Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2019 Oct;99:56–65. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.04.023

Identifying Indicators of Smartphone Addiction Through User-App Interaction

Beryl Noë a,, Liam D Turner a, David EJ Linden b, Stuart M Allen a, Bjorn Winkens c, Roger M Whitaker a
PMCID: PMC6686626  PMID: 31582873

Abstract

We introduce a new approach to monitoring the activity of smartphone users based on their physical interactions with the interface. Typical events are taps, scrolling and typing, carried out to interact with apps. As compared to other measures, this directly encapsulates potential problematic physical smartphone behaviour as a signal. The approach contrasts against conventions such as self-reporting or timing activity sessions, and it focusses on active rather than passive smartphone activity. Using this alternative method, we collected all user interface interaction events from a sample of 64 participants over a period of 8 weeks, using a bespoke monitoring app called Tymer. User Smartphone Addiction was seen to significantly correlate with high levels of interaction with Lifestyle apps, particularly for female users. Interactions with Social apps in general were also associated with Smartphone Addiction. In particular, user interactions with Snapchat correlated with Smartphone Addiction, represented across all types of interface interaction. This is significant given the widespread usage of Snapchat by teenagers, and we hypothesise that the app's design provides a particularly strong pathway in support of Smartphone Addiction.

Keywords: Smartphone addiction, Smartphone usage, User interface, Device interaction, Snapchat, Social media

Highlights

  • We assess the link between users' device interactions and Smartphone Addiction.

  • The approach distinguishes between passive and active Smartphone usage.

  • Lifestyle apps are associated with Smartphone Addiction, especially for female users.

  • Social apps in general are associated with Smartphone Addiction.

  • User interactions with Snapchat strongly correlate with Smartphone Addiction.

1. Introduction

Smartphone usage is now ubiquitous across much of the global population, with the smartphone offering a vast range of applications (apps) that help to extend human cognition (Clark & Chalmers, 1998). Typical daily usage levels are high (De-Sola Gutiérrez et al., 2016) and there is evidence of dependency and attachment to smartphone technology (Hoffner, Lee, & Park, 2016), combined with the potential disruptiveness of incoming notifications (Turner et al., 2015, 2017) and “checking habit” formation (Oulasvirta, Rattenbury, Ma, & Raita, 2012). It is now acknowledged that such uncontrolled and problematic behaviour can become harmful, being recognised as Smartphone Addiction (SA) (Pearson & Hussain, 2016). Problematic smartphone use has been linked to teenage depression and anxiety (Ha, Chin, Park, Ryu, & Yu, 2008; Lemola et al., 2014), and more widely, various relationships have been found concerning stress, depression, sleeping problems, anxiety, subjective well-being, and loneliness (Demirci, Akgönül, & Akpinar, 2015; Elhai, Dvorak, Levine, & Hall, 2017; Lee, Chang, You, & Cheng, 2014; Lemola et al., 2014).

Despite this research progress, detecting indicators of problematic smartphone behaviour is a challenge for two main reasons. Firstly, the range of utility that smartphone apps provide means that usage levels are generally high and that high usage is socially acceptable (Chotpitayasunondh and Douglas, 2016). Therefore behaviour correlating with SA can easily be hidden. Secondly, apps increasingly allow the smartphone to be passively used for large periods of time as a substitute for other devices (e.g., GPS navigation, TV, music player) meaning that as smartphone usage gets more diverse, high level metrics, such as time on the smartphone, may not represent the strongest indicator of problematic behaviour. These issues contribute to the invisibility of SA (Roberts et al., 2014) and the challenge of encouraging behaviour change to avoid it.

In this paper, we propose a new approach to identifying behaviours indicative of SA. By examining all interactions that a user performs through physically touching the screen (e.g., typing, scrolling, tapping), we are able to consider detailed user-app interactions as an indicator for SA. This allows us to gain new insight into the potential sources of problematic behaviour and pathways to addiction. In particular, our approach is based on measures of active usage than passive usage, which is determined by the extent of user engagement with the interface while the smartphone is operational. It also avoids the need for self-reporting and goes beyond the convention of monitoring screen or app time. Validating the approach required the development of a bespoke smartphone application called Tymer, which listens for and records the smartphone events that are triggered by human interaction, which was deployed on a cohort of 76 recruited participants over an eight week period (Noë, Turner, Linden, Allen, Maio, & Whitaker, 2017). Unlike previous work, this approach allows us to examine a wide range of apps within a single study, and make relative comparisons between different apps and classes of apps.

2. Related literature and hypotheses

Smartphone Addiction is an extension of Internet Addiction (Kimberly, 1998), which may also encompass other forms of behaviour bundled through the device, such as gaming, social networking, and online shopping (Block, 2008; Montag et al., 2015a). SA broadly refers to the condition leading to uncontrolled smartphone use despite the experience of negative repercussions on personal and social life. From an initial focus on the “cell-phone” as a means to communicate (e.g. (James & Drennan, 2005; Park, 2005)), the functionality provided by the latest generation of smartphones allows untethered Internet access, with applications optimised for mobile usage. The first smartphones with large touch-screens became globally available little over a decade ago: Apple's iPhone in 2007, followed by HTC's Dream in 2008, with the first studies on SA being published in 2011 (Chae and Lee, 2011; Woojae, Park, & Chang Lee, 2011).

Since then, interest in SA has grown strongly. Although, it has not yet received the clinical acknowledgement from the World Health Organisation (WHO) or American Psychiatric Association (APA), the WHO recently added gaming disorder to the ICD-11 (World Health Organization, 2018). Behavioural addictions share many similarities to substance abuse disorders, but differ inherently in the way they are caused not by a substance, but by a specific behaviour that is experienced as rewarding despite any associated negative consequences (Alavi et al., 2012). As such, it has been argued that any stimulating experience can potentially become addictive (Stanton & Brodsky, 1975).

Assessments for SA have resulted in various self-diagnostics (Kwon et al., 2013, Kwon, Kim, Cho, & Yang, 2013; Lin et al., 2014; Tossel, Kortum, Shepard, Rahmati, & Zhong, 2015), with the Smartphone Addiction Scale in particular combining assessment of substance use disorders, Internet Addiction, and the smartphone's own features. Current research related to SA frequently considers a particular application (e.g., Facebook (Ryan, Chester, Reece, & Xenos, 2014), WhatsApp (Montag et al., 2015b)) or a class of apps (e.g., social networking (Salehan & Negahban, 2013)). In this context it is important to note that SA can, like Internet Addiction (Griffiths, 2012), be argued to be both source and pathway to the causes of addiction.

Measures of smartphone activity typically involve self-reporting (e.g. (Elhai, Levine, Dvorak, & Hall, 2016)) or recording screen time (e.g. (Ferreira, Goncalves, Kostakos, Barkhuus, & Dey, 2014)). Although valid and worthy, these approaches have limitations, such as allowing subjects to filter their own reports of behaviour (King & Bruner, 2000) or including passive usage. Additionally, important details can be obfuscated that a user may consider insignificant, such as a dependency on a particular type of physical interaction with the device, or excessive repetition. Automated collection of user-data sets a new standard (Stachl et al., 2017), but this is more challenging to achieve and less well-established.

These issues motivate the new approach taken in this paper, which goes beyond monitoring high level metrics such as time on the device, to understand how the detailed interactions with the smartphone may coincide with SA. Furthermore, different apps, and different types of smartphone interaction may present different potential pathways to SA and different levels of risk. Our approach involves automatically recording all of the user's interactions with the user interface (UI) for all their applications and activity, over an eight week period. Related work taking this approach is scant: although tracking device interaction is an established technique in the human-computer interaction literature (Cao & Lin, 2017; Jesdabodi & Maalej, 2015; Peng, Zhao, & Zhu, 2016; Shin & Dey, 2013), detailed assessment of correlations with SA are limited, with usage typically being assessed at the higher level (e.g., time on device (Falaki et al., 2010; HintzeRainhard et al., 2014)) and correlated with other aspects such as personality (Gokul, Blom, & Gatica-Perez, 2013; Stachl et al., 2017) and mood (LiKamWa, Liu, Lane, & Zhong, 2013).

2.1. Motivating issues

Historically, high usage, measured by time, has been shown to be a key element of SA (e.g. (Bae, 2017; De-Sola Gutiérrez et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2014a; Lemola et al., 2014; Roberts et al., 2014; Rozgonjuk, Levine, Hall, & Elhai, 2018)). Differences in time spent on smartphones is often attributed to habit (Oulasvirta et al., 2012; Tossel et al., 2015) and usage frequency (Tossel et al., 2015). However, as smartphone capabilities continue to increase and support a wider variety of apps, it becomes important to determine the extent to which SA is related to particular types of use. Active usage, where the user is engaged through UI interactions, as compared to passive use (e.g., video streaming), remains an important distinction. To the best of our knowledge, this distinction has not been studied in the literature, due to the additional complexity required in assessing a user's interactions at a granular level. We focus on this and determine the interaction events occurring at the user interface. Based on previous observations regarding time, it is appropriate to hypothesise that high-scorers in SA have more UI interactions than low-scorers (H1); this can be considered across all applications.

It is challenging to assert with confidence how SA may manifest itself in terms of specific types of UI interactions, relative to the broader user population. Social media has been reported as one of the top reasons for smartphone use. In particular, social networking applications were found to be the second most downloaded type of application after games (Lim et al., 2015) and social networking accounted for the most application launches, representing 18% of the total number of launches in front of browser and search launches at 14% in recent assessments (Carrascal & Church, 2015). According to Statista (Statista, 2018), daily time spent on social networking is on the rise, reaching an estimated 135 min in 2017, representing an eighth of the time spent awake by an average adult. Social media has also been linked to SA in teenagers (Jeong, Kim, Yum, & Hwang, 2016) and adults (Enez Darcin et al., 2016). Large networks and high participation intensity in networks seem to contribute to the use of social media apps and SA (Salehan & Negahban, 2013). Facebook (Beyens, Frison, & Eggermont, 2016; Roberts et al., 2014; Ryan et al., 2014), Twitter (Roberts et al., 2014), and Instagram (Roberts et al., 2014) in particular have been identified as social media platforms whose use seems associated with SA. Recent work (Punyanunt-Carter et al., 2017), examining the user's relationship with the Snapchat social media app has posited that a user's functional and entertainment needs correlate with Internet Addiction. Interestingly, social media addiction also had the highest correlation to Internet Addiction (Montag et al., 2015a) in comparison to a number of addictions to other online activities: gaming, shopping, social networks, and pornography. We therefore hypothesise that high-scorers in SA have more UI interactions on social media applications (H2), with particular reference to Facebook, Instagram and Snapchat (Beyens et al., 2016; Punyanunt-Carter et al., 2017; Roberts et al., 2014; Ryan et al., 2014). This translates as a greater number of UI interactions registered across the “Social” apps category.

Closely related to social media, the communication function of smartphones has been retained as one of the central activities for users (Böhmer, Hecht, Johannes, Krüger, & Bauer, 2011; Carrascal & Church, 2015), despite the extended role of the smartphone as a pocket computer. Messaging (Lee et al., 2014a; Lemola et al., 2014; Yen et al., 2009) and phone calls (Roberts et al., 2014; Yen et al., 2009) have previously been associated with SA. Texting has also been found to be positively related to the mobile phone interfering with life (David, Kim, Brickman, Ran, & Curtis, 2015). In comparison to Facebook however, little evidence for links to SA have been found for Facebook Messenger and WhatsApp, two of the most popular communication apps worldwide. Overall, the messaging literature points to supporting the proposition that high-scorers in SA will have more UI interactions supporting communication activity (H3), detectable in our context from a larger number of user-generated events on the “Communication” apps category.

2.2. Summary of hypotheses

The hypotheses involve testing the extent to which UI interactions, at the most granular level possible, are indicative of SA. In summary:

H1

SA score is positively associated with the number of UI interactions with the device.

H2

SA score is positively associated with the number of UI interactions on apps of the Social app category, with particular reference to Facebook, Instagram, and Snapchat.

H3

SA score is positively associated with the number of UI interactions on apps of the Communication app category, with particular reference to WhatsApp and Facebook Messenger.

To the best of our knowledge, these hypotheses have not been previously tested and therefore the significance of UI interactions as an indicator of SA has not been established. We address the hypotheses in the context of an exploratory study, where wider observations are also presented. We investigate the hypotheses from three perspectives: the different types of UI interactions, as detected through machine-readable actions; the UI interactions with different apps and the UI interactions with different categories of apps. This also allows us to gain new potential insight into the sources of problematic behaviour and pathways to addiction. The specific interactions generated relate to tapping (short and long), writing, scrolling and text selection.

3. Methods

Seventy-six participants were recruited through posters and online advertisement at Cardiff University, UK. Participants were required to own a smartphone running Android 4.4 (KitKat) or higher and to have no history of mental illness. Composition of the full sample is presented in Table 1. Due to the voluntary nature of the participation in this study and the lack of control we had over participants' phones, certain participants were excluded as too little or no usage data was collected from them (e.g., due to the lack of space on their phone to store any data), resulting in the consideration of 64 participants in total. Thirty-four participants were male and 30 female, and they were aged between 19 and 46 (M=25.44,SD=5.87). A large sub-group are students (79%), who together with adolescents frequently make up samples for academic studies in this field (De-Sola Gutiérrez et al., 2016), consistent with their propensity to use smartphones. A significant proportion (41%) were employed.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic characteristics and Smartphone Addition scores (N = 64).

Age
M
SD
Years
25.4
5.87
Gender
N
%
Male 34 53.13
Female
30
46.88
Employment
N
%
Student 38 59.38
Student & employed 13 20.31
Employed 12 18.75
Unemployed
1
1.56
Education
N
%
High school, no diploma 1 1.56
High school diploma or the equivalent 5 7.81
Trade/technical/vocational training 1 1.56
Some undergraduate education, no degree 14 21.88
Bachelor's degree 19 29.69
Master's degree 21 32.81
Doctorate 2 3.13
No answer
1
1.56
Smartphone Addiction Scale -long version
M
SD
Score
87.8
20.26
Smartphone Addiction Scale -short version
N
%
Addicted 12 18.75
Not addicted 52 81.25

All participants were required to attend a briefing and a debriefing session. In the briefing session, participants gave their informed consent and downloaded our custom smartphone application Tymer. They were introduced to its functionality and were asked to keep the app installed and use it over a period of 8 weeks (56 days). In the debriefing session, participants received monetary compensation. In both briefing and debriefing sessions, participants were asked to complete 5 surveys: the Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS) (Kwon et al., 2013b), the Positive And Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Watson & Clark, 1994), the Big Five Inventory (BFI) (John & Srivastava, 1999), the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ) (Kirby & Petry, 2004), and a demographics and smartphone use questionnaire. The Smartphone Addiction Scale was the focus of assessment in this particular work, which is part of a wider set of analyses (Noë et al., 2017; Turner et al., 2019).

The SAS (Kwon et al., 2013b) was given the highest reliability score of the scales evaluated by De-Sola Gutiérrez and colleagues in their review (De-Sola Gutiérrez et al., 2016) and presents 33 questions each using a 6-point Likert scale. Total possible scores therefore range from 33 to 198, with a higher score indicating a higher likelihood of addiction. In our analyses, we use the score as a discrete variable. A shortened version (SAS-SV), composed of a subset of 10-questions is also available (Kwon et al., 2013a), for which the cut-off values of 31 for male and 33 for female participants have been tentatively proposed to determine SA. The responses of our participants to this subset of questions were therefore also evaluated separately and 18.75% of our sample was found to have reached this threshold (see Table 1).

A paired t-test was carried out to check whether the participant population's results for the Smartphone Addiction Scale tests had significantly changed between briefing (M=89.39,SD=23.41) and debriefing sessions (M=86.28,SD=19.22). This was not the case (p=.077) and the test values were highly correlated amongst themselves (r=.807,n=64,p<.001). For this reason, we used the mean of both tests for the analyses. The age and gender of participants are also taken into account in this study.

During the course of the study, the bespoke Tymer app functioned on each participant's device to accomplish two things. Firstly, to prompt the user for feedback on their current mood state (not featured in this analysis) and secondly, to track user interactions with their device. Each interaction is referred to as an event, recorded alongside a time stamp and the source of the event (i.e., the application with which the user interacts). The participants used Tymer for an average of 53.08 days (SD = 18.74).

Only UI interaction events were considered, excluding events that could be ambiguous (i.e., triggered by either the user or the smartphone, such as a screen off event, that can be caused by the user switching off the screen but also by the smartphone timing out the display). This was done to avoid including smartphone generated events as they do not constitute a real UI interaction, but also to exclude passive usage (e.g., streaming a video, using the phone as navigation system) which can count towards screen time, but does not constitute active interaction with the smartphone. The specific types of events considered are: tap, long tap, writing (e.g., striking the keyboard), scroll, and text selection (i.e., highlighting). We subsequently refer to the sum of all of these events together as “overall smartphone use”. Note large numbers of events were recorded over the period (Fig. 1), with scrolling making a particularly significant contribution.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Average number of daily events per event type and gender.

In Fig. 2 we present descriptive statistics concerning smartphone usage, for users classified by their addiction status, as determined by the SAS-SV (Kwon et al., 2013a). In the left panel, users are ordered by descending daily time spent on the smartphone, and notably, addiction is widely spread across the distribution. The same data is presented in the right panel, but with the users ordered by decreasing number of daily UI interactions. Here there appears to be increased clustering of SA for users at the head of the distribution. The relationship between daily time on the smartphone and UI interactions is further expressed in Fig. 3. Note that for the smartphone addicted sub-group, there is much greater variance in UI interactions, as compared to daily time on the smartphone.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Daily usage of non-addicted and addicted users, N=64.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Daily usage of non-addicted and addicted users by time spent on phone, n=64.

3.1. Analysis design

From comparing the number of events between participants, we analyse differences in participants' usage behaviour. The sum of events was chosen as the principle variable rather than a total session time length since it requires no interpretation of the data and represents the intensity of smartphone-user engagement. The data was normalised to obtain the average daily number of events, to account for any deviations in participation, such as from choosing to opt out or a delayed start. It should be noted that different apps generate different types and volumes of events. However this is not an issue in this study since we are principally concerned with analysing inter-personal differences relating to the same app or category of app.

Fig. 4 shows how user behaviour can be captured through total events of different types, that occur within different apps belonging to different categories of apps. These provide different levels of aggregation for a user's interaction and we consider their correlation with SA.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

The relationship between events, apps and categories of smartphone applications.

Events within app categories capture broad user behaviour, and can be modelled by the approximate taxonomy provided by the Google Play Store. No changes were made to these categories, with the exception of game app subcategories that were aggregated into a single Game category. Uncategorised apps were not included in the analyses pertaining to app categories. To allow cross comparison, app categories used by less than half of the participants were excluded, resulting in a list of 21 app categories. Considering events within apps provides insight into behaviour aligned with a particular bundle of functionality. Again, only sources from popular apps were included; apps that were absent from at least half of the participant's data and brand specific apps were excluded, resulting in the consideration of 29 apps (see Fig. 5). The total number of events across all apps was computed for each event type in isolation. These are potentially indicative of variations in overall usage between users. The types of events considered relate to a tap, long tap, writing, scroll, and text selection.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Percentage of participants having at least one recorded UI interaction event, i.e., being active on popular apps, n=64.

Gender and age are known characteristics likely to influence smartphone usage (De-Sola Gutiérrez et al., 2016; Roberts et al., 2014). To account for these confounding factors in each analysis, partial Spearman correlations with SA were first carried out separately for each gender, using age as a covariate. If the correlation coefficient was of the same sign, the analysis was repeated combining both male and female participants, and using both age and gender as covariates.

To correct for possible occurrence of type I errors, a False Discovery Rate (FDR) method was applied using the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure (Benjamini & Yosef, 1995).

4. Results

Participants received a mean score of 87.84 (SD=20.28) on the SAS. Neither gender was more or less likely to be addicted to smartphones than the other (t62=.257,p=.614). The mean SA score obtained by male participants was 89.01 (SD=21.10), while it was 86.51 (SD=19.57) for female participants. Moreover, age was not significantly correlated to SA (r=.219,n=64,p=.082).

When the daily amount of time spent on smartphones was evaluated in relation to SA, no significant association was found for males r=.073,p=.685, females r=.351,p=.062, and across gender r=.150,p=.244. This formalises the observations made on the descriptive statistics in Section 3 (Fig. 2) where SA is seen across participants with wide ranging daily time on the smartphone. This may indicate that passive usage, characterised by high time periods using the smartphone with low levels of UI interaction, could be a common use case in smartphone usage. This provides evidence that alone, time on the smartphone does not necessarily provide a strong indicator of problematic smartphone behaviour, and further motivates the analysis of UI interaction as an indicator of SA, as considered in the following analyses.

4.1. UI interactions in app categories and Smartphone Addiction

From considering total events by app category, analysis was undertaken based on gender, assessing each gender in isolation and also together (Table 2). These analyses allow investigation of Hypotheses H2 and H3.

Table 2.

Application categories use correlation with Smartphone Addiction for male (a), female (b), and all users (c).

App Categories M
F
M+F
r p r p r p
Auto And Vehicles .230 .198 .013 .948 .105 .416
Business .066 .713 .228 .234 .159 .216
Communication .100 .581 .011 .953 .048 .711
Entertainment .068 .708 .075 .700 .068 .601
Finance .068 .708 .187 .332 .200 .118
Game .062 .730 .093 .631 .109 .401
Health And Fitness .131 .468 -.066 .732
Lifestyle .230 .199 .680 .000a .446 .000a
Maps And Navigation .284 .109 .337 .074 .322 .011
Music And Audio .007 .971 .169 .381 .062 .630
News And Magazines .205 .252 .195 .310 .198 .122
Personalization .111 .537 .270 .157 .151 .241
Photography .165 .360 .011 .957 .117 .364
Productivity .148 .412 .036 .851 .120 .353
Shopping .064 .724 .142 .464 .085 .510
Social .364 .037 .445 .016 .421 .001a
Sports .234 .191 .014 .944 .123 .341
Tools .495 .003 -.117 .546
Travel And Local .177 .323 .040 .835 .143 .268
Video Players .098 .586 .085 .661 .090 .488

p < .05.

a

FDR < .05.

For female participants, SA was linked to Lifestyle (r=.680,p<.001) apps. When both genders were considered together, correlations were found with Lifestyle (r=.446,p=.001) and Social (r=.421,p=.001) app categories. This is consistent with previous findings (Montag et al., 2015a) concerning social media addiction.

These results support Hypothesis H2, however a significant correlation with social apps only occurs when combined analysis is performed on gender; this is likely due to limitation in sample size for the gender specific analyses. Interestingly, the results do not support H3, with the Communication app category not being significantly correlated to SA. It may well be that the multifaceted functionality of social media is permitting new channels through which traditional text based-communication can be undertaken.

4.2. UI interactions within apps and Smartphone Addiction

While considering the total UI interaction events generated in each app in isolation, analysis was undertaken based on gender, assessing each gender separately, followed by analyses across genders. Table 3 shows the correlation scores for the apps relevant to Hypotheses H2 and H3 and from the app categories that were found to be significantly correlated to SA. Consistent with the previous analysis of app categories, none of the Communication apps were significantly correlated to SA. However, Snapchat, an app from the Social category, was found to be significantly correlated to SA (r=.386,p=.002). This is an interesting finding, consistent with Snapchat providing more than only functional user experience relating to pleasure and entertainment. However, in contrast to previous research (Punyanunt-Carter et al., 2017), addiction to Snapchat appears more significant than as for other social media applications.

Table 3.

Application use correlation with Smartphone Addiction for male (a), female (b), and all users (c) for a subset of analysed apps.

Apps M
F
M+F
r p r p r p
Communication
Contacts .459 .007 -.182 .344
Facebook Messenger .204 .254 -.129 .505
Gmail -.037 .840 -.033 .867 -.037 .777
Google Chrome .177 .325 -.135 .486
Google Hangouts .084 .642 .126 .516 .058 .655
Skype .045 .805 -.318 .092
WeChat .094 .602 .281 .139 .183 .155
WhatsApp .159 .376 .331 .079 .210 .102

Social
Facebook .324 .066 .176 .361 .309 .014
Instagram .358 .041 -.017 .931
Snapchat .326 .064 .548 .002 .386 .002a

Lifestyle
Tinder .086 .634 .203 .292 .121 .348

p < .05.

a

FDR < .05.

These results partially support Hypothesis H2, but the non-significant findings for the social media applications Facebook and Instagram were not foreseen. While Snapchat is not classified in the app store as a Communication application, aspects of its functionality lend themselves to a process of targeted information exchange, lending some partial but weak support for Hypothesis H3.

It is also notable that, while positive correlations were found for the Lifestyle category, Tinder, the only app analysed in isolation from this category had no significant correlation to SA.

4.3. UI event types and Smartphone Addiction

To investigate Hypothesis H1, UI interaction events were considered by each different type (tap, long tap, writing, scroll, text selection and all combined, see Table 4). When both genders were combined, overall usage (r=.280,p=.028) and scrolling (r=.284,p=.025) events were found to be positively correlated to SA. However, these results were not significant for FDR < 0.05. This partially supports Hypothesis H1.

Table 4.

Correlation between app usage per event type and Smartphone Addiction for male (a), female (b), and all users (c) for a subset of analysed apps for which at least one p < .05 result was found.

Event type App M
F
M+F
r p r p r p
All
all
.303
.087
.253
.186
.280
.028
Tap all .170 .344 .241 .207 .226 .078
Contacts .427 .013 -.187 .331
Facebook .363 .038 .103 .596 .307 .015
Google Quick Search Box .344 .050 .242 .207 .315 .013
Instagram .345 .049 -.004 .984
Snapchat
.322
.068
.645
.000a
.459
.000a
Long tap all .142 .431 .241 .207 .210 .101
Contacts .485 .004a -.069 .720
Facebook .321 .068 .105 .587 .260 .042
Google Quick Search Box .284 .109 .307 .106 .283 .026
Snapchat
.240
.178
.498
.006
.368
.003a
Writing all -.053 .770 .327 .083
Contacts .496 .003 -.226 .239
Facebook .273 .124 .198 .304 .259 .042
Google Play Store .320 .069 .240 .210 .277 .030
Google Quick Search Box .412 .017 -.067 .728
Snapchat .281 .113 .620 .000a .432 .000a
Spotify
.106
.557
.481
.008
.261
.040
Scroll all .406 .019 .159 .410 .284 .025
Facebook .285 .024 .159 .409 .295 .020
Google Quick Search Box .265 .037 -.066 .734
Settings .359 .004 .246 .198 .326 .010
Snapchat .346 .006 .459 .012 .347 .006
WhatsApp
.260
.042
.322
.088
.276
.030
Text selection all .123 .497 .295 .120 .142 .270
Contacts .419 .015 -.343 .068
Facebook .373 .032 .119 .537 .287 .024
Google Play Store .404 .020 .168 .385 .316 .012
Google Quick Search Box .456 .008 -.074 .705
Snapchat .323 .067 .536 .003 .411 .001a
Spotify .153 .396 .418 .024 .262 .039

p < .05.

a

FDR < .05.

We note that even though tap, long tap, writing, and text selection events were not significantly associated with SA when all app data was considered, they were in the context of Snapchat. This is likely due to the overall strong correlation of Snapchat usage, regardless of the interaction type. This is especially true for female users.

Interestingly, long tap events (used to bring up context menus) in the Contacts built-in application were significantly correlated to SA for male users only. The strong correlations found for other event types (albeit not significant for FDR < .05) suggest that this result might be due to the overall correlation of Contacts use and SA. No significant correlations were however found for scroll events, which is of particular interest as they are the most used type of UI interaction overall (see Fig. 1). We also note that no significant correlation could be established for female users.

These results further provide some support for Hypotheses H1 and H2, with weak support for H3 when male usage of the Contacts app and the communication function of Snapchat is considered. Again it is notable that as compared to Facebook and Instagram, interaction with Snapchat has a more statistically significant correlation.

5. Discussion

The results provide some evidence for Hypothesis H1, noting that overall usage and scrolling events appear positively correlated to SA, when taken on aggregate (Section 4.3), although they do not withstand FDR correction. When considering events by their source application (Section 4.2), Snapchat was repeatedly found to be an origin from which particular events significantly positively correlate with SA. This also supports social media being a source of high activity for smartphone addicts (Hypothesis H2). Indeed, we find that across the categories of apps (Section 4.1), level of engagement (i.e., total user-generated events) in the Social category significantly correlates with SA, in support of Hypothesis H2. It is notable that the Lifestyle category is also significantly correlated with SA (Section 4.1). However, the findings in support of H2 are limited to correlation with activity in Snapchat, rather than a broader range of social media applications. This appears to provide a differential between Snapchat and other forms of social media, which was not anticipated based on the previous literature. Little support for Hypothesis H3 was found, which may be due to social media apps implicitly providing communication functionalities. However, usage of popular messaging apps, such as WhatsApp, was not found to be significantly correlated with SA.

Correlation between Lifestyle apps and SA was not hypothesised to be significant. Tinder is the only Lifestyle app that was analysed in isolation in our app analyses, however no significant relationship was found between its use and SA, for both genders. Given the diversity of potential Lifestyle apps, it is therefore difficult to establish any common characteristics of the Lifestyle apps used that contribute to correlation with SA, especially in the case of female users (r=.680). Since Lifestyle apps are diverse and indeed linked to a certain lifestyle that might widely differ per individual, it is perhaps the higher interaction with personalised, non-mainstream apps that is the basis of SA in this context. The gender difference might be explained by the greater brand commitment women showcase (Tifferet & Ram, 2012), the difference between how male and female users personalise their phones (Tossell, Kortum, Shepard, Ahmad, & Zhong, 2012), and that a large portion of top apps seem to target topics which in general, women display more interest in than men (Daly, Hogg, Sacks, Smith, & Zimring, 1983; Fichten & Sunerton, 1983).

Focusing on the findings for social media however, the results suggest that assessment of the user's device interaction is a useful proxy for Smartphone Addiction assessment. Importantly, the results show utility in observing UI interactions at a detailed level, and provide a means to filter false positive results that occur due to passive use of the smartphone. Our findings are consistent with the general literature that correlates social media usage with SA (David et al., 2015; Enez Darcin et al., 2016; Jeong et al., 2016; Ryan et al., 2014; Salehan & Negahban, 2013) and the fear of missing out (FoMO) (Alt, 2015; Przybylski, Murayama, DeHaan, & Gladwell, 2013), which has been previously aligned with problematic usage (Elhai et al., 2016). However, the prominence of Snapchat, relative to other social media apps, is a particularly interesting aspect in our results. This is significant because Snapchat is heavily used in the wider population, and is attractive to teenagers (Statista, 2016). While wider concerns have been voiced in the wider press on the addictive nature of Snapchat (Andersson, 2018; Kallie, 2018; Stewart, 2018; White, 2017), to the best of our knowledge, limited academic research has been conducted to date (Punyanunt-Carter et al., 2017).

5.1. Pathways to addiction

Smartphone usage provides pathways to addiction on several distinct levels. Firstly, the ease of access combined with the widespread and socially accepted use of smartphones, makes smartphone use ubiquitous. Secondly, the increasing number of functionalities smartphone (apps) offer, make users more reliant on the technology and incentivise smartphone usage over analog options or other digital devices. Thirdly, apps are designed to make users prolong their usage (e.g., through “infinite scrolling”, which lacks any stopping cues) or come back to them (e.g., notifications or daily rewards urging the user to open up the app). In our fine-grained analysis of UI interaction events within applications, we have attempted to look more closely at this third phenomenon. Observing UI interaction directly engages the potential problematic behaviour as a signal. This also reduces the opportunity for false positive problematic use classification to arise for users who have high levels of passive usage, such as occurs through streaming media or using the smartphone as a GPS device.

Beyond interaction with the device, the bundled nature of apps gives a multitude of ways through which app design and user experience may further contribute to problematic behaviour. FoMO is applicable across all social media apps and therefore characteristics beyond this are relevant. In particular, it is conceivable that the particular design of the Snapchat app contributes to the significant correlations that are repeatedly evident in our analyses, as compared to other social media apps (e.g., Facebook, Instagram).

We note that Snapchat is distinct from other social media in providing multimedia messaging whose main feature is the ephemeral nature of the text, picture or video messages. Snapchat has a combination of design features that promote high frequency usage. For example, “friend emojis” appear next to a user's friends' names only if they “snap” each other regularly and feedback is provided on when content has been consumed by the recipient. The app's functionality, in terms of filters used to visually edit content, also regularly changes. The self-deleting nature of content and the way in which messages are sent creates a perception of control and privacy (Utz, Muscanell, & Khalid, 2015). Incentives are also provided for users to increase their snapping frequency (i.e., send new content), by effectively making friends compete against each other for top position and incentivising them not to break their “Snapstreak” (which is only maintained if both interaction partners have sent each other a Snap within the last 24 hours). This latter form of gamification promotes usage, but it may also promote addiction (Hellman, Schoenmakers, Nordstrom, & Van Holst, 2013). Additionally, in contrast to some other social media platforms, Snapchat is only available for smartphones (as opposed to a general web application), channelling all usage through the handset.

Interestingly, the Snapchat app also connects with entertainment aspects of social communication, with “chatting through pictures” providing a strong emotional context as compared to text (Vaterlaus, Barnett, Roche, & Young, 2016). The combined need for the app to support fun and functionality has been previously linked to Snapchat addiction (Punyanunt-Carter et al., 2017), when Snapchat usage is considered in isolation. Based on our findings, we hypothesise that the design features of Snapchat, relative to other social media apps, are providing a strengthened environment for SA to take hold.

5.2. Limitations

Although equal or greater in sample size than other observational and task based studies (e.g. (Andrist, Ruis, & Shaffer, 2018; Marquet, Alberico, & Hipp, 2018; Peleg-Adler, Lanir, & Korman, 2018; Wang & Hung, 2018)), it is possible that, given the power of the present study, we did not detect all true associations, and thus replications with larger samples may reveal additional findings. We also note that due to the properties of different smartphone operating systems, only Android users were selected for this study. The nature of data collection through smartphones, based on an app, means that data collection was beyond our immediate control, leading to exclusion of certain participants or missing data (e.g., due to a low battery preventing the app from recording and/or sending data). Lastly, we acknowledge that this study was conducted through a non-clinical sample, recruited largely in a higher education environment. We thus would not infer that the smartphone usage levels and/or SAS scores indicate any impairment of everyday functioning.

6. Conclusion

We have introduced a new approach to monitoring user activity for the detection of problematic smartphone behaviour, by considering all the UI interactions made by a user. As compared to other measures, this directly engages the potential problematic physical behaviour as a signal. This approach also allows for a focus on exclusively active rather than passive smartphone activity. Our data indicates that the time a smartphone is active may not be a significant correlate of problematic behaviour, which is symptomatic of the smartphone being used for an increasingly wide variety of applications. Within heavy and diverse usage, a more detailed analysis of behaviour allows the potential sources of addiction to be better understood. Scrolling behaviour in particular, is worthy of further investigation.

Through this approach, using an exploratory study, we have been able to analyse indicators of Smartphone Addiction across a sample of smartphone users (N=64) over a period of 8 weeks. This has identified that Smartphone Addiction is associated with the usage of Lifestyle and Social applications. We have also discovered that Snapchat, one of the most popular social media applications in use today, may be particularly indicative of problematic smartphone usage, indicated across all types of UI interactions. To date, few academic studies have identified such issues and this is worthy of further consideration, particularly given the significant usage of Snapchat by teenagers. Preliminary analysis of the Snapchat app, compared to other social media, leads us to hypothesise that the app's design provides a particularly strong pathway towards Smartphone Addiction.

Research on the addictive elements of these apps, and in particular of Snapchat, is worth pursuing. Determining the direction of causality and further examining social and environmental factors that could be at play would be of great interest.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Wellcome Trust for funding this project through an Institutional Strategic Support Fund to Cardiff University and all participants who took part in our study.

References

  1. Alavi S.S., Ferdosi M., Jannatifard F., Eslami M., Alaghemandan H., Setare M. Behavioral addiction versus substance addiction: Correspondence of psychiatric and psychological views. International Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2012;3(4):290–294. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alt D. College students' academic motivation, media engagement and fear of missing out. Computers in Human Behavior. 2015;49:111–119. [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersson H. Jul 2018. Social media apps are ’deliberately’ addictive to users. [Google Scholar]
  4. Andrist S., Ruis A.R., Shaffer D.W. A network analytic approach to gaze coordination during a collaborative task. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018;89:339–348. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bae S.-man. The relationship between the type of smartphone use and smartphone dependence of Korean adolescents: National survey study. Children and Youth Services Review. 2017;81:207–211. [Google Scholar]
  6. Benjamini Y., Yosef H. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological) 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
  7. Beyens I., Frison E., Eggermont S. I don't want to miss a thing”: Adolescents' fear of missing out and its relationship to adolescents' social needs, Facebook use, and Facebook related stress. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;64:1–8. [Google Scholar]
  8. Block J.J. 2008. Issues for DSM-V: Internet addiction. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Böhmer M., Hecht B., Johannes S., Krüger A., Bauer G. Proceedings of the 13th international conference on Human computer interaction with mobile devices and services. ACM; 2011. Falling asleep with angry birds, facebook and kindle: A large scale study on mobile application usage; pp. 47–56. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cao H., Lin M. Pervasive and Mobile Computing; 2017. Mining smartphone data for app usage prediction and recommendations: A survey. [Google Scholar]
  11. Carrascal J.P., Church K. Vol. 1. 2015. An in-situ study of mobile app & mobile search interactions; pp. 2739–2748. (Proceedings of the ACM CHI’15 conference on human factors in computing systems). [Google Scholar]
  12. Chae S.W., Lee K.C. An empirical analysis of the effect of smartphone use on addiction: Usage and gratification approach. Information. 2011;14(9):3113–3126. [Google Scholar]
  13. Chotpitayasunondh V., Douglas K.M. How ”phubbing” becomes the norm: The antecedents and consequences of snubbing via smartphone. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;63:9–18. [Google Scholar]
  14. Clark A., Chalmers D. The extended mind. Analysis. 1998;58(1):7–19. [Google Scholar]
  15. Daly J.A., Hogg E., Sacks D., Smith M., Zimring L. Sex and relationship affect social self-grooming. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 1983;7(3):183–189. [Google Scholar]
  16. David P., Kim J.H., Brickman J.S., Ran W., Curtis C.M. New Media & Society. 2015;17(10):1661–1679. [Google Scholar]
  17. De-Sola Gutiérrez J., Rodríguez de Fonseca F., Rubio G. Cell-phone addiction: A review. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2016;7 doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Demirci K., Akgönül M., Akpinar A. Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2015;4(2):85–92. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Elhai J.D., Dvorak R.D., Levine J.C., Hall B.J. Problematic smartphone use: A conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2017;207:251–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Elhai J.D., Levine J.C., Dvorak R.D., Hall B.J. Fear of missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;63:509–516. [Google Scholar]
  21. Enez Darcin A., Kose S., Noyan C.O., Nurmedov S., Ylmaz O., Dilbaz N. Smartphone addiction and its relationship with social anxiety and loneliness. Behaviour & Information Technology. 2016;35(7):520–525. [Google Scholar]
  22. Falaki H., Mahajan R., Kandula S., Lymberopoulos D., Govindan R., Estrin D. Proceedings of the 8th international conference on Mobile systems, applications, and services. ACM; 2010. Diversity in smartphone usage; pp. 179–194. [Google Scholar]
  23. Ferreira D., Goncalves J., Kostakos V., Barkhuus L., Dey A.K. Proceedings of the 16th international conference on Human-computer interaction with mobile devices & services. ACM; 2014. Contextual experience sampling of mobile application micro-usage; pp. 91–100. [Google Scholar]
  24. Fichten C.S., Sunerton B. Popular horoscopes and the Barnum effect. The Journal of Psychology. 1983;114:123–134. [Google Scholar]
  25. Gokul C., Blom J., Gatica-Perez D. Mining large-scale smartphone data for personality studies. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing. 2013;17(3):433–450. [Google Scholar]
  26. Griffiths M.D. Facebook addiction: Concerns, criticism, and Recommendations—A response to Andreassen and colleagues. Psychological Reports. 2012;110(2):518–520. doi: 10.2466/01.07.18.PR0.110.2.518-520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ha J.H., Chin B., Park D.-H., Ryu S.-H., Yu J. Characteristics of excessive cellular phone use in Korean adolescents. CyberPsychology and Behavior. 2008;11(6):783–784. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hellman M., Schoenmakers T.M., Nordstrom B.R., Van Holst R.J. Is there such a thing as online video game addiction? A cross-disciplinary review. Addiction Research and Theory. 2013;21(2):102–112. [Google Scholar]
  29. Hintze D., Rainhard D.F., Scholz S., Mayrhofer R. Proceedings of the 12th international conference on advances in mobile computing and multimedia. ACM; 2014. Mobile device usage characteristics: The effect of context and form factor on locked and unlocked usage; pp. 105–114. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hoffner C.A., Lee S., Park S.J. I miss my mobile phone!: Self-expansion via mobile phone and responses to phone loss. New Media & Society. 2016;18(11):2452–2468. [Google Scholar]
  31. James D., Drennan J. Australian and New Zealand marketing academy conference, Perth, Australia. 2005. Exploring addictive consumption of mobile phone technology. [Google Scholar]
  32. Jeong S.H., Kim H.J., Yum J.Y., Hwang Y. What type of content are smartphone users addicted to?: SNS vs. games. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;54:10–17. [Google Scholar]
  33. Jesdabodi C., Maalej W. Proceedings of the 2015 ACM international joint conference on pervasive and ubiquitous computing. ACM; 2015. Understanding usage states on mobile devices; pp. 1221–1225. [Google Scholar]
  34. John O.P., Srivastava S. The Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. Handbook of personality: Theory and Research. 1999;2:102–138. [Google Scholar]
  35. Kallie K. Dec 2018. Don't put out the fire- a Snapchat streak addiction. [Google Scholar]
  36. Kimberly S.Y. Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder. CyberPsychology and Behavior. 1998;1(3):237–244. [Google Scholar]
  37. King M.F., Bruner G.C. Social desirability bias: A neglected aspect of validity testing. Psychology and Marketing. 2000;17(2):79–103. [Google Scholar]
  38. Kirby K.N., Petry N.M. Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction. 2004;99(4):461–471. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00669.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kwon M., Kim D.-J., Cho H., Yang S. The smartphone addiction scale: Development and validation of a short version for adolescents. PLoS One. 2013;8(12) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kwon M., Lee J.Y., Won W.Y., Park J.W., Min J.A., Hahn C. Development and validation of a smartphone addiction scale (SAS) PLoS One. 2013;8(2) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lee Y.K., Chang C.T., You L., Cheng Z.H. The dark side of smartphone usage: Psychological traits, compulsive behavior and technostress. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014;31(1):373–383. [Google Scholar]
  42. Lee U., Lee J., Ko M., Lee C., Kim Y., Yang S. Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems. ACM; 2014. Hooked on smartphones: An exploratory study on smartphone overuse among college students; pp. 2327–2336. [Google Scholar]
  43. Lemola S., Perkinson-Gloor N., Brand S., Julia F., Dewald-Kaufmann, Grob A. Adolescents' electronic media use at night, sleep disturbance, and depressive symptoms in the smartphone age. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2014;44(2):405–418. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0176-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. LiKamWa R., Liu Y., Lane N.D., Zhong L. Proceeding of the 11th annual international conference on Mobile systems, applications, and services. ACM; 2013. Moodscope: Building a mood sensor from smartphone usage patterns; pp. 389–402. [Google Scholar]
  45. Lim S.L., Bentley P.J., Kanakam N., Ishikawa F., Honiden S. Investigating country differences in mobile app user behavior and challenges for software engineering. IEEE Transactions on Software Engineering. 2015;41(1):40–64. [Google Scholar]
  46. Lin Y.-H., Chang L.-R., Lee Y.-H., Tseng H.-W., Terry B., Kuo J. Development and validation of the smartphone addiction inventory (SPAI) PLoS One. 2014;9(6) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Marquet O., Alberico C., Hipp A.J. Pokemon Go and physical activity among college students. A study using ecological momentary assessment. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018;81:215–222. [Google Scholar]
  48. Montag C., Bey K., Peng S., Li M., Chen Y.F., Liu W.Y. Is it meaningful to distinguish between generalized and specific Internet addiction? Evidence from a cross-cultural study from Germany, Sweden, Taiwan and China. Asia-Pacific Psychiatry. 2015;7(1):20–26. doi: 10.1111/appy.12122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Montag C., Błaszkiewicz K., Sariyska R., Lachmann B., Andone I., Trendafilov B. Smartphone usage in the 21st century: Who is active on WhatsApp? BMC Research Notes. 2015;8(331) doi: 10.1186/s13104-015-1280-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Noë B., Turner L.D., Linden D.E.J., Allen S.M., Maio G.R., Whitaker R.M. Timing rather than user traits mediates mood sampling on smartphones. BMC Research Notes. 2017;10(481) doi: 10.1186/s13104-017-2808-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Oulasvirta A., Rattenbury T., Ma L., Raita E. Habits make smartphone use more pervasive. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing. 2012;16(1):105–114. [Google Scholar]
  52. Park W.K. Mobile communications. Springer; 2005. Mobile phone addiction; pp. 253–272. [Google Scholar]
  53. Pearson C., Hussain Z. Smartphone addiction and associated psychological factors. Addicta: The Turkish Journal of Addictions. 2016;3(2):1–15. [Google Scholar]
  54. Peleg-Adler R., Lanir J., Korman M. The effects of aging on the use of handheld augmented reality in a route planning task. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018;81:52–62. [Google Scholar]
  55. Peng X., Zhao Y.C., Zhu Q. Investigating user switching intention for mobile instant messaging application: Taking WeChat as an example. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;64:206–216. [Google Scholar]
  56. Przybylski A.K., Murayama K., DeHaan C.R., Gladwell V. Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013;29(4):1841–1848. [Google Scholar]
  57. Punyanunt-Carter N.M., De La Cruz J.J., Wrench J.S. Investigating the relationships among college students' satisfaction, addiction, needs, communication apprehension, motives, and uses & gratifications with Snapchat. Computers in Human Behavior. 2017;75:870–875. [Google Scholar]
  58. Roberts J.A., Yaya L.H.P., Manolis C. The invisible addiction: Cell-phone activities and addiction among male and female college students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2014;3(4):254–265. doi: 10.1556/JBA.3.2014.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rozgonjuk D., Levine J.C., Hall B.J., Elhai J.D. The association between problematic smartphone use, depression and anxiety symptom severity, and objectively measured smartphone use over one week. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018;87:10–17. [Google Scholar]
  60. Ryan T., Chester A., Reece J., Xenos S. The uses and abuses of Facebook: A review of Facebook addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2014;3(3):133–148. doi: 10.1556/JBA.3.2014.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Salehan M., Negahban A. Social networking on smartphones: When mobile phones become addictive. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013;29:2632–2639. [Google Scholar]
  62. Shin C., Dey A.K. Proceedings of the 2013 ACM international joint conference on Pervasive and ubiquitous computing. ACM; 2013. Automatically detecting problematic use of smartphones; pp. 335–344. number September. [Google Scholar]
  63. Stachl C., Hilbert S., Au J.Q., Buschek D., Luca A.De, Bischl B. Personality traits predict smartphone usage. European Journal of Personality. 2017;31(6):701–722. [Google Scholar]
  64. Stanton P., Brodsky A. 1975. Love and addiction. [Google Scholar]
  65. Statista . 2016. Snapchat user demographics: Distribution of Snapchat users in the United States as of February 2016, by age. [Google Scholar]
  66. Statista . 2018. Daily time spent on social networking by internet users worldwide from 2012 to 2017. (in minutes) [Google Scholar]
  67. Stewart T. Jul 2018. Facebook, Snapchat, Twitter like ”behavioural Cocaine”, say silicon valley insiders. [Google Scholar]
  68. Tifferet S., Ram H. Gender differences in brand commitment, impulse buying, and hedonic consumption. The Journal of Product and Brand Management. 2012;21(3):176–182. [Google Scholar]
  69. Tossel C., Kortum P., Shepard C., Rahmati A., Zhong L. Exploring smartphone addiction: Insights from long-term telemetric behavioral measures. International Journal of Interactive Mobile Technologies (iJIM) 2015;9(2):37–43. [Google Scholar]
  70. Tossell C.C., Kortum P., Shepard C., Ahmad R., Zhong L. An empirical analysis of smartphone personalisation: Measurement and user variability. Behaviour & Information Technology. 2012;31(10):995–1010. [Google Scholar]
  71. Turner L.D., Allen S.M., Whitaker R.M. Proceedings of the 2015 ACM international joint conference on pervasive and ubiquitous computing. ACM; 2015. Interruptibility prediction for ubiquitous systems: Conventions and new directions from a growing field; pp. 801–812. [Google Scholar]
  72. Turner L.D., Allen S.M., Whitaker R.M. Reachable but not receptive: Enhancing smartphone interruptibility prediction by modelling the extent of user engagement with notifications. Pervasive and Mobile Computing. 2017;40:480–494. [Google Scholar]
  73. Turner L.D., Whitaker R.M., Allen S.M., Linden D.E.J., Tu K., Li J. Evidence to support common application switching behaviour on smartphones. Royal Society Open Science. 2019;6(3) doi: 10.1098/rsos.190018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Utz S., Muscanell N., Khalid C. Snapchat elicits more jealousy than Facebook: A comparison of Snapchat and Facebook use. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace. 2015;18(3):141–146. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Vaterlaus J.M., Barnett K., Roche C., Young J.A. Snapchat is more personal: An exploratory study on Snapchat behaviors and young adult interpersonal relationships. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;62:594–601. [Google Scholar]
  76. Wang C.-C., Hung J.C. Comparative analysis of advertising attention to Facebook social network: Evidence from eye-movement data. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018 https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0747563218303820 in press, corrected proof. [Google Scholar]
  77. Watson D., Clark L.A. 1994. The PANAS-X manual for the positive and negative affect Schedule -Expanded form. Unpublished manuscript, University of Iowa, Iowa City. [Google Scholar]
  78. White S. Mar 2017. Snapchat addiction could lead to serious health problems as users can't get enough of the site. [Google Scholar]
  79. Woojae K., Park B.W., Chang Lee K. Measuring smartphone dependence: A first step with emphasis on factor analytic evidence. Information. 2011;14(9):3031–3047. [Google Scholar]
  80. World Health Organization International Classification of Diseases. 2018;11 [Google Scholar]
  81. Yen C.-F., Tang T.-C., Yen J.-Y., Lin H.-C., Huang C.-F., Liu S.-C. Symptoms of problematic cellular phone use, functional impairment and its association with depression among adolescents in Southern Taiwan. Journal of Adolescence. 2009;32(4):863–873. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES