Schaik & van Hooff, 1983; Whitehead & Dufault, 1999]. Here, I adopt the nomenclature from Kappeler & van Schaik [2002] to characterize social systems:

Social organization

A group's number of individuals, sex composition, and spatiotemporal relationships. This category describes whether individuals are gregarious, and if so, which sex(es) permanently associate with one another. Kappeler & van Schaik [2002] outlined three fundamental types of social organization: solitary, pair‐living, and group‐living.

Mating system

The number of mating males and females in a group and the sexual relationships present (e.g., monogamous, polygamous). This component can be dissected into two discrete, but related, traits: (i) the behavioral mating system (observed copulations, sexual behavior); and (ii) the genetic mating system (which animals actually produce offspring). While the genetic mating system will be more informative as to patterns of reproductive success and evolution, reports on primate behavioral mating systems are more widespread in the literature. Knowledge of both components is ideal for characterizing a species' mating system. Studies assessing both components are currently small in number, yet increasing [Di Fiore, 2009; Huck et al., 2014].

Social structure

The character of social relationships between conspecifics, including inter‐ and intrasexual relationships within and between groups. This aspect describes which individuals exhibit affiliative and agonistic social interactions.

Parceling these separate components into discrete entities has an obvious utility for describing and comparing primate species. However, it is important to note that these components are highly interrelated. For instance, mating system often roughly corresponds to social organization [Kappeler & van Schaik, 2002] and female‐female bonded social structures are not possible in social organizations lacking multiple females. Yet all components are needed to provide a complete picture of a species' social system. As an example, pair‐living groups can display varying social structures, such as strong reciprocal pair‐bonds, asymmetrical bonds maintained solely by one member, or infrequent social interactions between pair members [Fuentes, 1998, 2002; Mock & Fujioka, 1990]. In these cases, knowing the selective forces leading to social organization alone will tell us little about the social structure dominating these groups. Group history can also provide vital insight into variation in social systems [e.g., Bartlett, 2003]. For instance, incorporating multiple adults into the pair via retained offspring versus immigration of unrelated individuals will lead to similar social organizations, but will likely yield quite disparate social structures and mating systems. Intraspecific variation can further complicate characterization of these components, as species can display multiple social organizations, mating systems, or social structures. Using the case study of variable pair‐living in white‐faced saki monkeys, I aim to demonstrate that considering these three components holistically can provide insight into the factors generating intraspecific social variability.

The Evolution of Pair‐Living

The evolution of pair‐living in primates benefits from a strong and well developed background. Since these models have been reviewed extensively elsewhere [Brotherton & Komers, 2003; Fuentes, 2002; van Schaik & Dunbar, 1990; van Schaik & Kappeler, 2003] I only briefly outline them here: (i) Widely dispersed females [Rutberg, 1983; van Schaik & van Hooff, 1983; Wrangham, 1980]. Females are distributed such that males can only monopolize one at a time and males gain more reproductive success through associating with one female than ranging widely and mating promiscuously; (ii) Obligate paternal care [Kleiman, 1977; Wittenberger & Tilson, 1980]. Male assistance is required to successfully rear offspring, leading males to remain with one female to assure paternity of reared offspring; (iii) Male defense against predators or defense against resource competition [van Schaik & Dunbar, 1990; Wittenberger & Tilson, 1980]. Females choose to associate with males that provide protection from predation and/or access to greater amounts of food (i.e., allow females to avoid the costs of resource competition); (iv) Mate guarding [Brotherton & Manser, 1997; Palombit, 1999, 1996]. Males maintain pair‐bonds with females to ensure they sire offspring and prevent copulations with competing males; (v) Bodyguard hypothesis [Emlen & Wrege, 1986; Mesnick, 1997; Smuts & Smuts, 1993]. Females choose to pair with males that provide protection from harassment and coercion by conspecific males; (vi) Infanticide prevention hypothesis [Palombit, 2000; van Schaik, 2000; van Schaik & Dunbar, 1990; van Schaik & Kappeler, 1997]. Females maintain pair‐bonds with males to acquire protection from infanticidal males.

Analyses have found conflicting support for these hypotheses as selective forces leading to primate and mammalian pair‐living. There is consistent evidence supporting paternal care as a pressure that maintains, but did not initially generate, pair‐living [Komers et al., 1997; Lukas & Clutton‐Brock, 2013; Opie et al., 2013a]. However, the roles of the infanticide prevention and widely dispersed female hypotheses have been under intense dispute. Several studies have supported infanticide as the main selective pressure leading to pair‐living in primates [Opie et al., 2013a,2013b, 2014; van Schaik & Dunbar, 1990; van Schaik & Kappeler, 2003], but others have failed to support it [Fuentes, 2002; Lukas & Clutton‐Brock, 2014], instead favoring the widely dispersed female hypothesis [Komers et al., 1997; Lukas & Clutton‐Brock, 2013]. Another study by Dobson et al. [2010] concluded that there was not a single predominate selective pressure leading to pair‐living for all mammals. Given this ongoing lack of consensus (despite decades of research on primate and mammalian pair‐living), in addition to the high ecological and physiological diversity of primates displaying pair‐living, it is seems prudent to begin incorporating species‐specific approaches into our examinations of pair‐living.

Sources of Intraspecific Variation

The above conceptual models outline selective forces that favor pair‐living. However, the dynamics shaping realized social phenotypes are more numerous and complex. For pair‐living primates, a number of factors could impact whether additional adults are incorporated into the group. Here, I put forth three selective factors that may lead to variability in pair‐living social organizations: (i) A battle of the sexes [Arnqvist & Rowe, 2005; Davies, 1992; Parker, 2006]. It is notable that many of the basic selective pressures proposed in the conceptual models above only explain why one sex benefits from a pair‐living scenario. Since males and females maximize reproductive success differently (males by gaining access to females, females by gaining access to food or infant caregivers), both sexes may not share a common benefit from a pair‐living arrangement. For instance, in the mate guarding and bodyguard hypotheses, females benefit from associating with one male, but males will still benefit from incorporating multiple females into the group, if possible. Alternately, under an obligate paternal care scenario, females will benefit from the presence of more than one male in the group, to the detriment of these males' reproductive success [e.g., Goldizen, 2003, 1990]. Stronger selective pressures favoring pair‐living for one sex but not the other could lead to a tug‐of‐war scenario in which the displayed social organization represents a compromise between differing male and female fitness optima; (ii) Decisions are made on the individual level [Clutton‐Brock, 1989; Kenrick et al., 2003; Parrish & Edelstein‐Keshet, 1999]. The actions of individuals can prevent other animals from achieving their optimal social organization. As an example, sharing mating access to a female would reduce the reproductive success of a male already in an established breeding pair. However, this shared access would represent a potential increase in reproductive success for a solitary floater male. If a solitary male joins a breeding pair (and perhaps has support of the female to do so), the established male may be unable to exclude this new immigrant. In such a case, animals are not able to reach their fitness optima due to the decisions of other animals [e.g., Clutton‐Brock, 1998]; (iii) Conflicting selection pressures [Schluter et al., 1991]. Primates are subject to a wide range of selective forces, which may or may not favor pair‐living. For instance, to gain sole reproductive access to females, established males should exclude extra‐group males. Yet incorporating extra males into the group could provide alternate benefits such as more successful resource defense, increased protection from predators (through increased vigilance and selfish herd/dilution effects), or additional help rearing offspring [Nunn, 2000; van Schaik & Kappeler, 2006]. The social organization adopted by animals may represent a compromise between these competing selective forces [e.g., Savini et al., 2009]. In addition to the above factors, it has also been previously acknowledged that environmental variation can impact social organization [Chapman & Rothman, 2009; Savini et al., 2009]. Differences in habitat quality, population density, presence and abundance of predators, or interspecific competition can all impact which social strategy best enables animals to reach their fitness optima in specific localities. While not exhaustive, this list outlines major themes to consider when analyzing sources of variation in sociality. Considering these pressures in conjunction with the above pair‐living models can assist in reconstructing the selective pressures generating variably pair‐living social systems.

WHITE‐FACED SAKIS AS A CASE STUDY IN VARIABLE PAIR‐LIVING

White‐Faced Saki Social System

White‐faced sakis (Pithecia pithecia) provide an intriguing case to assess variable pair‐living. While the pitheciines have traditionally been an understudied group, continued research has allowed us to gain a more substantial understanding of white‐faced saki social behavior. Pithecia represents an evolutionary intermediate in a large‐scale phylogenetic shift in social organization, being sister taxa to both the consistently pair‐living titi monkeys (Callicebus) and the large grouped multi‐male, multi‐female bearded sakis (Chiropotes) and uakaris (Cacajao) [Thompson & Norconk, 2011]. White‐faced sakis also display a unique mix of traits both indicative and non‐indicative of ‘typical’ monogamous primates [i.e., Fuentes, 1998], having strong male‐female social bonds and territoriality, yet lacking sexual monomorphism and paternal care (see below). Given our present knowledge, the three components outlined by Kappeler & van Schaik [2002] can be used to characterize this species' social system. While research has been published on several aspects of white‐faced saki social behavior independently, the current work represents the first integrative characterization of this species' social system as a whole. This research adhered to legal and ethical standards outlined by the American Society of Primatologists.

Social organization

Several long‐term and census studies have now reported that white‐faced sakis naturally occur in both male‐female pairs and small multi‐male, multi‐female groups [Kessler, 1998; Lehman et al., 2001; Mittermeier, 1977; Muckenhirn et al., 1975; Norconk et al., 2003; Oliveira et al., 1985; Thompson & Norconk, 2011; Vié et al., 2001]. White‐faced saki groups average 3.2 individuals (range = 2–12) [Norconk, 2011] and a survey in Guyana reported that roughly 26% (N = 19 observed bisexual groups) of groups were pair‐living, while 74% contained more than one adult male and/or female [Lehman et al., 2001]. These small multi‐male, multi‐female groups likely form via two means: (i) the maturation and subsequent breeding of offspring within their natal group, and/or; (ii) immigration of unrelated adults into established groups [Norconk, 2006; Soini, 1986; Thompson et al., 2010]. These mechanisms likely lead to differing social dynamics within the group, reinforcing that group history can be an important factor shaping social structure and mating systems. The factors constraining groups to small sizes are unclear, however white‐faced sakis' cryptic strategy suggests predator avoidance may be a factor [Gleason & Norconk, 2002]. Although this review focuses on white‐faced sakis (due to the relatively larger amount known about their social behavior), similar variation has also been reported for other Pithecia sp. [reviewed in Norconk & Setz, 2013].

In addition to pairs and small multi‐adult groups, the presence of solitary floater individuals (of both sexes) has been reported in multiple populations from both censuses and studies with sustained observation [Mittermeier et al., 1977; Muckenhirn, 1975; Thompson et al., 2010; Vié et al., 2001; for similar data on other Pithecia sp. see Di Fiore et al., 2007; Soini, 1986].

Mating system

Two studies have now documented concurrent pregnancies of >1 white‐faced saki females within a single group [Norconk, 2006; Thompson, 2013]. Reports of copulations from a 17‐month study in Suriname have shown that both pair‐living and multi‐adult groups can exhibit monogamous copulation patterns [Thompson, 2013]. However multi‐adult groups can also display polygamous copulations, with both males and females copulating with more than one partner. Behaviors indicative of within‐group mating competition (copulation interference, female‐directed sexual aggression) have also been reported for multi‐adult groups [Thompson, 2013]. White‐faced sakis continuously cycle and births occur throughout the year, although there is a slight peak in births November through April [Norconk, 2006; Savage et al., 1995]. Both birth and group membership sex ratios are male biased [Norconk, 2006]. No published studies to date have measured mating success from genetic data to determine if observed copulations correspond to actual siring of offspring.

Social structure

Details regarding social structure have emerged from long‐term studies of white‐faced sakis in Venezuela [Harrison & Norconk, 1997, 1999; Norconk et al., 1999; Norconk, 2006] and Suriname [Norconk et al., 2003; Thompson & Norconk, 2011; Thompson et al., 2012]. The strongest social bonds in white‐faced saki groups are between a single male‐female reproductive pair (with females being responsible for grooming and maintaining proximity to males), regardless of the presence of same‐sex kin or other sexually active dyads [Thompson & Norconk, 2011]. This affiliative male‐female bond lies in stark contrast to the consistent same‐sex aggression exhibited between groups [Norconk, 2006; Thompson et al., 2012; Thompson & Norconk, 2013]. Encounter rates range from almost daily by one report, to an average of one encounter per 7.5 days and can be intense, involving chases, animals biting one another, and falling from trees [Norconk, 2006; Thompson et al., 2012; Thompson & Norconk, 2013]. Although males are the primary participants in this aggression, females have also been reported to participate [Norconk, 2006]. There is evidence this aggression is tied to both mate guarding of females and resource defense [Cunningham & Janson, 2007; Thompson et al., 2012]. Within‐group aggression is relatively low [Harrison & Norconk, 1997; Norconk et al., 1999], exhibited between members of the same‐sex, and generally directed toward older members of the group by younger individuals [Norconk, 2006]. Male‐male aggression in sexual contexts is also directed from younger toward older males [Thompson, 2013]. Norconk [2006] proposed that this same‐sex aggression may reflect efforts by younger group members to dispose older individuals from breeding positions.

Applying the Models

Evaluating the specific evolutionary pressures driving pair‐living in white‐faced sakis may help elucidate the factors underlying social variation. However, comparing available data with each models' expectations yields only partial support for most models (Table I). Given the deviation of white‐faced sakis from a rigidly pair‐living social system, this mixed result is not entirely unexpected. Yet some clear trends do result from this exercise. Our currently available data show more superficial support for the infanticide hypothesis than others (Table I), however there are some justified reservations to fully accepting this hypothesis. Namely, infanticide has never been observed in wild white‐faced sakis. It is notable that there have been relatively few long‐term studies on Pithecia sp., decreasing the chances infanticide would have been observed if it is a rare behavior. As outlined above, infanticide has been proposed as a main factor contributing to both pair‐living and other permanent male‐female associations in primates [Opie et al., 2013a; Palombit, 2000; van Schaik & Kappeler, 1997, 2003]. Accordingly, it may be tempting to invoke past infanticidal pressures as a selective force that shaped the currently observed social system (i.e., ‘ghost of selection past’) [Fuentes, 2002; Opie et al., 2013a; van Schaik & Dunbar, 1990]. While this may be a plausible explanation, empirically testing for past selection pressures is often either very difficult or unfeasible, putting researchers in an uncomfortable position of accepting an explanation without hard evidence. Yet, pair‐living primates do tend to have lower levels of infanticide than species exhibiting other social organizations [Opie et al., 2013a], consistent with the idea that pair‐living is an effective infanticide prevention strategy. If a similar strategy is occurring in white‐faced sakis as in other variably uni‐ or bi‐male groups such as gorillas, the presence of extra males may help further reduce infanticide risk [Robbins, 1995; Yamagiwa et al., 2003]. This is consistent with the idea that a greater number of white‐faced saki groups are multi‐adult, compared to pair‐living [statistics provided above; Lehman et al., 2001]. Despite ongoing debate over the role of infanticide in primate and mammalian pair‐living (see above), these larger taxonomic trends don't preclude a role for infanticide in shaping white‐faced saki social organization specifically, since separate selective pressures may be acting on different primate species. However there are currently no data to support infanticide as a direct motivation for between‐group aggression in white‐faced sakis [Thompson et al., 2012]. The risk of infanticide posed from within white‐faced groups is low given the overall low levels of aggression, low number of males per group, and potential relatedness of males [although recent unrelated male immigrants may pose a more viable threat: e.g., Cheney et al., 2004; Fedigan et al., 2003; Knopff et al., 2004]. It is also possible that multiple competing females within the group could present a risk of female infanticide, as seen in marmosets [Digby, 1995; Lazaro‐Perea et al., 2000]. Lastly, floater populations could pose a high risk of infanticide. In three observed instances of interactions between floaters and established groups, established males were always aggressive toward floater males [Thompson, pers. obs.]. Unfortunately, there is not currently enough data to investigate whether floaters target groups during times of infant vulnerability and aggression towards floaters can serve multiple functions other than infanticide. It is also worth considering that the mixed support for the infanticide hypothesis (Table I) may result from unfit predictions or because predictions do not mutually exclude hypotheses.

Table I.

Expectations of Models Explaining Pair‐living and an Assessment for White‐Faced Sakis

Model Predictions a Supported?
Females as a widely dispersed resource 1) females exhibit range defense N b
2) males cannot achieve more copulations under a roving male strategy ?
3) species occur exclusively in two‐adult groups N
4) females are more energetically limited than males ? c
Obligate paternal care 1) males display caretaking of immature N d
2) females without males will have lowered reproductive success ?
Male defense against predators/conspecifics 1) predation is a significant selective pressure ? e
2) vigilance behavior is costly & males are more vigilant ? e
3) males exhibit predator defense N
4) there is intergroup competition for resources Y
5) group size is small Y
6) both sexes actively defend range from conspecifics N b
7) solitary females will have smaller ranges and lose interspecific contests ?
Mate guarding 1) males will be primarily responsible for maintaining the pair‐bond N
2) male‐male aggression should be high Intergroup: Y; Intragroup: N
3) females should be dispersed/limited N
Bodyguard 1) females are responsible for maintaining the pair‐bond Y
2) males direct substantial aggression to females N
3) males are larger or somehow more capable of inflicting damage to females ? f
Infanticide prevention 1) infanticide occurs in the species N d
2) male‐female associations last longer than one interbirth interval Y
3) females are unable to defend young from infanticidal males ? g
4) male floater population and/or skewed sex ratio exists Y
5) females are responsible for maintaining the pair‐bond Y
6) adults should be wary of extra‐group males Y h
7) adults should not be wary of extra‐group females ? h
a

Models' predictions are from Fuentes [2002].

b

Males are the primary participants in between‐group aggression, but females do participate in a limited number of cases [Norconk, 2006; Thompson et al., 2012; Thompson & Norconk, 2013].

c

Female mammals are presumed to be more energetically limited than males due to the constraints of internal gestation and lactation, however there is no reason to believe this is more exacerbated in white‐faced sakis than other primates. Male white‐faced sakis are slightly larger in body size than females (see footnotes e, f below), which may increase their absolute energy requirements.

d

No cases have been reported in the literature.

e

Although their relatively small body size [x¯=1.68kg: Ford & Davis, 1992] makes white‐faced sakis susceptible to a variety of predators, it in unknown whether predation pressure is higher than expected for an arboreal primate. The cost of vigilance behavior has not been evaluated in white‐faced sakis. Cunningham et al. [2013] found that males often assumed more vulnerable positions while traveling, but not while feeding. Males are more vigilant than females before entering the sleeping tree, although reactions to predator sightings (mobbing, alarm calls) are more often initiated by females [Thompson, pers. obs.]. See Gleason & Norconk [2002] for a review of anti‐predator behavior.

f

Body size dimorphism: 1.14 (M:F); canine size dimorphism: 1.24 [Hershkovitz, 1987; Ford & Davis, 1992; Ford, 1994].

g

No evidence exists on defensive ability of females against males. Although body size dimorphism exists (see f above), it is relatively small.

h

During intergroup encounters, females generally hang back from the encounter area and avoid the males at the front of the encounter, indicating that both sexes are wary of males approaching from the opposing group. However, since females avoid interactions with the opposing group, the reaction of either sex to an unknown female is currently unknown [Thompson et al., 2012; Thompson & Norconk, 2013].

In addition to the above citations, the table was based on these additional references: [Buchanan et al., 1981; Cunningham & Janson, 2007; Lehman et al., 2001; Norconk, 2006; Norconk et al., 1999; Thompson, 2013; Thompson & Norconk, 2011, 2013; Thompson et al., 2010; Vié, et al., 2001].

The partial support for the infanticide hypothesis rests on data indicating that male‐male between‐group aggression plays a central role in white‐faced saki behavior. This trait emerges as a common theme with other partially supported hypotheses, namely defense against resource competition from conspecifics and mate guarding (Table I). There is additional evidence for these partially supported models, as between‐group aggression has been linked to both female reproductive status and resource defense in white‐faced sakis [Cunningham & Janson, 2007; Thompson et al., 2012]. This commonality suggests that male defense from extra‐group individuals, whether for infanticide prevention, mate‐guarding, or resource defense, is a key component in the white‐faced saki social system. These motivations are not mutually exclusive and selection pressures from all three may influence the social patterns of white‐faced sakis to greater or lesser degrees. The influence of multiple, concurrent selection pressures on social organization has also been noted in other primates [Kappeler, 1997; Hill & Lee, 1998; Hilgartner et al., 2012]. For instance in Lepilemur, there is concurrent selection for males to monopolize single females in order to gain information on females' relatively short estrus period and to minimize the energy expenditure posed by a roving‐male strategy [Hilgartner et al., 2012]. It is additionally worth noting (as above) that the original selective pressure favoring these behaviors could differ from the selective pressure currently maintaining them [Dobson et al., 2010; Opie et al., 2013a]. This mix of selective pressures may actually warrant reconsideration of the infanticide hypothesis, as year‐round defense for resources or mate guarding should functionally provide protection from infanticide, without leaving a clear, detectable signal of defense only when infants are present.

Thus, a systematic assessment of models has given us insight into the central themes shaping pair‐living in white‐faced sakis. Primarily, that high between‐group aggression is a central factor in their social organization (although the ultimate drive(s) for this aggression are unresolved). Examining potential sources of variation within this framework can provide additional perspective. If excluding extra‐group males is a key component of white‐faced saki social strategy (regardless of the selective pressure(s) favoring it), then these high levels of same sex and between‐group aggression should prevent immigration into the group. Yet males may still experience competing advantages of including versus excluding extra adults into/from the group [Savini et al., 2009]. In pair‐living or uni‐male groups, males will benefit from sole reproductive access to females, especially under conditions when other resident males do not provide beneficial services such as infant caregiving or resource and mate defense. On the other hand, adding extra males to the group may increase established males' competitive ability through cooperative defense [Savini et al., 2009; Port et al., 2011]. It is also worth noting that if extra group members are not reproductively active (as may be the case for retained adult offspring if their parents are still active breeders in the group), there will be lower costs of incorporating them into the pair. This is likely the case with Cebus capucinus groups, in which the dominant male sires most of the offspring regardless of other males being present [Jack & Fedigan, 2005]. A similar phenomenon has been well demonstrated for variably uni‐ or bi‐male harems in howler monkeys (Alouatta) and gorillas (Gorilla), in which extra adult males stabilize the group and extend the tenure of the alpha male [Pope, 1990; Robbins, 1995; Snyder‐Mackler et al., 2012; Watts, 2000; Yamagiwa et al., 2003]. In this scenario there are multiple ways for animals to maximize their fitness, either through: (i) less effective defense, but exclusive reproductive access in a pair‐living scenario, or (ii) more effective defense, but shared reproductive access in a multi‐adult group. Additionally, the opportunity to adopt cooperative group defense likely varies on an individual basis. For example, males may not have a suitable coalitionary partner until they have surviving adult offspring that choose to stay in the group. Alternately, established males may be unable to exclude males that chose to immigrate into the group. These types of individual decisions and group history scenarios undoubtedly affect the social organization (pair‐living or multi‐adult) exhibited by white‐faced saki groups.

A Holistic Approach to Sociality

Analyzing the three components of sociality (social organization, mating system, and social structure) in conjunction can provide a more complete picture of the forces generating variation in pair‐living. For white‐faced sakis these elements fit together congruously (Fig. 1). Figure 1 shows the dynamics in which variable pair‐living could be maintained as an evolutionarily stable strategy in white‐faced sakis. By analyzing social structures, it is revealed that high between‐group aggression can strengthen the male‐female affiliation observed in this species. Females have an incentive to remain and form bonds with males who will protect access to feeding resources. Female choice for high quality males should also reinforce high male‐male competition and aggression. Since this aggression provides exclusive or near‐exclusive sexual access to female(s), it should also reinforce the male's commitment to the female by increasing paternity certainty, further enabling strong male‐female bonds. High levels of male‐male aggression may also make a roving male strategy unfeasible, if females are guarded. On the other hand, if a single male is unable to provide adequate defense, females should favor incorporating additional males into the group (Fig. 1). However, factors such as heightened within‐group feeding competition may ultimately limit overall group size. Additionally, if females mate with a large number of males (and thus reduce paternity certainty), these males will have little incentive to defend resources for females. Lastly, although the same‐sex aggression displayed by white‐faced sakis (both male and female) may promote pair‐living via excluding new members, this force may also lead to multi‐adult groups when offspring delay dispersal and attempt to establish a reproductive role within the group (if the potential for non‐incestuous mating arises) (Fig. 1). Thus, some portion of multi‐male white‐faced saki groups are likely comprised of related father‐son pairs, although the documentation of polygamous mating suggests this may not be true for all groups. In sum, white‐faced saki social organization appears to result from the interaction between the selective benefits of pair‐living versus incorporating extra adults into the group.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Interrelationships between components of the white‐faced saki social system. Bold lettering indicates the three components of social system. Boxes with solid borders indicate traits reported in the literature; dotted borders represent proposed relationships between traits. Arrows indicate direction of impact. 1The presence of floater populations and aggression toward extra‐group members implies that breeding positions are limited.

This analysis suggests that the primary white‐faced saki strategy is for males to limit access to females by non‐group members, but also possibly tolerate minimal within‐group mating and/or feeding competition, if it achieves better group defense. This may occur when the threat posed by floater animals is high, when between‐group feeding competition is high, or when males are unable to exclude extra adult males from the group (either from immigration or delayed dispersal by offspring). This cooperative defense likely provides benefits to females through reducing infanticide risk, reducing harassment from floaters, or increasing access to resources. Receiving these advantages provides a testable explanation for why females choose to cultivate close social bonds with (presumably) high‐quality males. If the current assertion is correct, then: (i) multi‐adult groups should be more effective in home range and infanticide defense than pairs; (ii) females should have or attempt to cultivate stronger bonds with males that participate in defense and/or are more effective in group defense; (iii) females in multi‐adult groups should receive greater benefits of defense (increased access to resources or protection) than those in pairs [see Santos & Nakagawa, 2013].

Analyzing these separate components of social behavior has provided corroboratory evidence to the insights gained from our conceptual models regarding variable pair‐living in white‐faced sakis. This species‐level approach has utility across a range of social animals, since: (i) an overarching model for the variability displayed in social and mating patterns of pair‐living is currently lacking, and (ii) factors selecting for pair‐living versus small multi‐adult groups may differ between species and likewise the stimuli for variation will also be species‐specific. In addition to helping generate species‐specific models of social system (i.e., Fig. 1) this approach can supply testable predictions to assess the accuracy of these models and guide future research.

Species such as white‐faced sakis that exhibit variable social organizations present an ideal natural experimental design to directly compare the factors, pressures, and advantages driving pair‐living and multi‐adult groups. The current exercise has demonstrated how conceptual models, a consideration of sources of variation, and a holistic examination of social systems can provide insight into the selective forces driving variation in pair‐living.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I am grateful to Adrian Barnett and Sarah Boyle for the invitation to participate in this special issue. I also appreciate the helpful feedback on this manuscript from Sarah Boyle, Paul Garber, and two anonymous reviewers. I would like to thank Marilyn Norconk, Joan Silk, Richard Meindl, and C. Owen Lovejoy for their guidance in all my saki endeavors. Field data making this publication possible was provided by Scott Dettloff, Chris and Freya Gordon, Rose Hores, Reshma Jankispersad, Guno Marjanom, and Helen Smith.

Conflict of interest: None.

REFERENCES

  1. Aquino R, Puertas P, Encarnación F. 1990. Supplemental notes on population parameters of northeastern Peruvian night monkeys, genus Aotus (Cebidae). American Journal of Primatology 21:215–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnqvist G, Rowe L. 2005. Sexual conflict: monographs in behavior and ecology. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bartlett TQ. 2003. Intragroup and intergroup social interactions in white‐handed gibbons. International Journal of Primatology 24:239–259. [Google Scholar]
  4. Brotherton PNM, Komers PE. 2003. Mate guarding and the evolution of social monogamy in mammals In: Reichard UH, Boesch C, editors. Monogamy: mating strategies and partnerships in birds, humans and other mammals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 42–80. [Google Scholar]
  5. Brotherton PNM, Manser MB. 1997. Female dispersion and the evolution of monogamy in the dik‐dik. Animal Behavior 54:1413–1424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Buchanan DB, Mittermeier RA, van Roosmalen MGM. 1981. The saki monkeys, genus Pithecia In: Coimbra‐Filho AF, Mittermeier RA, editors. Ecology and behavior of Neotropical primates. Rio de Janeiro: Academia Brasileira de Ciencias; p 391–417. [Google Scholar]
  7. Chapman CA, Rothman JM. 2009. Within‐species differences in primate social structure: evolution of plasticity and phylogenetic constraints. Primates 50:12–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cheney D, Seyfarth R, Fischer J, et al. 2004. Factors affecting reproduction and mortality among baboons in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. International Journal of Primatology 25:401–428. [Google Scholar]
  9. Clutton‐Brock TH. 1989. Review lecture: mammalian mating systems. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. Biological Sciences 236:339–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Clutton‐Brock TH. 1998. Reproductive skew, concessions and limited control. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 13:288–292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cunningham EP, Harrison‐Levine AL, Norman RG. 2013. Finding the balance: optimizing predator avoidance and food encounters through individual positioning in Pithecia pithecia during travel In: Barnett AA, Veiga LM, Ferrari SF, Norconk MA, editors. Evolutionary biology and conservation of titis, sakis and uacaris. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 272–276. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cunningham E, Janson C. 2007. Integrating information about location and value of resources by white‐faced saki monkeys (Pithecia pithecia). Animal Cognition 10:293–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Davies N. 1992. Dunnock behaviour and evolution. London: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Di Fiore A, Fernandez‐Duque E, Hurst D. 2007. Adult male replacement in socially monogamous equatorial saki monkeys (Pithecia aequatorialis). Folia Primatologica 78:88–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Di Fiore A. 2009. Genetic approaches to the study of dispersal and kinship in New World primates In: Garber P, Estrada A, Bicca‐Marques JC, Heymann EW, Strier KB, editors. South American primates: comparative perspectives in the study of behavior, ecology, and conservation. Springer; p 211–250. [Google Scholar]
  16. Digby LJ, Ferrari SF, Saltzman W, et al. 2011. Callitrichines: the role of competition in cooperatively breeding species In Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Bearder SK, Stumpf RM, editors. Primates in perspective. New York: Oxford University Press; p 91–107. [Google Scholar]
  17. Digby LJ. 1995. Infant care, infanticide, and female reproductive strategies in polygynous groups of common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 37:51–61. [Google Scholar]
  18. Dobson FS, Way BM, Baudoin C. 2010. Spatial dynamics and the evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Behavioral Ecology 21:747–752. [Google Scholar]
  19. Eisenberg JF, Muckenhirn NA, Rudran R. 1972. The relation between ecology and social structure in primates. Science 176:863–874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Emlen ST, Wrege PH. 1986. Forced copulations and intra‐specific parasitism: two costs of social living in the white‐fronted bee‐eater. Ethology 71:2–29. [Google Scholar]
  21. Fan P, Jiang X. 2010. Maintenance of multifemale social organization in a group of Nomascus concolor at Wuliang Mountain, China. International Journal of Primatology 31:1–13. [Google Scholar]
  22. Fan P, Fei H, Xiang Z, et al. 2010. Social structure and group dynamics of the cao vit gibbon (Nomascus nasutus) in Bangliang, Jingxi, China. Folia Primatologia 81:245–253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fedigan LM. 2003. Impact of male takeovers on infant deaths, births and conceptions in Cebus capucinus at Santa Rosa, Costa Rica. International Journal of Primatology 24:723–741. [Google Scholar]
  24. Ford SM. 1994. Evolution of sexual dimorphism in body weight in platyrrhines. American Journal of Primatology 34:221–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ford SM, Davis LC. 1992. Systematics and body size: implications for feeding adaptations in New World monkeys. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 88:415–468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fuentes A. 1998. Re‐evaluating primate monogamy. American Anthropologist 100:890–907. [Google Scholar]
  27. Fuentes A. 2000. Hylobatid communities: changing views on pair bonding and social organization in hominoids. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 113:33–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fuentes A. 2002. Patterns and trends in primate pair bonds. International Journal of Primatology 23:953–978. [Google Scholar]
  29. Gleason TM, Norconk MA. 2002. Predation risk and antipredator adaptations in white‐faced sakis, Pithecia pithecia In: Miller LE, editor. Eat or be eaten: predator sensitive foraging among primates. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 169–184. [Google Scholar]
  30. Goldizen AW, Boesch C. 2003. In: Reichard UH, editor. Social monogamy and its variations in callitrichids: do these relate to the costs of infant care. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 232–247. [Google Scholar]
  31. Goldizen AW. 1990. A comparative perspective on the evolution of tamarin and marmoset social systems. International Journal of Primatology 11:63–83. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gursky S. 2000. Sociality in the spectral tarsier, Tarsius spectrum . American Journal of Primatology 51:89–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Harcourt CS, Nash LT. 1986. Social organization of galagos in Kenyan coastal forests: I Galago zanzibaricus . American Journal of Primatology 10:339–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Harrison AL, Norconk MA. 1999. Social dominance in a group of white‐faced sakis (Pithecia pithecia) in the context of a rare and limited resource. American Journal of Primatology 49:60. [Google Scholar]
  35. Harrison AL, Norconk MA. 1997. Feeding party dynamics in response to food availability in white‐faced saki of Lago Guri, Venezuela. American Journal of Primatology 42:114–115. [Google Scholar]
  36. Hershkovitz P. 1987. Taxonomy of South American sakis, genus Pithecia (Cebidae, Platyrrhini): a preliminary report and critical review with the description of a new species and a new subspecies. American Journal of Primatology 12:387–468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hilgartner R, Fichtel C, Kappeler PM, Zinner D. 2012. Determinants of pair‐living in red‐tailed sportive lemurs (Lepilemur ruficaudatus). Ethology 118:466–479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hill R, Lee P. 1998. Predation risk as an influence on group size in cercopithecoid primates: implications for social structure. Journal of Zoology 245:447–456. [Google Scholar]
  39. Huck M, Fernandez‐Duque E, Babb P, Schurr T. 2014. Correlates of genetic monogamy in socially monogamous mammals: insights from Azara's owl monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. Biological Sciences 281:20140195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Isbell LA. 2004. Is there no place like home? Ecological bases of female dispersal and philopatry and their consequences for the formation of kin groups In: Berman CM, editor. Kinship and behavior in primates. Oxford: Oxford University Press; p 71–108. [Google Scholar]
  41. Jolly A. 1998. Pair‐bonding, female aggression and the evolution of lemur societies. Folia Primatologica 69:1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kappeler PM. 1997. Determinants of primate social organization: comparative evidence and new insights from Malagasy lemurs. Biological Reviews 72:111–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kappeler PM. 2000. Causes and consequences of unusual sex ratios among lemurs In: Kappeler PM, editor. Primate males: causes and consequences of variation in group composition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 55–63. [Google Scholar]
  44. Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP. 2002. Evolution of primate social systems. International Journal of Primatology 23:707–740. [Google Scholar]
  45. Kenrick DT, Li NP, Butner J. 2003. Dynamical evolutionary psychology: individual decision rules and emergent social norms. Psychological Review 110:3–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kessler P. 1998. Primate densities in the natural reserve of Nouragues, French Guiana. Neotropical Primates 6:45–46. [Google Scholar]
  47. Kleiman DG. 1977. Monogamy in mammals. Quarterly Review of Biology 52:39–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Knopff KH, Knopff ARA, Pavelka MSM. 2004. Observed case of infanticide committed by a resident male central American black howler monkey (Alouatta pigra). American Journal of Primatology 63:239–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Koenig A, Borries C. 2009. The lost dream of ecological determinism: time to say goodbye?… or a white queen's proposal? Evolutionary Anthropology 18:166–174. [Google Scholar]
  50. Komers PNM, Brotherton P. 1997. Female space use is the best predictor of monogamy in mammals. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. Biological Sciences 264:1261–1270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lappan S. 2007. Social relationships among males in multimale siamang groups. International Journal of Primatology 28:369–387. [Google Scholar]
  52. Lawler RR. 2011. Feeding competition, cooperation, and the causes of primate sociality: a commentary on Sussman et al. American Journal of Primatology 73:84–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lazaro‐Perea C, Castro CS, Harrison R, et al. 2000. Behavioral and demographic changes following the loss of the breeding female in cooperatively breeding marmosets. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 48:137–146. [Google Scholar]
  54. Lehman SM, Prince W, Mayor M. 2001. Variations in group size in white‐faced sakis (Pithecia pithecia): evidence for monogamy or seasonal congregations? Neotropical Primates 9:96–101. [Google Scholar]
  55. Lukas D, Clutton‐Brock T. 2014. Evolution of social monogamy in primates is not consistently associated with male infanticide. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111:E1674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Lukas D, Clutton‐Brock TH. 2013. The evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Science 341:526–530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. MacKinnon J, MacKinnon K. 1980. The behavior of wild spectral tarsiers. International Journal of Primatology 1:361–379. [Google Scholar]
  58. Mesnick SL. 1997. Sexual alliances: evidence and evolutionary implications In: Gowaty P, editor. Feminism and evolutionary biology. New York: Springer; p 207–260. [Google Scholar]
  59. Mittermeier RA. 1977. Distribution, synecology and conservation of Surinam monkeys. Dissertation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. [Google Scholar]
  60. Mock DW, Fujioka M. 1990. Monogamy and long‐term pair bonding in vertebrates. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 5:39–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Muckenhirn NA, Mortensen BK, Vessey S, Fraser CEO, Singh B. 1975. Report on a primate survey in Guyana. Washington, D.C: Pan American World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
  62. Müller A, Thalmann U. 2000. Origin and evolution of primate social organisation: a reconstruction. Biological Reviews 75:405–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Norconk MA. 2011. Sakis, uakaris, and titi monkeys: behavioral diversity in a radiation of primate seed predators In: Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Bearder SK, Stumpf RM, editors. Primates in perspective. New York: Oxford University Press; p 122–139. [Google Scholar]
  64. Norconk MA. 2006. Long‐term study of group dynamics and female reproduction in Venezuelan Pithecia pithecia . International Journal of Primatology 27:653–674. [Google Scholar]
  65. Norconk MA, Setz EZ. 2013. Ecology and behavior of saki monkeys (genus Pithecia) In: Barnett AA, Veiga LM, Ferrari SF, Norconk MA, editors. Evolutionary biology and conservation of titis, sakis and uacaris. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 262–271. [Google Scholar]
  66. Norconk MA, Gleason TMH. 1999. Feeding rates and social dominance among white‐faced saki females. American Journal of Physical Anthropology S28:212. [Google Scholar]
  67. Norconk MA, Raghanti MA, Martin SK, et al. 2003. Primates of Brownsberg Natuurpark, Suriname, with particular attention to the pitheciins. Neotropical Primates 11:94–100. [Google Scholar]
  68. Nunn CL. 2000. Collective benefits, free‐riders, and male extra‐group conflict In: Kappeler PM, editor. Primate males: causes and consequences of variation in group composition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 192–204. [Google Scholar]
  69. Oliveira JMS, Lima MG, Bonvincin C, Ayres JM, Fleagle JG. 1985. Preliminary notes on the ecology and behavior of the Guianan saki (Pithecia pithecia, Linnaeus 1766 Cebidae Primate). Acta Amazonica 15:249–263. [Google Scholar]
  70. Opie C, Atkinson QD, Dunbar RIM, Shultz S. 2013a. Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110:13328–13332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Opie C, Atkinson QD, Dunbar RIM, Shultz S. 2013b. Reply to Dixson: Infanticide triggers primate monogamy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110:E4938–E4938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Opie C, Atkinson QD, Dunbar RI, Shultz S. 2014. Reply to Lukas and Clutton‐Brock: Infanticide still drives primate monogamy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111:E1675–E1675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Palombit RA. 2000. Infanticide and the evolution of male‐female bonds in animals In: Janson CH, editor. Infanticide by males and its implications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 239–268. [Google Scholar]
  74. Palombit RA. 1999. Infanticide and the evolution of pair bonds in nonhuman primates. Evolutionary Anthropology 7:117–129. [Google Scholar]
  75. Palombit R. 1996. Pair bonds in monogamous apes: a comparison of the siamang Hylobates syndactylus and the white‐handed gibbon Hylobates lar . Behaviour 133:321–356. [Google Scholar]
  76. Parker GA. 2006. Sexual conflict over mating and fertilization: an overview. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. Biological Sciences 361:235–259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Parrish JK, Edelstein‐Keshet L. 1999. Complexity, pattern, and evolutionary trade‐offs in animal aggregation. Science 284:99–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Pope TR. 1990. The reproductive consequences of male cooperation in the red howler monkey: paternity exclusion in multi‐male and single‐male troops using genetic markers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 27:439–446. [Google Scholar]
  79. Port M, Kappeler PM, Johnstone RA. 2011. Communal defense of territories and the evolution of sociality. American Naturalist 178:787–800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Reichard UH. 2003. Social monogamy in gibbons: the male perspective In: Reichard UH, Boesch C, editors. Monogamy: mating strategies and partnerships in birds, humans and other mammals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 190–213. [Google Scholar]
  81. Reichard UH. 2009. The social organization and mating system of Khao Yai white‐handed gibbons: 1992–2006 In: Whittaker DJ, editor. Gibbons: new perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology. New York: Springer; p 347–384. [Google Scholar]
  82. Robbins MM. 1995. A demographic analysis of male life history and social structure of mountain gorillas. Behaviour 132:21–47. [Google Scholar]
  83. Rutberg AT. 1983. The evolution of monogamy in primates. Journal of Theoretical Biology 104:93–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Santos ES, Nakagawa S. 2013. Breeding biology and variable mating system of a population of introduced dunnocks (Prunella modularis) in New Zealand. PLOS One 8:e69329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Savage A, Lasley BL, Vecchio AJ, Miller AE, Shideler SE. 1995. Selected aspects of female white‐faced saki (Pithecia pithecia) reproductive biology in captivity. Zoo Biology 14:441–452. [Google Scholar]
  86. Savini T, Boesch C, Reichard UH. 2009. Varying ecological quality influences the probability of polyandry in white‐handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) in Thailand. Biotropica 41:503–513. [Google Scholar]
  87. Schluter D, Price TD, Rowe L. 1991. Conflicting selection pressures and life history trade‐offs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. Biological Sciences 246:11–17. [Google Scholar]
  88. Setchell JM. 2013. Editorial: the top 10 questions in primatology. International Journal of Primatology 34:1–15. [Google Scholar]
  89. Snyder‐Mackler N, Alberts SC, Bergman TJ. 2012. Concessions of an alpha male? Cooperative defence and shared reproduction in multi‐male primate groups. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. Biological Sciences 279:3788–3795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Smuts BB, Smuts RW. 1993. Male aggression and sexual coercion of females in nonhuman primates and other mammals: evidence and theoretical implications. Advances in the Study of Behavior 22:1–63. [Google Scholar]
  91. Soini P. 1986. A synecological study of a primate community in the Pacaya‐Samiria National Reserve Peru. Primate Conservation 7:63–71. [Google Scholar]
  92. Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP. 1997. The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 41:291–309. [Google Scholar]
  93. Strier KB. 2009. Seeing the forest through the seeds: mechanisms of primate behavioral diversity from individuals to populations and beyond. Current Anthropology 50:213–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Strier KB. 2003. Primate behavioral ecology: from ethnography to ethology and back. American Anthropologist 105:16–27. [Google Scholar]
  95. Struhsaker TT. 2000. Variation in adult sex ratios of red colobus monkey social groups: implications for interspecific comparisons In: Kappeler PM, editor. Primate males: causes and consequences of variation in group composition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 108–119. [Google Scholar]
  96. Struhsaker TT. 2008. Demographic variability in monkeys: implications for theory and conservation. International Journal of Primatology 29:19–34. [Google Scholar]
  97. Struhsaker TT, Marshall AR, Detwiler K, et al. 2004. Demographic variation among Udzungwa red colobus in relation to gross ecological and sociological parameters. International Journal of Primatology 25:615–658. [Google Scholar]
  98. Sussman RW, Garber PA. 2011. Cooperation, collective action, and competition in primate social interactions In: Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Bearder SK, Stumpf RM, editors. Primates in Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press; p 587–598. [Google Scholar]
  99. Sussman R, Garber P, Cheverud J. 2011. Reply to Lawler: feeding competition, cooperation, and the causes of primate sociality. American Journal of Primatology 73:91–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Tenaza RR, Fuentes A. 1995. Monandrous social organization of pigtailed langurs (Simias concolor) in the Pagai Islands. Indonesia. 16:295–310. [Google Scholar]
  101. Thierry B. 2008. Primate socioecology, the lost dream of ecological determinism. Evolutionary Anthropology 17:93–96. [Google Scholar]
  102. Thompson CL. 2013. Non‐monogamous copulations and potential within‐group mating competition in white‐faced saki monkeys (Pithecia pithecia). American Journal of Primatology 78:817–824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Thompson CL, Norconk MA. 2011. Within‐group social bonds in white‐faced saki monkeys (Pithecia pithecia) display male‐female pair preference. American Journal of Primatology 73:1051–1061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Thompson CL, Norconk MA. 2013. Testing models of social behavior with regard to inter‐ and intragroup interactions in free‐ranging white‐faced sakis In: Barnett AA, Veiga LM, Ferrari SF, Norconk MA, editors. Evolutionary biology and conservation of titis, sakis and uacaris. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 277–284. [Google Scholar]
  105. Thompson CL, Norconk MA, Anzelc A, et al. 2010. Group dynamics of free‐ranging white‐faced sakis (Pithecia pithecia) at Brownsberg Naturepark Suriname. American Journal of Primatology 72:59. [Google Scholar]
  106. Thompson CL, Norconk MA, Whitten PL. 2012. Why fight? Selective forces favoring between‐group aggression in a variably pair‐living primate, the white‐faced saki (Pithecia pithecia). Behaviour 149:795–820. [Google Scholar]
  107. van Schaik CP. 1989. The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates In: Standen V, Foley RA, editors. Comparative socioecology: the behavioural ecology of humans and other mammals. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific Publications; p 195–218. [Google Scholar]
  108. van Schaik CP. 2000. Infanticide by male primates: the sexual selection hypothesis revisited In: van Schaik CP, Janson CH, editors. Infanticide by males and its implications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 27–60. [Google Scholar]
  109. van Schaik CP, Kappeler PM. 1997. Infanticide risk and the evolution of male–female association in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. Biological Sciences 264:1687–1694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. van Schaik CP, Kappeler PM. 2003. The evolution of social monogamy in primates In: Reichard UH, Boesch C, editors. Monogamy: mating strategies and partnerships in birds, humans, and other mammals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 59–80. [Google Scholar]
  111. van Schaik CP, Kappeler PM. 2006. Cooperation in primates and humans: closing the gap In: Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP, editors. Cooperation in primates and humans: mechanisms and evolution. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer; p 3–21. [Google Scholar]
  112. van Schaik CP, Dunbar RIM. 1990. The evolution of monogamy in large primates: a new hypothesis and some crucial tests. Behaviour 115:30–62. [Google Scholar]
  113. van Schaik CP, van Hooff JARAM. 1983. On the ultimate causes of primate social systems. Behaviour 85:91–117. [Google Scholar]
  114. Vié J, Richard‐Hansen C, Fournier‐Chambrillon C. 2001. Abundance, use of space, and activity patterns of white‐faced sakis (Pithecia pithecia) in French Guiana. American Journal of Primatology 55:203–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Watts DP. 2000. Causes and consequences of variation in male mountain gorilla life histories and group membership In: Kappeler PM, editor. Primate males: causes and consequences of variation in group composition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; p 169–180. [Google Scholar]
  116. Whitehead H, Dufault S. 1999. Techniques for analyzing vertebrate social structure using identified individuals: review and recommendations. Advances in the Study of Behavior 28:33–74. [Google Scholar]
  117. Wittenberger JF, Tilson RL. 1980. The evolution of monogamy: hypotheses and evidence. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 11:197–232. [Google Scholar]
  118. Wrangham RW. 1980. An ecological model of female‐bonded primate groups. Behaviour 75:262–299. [Google Scholar]
  119. Yamagiwa J, Hill DA. 1998. Intraspecific variation in the social organization of Japanese macaques: past and present scope of field studies in natural habitats. Primates 39:257–273. [Google Scholar]
  120. Yamagiwa J, Kahekwa J, Basabose AK. 2003. Intra‐specific variation in social organization of gorillas: implications for their social evolution. Primates 44:359–369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Primatology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES