Abstract
Background:
Humans typically walk in ways that minimize energy cost. Recent work has found that healthy adults will even adopt new ways of walking when a new pattern costs less energy. This suggests potential for rehabilitation to drive changes in walking by altering the energy costs of walking patterns so that the desired pattern becomes energetically optimal (i.e., costs least energy of all available patterns).
Objective:
We aimed to change gait symmetry in healthy adults and persons post-stroke by creating environments where changing symmetry allowed the participants to save energy.
Methods:
Across three experiments, we tested healthy adults (n=12 in Experiment 1, n=20 in Experiment 2) and persons post-stroke (n=7 in Experiment 3) in a novel treadmill environment that linked asymmetric stepping and gait speed – two factors that influence energy cost – to create situations where walking with one’s preferred gait symmetry (or asymmetry, in the case of the persons post-stroke) was no longer the least energetically costly way to walk.
Results:
Across the three experiments, we found that most participants changed their gait when experiencing the new energy landscape. Healthy adults often adopted an asymmetric gait if it saved energy, and persons post-stroke often began to step more symmetrically than they prefer to walk in daily life.
Conclusions:
We used a novel treadmill environment to show that people with and without stroke change clinically-relevant features of walking to save energy. These findings suggest that rehabilitation approaches aimed at making symmetric walking energetically “easier” may promote gait symmetry after stroke.
Keywords: gait, stroke, feedback, locomotion, cost, rehabilitation
INTRODUCTION
Humans are essentially walking experts by adulthood. Our walking patterns are suitable for nearly any terrain or situation: we can walk on sand or ice, walk while wearing sandals or high heels, and navigate steep hills or narrow balance beams. Despite this flexibility in locomotor control, each person’s gait is so distinct that we can recognize friends simply by how they walk1.
One reason that we walk in such stereotyped ways is that humans (and many other animals2–6) tend to walk in ways that conserve energy. Our gait parameters – speed7,8, cadence9,10, step length11, and step width12 – appear tuned to minimize the energy used to walk a certain distance (i.e., cost of transport) within the constraints of our musculoskeletal system13. Indeed, people adjust gait parameters to minimize energy expenditure when they walk in new environments14. While the precise role of energetics in locomotor control remains debatable – other objectives (e.g., maintaining stability, avoiding pain) also influence locomotion – many studies suggest that energy optimization is important for gait parameter selection14–20.
Walking often costs more energy after injury21,22 or disease23, including stroke24. Accordingly, interest in understanding how to manipulate or leverage cost of transport to improve patient walking has grown rapidly25–27. Most rehabilitation studies consider cost of transport as an outcome measure of exercise interventions or device implementation. As examples, prior studies have asked questions about whether locomotor training28 or a robotic exoskeleton29 may decrease cost of transport after stroke. Studies of this type aim to make walking energetically “easier” by changing movement to decrease cost of transport.
Here we suggest that rehabilitation may also benefit from the reverse approach where cost of transport is leveraged to change movement14. In other words, cost of transport should not only be considered as an outcome of rehabilitation, but potentially a driving force behind it. Consider that several neurologic populations exhibit gait asymmetry30–32 and restoration of gait symmetry is a common goal for rehabilitation33,34. Prior work has found that cost of transport increases with gait asymmetry in persons post-stroke35,36. However, recent findings suggest that asymmetric walking may be energetically costly after stroke, but not necessarily energetically suboptimal37. Furthermore, persons post-stroke can often walk more symmetrically than they do in daily life33,37. This led us to ask: will persons post-stroke walk more symmetrically if symmetric walking costs less energy than asymmetric walking?
We aimed to study whether healthy adults and persons post-stroke would change gait symmetry to save energy. In Experiments 1 and 2, we asked whether energy cost could drive healthy adults to adopt an unusual asymmetric gait. To test this, we developed a novel treadmill controller that linked foot placement asymmetry to gait speed. This created an environment wherein participants must deviate from their preferred gait symmetry to achieve the least energetically costly pattern. We hypothesized that healthy adults would walk asymmetrically when doing so cost less energy than walking symmetrically. In Experiment 3, we explored whether energy cost could drive changes in foot placement asymmetry in persons post-stroke with gait asymmetry. We hypothesized that persons post-stroke would adopt a more symmetric pattern when symmetric walking cost less energy than asymmetric walking.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
General methods
Data collection and analysis
All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Johns Hopkins Medicine Institutional Review Board prior to participating, participated in only one of the three experiments, and received monetary compensation. The participants reported no neurological (other than stroke in Experiment 3), musculoskeletal, or cardiovascular conditions. Participants walked on a treadmill with two belts (one for each foot) moving at the same speeds (Woodway USA, Waukesha, WI). A thin partition approximately 12” tall ran between the belts to prohibit stepping on both belts simultaneously, allowing accurate heel-strike detection. Participants held onto handrails and wore a safety harness that did not provide body weight support. We measured self-selected walking speed as the mean speed of either five (Experiments 1 and 2) or three (Experiment 3) overground ten-meter walk tests prior to treadmill walking. In Experiment 3, we also measured fast walking speed as the mean of three overground ten-meter walk tests where participants walked at their fastest comfortable pace.
We recorded kinematic data using an Optotrak Certus system (Northern Digital, Waterloo, ON; 100 Hz). We placed markers bilaterally over the fifth metatarsal head, lateral malleolus, lateral femoral epicondyle, greater trochanter, iliac crest, and acromion process. All participants wore comfortable shoes and form-fitting clothing. We calculated foot placement relative to the body by calculating the sagittal angle between vertical and a vector connecting the hip and ankle markers at heel-strike (i.e., alpha angle). We calculated foot placement asymmetry as the difference between the alpha angle of the limb stepping further ahead and the alpha angle of the contralateral limb.
We collected metabolic data using a TrueOne 2400 system (Parvomedics, Sandy, UT). The system warmed up for at least 30 minutes before data collection and was calibrated to manufacturer specifications. We sampled oxygen consumption breath-by-breath and calculated the rate of oxygen consumption. We used the short form of a traditional equation38 to calculate metabolic power by normalizing rate of oxygen consumption to body mass and total cost of transport (herein referred to as cost of transport) by normalizing rate of oxygen consumption to body mass and treadmill speed. We analyzed the last two minutes of each five-minute trial (in Experiments 1 and 2) and last minute of each four-minute trial (in Experiment 3) to ensure steady state measurement.
Visual display
Our visual feedback display (WorldViz, Santa Barbara, CA; Figure 1A) showed 16 vertically-arranged virtual targets on a 56” television in front of the treadmill39. Red and blue circles moved up and down the left and right halves of the display, respectively, to show realtime anterior-posterior positions of the left and right ankle markers. The left and right ankle markers could be up to 10 cm apart and remain inside the same target. The virtual targets spanned the length of the treadmill with numbers ascending from back (target 0) to front (target 15). When participants stepped to a particular target, the white number centered inside the target changed color (red at left heel-strike, blue at right heel-strike) to indicate which target had been stepped to with each foot (Figure 1B).
In Experiment 3, we also used a similar display that provided step length feedback. The left and right ankle markers could again be up to 10 cm apart and remain inside the same target. We detected heel-strikes in real time using the force plates under each treadmill belt.
Experiment 1
Protocol
The objective of Experiment 1 was to understand whether healthy young adults would adopt an asymmetric gait over a fixed time interval if asymmetric walking cost less energy than symmetric walking. Twelve healthy adults participated in Experiment 1 (3M/9F, age (mean ± standard deviation): 26±4 years, self-selected walking speed: 1.34±0.18 m/s) over two testing days. On Day 1, participants walked for five minutes at self-selected speeds followed by five minutes at 0.5 m/s, 1.0 m/s, 1.5 m/s, and 2.0 m/s (display off, order randomized). Participants rested after each trial and breathing returned to baseline. We then turned on the display and participants tested it briefly by stepping with the treadmill stationary. We then set the treadmill speed to the self-selected speed for the remainder of Day 1 and participants performed four asymmetric walking patterns for five minutes each (order randomized). The instructions were:
“Walk so that your right (left) foot lands at least four targets ahead of the left (right)”
“Walk so that your right (left) foot lands two or three targets ahead of the left (right)”
Day 1 concluded with two minutes of walking at self-selected speed with the display turned off and without instruction to investigate aftereffects following the asymmetric walking patterns.
On Day 2, we paired each speed with a specific target asymmetry:
Symmetric walking: difference of zero or one target, speed=0.5 m/s
Small left limp: left foot two or three targets ahead of right, speed=2.0 m/s
Small right limp: right foot two or three targets ahead of left, speed=1.0 m/s
Large left limp: left foot at least four targets ahead of right, speed=1.5 m/s
Large right limp: right foot at least four targets ahead of left, speed=self-selected
Participants walked with each asymmetry while an investigator set the treadmill at its paired speed for five minutes each. We then activated our treadmill controller that changed the treadmill speed to the speed paired with the asymmetry used most frequently over the prior ten strides (this ensured that the speed did not change if the participants made a misstep).
Participants then explored the controller-driven landscape for ten minutes. Participants did not wear the metabolic equipment during the exploration periods of Experiments 1–3 so that they could ask questions during their initial experience with the controller. We guided the participants through the combinations (e.g., “walk so that both feet land in the same target…now walk with the right foot landing at least four targets ahead of the left…”) for the first five minutes. During the final five minutes, we asked participants to try the combinations at their own paces and discover which combination they preferred, as they would later walk for five minutes using their preferred combination. Following exploration, the controller remained active and participants walked for five minutes using their preferred combination (test period). After the test period, we turned the display off and told participants to walk however felt comfortable for five minutes with the speed set to that picked during the test period.
Statistical analysis
We performed repeated measures ANOVAs to compare cost of transport among the five different asymmetry/speed combinations set by the treadmill controller (Shapiro-Wilk tests were not significant). Sample sizes in Experiments 1–3 were determined by the number of participants required to observe a significant reshaping of the energy landscapes by pairing asymmetry and speed. In all experiments, we set α≤0.05, performed Mauchly’s tests of sphericity (Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied when sphericity was violated), and applied Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons where appropriate.
Experiment 2
Protocol
We also considered that, in daily life, people walk to travel a set distance. We posited that the five-minute time limit in Experiment 1 could have affected the participant choices during the test period if they chose to minimize total energy cost (i.e., cost per time) rather than cost of transport (i.e., cost per distance). In Experiment 2, we investigated how participants preferred to walk in a reshaped energy landscape when required to walk a set distance. Twenty healthy adults participated in Experiment 2 (8M/12F, age: 24±5 years, self-selected walking speed: 1.26±0.14 m/s). In Experiment 2, the protocol was similar to Day 2 of Experiment 1 except we reorganized the asymmetry/speed combinations and participants walked for 1.0 km instead of five minutes during the test period. The new target asymmetry/speed combinations were:
Symmetric walking: difference of zero or one target, speed=0.5 m/s
Small left limp: left foot two or three targets ahead of right, speed=2.0 m/s
Small right limp: right foot two or three targets ahead of left, speed=1.5 m/s
Large left limp: left foot at least four targets ahead of right, speed=1.0 m/s
Large right limp: right foot at least four targets ahead of left, speed=self-selected
We showed participants the distance traveled during the exploration and test periods by displaying a horizontal green bar that began adjacent with the word “BEGIN” in the upper left corner of the display. The bar grew from left to right to indicate total distance walked. The bar reached the word “END” at the right edge of the display after participants walked one kilometer.
Before the experiment, we informed participants that they would neither receive additional compensation for extending the experiment by walking slowly nor leave the experiment sooner by walking fast. Participants did not wear the metabolic equipment during the test period of Experiment 2 to minimize the possibility that the equipment might influence the combination picked (e.g., we considered that participants may walk faster to complete the experiment and remove the equipment sooner). Following each combination, we recorded perceived exertion using the Borg Rating of Perceived Exertion Scale (RPE)40 to assess whether perceived exertion influenced the combination picked during the test period.
Statistical analysis
We performed repeated measures ANOVAs to compare cost of transport among the five different asymmetry/speed combinations set by the treadmill controller (Shapiro-Wilk tests were not significant). We performed Pearson’s correlations to assess relationships between Borg RPE and cost of transport and between Borg RPE and metabolic power.
Experiment 3
Protocol
The objective of Experiment 3 was to understand whether persons post-stroke would walk more symmetrically if symmetric walking cost less energy than asymmetric walking. Seven persons post-stroke participated in Experiment 3 over two days (Table 1). One participant wore a functional electrical stimulation device (tibialis anterior of paretic leg). On Day 1, participants walked for four minutes at three speeds (all with the display turned off, order randomized): self-selected, slow (0.4 m/s), and fastest comfortable. Participants rested after each trial and breathing returned to baseline.
Table 1.
Participant |
Sex |
Age (y) |
LEFM |
SS (m/s) |
FS (m/s) |
AFO |
MPS |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | F | 49 | 25 | 0.81 | 0.90 | no | 10 |
2 | M | 78 | 31 | 0.94 | 1.08 | no | 12 |
3 | M | 50 | 22 | 0.77 | 0.93 | no | 40 |
4 | M | 62 | 30 | 1.06 | 1.15 | no | 110 |
5 | F | 68 | 24 | 0.96 | 1.10 | no | 90 |
6 | M | 68 | 23 | 0.83 | 0.89 | yes | 45 |
7 | F | 63 | 17 | 0.80 | 0.89 | yes | 99 |
LEFM: Lower extremity Fugl-Meyer. SS: Self-selected walking speed. FS: Fast walking speed. AFO: Ankle-foot orthotic. MPS: Months post-stroke.
After the speed manipulation trials, we turned on the foot placement feedback and participants tested it briefly (similar to Experiments 1 and 2). We set the treadmill speed to the self-selected walking speed for the remainder of Day 1 and participants performed three patterns for four minutes each (order randomized): walking with preferred asymmetry (measured during the self-selected speed trial), more symmetric than preferred, and less symmetric than preferred. Participants were not informed about their preferred symmetry and its relationship to the visual feedback. For example, if a participant walked with the right foot stepping one target ahead of the left (unseen by the participant as the feedback was turned off) during self-selected walking, the three conditions would be “walk with your right foot landing one target ahead of the left” (preferred), “walk with both feet landing inside the same target” (more symmetric), and “walk with your right foot landing two targets ahead of the left” (less symmetric). Participants in Experiment 3 also underwent a similar protocol using step length feedback. We added the step length asymmetry manipulation because step length asymmetry is a common target in poststroke rehabilitation and its effect on cost of transport may differ from that of foot placement asymmetry. We calculated step length as the anterior-posterior distance between the ankle markers at heel-strike and step length asymmetry as:
where the longer and shorter step lengths were defined based on the average step length of each limb over the four minutes of walking at self-selected speed.
Day 2 of Experiment 3 began with four minutes of walking at self-selected speed. Each speed was then paired with a specific foot placement asymmetry. Participants first walked with each asymmetry while an investigator set the treadmill at its paired speed for four minutes each.
The target asymmetry/speed combinations were as follows:
Preferred symmetry: baseline asymmetry, speed=0.4 m/s
More symmetric: more symmetric than baseline, speed=self-selected
Less symmetric: less symmetric than baseline, speed=fast
We chose foot placement because 1) foot placement and step length asymmetries showed similar costs of transport on Day 1, and 2) online feedback was easier for participants to control.
We then activated the controller and participants explored the landscape for ten minutes. Following exploration, participants walked 0.25 km using their preferred combination during the test period. Similar to Experiment 2, we showed participants the real-time distance traveled during the exploration and test periods. Participants did not wear the metabolic equipment during the test period.
Statistical analysis
We performed Friedman tests to compare costs of transport among the three walking speeds, as the results of the Shapiro-Wilk test of normality were significant for these data. We performed repeated measures ANOVAs on 1) mean alpha angle difference over the last 30 strides of the three foot placement asymmetry patterns on Day 1, 2) mean step length asymmetry over the last 30 strides of the three step length asymmetry patterns on Day 1, 3) costs of transport among the three foot placement asymmetry patterns on Day 1, 4) costs of transport among the three step length asymmetry patterns on Day 1, and 5) costs of transport among the three different asymmetry/speed combinations set by the treadmill controller on Day 2. Shapiro-Wilk tests were not significant for these data.
RESULTS
Experiment 1
On Day 1 of Experiment 1 (Figure 1C), we confirmed that cost of transport was lowest near self-selected walking speeds (Figure 1D) and increased with asymmetry (Figure 1E). On all relevant figures, asymmetry is shown as the left target minus the right target. The prescribed asymmetric patterns were distinct from normal walking (Figure 2). We collected 3,382 total strides across all 12 participants’ five-minute self-selected speed walking trials. Participants walked with one foot two or three targets ahead of the other in only 13 strides (~0.4%) and one foot more than three targets ahead of the other in only three strides (~0.09%).
We then used the Day 1 findings to design the Day 2 controller combinations (Figure 3A). The participants performed all combinations well (i.e., hit the desired difference in targets at each speed with above the 50% accuracy needed to change the speed using the controller). We designed the Day 2 asymmetry/speed combinations so that those with the lowest cost of transport required asymmetry and non-preferred speeds (Figure 3B). The small right limp+1.0 m/s and small left limp+2.0 m/s combinations showed lower costs of transport than all other combinations (F(4,44)=13.494, all p<0.05 except p=0.076 for small right limp+1.0 m/s vs. large left limp+1.5 m/s; p=1.00 between the two low cost combinations).
Following controller activation and the subsequent exploration periods, participants performed the test period where the controller remained active and they walked for five minutes using the combination that felt most comfortable. Nine of 12 participants picked the low cost small right limp+1.0 m/s combination (Figure 3C), even though it required a limp and a nonpreferred speed. The three remaining participants picked the symmetric+0.5 m/s combination. Upon follow-up questioning, all participants in Experiments 1–3 said they would not change the selected combination if they were to repeat the test period.
Experiment 2
The single-day Experiment 2 protocol was identical to Day 2 from Experiment 1 with two exceptions: 1) we reorganized the asymmetry/speed combinations to test whether participants would still converge on a low cost combination under different conditions, and 2) participants walked for one kilometer rather than five minutes during the test period (Figure 3D). Participants again performed the combinations well. The small right limp+1.5 m/s combination showed lower cost of transport than all other combinations (Figure 3E; F(4,76)=37.127, all p<0.01).
In Experiment 2, participants again tended to prefer low-cost, asymmetric patterns. Eleven of 20 participants picked the lowest cost combination (small right limp+1.5 m/s) and two picked a similarly low cost combination (small left limp+2.0 m/s; Figure 3F). Three participants picked the symmetric+0.5 m/s combination. The remaining participants picked more costly combinations (three participants picked the large left limp+1.0 m/s combination, one participant picked the large right limp+self-selected speed combination). Across Experiments 1 and 2, all participants that picked an asymmetric pattern during the test period reported that the symmetric combination was “too slow” when asked after the experiment. We did not observe aftereffects following the test period in Experiments 1 and 2, as participants only walked asymmetrically when they achieved an energy benefit (i.e., when the controller was on).
We then wondered why a few participants picked costly patterns in Experiment 2. The four participants that picked either the symmetric combination or the self-selected speed combination were among the seven slowest walkers studied. Some people may prefer a slow walking speed at the expense of high energy cost, a finding supported by work on cost of transport in persons post-stroke41.
Perceived exertion did not influence how the participants preferred to walk during the test period. Cost of transport and perceived exertion were weakly related (r=−0.27, p=0.007; Figure 4A), suggesting that cost of transport actually decreases as perceived exertion increases. Unsurprisingly, perceived exertion was positively associated with metabolic power, a measure of energy expenditure per unit time (r=0.76, p<0.001; Figure 4A). Because pilot testing revealed that some participants desired to change the combination upon seeing the distance traveled, we allowed participants to change their gait during the test period in Experiment 2. Only 5 of 20 participants changed their walking during the test period (Figure 4B). When this occurred, we determined the combination picked to be the combination used to walk the majority of the kilometer.
Experiment 3
In Experiment 3, we asked whether persons post-stroke would also change foot placement symmetry if doing so reduced cost of transport (Day 1 protocol shown in Figure 5A). First, we observed a significant effect of walking speed on cost of transport (χ2(2)=10.571, p<0.01; representative participant data shown in Figure 5B, group data shown in Figure 5C). Post-hoc analyses revealed marginally significant increases in cost of transport during slow walking as compared to preferred or fast speeds (both p=0.018; significance level set at p<0.017 after Bonferroni correction).
Unlike in healthy adults, cost of transport was unaffected by foot placement or step length symmetry in persons post-stroke37. We observed similar costs of transport when persons post-stroke walked with preferred asymmetry or with patterns that were more/less symmetric than preferred. This was consistent across asymmetries in foot placement (F(2,12)=1.036) and step length (F(2,12)=0.887, p>0.40 for all foot placement or step length asymmetry comparisons; Figures 5D and 5E). We confirmed that participants manipulated foot placement (Figure 5F) and step length (Figure 5G) asymmetry in accordance with the instructions (F(2,12)=44.333, p<0.01 for all foot placement comparisons; F(2,12)=9.807, p=0.045 for more symmetric vs. less symmetric step length comparison, p=0.169 for less symmetric vs. preferred step length comparison, and p=0.074 for more symmetric vs. preferred step length comparison; Figure 5H).
We then designed the Day 2 combinations so that those with the lowest cost of transport required the post-stroke participants to adopt a new foot placement symmetry (Day 2 protocol shown on Figure 6A). Similar to Experiments 1 and 2, participants performed all combinations well (Figure 6B). The more symmetric+self-selected speed and less symmetric+fast speed combinations showed lower costs of transport than the preferred asymmetry+slow speed combination (F(2,12)=9.160, p=0.036 for more symmetric+self-selected speed vs. preferred asymmetry+slow speed comparison, p=0.101 for less symmetric+fast speed vs. preferred asymmetry+slow speed comparison, p=1.0 between the two low cost combinations; Figure 6C). We then activated the controller and participants explored the landscape for ten minutes. During exploration, they were informed that they would later walk 0.25 km using the most comfortable pattern. Participants then walked for 0.25 km using the preferred combination during the test period.
During the test period, all seven participants adopted one of the low cost combinations that deviated from their preferred foot placement symmetry. Five of the seven participants walked with the more symmetric+self-selected speed combination and two walked with the less symmetric+fast speed combination (Figure 6D). Similar to the healthy adults in Experiments 1 and 2, persons post-stroke did not show aftereffects once the controller was turned off.
DISCUSSION
It has become clear that the nervous system will change the fundamental nature of walking to save energy14. Here we asked whether we could leverage cost of transport to drive changes in gait symmetry in healthy adults and persons post-stroke. We observed: 1) most healthy adults (75% in Experiment 1, 65% in Experiment 2) walk asymmetrically when asymmetric walking costs less energy than normal walking, and 2) persons post-stroke change their gait symmetry to walk faster with a lower cost of transport.
The results of Experiments 1 and 2 reveal that the human preference for symmetric walking is not strong. Most healthy participants tested here readily adopted asymmetric walking patterns when asymmetric walking cost less energy than symmetric walking. This occurred regardless of whether participants were asked to walk for a set time or set distance, suggesting that people optimize energy spent per unit distance regardless of the parameters of the walking task (i.e., they would have picked the slowest walking speed in Experiment 1 had they aimed to minimize total energy expenditure). These findings are consistent with prior work that revealed the people will change how they walk to minimize energy expenditure14.
We are particularly interested in the potential clinical implications of our findings. Our results may offer an explanation as to why some patients settle on certain abnormal walking patterns. Persons post-stroke can often walk faster41 or more symmetrically33,37 than they do in daily life, as observed in Experiment 3. Why then do persons post-stroke walk in seemingly undesirable ways when they are capable of “better” patterns? There are many objectives that can influence human locomotor control (e.g., stability maximization42, energy optimization14, pain avoidance43–46). These factors are likely weighted differently among different patients. Patients with balance impairment may prioritize stability, and those with pain may adopt patterns that are less painful. However, we and others47 suggest that minimizing cost of transport may strongly influence walking in patients that are minimally affected by these other confounding issues.
In Experiment 3, we found that manipulating foot placement or step length symmetry had little effect on cost of transport in persons post-stroke within a single walking session. These data Symmetry and energy cost during walking are consistent with recent findings37 and conflict with a longstanding assumption that asymmetric walking after stroke is energetically suboptimal. This assumption is drawn largely from work indicating that cost of transport increases after stroke24,48 and from between-subject comparisons showing that patients with larger asymmetry exhibit higher costs of transport35,36. Given the asymmetric neuromuscular impairments commonly observed after stroke, we considered that persons post-stroke may experience energy landscapes similar to those we designed in Experiments 1 and 2 where cost of transport is elevated but an asymmetric pattern is the least costly option. We did not find this to be the case exactly – cost of transport was similarly high no matter how persons post-stroke manipulated foot placement or step length symmetry – but the data confirmed that more symmetric stepping is not less costly for persons post-stroke with hemiparesis within a single walking session.
Importantly, when we reshaped the energy landscape so that walking more/less symmetrically than preferred reduced the cost of transport in Experiment 3, all seven post-stroke participants changed their foot placement symmetry to a less costly pattern. No participant chose the combination that allowed for walking with preferred asymmetry and also likely posed the least challenging demands on balance. We suggest that rehabilitation approaches for stroke and other neurological disorders may be most successful when the desired walking pattern becomes energetically optimal.
Restoring “normal” function is a common goal of rehabilitation. Our findings support the idea that it is important for “normal” movement patterns to have value to the patient49. If the desired gait pattern saves energy, this may lead to longer-lasting improvements than conventional therapy because the desired pattern now benefits the patient. After stroke, designing rehabilitation to reshape the energy landscape so that symmetric walking (which was, on average, approximately 35% more costly at self-selected speeds compared to healthy adults) is energetically optimal may incentivize more symmetric walking.
It is important to note that this approach is unlikely to benefit all patients. The persons post-stroke included in this study exhibited mild-to-moderate disability, relatively fast gait speeds, and capacity to change their walking patterns. Subgroups of persons post-stroke with balance impairment, slow gait speeds, deconditioned cardiovascular fitness, or lower functional capacity may not benefit from energy-driven approaches. For example, it is unlikely that a patient with poor balance would minimize cost of transport at the expense of stability. However, if therapy can first address primary deficits (e.g., balance, fitness), an energy-driven approach may become useful. It is also possible that some participants in Experiment 3 changed their gait asymmetry during the test period to achieve their preferred walking speeds rather than to minimize cost of transport. Given that this did not occur in Experiments 1 and 2 – and some participants in Experiment 3 picked a low cost pattern that was not paired with preferred walking speed – we consider this unlikely.
Moving forward, it will be important to understand how the nervous system computes cost of transport during walking. Our findings demonstrate that people walk in ways that save energy but do not feel easiest. Therefore, people must compute cost of transport more objectively. Physiological measures that correlate with gait speed (e.g., respiratory rate or heart rate) may influence ratings of exertion whereas objective measurements from physiological sensors may provide information about cost of transport, though this process may be complex50. Understanding how the nervous system computes cost of transport and uses this signal to adjust walking patterns will be critical for leveraging energetics to improve rehabilitation.
This study was not without limitations. The energy landscapes designed in Experiments 1–3 resulted in new walking patterns becoming energetically optimal; however, all of the available patterns cost more energy than preferred symmetry at preferred speed (which was no longer an option in any of the altered landscapes). Whether people would prefer a new, less costly pattern over their habitual gait pattern is unknown. Use of the handrails may have provided the post-stroke participants with additional stability that would not be available during overground walking and, in turn, may have made them more likely to change their gait pattern during the test period on the treadmill. This study included a relatively small sample size of persons post-stroke who, as mentioned above, exhibit only mild-to-moderate impairment.
CONCLUSIONS
Healthy adults and persons post-stroke adjusted gait symmetry away from their preferred symmetry when new gait patterns reduced the cost of transport during walking. We find these results exciting because they raise questions about how energetics can influence gait patterns and rehabilitation in clinical populations. Future work will aim to understand how energetics can be leveraged to improve rehabilitation and will pursue training mechanisms targeted at reshaping energy landscapes during walking.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grants R21AG059184 to RTR and R37NS090610 to AJB. We thank Max Donelan, Sabrina Abram, Surabhi Simha, Jeremy Wong, and Jessica Selinger for helpful discussions. We also thank Tziporah Thompson for illustrations on Figure 1.
REFERENCES
- 1.Cutting JE, Kozlowski LT. Recognizing friends by their walk: gait perception without familiarity clues. Bull Psychon Soc 1977;9(5):353–356. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Rubenson J, Heliams DB, Lloyd DG, Fournier PA. Gait selection in the ostrich: mechanical and metabolic characteristics of walking and running with and without an aerial phase. Proc Biol Sci 2004;271(1543):1091–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.O’Neill MC. Gait-specific metabolic costs and preferred speeds in ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta), with implications for the scaling of locomotor costs. Am J Phys Anthropol 2012;149(3):356–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Langman VA et al. Moving cheaply: energetics of walking in the African elephant. J Exp Biol 1995;198(3):629–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Zani PA, Kram R. Low metabolic cost of locomotion in ornate box turtles, Terrapene ornata. J Exp Biol 2008;211(23):3671–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Griffin TM, Kram R. Penguin waddling is not wasteful. Nature 2000;408:929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bornstein MH, Bornstein HG. The pace of life. Nature 1976;259:557–559. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ralston HJ. Energy-speed relation and optimal speed during level walking. Int Z Angew Physiol 1958;17(4):277–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Atzler E, Herbst R. Arbeitsphysiologische Studien III. Pflugers Arch 1928;215:292–328. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Molen NH, Rozendal RH, Boon W. Graphic representation of the relationship between oxygen-consumption and characteristics of normal gait of the human male. Proc K Ned Akad Wet C 1972;75(4):305–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Zarrugh MY, Todd FN, Ralston HJ. Optimization of energy expenditure during level walking. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol 1975;33(4):293–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Donelan JM, Kram R, Kuo AD. Mechanical and metabolic determinants of the preferred step width in human walking. Proc Biol Sci 2001;268(1480):1985–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Collins SH, Wiggin MB, Sawicki GS. Reducing the energy cost of human walking using an unpowered exoskeleton. Nature 2015;522:212–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Selinger JC, O’Connor SM, Wong JD, Donelan JM. Humans Can Continuously Optimize Energetic Cost during Walking. Curr Biol 2015;25(18):2452–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bertram JE, Ruina A Multiple walking speed-frequency relations are predicted by constrained optimization. J Theor Biol 2001;209(4):445–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kuo AD. A simple model of bipedal walking predicts the preferred speed-step length relationship. J Biomech Eng 2001;123(3):264–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Gottschall JS, Kram R. Energy cost and muscular activity required for leg swing during walking. J Appl Physiol (1985) 2005;99(1):23–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Snaterse M, Ton R, Kuo AD, Donelan JM. Distinct fast and slow processes contribute to the selection of preferred step frequency during human walking. J Appl Physiol (1985) 2011;110(6):1682–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.O’Connor SM, Donelan JM. Fast visual prediction and slow optimization of preferred walking speed. J Neurophysiol 2012;107(9):2549–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Long LL, Srinivasan M. Walking, running, and resting under time, distance, and average speed constraints: optimality of walk-run-rest mixtures. J R Soc Interface 2013;10(81):20120980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Imms FJ, MacDonald IC, Prestidge SP. Energy expenditure during walking in patients recovering from fractures of the leg. Scand J Rehabil Med 1976;8(1):1–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Veicsteinas A, Aghemo P, Margaria R, Cova P, Pozzolini M Energy cost of walking with lesions of the foot. J Bone Joint Surg Am 1979;61(7):1073–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Christiansen CL, Schenkman ML, McFann K, Wolfe P, Kohrt WM. Walking economy in people with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2009;24(10):1481–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Zamparo P, Francescato MP, De Luca G, Lovati L, di Prampero PE. The energy cost of level walking in patients with hemiplegia. Scand J Med Sci Sports 1995;5(6):348–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gordon KE, Ferris DP. Learning to walk with a robotic ankle exoskeleton. J Biomech 2007;40(12):2636–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Mooney LM, Rouse EJ, Herr HM. Autonomous exoskeleton reduces metabolic cost of human walking. J Neuroeng Rehabil 2014;11:151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Louie DR, Eng JJ. Powered robotic exoskeletons in post-stroke rehabilitation of gait: a scoping review. J Neuroeng Rehabil 2016;13:53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Reisman DS, Binder-MacLeod S, Farquhar WB. Top Stroke Rehabil 2013;20(2):161–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Awad LN, Bae J, O’Donnell K, De Rossi SMM, Hendron K, Sloot LH, et al. A soft robotic exosuit improves walking in patients after stroke. Sci Transl Med 2017;9(400): pii:eaaai9084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Knutsson E An analysis of Parkinsonian gait. Brain 1972;95(3):475–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Knutsson E, Richards C. Different types of disturbed motor control in gait of hemiparetic patients. Brain 1979;102(2):405–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Olney SJ, Richards C. Hemiparetic gait following stroke. Part I: characteristics. Gait Posture 1996;4:136–148. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Reisman DS, Wityk R, Silver K, Bastian AJ. Locomotor adaptation on a split-belt treadmill can improve walking symmetry post-stroke. Brain 2007;130(7):1861–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bohannon RW, Andrews AW, Smith MB. Rehabilitation goals of patients with hemiplegia. Int J Rehabil Res 1988;11:181–183. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Awad LN, Palmer JA, Pohlig RT, Binder-Macleod SA, Reisman DS. Walking speed and step length asymmetry modify the energy cost of walking after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2015;29(5):416–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Finley JM, Bastian AJ. Associations between foot placement asymmetries and metabolic cost of transport in hemiparetic gait. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2017;31(2):168–177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sánchez N, Finley JM. Individual differences in locomotor function predict the capacity to reduce asymmetry and modify the energetic cost of walking poststroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2018;32(8):701–713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Brockway JM. Derivation of formulae used to calculate energy expenditure in man. Hum Nutr Clin Nutr 1987;41(6):463–471. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Roemmich RT, Long AW, Bastian AJ. Seeing the errors you feel enhances locomotor performance but not learning. Curr Biol 2016;26(20):2707–2716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Borg G Borg’s perceived exertion and pain scales, 1998; Human Kinetics, Champaign. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Reisman DS, Rudolph KS, Farquhar WB. Influence of speed on walking economy poststroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2009;23(6):529–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Latt MD, Menz HB, Fung VS, Lord SR. Walking speed, cadence and step length are selected to optimize the stability of head and pelvis accelerations. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(2):201–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Phillips A, McClinton S. Gait deviations associated with plantar heel pain: A systematic review. Clin Biomech 2017;42:55–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Arazpour M, Bahramian F, Abutorabi A, Nourbakhsh ST, Alidousti A, Aslani H. The effect of patellofemoral pain syndrome on gait parameters: a literature review. Arch Bone Jt Surg 2016;4(4):298–306. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Al-Obaidi SM, Al-Zoabi B, Al-Shuwaie N, Al-Zaabie N, Nelson RM. The influence of pain and pain-related fear and disability beliefs on walking velocity in chronic low back pain. Int J Rehabil Res 2003;26(2):101–08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hurwitz DE, Ryals AR, Block JA, Sharma L, Schnitzer TJ, Andriacchi TP. Knee pain and joint loading in subjects with osteoarthritis of the knee. J Orthop Res 2000;18(4):572–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kuo AD, Donelan JM. Dynamic principles of gait and their clinical implications. Phys Ther 2010;90(2):157–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Platts MM, Rafferty D, Paul L. Metabolic cost of over ground gait in younger stroke patients and healthy controls. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2006;38(6):1041–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Latash ML, Anson JG. What are “normal movements” in atypical populations? Behav Brain Sci 1996;19(1):55–68. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wong JD, O’Connor SM, Selinger JC, Donelan JM. Contribution of blood oxygen and carbon dioxide sensing to the energetic optimization of human walking. J Neurophysiol 2017;118(2):1425–1433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]