Skip to main content
Evolutionary Applications logoLink to Evolutionary Applications
. 2018 Dec 18;12(7):1329–1343. doi: 10.1111/eva.12721

Urbanization erodes niche segregation in Darwin's finches

Luis F De León 1,2,, Diana M T Sharpe 3, Kiyoko M Gotanda 3,4, Joost A M Raeymaekers 5,6, Jaime A Chaves 7,8, Andrew P Hendry 3, Jeffrey Podos 9
PMCID: PMC6691225  PMID: 31417618

Abstract

Urbanization is influencing patterns of biological evolution in ways that are only beginning to be explored. One potential effect of urbanization is in modifying ecological resource distributions that underlie niche differences and that thus promote and maintain species diversification. Few studies have assessed such modifications, or their potential evolutionary consequences, in the context of ongoing adaptive radiation. We study this effect in Darwin's finches on the Galápagos Islands, by quantifying feeding preferences and diet niche partitioning across sites with different degrees of urbanization. We found higher finch density in urban sites and that feeding preferences and diets at urban sites skew heavily toward human food items. Furthermore, we show that finches at urban sites appear to be accustomed to the presence of people, compared with birds at sites with few people. In addition, we found that human behavior via the tendency to feed birds at non‐urban but tourist sites is likely an important driver of finch preferences for human foods. Site differences in diet and feeding behavior have resulted in larger niche breadth within finch species and wider niche overlap between species at the urban sites. Both factors effectively minimize niche differences that would otherwise facilitate interspecies coexistence. These findings suggest that both human behavior and ongoing urbanization in Galápagos are starting to erode ecological differences that promote and maintain adaptive radiation in Darwin's finches. Smoothing of adaptive landscapes underlying diversification represents a potentially important yet underappreciated consequence of urbanization. Overall, our findings accentuate the fragility of the initial stages of adaptive radiation in Darwin's finches and raise concerns about the fate of the Galápagos ecosystems in the face of increasing urbanization.

Keywords: adaptive divergence, Galápagos, human disturbances, maladaptation, niche partitioning, resource distribution, urban ecology, urbanization

1. INTRODUCTION

One of the hallmarks of adaptive radiation is niche segregation, whereby closely related populations or species evolve to specialize on distinct ecological resources (Grant, 1999; Lack, 1947; Schluter, 2000; Simpson, 1953). Niche segregation is thought to emerge principally from competition for shared ecological resources (Gause, 1934; Hardin, 1960; Macarthur & Levins, 1967; Roughgarden, 1976; Schoener, 1968) and is determined jointly by the availability of ecological resources and the ability of consumer populations to exploit those resources. Accordingly, variation in resource distributions can facilitate niche segregation between populations or species in a given environment (De León, Podos, Gardezi, Herrel, & Hendry, 2014; Levine & HilleRisLambers, 2009; Pianka, 1973; Schoener, 1974; Tilman, 1982). Niche segregation can also be favored by additional factors, including behavioral flexibility or phenotypic plasticity, whereby individuals explore novel resources within shared environments (Boogert, Monceau, & Lefebvre, 2010; Ducatez, Clavel, & Lefebvre, 2015; Inouye, 1978; Nicolakakis, Sol, & Lefebvre, 2003; Sol, González‐Lagos, Moreira, Maspons, & Lapiedra, 2014; Wright, Eberhard, Hobson, Avery, & Russello, 2010) or by genetically based phenotypic variability, whereby individuals with different trait values exploit and diverge into resources to which they are best adapted (Bolnick & Paull, 2009; Bolnick, Svanbäck, Araújo, & Persson, 2007; De León, Rolshausen, Bermingham, Podos, & Hendry, 2012).

The process of divergence via niche segregation can be conceptualized as the splitting of populations along a “rugged” adaptive landscape—a surface relating the mean fitness of populations or species to mean trait values, with “ruggedness” arising from distinct alternative fitness peaks that correspond to different ecological resources (Simpson, 1953; Svensson & Calsbeek, 2012). As such, alterations to resource availability and resource distributions are viewed as affecting the shape of adaptive landscapes underlying diversification (De León et al., 2011; Hendry et al., 2006). Alterations to adaptive landscapes can be particularly drastic in the case of human disturbances such as urbanization, where large swathes of natural environments—and the resources they contain—are altered by many factors, including infrastructure development, the introduction of exotic species, and human food availability (Alberti, 2015; Aronson et al., 2014; Gaston, 2010; Gotanda, Hendry, & Svensson, 2017; McKinney, 2002, 2006 ; Penick, Savage, & Dunn, 2015). However, despite the rapid increase in urbanization worldwide (Grimm et al., 2008; Seto, Sánchez‐Rodríguez, & Fragkias, 2010; Wigginton, Fahrenkamp‐Uppenbrink, Wible, & Malakoff, 2016), and accumulating evidence that some species can adapt accordingly (Donihue & Lambert, 2014; Johnson & Munshi‐South, 2017; Kettlewell, 1955; Littleford‐Colquhoun, Clemente, Whiting, Ortiz‐Barrientos, & Frère, 2017; Lowry, Lill, & Wong, 2013; Slabbekoorn & Peet, 2003; Winchell, Reynolds, Prado‐Irwin, Puente‐Rolón, & Revell, 2016), the exploitation of human‐introduced ecological resources has not yet been linked to the alteration of specific ecological niches or adaptive landscapes that drive diversification in nature. Here, we explore such links in Darwin's ground finches (Geospiza spp.) across sites with different degrees of urbanization on Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos, Ecuador. Specifically, we ask the following: (a) Has the availability of novel human foods in urban areas altered finch diets?; and, if so, (b) Do finches in urban environments prefer human foods over natural foods?; (c) Do finches in urban areas respond differently to the presence of people?; and (d) What are the consequences of finches' use of human foods for the persistence of ecological differences underlying the finch adaptive radiation?

In the adaptive radiation of Darwin's finches, beak morphology has diversified as a consequence of adaptation to different ecological resources. For instance, in the ground finches, divergent beak sizes and shapes are considered adaptations to exploit different seed types (Supporting Information Figure S1), presumably corresponding to different peaks on their adaptive landscape (Abbott, Abbott, & Grant, 1977; Bowman, 1961; Grant & Grant, 2008; Lack, 1947; Schluter & Grant, 1984). Specifically, the small, medium, and large ground finches (Geospiza fuliginosa, G. fortis, and G. magnirostris) feed on small/soft, medium, and large/hard seeds, respectively, and accordingly have evolved small, medium, and large beaks. The closely related cactus finch (Geospiza scandens) specializes on the nectar, pollen, and seeds of Opuntia cacti and has evolved an elongated beak (Supporting Information Figure S1). Resource partitioning has also promoted intra‐specific adaptive divergence within the medium ground finch, where two beak‐size morphs on Santa Cruz Island have diverged significantly in ecological (De León et al., 2014, 2012), morphological (Hendry et al., 2006; Hendry, Huber, León, Herrel, & Podos, 2009; Huber, Leon, Hendry, Bermingham, & Podos, 2007), and genetic attributes (Chaves et al., 2016; De León, Bermingham, Podos, & Hendry, 2010; Huber et al., 2007). Resource partitioning has also been associated with morphological divergence between highland and lowland populations of the small ground finches on Santa Cruz Island (Kleindorfer, Chapman, Winkler, & Sulloway, 2006; Sulloway & Kleindorfer, 2013). Studies on the continuum of intra‐ to inter‐specific divergence in the ground finches can help reveal the processes underlying adaptive divergence and how it might be influenced by urbanization.

Previous work on the medium ground finch suggests that divergence of the morphs has recently diminished at sites adjacent to a human settlement (Hendry et al., 2006). It has been hypothesized that the apparent recent fusion of beak‐size distributions of the morphs, from bimodal to unimodal, was due to the introduction and ready availability of human foods, which might be flattening the adaptive landscape and thereby reducing selection against intermediate forms (De León et al., 2011; Hendry et al., 2006). A critical test of this hypothesis would involve asking whether niche segregation and feeding preferences actually differ between urban and non‐urban contexts. In the present paper, we offer such a test, by conducting feeding observations and field experiments on coexisting ground finch species at sites that span different degrees of urbanization.

2. METHODS

2.1. Field sites

Sampling and experiments took place between January and March of 2014 and 2015 at four sites on Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos, Ecuador (Figure 1). All four sites are located within the low‐elevation arid zone of the island (Wiggins & Porter, 1971) and differed in their degrees of urbanization as well as human‐associated activities (i.e., the tendency of people to feed finches; see Table 1 for details). The foraging ecology and food preferences of finches are likely to differ in transition or high‐elevation sites, and therefore, responses to urbanization may also differ in these zones.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Finches feeding on human foods at urban sites. Panels show a female medium ground finch eating dry rice from a feeder (a). Study sites on Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos, Ecuador, with urban (black dots) sites, non‐urban sites (gray dots), and roads (lines) designed for vehicular traffic (b). Santa Rosa and Bellavista were not included in our study, but are shown here for illustrative purpose only. A group of small ground finches feeding crumbs off a plate at restaurant in Puerto Ayora (c). Photo credit: L. F. De León

Table 1.

Level of urbanization and human behavior at each study site on Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos, Ecuador

Site Urbanization level Annual visitors Tendency of feeding Human contact
El Garrapatero Non‐urban Only scientists visit this site No human feeding; low human density Minimal contact with humans, except for scientists and park rangers
El Garrapatero Beach Non‐urban tourist 38,542 Regular human feeding; low human density Humans visit beach for the day and bring snacks/picnics as there are no shops
Academy Bay Intermediate urban 78,555 Little human feeding, but finches feed opportunistically on food scraps, and are likely to be intentionally fed on occasions; high human density Humans visit research center and are advised not to feed the finches, but finches are often within close proximity to large groups of people
Puerto Ayora Urban 158,339 Regular human feeding; high human density Humans in city generate food scraps on an hourly basis

Sites are ranked according to the tendency of humans to feed finches (tendency of feeding) and the degree of interaction (human contact) with humans at each site. Data for the annual number of visitors were obtained from Dirección del Parque Nacional Galápagos & Observatorio de Turismo de Galápagos (2016).

The first site, El Garrapatero (EG; “non‐urban” site; 0°41'16.6"S 90°13'19.4"W), is located 1–2 km inland of the island's eastern coast and is about 10 km from the nearest major human settlement (Bellavista, Figure 1). Introduced plant species and human foods are rare at this site (De León et al., 2011), although human activity has increased since 2009 due to the paving of a road that provides access to the coast. On a typical day, dozens of vehicles now pass through the site. Browsing by feral goats and donkeys was historically common at EG, but eradication efforts led by the Galápagos National Park (Phillips, Wiedenfeld, & Snell, 2012) have resulted in decreased grazing disturbance. Surveys at this site were performed in an area of ~0.5 km2 eastward from EG road.

The second site, EG Beach (“non‐urban tourist” site; 0°41'39.9"S 90°13'15.8"W), hereafter referred to as “EG Beach,” is located on the eastern shore of the island adjacent to the first site (Figure 1). The number of visitors to this site has increased markedly due to the newly paved road, which provides direct access to the beach. Although this site supports no permanent human presence, infrastructure has expanded over the past 6 years to include a gravel parking lot, cobblestone paths, a ranger outpost, and an overnight camping ground. Typically, 5–20 tourists a day (although sometimes many more) visit the beach to swim, kayak, picnic, and observe wildlife. We included this site to help disentangle two urban factors: the large‐scale alteration of habitats (absent here) versus the occasional to regular presence of humans themselves (present here).

The third site, Academy Bay (AB; “intermediate urban” site; 0°44'31.6"S 90°18'15.3"W), is situated on the south coast of the island and is contiguous with the town of Puerto Ayora (PA). Human influences at AB include wide cobblestone and dirt paths, a high abundance of exotic plant species, and the presence of human foods associated mainly with concessions for tourists visiting the Charles Darwin Research Station (CDRS), as well as a few local residents and dorms. Finches at this site are regularly observed consuming human foods (Figure 1) and drinking freshwater from tortoise pens and broken pipes (De León et al., 2011). Surveys at this site were performed in an area of ~0.5 km2 encompassing trails from the public entrance of the Galápagos National Park eastward to a cliff behind the animal facilities of the CDRS and did not include the beach areas bordering the CDRS.

The fourth site, PA (“urban” site; 0°44'34.8"S 90°18'43.4"W), is the largest human settlement on Galápagos with over 12,000 inhabitants (Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Censos). Puerto Ayora also receives many more tourists than other islands in the Archipelago, with ~218,365, and 241,800 recorded visitors in 2016 and 2017, respectively (Dirección del Parque Nacional Galápagos & Observatorio de Turismo de Galápagos, 2016, 2017). At this site, we have seen finches feeding on a wide variety of introduced plant species and human foods (Figure 1), including bread, potato chips, ice‐cream cones, rice, and beans (De León et al., 2011). Surveys at this site were performed in an area of ~1.0 km2 encompassing the fire station, the farmer's market, the cemetery, the public dock, and the entrance to the trail to Tortuga Bay.

Given the complex ways in which finches could interact with humans on Santa Cruz Island (Table 1), we cannot consider our four sampling sites as representing a simple urbanization gradient. We rather consider them as four discrete sites that vary independently in both the degree of urbanization (as given by human infrastructure and population density) and the potential for human interaction with finches (a function of both the number and behavior of tourists).

Regarding human interaction with finches, reports from the Galapagos National Park indicate that the number of visitors to the Galápagos is high during the entire year, with two inter‐annual peaks: the first between July and September, and the second in March (Dirección del Parque Nacional Galápagos & Observatorio de Turismo de Galápagos, 2016, 2017 ). Thus, our sampling period (January–March) occurred just before the onset of the second largest peak in the number of visitors. Although we did not quantify food availability in the current study, finches in urban areas are likely to enjoy a surplus of human foods throughout the year. However, we do not expect that this supply was any higher during our sampling period. Further studies will be necessary, however, to better understand how finch preferences for human foods might respond to temporal variation in the availability of both natural and human foods.

2.2. Feeding observations

Our first goal was to quantify the diets at our study sites of the four Geospiza species (G. fortis, G. fuliginosa, G. scandens, and G. magnirostris). Toward this end, we employed a point observation method (De León et al., 2014, 2011, 2012 ). Briefly, during morning or afternoon hours, we walked along predetermined transects and used binoculars to identify birds (to the species level) and, if possible, the food items on which they were feeding. At three of our sites, we surveyed along a total of 74 transects covering a linear distance of 30.74 km: EG (n = 22, mean length = 436.80 m), AB (n = 22, 476.75 m), and PA (n = 30, 353.84 m). No feeding observations or estimations of bird density were performed at EG Beach because this site is represented by an open sandy beach with little natural vegetation. Transect courses were determined randomly at the beginning of each walk, but they were limited to a series of preexisting trails that facilitated access to sites with dense vegetation (EG and AB). In the town of PA, transects were determined by following both large and small streets through the middle and around the town, including surrounding neighborhoods (Miraflores and El Edén). Observations in this area included finches found on the streets, sidewalks, and restaurant areas, as well as on the natural vegetation of parks, gardens, and roadsides.

Food items included specific plant species and plant parts (i.e., seeds, fruits, leaves, or flowers) as well as different types of human foods. We also recorded the category “ground,” when birds were feeding on the ground, but the exact food items could not be identified owing to their small size. After a feeding event was recorded, we moved immediately onto the next individual to avoid pseudoreplication. Our data therefore represent counts of discrete observations of individual birds feeding on particular food items (De León et al., 2014, 2012 ). Finally, we generated rarefaction curves to visualize how the cumulative numbers of food items observed varied in relation to our sampling efforts at each site.

2.3. Bird density

Our second goal was to estimate variation in bird density across sites. For this task, we used bird count data for our focal species from our feeding observation transects, given that we recorded all birds within 30 m at each side of the observer, whether they were feeding or not. We then used these values to estimate the number of individuals per unit area (Emlen, 1977). Two factors could have affected our estimates of bird density. The first is bird detectability at sites with dense vegetation, such as EG and AB, in contrast to the more open urban site (PA). And second, combining feeding observations and bird count along the same transect might not be as accurate as surveys dedicated to bird counts alone. However, our main goal here was to estimate relative differences in bird abundance between urban and non‐urban environments, rather than providing a precise value of bird density at each site. In addition, to reduce autocorrelation effects and to improve detectability we recorded only one feeding event per individual (see above), and only within 30 m of the observer. These types of observations were also facilitated by the fact that Darwin's finches are tame and can be easily observed at short distances with little interference (De León et al., 2014; Grant, 1999).

2.4. Finch response to food cues

To test whether and how finches across our study sites respond to the presence of people, we developed a “finch–human interaction” experiment. We recorded finch responses to two different human stimuli: a visual stimulus (an experimenter standing or sitting still and quietly in an open space) and an audiovisual stimulus (an experimenter standing or sitting still and noisily rustling a bag of potato chips, generating a “crinkle” sound typically associated with packaged human foods). Including this second stimulus was inspired by observations of finches approaching humans opening and/or handling packaged foods. The stimuli were presented sequentially and in the same spot, with the visual stimulus first (5 min) and the audiovisual stimulus second (five more minutes). During stimulus presentation, we recorded the number, species, and sex of all birds that approached within 1.5 m of the experimenter, including birds that perched above the experimenter. Presentation locations within our sites were selected haphazardly and were conducted at least 100 m apart from each other during a given day. Data for this experiment were collected at all four sites: EG (non‐urban, n = 22), EG beach (non‐urban tourist, n = 37), AB (intermediate urban, n = 14), and PA (urban, n = 30).

2.5. Feeding preference experiment

To quantify finch feeding preferences, and whether and how they varied with the degree of urbanization, we performed replicate “cafeteria” experiments, in which finches were presented with a choice of human and native food items. We constructed cardboard feeding trays (30 cm × 30 cm) with nine sections (3 × 3 grid pattern) into which food could be placed. A rock was placed in the center section of each tray as an anchor. Each tray was stocked with 2.5 g of each of six natural or human foods commonly observed previously (De León et al., 2014, 2011, 2012 ). To control for any potential biases associated with the location of food on the tray, food items were placed randomly in six of the eight available sections of the tray. Human food items included uncooked white rice, potato chips, and coconut cookies, the latter two of which we crumbled into small pieces to mimic the size and shape commonly seen in urban sites. For natural native foods, we included fruits from Cryptocarpus pyriformis, Tournefortia psilostachya, and Scutia spicata. We choose these three plant species because they occur commonly at each of our sites and are eaten frequently by all ground finches, regardless of their beak size (De León et al., 2014). Furthermore, they all produce small and soft seeds, and therefore are comparable to human foods in terms hardness, and are unlikely to impose functional constraints on finch feeding. We did not include additional soft food items such as insect larvae in our experiments because their most important contribution to finch diet is limited to the onset of the rainy season (De León et al., 2014); thus, they represent a less stable food resource for finches when compared with other natural and human foods items. The tray was placed on the ground, and observers moved at least 10 m away or concealed themselves at least 5 m away to record feeding activity. We did not present empty trays in our trials because we were interested in finch preference for different types of foods rather than finches' reactions to the presence of food in general. Trial sites were selected haphazardly, and no trials were conducted within 100 m of each other during the same day. Trays were left out for a maximum of 20 min if no finch approached. If a finch approached and fed from the tray, a timer was started and observations recorded for 10 min from the first finch feeding. We recorded the total number of finches that approached, perched on, and/or fed at the tray, and their species identity. At the end of each trial, trays were collected and the food that remained in each section re‐weighed. We performed trials at all four sites: EG (n = 34), EG beach (n = 40), AB (n = 31), and PA (n = 46). Overall, our experiments were not designed to disentangle the mechanisms underlying natural feeding preferences in Darwin's finches, but rather to test whether or not Darwin finches show preferences for human foods over natural foods in both urban environments and non‐urban environments.

2.6. Data analysis

To characterize variation in finch diets across sites, we performed correspondence analysis (CA) on our feeding observation data. CA is a multivariate descriptive analysis based on matrices of frequency data (Benzécri, 1973). We here used CA to visualize and determine the contribution of each food item to the total diet of each species at each site. We then used cosine‐squared (Cos2) correlations (R package FactorMineR; Lê, Josse, & Husson, 2008) to test for associations among food items and finch diets. To test for variation in bird density across sites, we performed a Kruskal–Wallis H test, followed by a post hoc Dunn test for multiple comparisons.

To assess among‐site variation in finch response to humans, we ran two tests. First, we tallied the number of birds approaching human experimenters in the response trials and performed a Kruskal–Wallis test on these tallies. Second, for each site we calculated the proportion of trials in which at least one finch approached the stimulus, relative to the total number of trials performed (proportion positive response) and performed a chi‐squared test on these proportions.

To analyze feeding preferences from our cafeteria experiment, we constructed a nested linear model to test for variation in the amount of food eaten (in grams) across sites, food category (human vs. natural), food item nested within food category (rice vs. chips vs. cookies for human foods and C. pyriformis vs. T. psilostachya vs. S. spicata for natural foods), and the two‐way interactions (site × food category and site × food item). The number of birds that approached trays in the cafeteria experiments provided us with a third metric of response, beyond the two noted in the previous paragraph.

To test for site‐specific variation in dietary niche partitioning, we estimated at each site two commonly considered niche parameters: niche width within each species, and niche overlap between each species pair. Analyses were performed across all ground finches together (n = 1,063) and for the two ground finch species that had the highest sample sizes (G. fortis [n = 571] and G. fuliginosa [n = 349]) (Table 2). To estimate niche width, we calculated Shannon's (Colwell & Futuyma, 1971) and Levin's (Levins, 1968) niche width indices as implemented in the R package spaa v.0.2.2 (Zhang, 2016). To estimate niche overlap, we calculated Pianka's (1973) niche overlap indices as implemented in the R package EcoSimR (Gotelli & Ellison, 2013). These indices range from zero to one, with zero indicating no overlap in food items across species and one indicating complete overlap. Following De León et al. (2014), we next used EcoSimR to generate null models of expected niche overlap under different randomization algorithms (Gotelli & Entsminger, 2009). With these models, we tested whether observed niche overlap between species differed significantly from random expectations. We generated 1,000 permutations under the RA3 algorithm (Lawlor, 1980). The RA3 algorithm is recommended for niche overlap estimates because it randomizes each species' prey items, while maintaining its overall niche breadth, thus generating a random utilization matrix with similar dimensions as the observed matrix (Gotelli & Entsminger, 2009; Lawlor, 1980). All data analysis and graphing were performed in R version 3.3.0 (R Core Team, 2013). Research permits for this work were obtained from the Ministry of Environment of Ecuador and the Galápagos National Park (Permits No. PC‐29‐14 and PC‐25‐15). This study was also conducted following the guidelines of the animal care protocol of the University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

Table 2.

Number of feeding observations across three sites and 11 passerine bird species on Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos, Ecuador

Species Common name El Garrapatero Academy Bay Puerto Ayora Total
Human Natural Human Natural Human Natural
Geospiza fortis Medium ground finch 3 102 9 173 227 57 571
Geospiza fuliginosa Small ground finch 31 11 133 152 22 349
Geospiza magnirostris Large ground finch 3 3 5 11
Geospiza scandens Cactus finch 6 4 30 7 8 55
Geospiza spp. Ground finch 16 6 24 30 1 77
Geospiza parvula Small tree finch 2 7 10 1 20
Platyspiza crassirostris Vegetarian finch 5 3 72 11 15 106
Crotophaga ani Smooth‐billed ani 1 1 2
Setophaga petechia Yellow warbler 1 8 3 12
Mimus parvulus Galápagos mockingbird 1 5 6
Myiarchus magnirostris Galápagos flycatcher 2 2 4
Total 3 163 34 457 441 115 1,213

Observations of other species are included here for reference, but only data from ground finches (Geospiza spp.) were included in our statistical analyses. When identification to species level was not possible, we included a Geospiza spp. category. For each observation, we reported each food item's origin, natural versus human.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Feeding observations and bird density

During the 2 years of our study, we collected a total of 1,213 feeding observations: 166 at EG, 491 at AB, and 556 at PA (Table 1; Supporting Information Table S1). The total number of different food items we observed finches eating corresponded roughly to degrees of urbanization, from 13 at EG to 31 at AB to 36 at PA (Table 1 and Supporting Information Table S1). The greater diversity of food types at the latter sites is due mostly to the presence at these sites of a variety (11 items) of human foods (Table 1; Supporting Information Table S1). By contrast, the diversity of native plant species consumed was similar across sites, consistent with findings from our previous 5‐year survey at EG and AB (De León et al., 2014). Although the number of feeding observations varied across sites, rarefaction curves showed evidence for saturation at a relatively low number of observations (~150) for each site (Supporting Information Figure S2), suggesting that our sampling effort was sufficient to estimate dietary niches in ground finches. This was also consistent with a previous 5‐year study of feeding preference at two of the same sites (EG and AB) (De León et al., 2014).

For our transect surveys, we observed a total of 3,343 individuals (both feeding and non‐feeding) during 75.1 hr (Supporting Information Table S2). The density of ground finches varied significantly across sites, mean ± SE: 761.37 ± 81.55 bird/km2 at EG, 923.69 ± 75.33 bird/km2 at AB and 1,019.17 ± 104.65 bird/km2 at PA; Kruskal–Wallis test: χ 2 (2) = 8.23, p = 0.016, with post hoc tests confirming a significant difference between the most‐urbanized sites (PA) and both the least‐urbanized site (EG, p = 0.003) and the intermediate‐urbanized site (AB, p = 0.027).

Correspondence analyses provided a further illustration of how finches consumed human food items with increasing urbanization (Figure 2, note how species at the urban site, PA, cluster near the human food items, shown as red filled circles). Some human foods contributed disproportionately to the finch diet at urban sites, but almost never at non‐urban sites (Figure 2; Supporting Information Table S1). At PA in particular, finches were observed feeding almost exclusively (78.6% of all observations) on human foods, including crackers (3.7%), rice (5.7%), bread (6.6%), and unidentified crumbs (46%), as well as introduced garden species such as Hibiscus sp. (1.6%), and Delonix regia (Flamboyant; 10.5%). At AB, finches often fed on native plant species such as Boerhavia caribaea, S. spicata, Cryptocarpus pyriformis, and Cordia lutea, but they also were seen feeding on some human food items such as bread and crumbs (Supporting Information Table S1). Finches at this site were also seen drinking freshwater from broken pipes. In contrast, at the non‐urban site (EG) finches were seen feeding almost exclusively on native species such as S. spicata, C. pyriformis, Cordia leucophlyctis, and T. psilostachya (Figure 2, Supporting Information Table S1). The only exceptions were three birds on the side of the road to the EG, which were seen pecking at a candy wrapper and crumbs left by tourists (Figure 2, Supporting Information Table S1).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Diet of ground finches across sites with different degrees of urbanization on Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos, Ecuador. The graph represents correspondence analysis (CA) based on feeding observation data. Colors represent natural (green) and human (red) food items. Black labels represent species and site centroid combinations: El Garrapatero (EG), Academy Bay (AB), Puerto Ayora (PA), Geospiza fortis (FOR), Geospiza fuliginosa (FUL), Geospiza magnirostris (MAG), and Geospiza scandens (SCA). Food items labels and points were slightly offset to facilitate readability. In this graph, the position of each species/site combination (filled triangles) corresponds to the food items favored in its diet. Finches at the urban site, PA, cluster near the human food items

3.2. Finch response to food cues

Our finch–human interaction trials revealed significant variation across sites in finches' response to the presence of people, Figure 3; Kruskal–Wallis test: χ 2 (3) = 60.68, p < 0.001. Few finches approached the experimenters at EG (non‐urban site) and AB (intermediate‐urban site), yet many finches approached the experimenter at PA (urban site), suggesting that urban birds are indeed more accustomed to the presence of humans. Interestingly, an even stronger response (approach to an experimenter) was observed at the non‐urban tourist site, EG Beach (Figure 3; Supporting Information Table S3). Within each site, we did not find statistical differences in finch response between the audiovisual the visual stimulus alone (Figure 3; p > 0.05 for all comparisons). However, because the order in which we presented the stimuli (visual first and then audiovisual) was fixed rather than randomized, we were unable to distinguish the contribution of either of the stimulus from effect of longer exposure to the first stimulus to finch response. Nevertheless, the overall differences among sites in finch response to the presence of humans were corroborated by the proportion of positive responses (the number of trials in which at least one finch approached the stimulus), which varied significantly across sites, χ 2 (3) = 56.27, p < 0.001, with up to 73%, 35%, and 20% at EG Beach, PA, and AB, respectively, and only 7% at EG.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Finch response to humans across sites with different degrees of an urbanization and human behavior on Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos, Ecuador. The data represent the number of finches (mean ± SD) that approached the human experimenter (top panel, food preference tests) and the food tray (bottom panel, cafeteria experiment) at the four study sites. Site labels are El Garrapatero (EG), El Garrapatero Beach (EG Beach), Academy Bay (AB), and Puerto Ayora (PA)

3.3. Feeding preference experiment

The cafeteria experiment data revealed a striking variation in feeding preferences across sites with different degrees of urbanization (Figure 4; Supporting Information Movies S1 and Movie S2). The amount of food consumed from our experimental trays varied substantially across sites, food categories, and food items, with “site” interacting significantly with both food variables (Table 3). At EG (non‐urban site), only one individual inspected our feeding trays (Figure 3), but no food consumption was observed across any of the trials (Figure 4), even when finches were in the vicinity. At EG Beach (non‐urban, tourist site), finches preferentially consumed human foods over natural foods (Tukey's HSD: p < 0.001). At AB (intermediate urban site), finches showed intermediate responses to our feeding trays (Figure 4; Tukey's HSD: p = 0.948), with an apparent strong preference for rice over all other native and human food items (Figure 4). At PA (urban site), finches showed the strongest preference for human foods over native food items (Figure 4; Tukey's HSD: p < 0.001). We found that the number of finches approaching the experimental trays was lower at EG and AB than at PA and EG Beach (Figure 3). This variation was significant across sites, χ 2 (3) = 57.14, p < 0.001, and showed strong differences between EG (non‐urban) and PA (urban site) as well as between EG and EG Beach (non‐urban but tourist site) (Supporting Information Table S3).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Finches prefer human food at sites where they are fed by humans. The graph shows the Mean ± SE of food eaten from experimental cafeteria trays at four different sites with different degrees of an urbanization and human behavior on Santa Cruz Island in the Galápagos. White indicates human foods, and gray indicates natural foods

Table 3.

Finch feeding preferences for human versus natural foods across sites

Factors SS df F p
Site 2.09 3 40.723 <0.001
Food category 0.63 1 36.710 <0.001
Food type (Nested) 0.56 4 8.237 <0.001
Site*food category 0.95 3 18.588 <0.001
Site*food type 0.45 12 2.176 0.011
Residuals 14.26 834

Results of a nested linear model examining variation in the amount of food eaten in cafeteria experiments across sites with different degrees of urbanization on Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos, Ecuador. Values in bold represent statistically significant differences. The direction of the effects has been included in the main text.

3.4. Dietary niche partitioning

Across sites, variations in resource use led to highly variable estimates of niche breadth within species and niche overlap between species. In the case of G. fortis and Gfuliginosa, niche breadths were lowest at EG (non‐urban site), intermediate at AB (intermediate urban site), and highest at PA (urban site; Figure 5), consistent with variation in food type diversity. With respect to diet partitioning between these two species, finches at EG (non‐urban site) and AB (intermediate urban site) showed lower diet overlap than expected from random simulations (Figure 5), whereas the two finch species at PA (urban site) showed nearly 100% diet overlap, greater than expected at random (Figure 5). Similar trends emerged in analyses of all ground finch species, both for niche breadth (Supporting Information Figure S3) and for niche overlap (Supporting Information Figure S4).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Niche characteristics of ground finches (Geospiza fortis and Geospiza fuliginosa) across sites with different degrees of urbanization. The data represent the Shannon–Wiener's niche breadth (top panel) and Pianka's niche overlap (bottom panel) index estimated from feeding observations at three sites on Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos, Ecuador. Site labels are El Garrapatero (EG), Academy Bay (AB), and Puerto Ayora (PA)

4. DISCUSSION

Our main results were as follows: (a) finch density was notably higher at urban sites than at non‐urban sites; (b) food type availability and the diet of finches at urban sites were notably broader than at other sites and included many human foods; (c) finches at sites frequented by people (the urban sites and the non‐urban tourist site) were willing to approach people and novel objects (food trays), much more so than were finches at the non‐urban site; (d) the degree of urbanization and the presence of humans associate closely with strong preferences for human foods, with finches at urban sites feeding predominantly on human foods. From these findings, we can surmise that finch preferences for human foods in urban environments, and corresponding alterations to their behavior, have at least for the moment collapsed niche differences that normally characterize the adaptive radiation of Darwin's ground finches. These results also suggest that urbanization and the introduction of novel ecological resources are modifying finch adaptive landscapes and behavior, in ways and to degrees that seem likely to undermine the natural processes that drive adaptive diversification.

4.1. Behavioral flexibility and adaptation to urban environments

Urbanization often alters the distribution of ecological resources (McKinney, 2002, 2006 ; Schochat, Warren, Faeth, McIntyre, & Hope, 2006) and represents a novel and strong agent of selection to which organisms might or might not adapt (Alberti, 2015; Atwell et al., 2012; Smith & Bernatchez, 2008). One way that organisms could cope with altered resources is through behavioral flexibility, defined as the ability of organisms to alter their behavior in response to changing environments (Coppens, Boer, & Koolhaas, 2010). Behavioral flexibility is expected to facilitate the exploration of novel ecological resources (Inouye, 1978; Sol et al., 2014; Sol, Lefebvre, & Rodríguez‐Teijeiro, 2005; Sol, Timmermans, & Lefebvre, 2002; Tebbich, Sterelny, & Teschke, 2010; Wright et al., 2010), in both natural environments (Liebl & Martin, 2014; Nicolakakis et al., 2003; Sol et al., 2005) and urban environments (Bowers & Breland, 1996; Gotanda, Sharpe, & Léon, 2015; Lowry et al., 2013; Martin & Fitzgerald, 2005; Schochat et al., 2006; Sol et al., 2014). In birds, behavioral flexibility in urban environments is sometimes associated with a reduction in neophobia (Atwell et al., 2012; Boogert, Reader, & Laland, 2006; Martin & Fitzgerald, 2005; Sol et al., 2014) and the incorporation of novel human foods into diets (Boogert et al., 2010; Ducatez et al., 2015; Shochat, Lerman, Katti, & Lewis, 2004; Sol et al., 2002; Sol, Griffin, Bartomeus, & Boyce, 2011; Sol, Lapiedra, & González‐Lagos, 2013).

Consistent with these findings from other systems, we found that Darwin's ground finches in urban environments are accustomed to the presence of people and also display a strong preference for human foods, in contrast to finches from non‐urban environments. In addition, the fact that finches at urban sites largely ignored natural foods from experimental trays suggests that behavioral flexibility is facilitating specialization on high‐calorie and easily accessible human foods. For instance, the strong preference for rice at AB (Figure 4) could reflect a canalized/learned behavior, given that finches have previously—on many occasions—eaten rice at this site, furnished by sources such as bird feeders, direct feeding by people, and local restaurants (De León et al., 2011). The current finch preference for human foods could also reflect the influences of both behavioral flexibility and experience operating at different points of time. For example, behavioral flexibility may have been most important as the first generation of urban “pioneers” expanded their diets to include human foods. However, in subsequent generations, urban nestlings raised on human diets may have developed a preference for these foods simply based on experience/familiarity. Nevertheless, we cannot disentangle the relative importance of these two mechanisms with the current data. Overall, however, these findings support the likely importance of behavioral flexibility in promoting Darwin's finch adaptive radiation (Grant & Grant, 2008; Tebbich et al., 2010).

Exploiting urban environments might present additional challenges for organisms (Chamberlain et al., 2009; Lowry et al., 2013; McKinney, 2006; McLaughlin, Janousek, McCarty, & Wolfenbarger, 2014; Slabbekoorn & Peet, 2003), including negative effects on health that might reduce lifespan and probabilities of survival (Salmo, Nilsson, Nord, Bensch, & Isaksson, 2016). In addition, consuming highly processed human foods such as bread and crackers could have negative impacts on finch health or physiological condition (Jones, 2011; Murray et al., 2018), a possibility that should be explored in further studies. Indeed, urban environments could constitute effective ecological traps where organisms exploit environments with negative fitness consequences (Dwernychuk & Boag, 1972). In short, while our results clearly show a shift to human foods in urban sites, the adaptive significance of that shift remains to be determined. Examining the physiological and health effects of consuming human foods seems crucial to understanding the potential fitness and evolutionary consequences of urbanization on Darwin's finches.

4.2. The urban finch and the future of adaptive radiation

Accumulating evidence illustrates that some species are able to exploit novel resources provided by urban environments (Donihue & Lambert, 2014; Johnson & Munshi‐South, 2017; Kettlewell, 1955; Littleford‐Colquhoun et al., 2017; Sol et al., 2002; Winchell et al., 2016). Less clear, however, are the evolutionary consequences of using these novel resources for populations or species undergoing adaptive radiation. In Darwin's finches, the interaction between resource availability and competition for resources is thought to be essential for promoting diversification and then maintaining coexistence among closely related species (Bowman, 1961; De León et al., 2014; Grant, 1999; Lack, 1947; Schluter, 2000). Indeed, these processes have led to the formation of a number of species whose beak morphology is differentially adapted to feed on different food resources (Bowman, 1961; Grant, 1999; Lack, 1947; Schluter, 2000). However, Darwin's finches might also be considered opportunistic or “imperfect generalists” (sensu De León et al., 2014) because, during benign periods, their diets tend to converge on foods that are abundant and easily accessible, regardless of their beak morphology, resulting in temporary weakening of selection (Abbott et al., 1977; Smith, Grant, Grant, Abbott, & Abbott, 1978). Nevertheless, strong selection on beak morphology re‐emerges during periods of drought or scarcity, when finches specialize on the food types for which they are best adapted. As a consequence, year‐round availability of soft and highly abundant human foods in urban environments is likely to affect the very ecological and evolutionary processes that promote species and phenotypic diversification in Darwin's finches (De León et al., 2011; Hendry et al., 2006).

As suggested by our results, in the presence of an abundance of calorie‐rich and readily available human foods, natural inter‐species ecological differences might be eroding, leading to smoothing of the previously rugged adaptive landscape and a corresponding weakening of selection underlying divergence. This process has been inferred previously adjacent to urban environments on Santa Cruz Island, where divergent morphs of the medium ground finch have been progressively converging as the human population has increased (De León et al., 2011; Hendry et al., 2006). Our present study shows how the effects of urbanization and human behavior might also extend to other ground finch species. Specifically, besides eroding ecological differences between the small (G. fuliginosa) and medium (G. fortis) ground finch, we observed a substantial number of cactus finch (G. scandens, 11 individuals) and large ground finch (G. magnirostris, 55 individuals) feeding on and responding to human foods at the urban site. Furthermore, our finding that finches at a non‐urban but tourist site (EG Beach; Supporting Information Movie S1) also elicited a strong preference for human foods suggests that ecological niches might be more susceptible to human disturbances than previously thought. As such, changes in beak morphology within and across species in urban environments could be shaped by reduced survival disadvantages of intermediate beak‐size birds, including hybrid individuals, which under natural circumstances are unlikely to survive (i.e., because they fall in valleys of low fitness). One remaining question is what the consequences of urbanization and the presence of humans at the local scale might be for the adaptive radiation of Darwin's finches as a whole?

One likely short‐term consequence of urbanized adaptive landscapes will be the convergence of previously distinct species through introgressive hybridization (Rhymer & Simberloff, 1996; Seehausen, Takimoto, Roy, & Jokela, 2008; Taylor et al., 2006). In the ground finches, hybridization is common and has been detected in non‐urban habitats (Chaves et al., 2016; Grant, 1993; Grant & Grant, 1989, 1996 ; Lamichhaney et al., 2015), suggesting a lack of intrinsic genetic incompatibilities among species. During extreme climatic conditions (i.e., high rainfall) on Daphne Major, when natural foods abound, hybridization has led to convergence of species such as the cactus finch and the medium ground finch (Grant & Grant, 2002; Grant, Grant, Markert, Keller, & Petren, 2004), and also for the small and the medium ground finch (Grant & Grant, 2016). In tree finches (Camarhynchus spp.), hybridization has also been detected on Floreana Island, likely associated with the introduction of the Philornis parasite (Kleindorfer et al., 2014; Peters, Myers, Dudaniec, O'Connor, & Kleindorfer, 2017). Overall, these studies suggest that introgressive hybridization in Darwin's finches is widespread. If it is also strong and persistent, it could lead to the collapse of species boundaries in human‐altered environments. Another potential consequence of hybridization in urban environments is the generation of novel genetic variation that could facilitate further diversification in Darwin's fiches. This could occur, for instance, if novel hybrid individuals with intermediate beak sizes are able to experience high fitness by specializing on food items of intermediate size/hardness, such as human foods. It is important to mention that despite evidence of hybridization, other axes of divergence such as differences in song types and vocal performance (Grant, 1999; Huber & Podos, 2006; Podos, 2001) could also help maintain species boundaries in the face of increasing ecological disturbance. However, both song types and vocal performance are tightly associated with beak morphology (Podos, 2001), suggesting that changes in selection pressure on beaks via alteration of food resources could also affect other axes of divergence.

Alteration of ecological resources at local scales such as a single urban site on Santa Cruz Island could potentially have broader implications for the ground finch radiation across the entire island. For instance, increasing finch population at the urban site could promote merging of species via gene flow and interspecific hybridization (as above). This possibility was hinted at by our previous study that showed that genetic differences among ground finches (G. fortis, G. fuliginosa, and G. magnirostris) are smaller at AB (the intermediate‐urban site) than at EG (the non‐urban site) (De León et al., 2010), possibly due to higher gene flow among species at the intermediate‐urban site. Interestingly, G. fortis and G. fuliginosa are both the most abundant and the most closely related species across sites (Chaves et al., 2016; De León et al., 2010), which is likely to amplify the merging effect of hybridization in urban environments. In addition, urban finch populations could be a source of maladaptive gene flow (Hendry, Taylor, & McPhail, 2002), leading to changes in the optimal beak‐size distribution of non‐urban finch populations. We refer to maladaptive traits as those that reduce fitness under a given environmental condition. For instance, urban finches could ultimately evolve an “urban beak morphology” (e.g., a small and soft bill) adapted to exploit soft human foods in urban environments. But individuals with that morphology would face a lower fitness if they migrated to natural environments where seeds are larger and harder than human foods. In this context, hybridization (or gene flow) from urban environments could render non‐urban finch populations unable to cope with drastic environmental changes under natural conditions and could reinforce genetic differences between urban and non‐urban populations. Thus, we postulate that maladaptation could be another unintended consequence of urbanization.

4.3. Possible evolutionary consequences of human behavior

Studies of urbanization often highlight human population density, the presence of impervious surfaces or the development of infrastructure as main drivers of effects on local biodiversity (Alberti, 2015; Gaston, 2010; Gotanda et al., 2017; Johnson & Munshi‐South, 2017; McKinney, 2006). However, human behavior and the way we interact with local biodiversity could expand impacts of urbanization beyond city centers. This was suggested by our finding of strong finch preferences for human foods at EG Beach, a non‐urban but tourist site located ~12 km away from the town of PA. This also suggests that human behavior rather than human population density is the main driver of finch preference for human foods. Although additional replication is needed to statistically disentangle these factors, we argue the effect of human behavior on finch diets is likely facilitated by our tendency to feed birds either directly (via feeders) or inadvertently (via food dropping or littering), both of which are often seen at both urban centers and non‐urban tourist sites (L F. De León, per. obs). In addition, although the Galápagos are a protected area, and human infrastructure is rather localized, popular tourist sites outside the urban areas appear to also be affected by the way human interact with finches at those sites.

The ecological and conservation implications of wildlife feeding and food provisioning have been explored extensively across a diversity of taxa (Cox & Gaston, 2018; Dubois & Cheptou, 2017; Murray et al., 2018). However, less is known about the evolutionary implications of such common human behaviors. Here, we showed that wildlife feeding and food provisioning in Darwin's finches could impact the very evolutionary process the drive diversification in this iconic birds.

In conclusion, our study focused on single urban center on a single island, and the lack of replication limits our ability to draw general inferences. Yet, our study represents a first attempt to explore the potential impacts of urbanization and human behavior on the ongoing adaptive radiation of Darwin's finches. We also hypothesize that a similar phenomenon might currently be affecting finches in urban environments across the Galápagos archipelago—a possibility that we are exploring currently in the coastal and agricultural zones of other islands. Moreover, urban effects on finch evolution might be comparatively strong, given their persistence (continued access to human foods) as opposed to the episodic nature of extreme climatic events. Our study also adds to the increasing evidence of human effects on Galápagos biodiversity. This includes the near‐extinction of the Mangrove finch resulting from the introduction of a parasitic fly (Fessl et al., 2010; Fessl, Dvorak, Vargas, & Young, 2011), and the collapse of multiple unique plant and animal species due to habitat modification, and the introduction and proliferation of alien species (Mauchamp, 1997; Rentería, Gardener, Panetta, Atkinson, & Crawley, 2012; Trueman, Atkinson, Guézou, & Wurm, 2010; Watson, Trueman, Tufet, Henderson, & Atkinson, 2010).Our findings thus accentuate the fragility of the initial stages of adaptive radiation in Darwin's finches and raise concerns about the fate of the Galápagos ecosystems in the face of increasing urbanization and human presence. Ultimately, understanding the unexpected consequences of urbanization on ecological niches might guide strategies for preserving biodiversity and the processes that generate it.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

None declared.

Supporting information

 

 

 

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Logistical support and permits were provided by the Galápagos National Park Service and the Charles Darwin Research Station. Funding was provided by the Earthwatch Institute in the form of a grant to LFD. Additional funding support was provided by Secretaría Nacional de Ciencia, Tecnología e Innovación and Sistema Nacional de Investigación (LFD and DMTS), Sigma Xi (KMG), the National Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada Vanier Scholarship (KMG), the European Society for Evolutionary Biology Godfrey Hewitt Mobility Award (KMG), McGill University (KMG), and GAIAS‐USFQ Grant (JAC). We thank the amazing group of Earthwatch volunteers who offered assistance in the field. Additional field assistance was provided by S. Carvajal‐Endara, D. Hanson, and V. Vysna.

De León LF, Sharpe DMT, Gotanda KM, et al. Urbanization erodes niche segregation in Darwin's finches. Evol Appl. 2019;12:1329–1343. 10.1111/eva.12721

DATA ACCESSIBILITY

Data are available in the Supporting Information.

REFERENCES

  1. Abbott, I. , Abbott, L. K. , & Grant, P. R. (1977). Comparative ecology of Galapagos Ground finches (Geospiza Gould): Evaluation of the importance of floristic diversity and interspecific competition. Ecological Monographs, 47, 151–184. 10.2307/1942615 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alberti, M. (2015). Eco‐evolutionary dynamics in an urbanizing planet. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 30, 114–126. 10.1016/j.tree.2014.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aronson, M. F. J. , La Sorte, F. A. , Nilon, C. H. , Katti, M. , Goddard, M. A. , Lepczyk, C. A. , … Winter, M. (2014). A global analysis of the impacts of urbanization on bird and plant diversity reveals key anthropogenic drivers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281, 20133330 10.1098/rspb.2013.3330 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Atwell, J. W. , Cardoso, G. C. , Whittaker, D. J. , Campbell‐Nelson, S. , Robertson, K. W. , & Ketterson, E. D. (2012). Boldness behavior and stress physiology in a novel urban environment suggest rapid correlated evolutionary adaptation. Behavioral Ecology, 23, 960–969. 10.1093/beheco/ars059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Benzécri, J. (1973). L'Analyse des donnees: II. La Taxonomie. Paris, France: Dunod. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bolnick, D. I. , & Paull, J. S. (2009). Morphological and dietary differences between individuals are weakly but positively correlated within a population of threespine stickleback. Evolutionary Ecology, 11, 1217–1233. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bolnick, D. I. , Svanbäck, R. , Araújo, M. S. , & Persson, L. (2007). Comparative support for the niche variation hypothesis that more generalized populations also are more heterogeneous. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 10075–10079. 10.1073/pnas.0703743104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boogert, N. J. , Monceau, K. , & Lefebvre, L. (2010). A field test of behavioural flexibility in Zenaida doves (Zenaida aurita). Behavioural Processes, 85, 135–141. 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.06.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boogert, N. J. , Reader, S. M. , & Laland, K. N. (2006). The relation between social rank, neophobia and individual learning in starlings. Animal Behavior, 72, 1229–1239. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.02.021 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bowers, M. A. , & Breland, B. (1996). Foraging of gray squirrels on an urban‐rural gradient: Use of the gud to assess anthropogenic impact. Ecological Applications, 6, 1135–1142. 10.2307/2269597 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bowman, R. I. (1961). Morphological differentiation and adaptation in the Galápagos finches. University of California Publications in Zoology, 58, 1–326. [Google Scholar]
  12. Chamberlain, D. E. , Cannon, A. R. , Toms, M. P. , Leech, D. I. , Hatchwell, B. J. , & Gaston, K. J. (2009). Avian productivity in urban landscapes: A review and meta‐analysis. Ibis (Lond, 1859), 151, 1–18. 10.1111/j.1474-919X.2008.00899.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chaves, J. A. , Cooper, E. A. , Hendry, A. P. , Podos, J. , De León, L. F. , Raeymaekers, J. A. M. , … Uy, J. A. C. (2016). Genomic variation at the tips of the adaptive radiation of Darwin's finches. Molecular Ecology, 25, 5282–5295. 10.1111/mec.13743 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Colwell, R. K. , & Futuyma, D. J. (1971). On the measurement of niche breadth and overlap. Ecology, 52, 567–576. 10.2307/1934144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Coppens, C. M. , de Boer, S. F. , & Koolhaas, J. M. (2010). Coping styles and behavioural flexibility: Towards underlying mechanisms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365, 4021–4028. 10.1098/rstb.2010.0217 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cox, D. T. C. , & Gaston, K. J. (2018). Human‐nature interactions and the consequences and drivers of provisioning wildlife. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 373, 20170092 10.1098/rstb.2017.0092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. De León, L. F. , Bermingham, E. , Podos, J. , & Hendry, A. P. (2010). Divergence with gene flow as facilitated by ecological differences: Within‐island variation in Darwin's finches. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365, 1041–1052. 10.1098/rstb.2009.0314 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. De León, L. F. , Podos, J. , Gardezi, T. , Herrel, A. , & Hendry, A. P. (2014). Darwin's finches and their diet niches: The sympatric coexistence of imperfect generalists. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 27, 1093–1104. 10.1111/jeb.12383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. De León, L. F. , Raeymaekers, J. A. M. , Bermingham, E. , Podos, J. , Herrel, A. , & Hendry, A. P. (2011). Exploring possible human influences on the evolution of Darwin's finches. Evolution, 65, 2258–2272. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01297.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. De León, L. F. , Rolshausen, G. , Bermingham, E. , Podos, J. , & Hendry, A. P. (2012). Individual specialization and the seeds of adaptive radiation in Darwin's finches. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 14, 365–380. [Google Scholar]
  21. Dirección del Parque Nacional Galápagos & Observatorio de Turismo de Galápagos (2016).Informe anual de visitantes a las áreas protegidas de Galápagos. Ecuador: Galápagos. [Google Scholar]
  22. Dirección del Parque Nacional Galápagos & Observatorio de Turismo de Galápagos (2017).Informe anual de visitantes a las áreas protegidas de Galápagos. Ecuador: Galápagos. [Google Scholar]
  23. Donihue, C. M. , & Lambert, M. R. (2014). Adaptive evolution in urban ecosystems. Ambio, 44, 194–203. 10.1007/s13280-014-0547-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dubois, J. , & Cheptou, P.‐O. (2017). Effects of fragmentation on plant adaptation to urban environments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372, 20160038 10.1098/rstb.2016.0038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ducatez, S. , Clavel, J. , & Lefebvre, L. (2015). Ecological generalism and behavioural innovation in birds: Technical intelligence or the simple incorporation of new foods? Journal of Animal Ecology, 84, 79–89. 10.1111/1365-2656.12255 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dwernychuk, L. W. , & Boag, D. A. (1972). Ducks nesting in association with gulls—an ecological trap? Canadian Journal of Zoology, 50, 559–563. 10.1139/z72-076 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Emlen, J. T. (1977). Estimating breeding season bird densities from transect counts. The Auk, 94, 455–468. [Google Scholar]
  28. Fessl, B. , Dvorak, M. , Vargas, F. H. , & Young, H. G. (2011). Recent conservation efforts and identification of the critically endangered mangrove finch Camarhynchus heliobates in Galápagos. Continga, 33, 27–33. [Google Scholar]
  29. Fessl, B. , Young, G. H. , Young, R. P. , Rodriguez‐Matamoros, J. , Dvorak, M. , Tebbich, S. , & Fa, J. E. (2010). How to save the rarest Darwin's finch from extinction: The mangrove finch on Isabela Island. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 365, 1019–1030. 10.1098/rstb.2009.0288 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gaston, K. J. (2010). Urban ecology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  31. Gause, G. F. (1934). The struggle for existence. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins Co. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gotanda, K. M. , Hendry, A. P. , & Svensson, E. I. (2017). Human influences on evolution, and the ecological and societal consequences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372, 20160028 10.1098/rstb.2016.0028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gotanda, K. M. , Sharpe, D. M. T. , & De Léon, L. F. (2015). Galapagos mockingbird (Mimus parvulus) preys on an invasive mammal. Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 127, 138–141. 10.1676/14-055.1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gotelli, N. , & Ellison, A. (2013). EcoSimR 1.00. Retrieved from https://www.uvm.edu/~ngotelli/EcoSim/EcoSim.html
  35. Gotelli, N. J. , & Entsminger, G. L. (2009). EcoSim: Null models software for ecology. Version 7. Jericho, VT: Acquired Intelligence Inc. & Kesey‐Bear; Retrieved from https://garyentsminger.com/ecosim.htm [Google Scholar]
  36. Grant, P. R. (1993). Hybridization of Darwin's finches on Isla Daphne Major, Galapagos. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 340, 127–139. 10.1098/rstb.1993.0052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Grant, P. R. (1999). Ecology and evolution of Darwin's finches. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  38. Grant, B. R. , & Grant, P. R. (1989). Evolutionary dynamics of a natural population. The large Cactus finch of the Galapagos. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Grant, B. R. , & Grant, P. R. (1996). High survival of Darwin's finch hybrids: Effects of beak morphology and diets. Ecology, 77, 500–509. 10.2307/2265625 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Grant, P. R. , & Grant, B. R. (2002). Unpredictable evolution in a 30‐year study of Darwin's finches. Science, 296, 707–711. 10.1126/science.1070315 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Grant, P. R. , & Grant, B. R. (2008). How and why species multiply: The radiation of Darwin's finches. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  42. Grant, P. R. , & Grant, B. R. (2016). Introgressive hybridization and natural selection in Darwin's finches. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 117, 812–822. 10.1111/bij.12702 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Grant, P. R. , Grant, B. R. , Markert, J. A. , Keller, L. F. , & Petren, K. (2004). Convergent evolution of Darwin's finches caused by introgressive hybridization and selection. Evolution, 58, 1588–1599. 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2004.tb01738.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Grimm, N. B. , Faeth, S. H. , Golubiewski, N. E. , Redman, C. l. , Wu, J. , Bai, X. , & Briggs, J. M. (2008). Global change and the ecology of cities. Science, 319, 756–760. 10.1126/science.1150195 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hardin, G. (1960). The competitive exclusion principle. Science, 131, 1292–1297. 10.1126/science.131.3409.1292 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hendry, A. P. , Grant, P. R. , Rosemary Grant, B. , Ford, H. A. , Brewer, M. J. , & Podos, J. (2006). Possible human impacts on adaptive radiation: Beak size bimodality in Darwin's finches. Proceedings of the Royal Society B‐Biological Sciences, 273, 1887–1894. 10.1098/rspb.2006.3534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hendry, A. P. , Huber, S. K. , De León, L. F. , Herrel, A. , & Podos, J. (2009). Disruptive selection in a bimodal population of Darwin's finches. Proceedings of the Royal Society B‐Biological Sciences, 276, 753–759. 10.1098/rspb.2008.1321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hendry, A. P. , Taylor, E. B. , & McPhail, J. D. (2002). Adaptive divergence and the balance between selection and gene flow: Lake and stream stickleback in the misty system. Evolution, 56, 1199–1216. 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2002.tb01432.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Huber, S. K. , Leon, L. F. D. , Hendry, A. P. , Bermingham, E. , & Podos, J. (2007). Reproductive isolation of sympatric morphs in a population of Darwin's finches. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274, 1709–1714. 10.1098/rspb.2007.0224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Huber, S. K., & Podos, J. (2006). Beak morphology and song features covary in a population of Darwin's finches (Geospiza fortis). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 88, 489–498. [Google Scholar]
  51. Inouye, D. W. (1978). Resource partitioning in bumblebees: Experimental studies of foraging behavior. Ecology, 59, 672–678. 10.2307/1938769 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Johnson, M. T. J. , & Munshi‐South, J. (2017). Evolution of life in urban environments. Science, 358, eaam8327. 10.1126/science.aam8327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Jones, D. (2011). An appetite for connection: Why we need to understand the effect and value of feeding wild birds. Emu, 111, i–vii. 10.1071/MUv111n2_ED [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kettlewell, H. B. D. (1955). Selection experiments on industrial melanism in the Lepidoptera. Heredity, 9, 323–342. 10.1038/hdy.1955.36 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kleindorfer, S. , Chapman, T. W. , Winkler, H. , & Sulloway, F. J. (2006). Adaptive divergence in contiguous populations of Darwin's small ground finch (Geospiza fuliginosa). Evolutionary Ecology Research, 8, 357–372. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kleindorfer, S. , O'Connor, J. A. , Dudaniec, R. Y. , Myers, S. A. , Robertson, J. , & Sulloway, F. J. (2014). Species collapse via hybridization in Darwin's tree finches. American Naturalist, 183, 325–341. 10.1086/674899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lack, D. (1947). Darwin's finches. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  58. Lamichhaney, S. , Berglund, J. , Almén, M. S. , Maqbool, K. , Grabherr, M. , Martinez‐Barrio, A. , … Andersson, L. (2015). Evolution of Darwin's finches and their beaks revealed by genome sequencing. Nature, 518, 371–375. 10.1038/nature14181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lawlor, L. R. (1980). Overlap, similarity, and competition coefficients. Ecology, 61, 245–251. 10.2307/1935181 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lê, S. , Josse, J. , & Husson, F. (2008). FactoMineR: An R package for multivariate analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 25, 1–18. [Google Scholar]
  61. Levine, J. M. , & HilleRisLambers, J. (2009). The importance of niches for the maintenance of species diversity. Nature, 461, 254–257. 10.1038/nature08251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Levins, R. (1968). Evolution in changing environments: Some theoretical explorations. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  63. Liebl, A. L. , & Martin, L. B. (2014). Living on the edge: Range edge birds consume novel foods sooner than established ones. Behavioral Ecology, 25, 1089–1096. 10.1093/beheco/aru089 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Littleford‐Colquhoun, B. L. , Clemente, C. , Whiting, M. J. , Ortiz‐Barrientos, D. , & Frère, C. H. (2017). Archipelagos of the Anthropocene: Rapid and extensive differentiation of native terrestrial vertebrates in a single metropolis. Molecular Ecology, 26, 2466–2481. 10.1111/mec.14042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lowry, H. , Lill, A. , & Wong, B. B. M. (2013). Behavioural responses of wildlife to urban environments. Biological Reviews, 88, 537–549. 10.1111/brv.12012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Macarthur, R. , & Levins, R. (1967). The limiting similarity, convergence, and divergence of coexisting species. American Naturalist, 101, 377–385. 10.1086/282505 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Martin, L. B. , & Fitzgerald, L. (2005). A taste for novelty in invading house sparrows, Passer domesticus . Behavioral Ecology, 16, 702–707. 10.1093/beheco/ari044 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Mauchamp, A. (1997). Threats from alien plant species in the Galapagos Islands. Conservation Biology, 11, 260–263. 10.1046/j.1523-1739.1997.95356.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. McKinney, M. L. (2002). Urbanization, biodiversity, and conservation. BioScience, 52, 883 10.1641/0006-3568(2002)052[0883:UBAC]2.0.CO;2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. McKinney, M. L. (2006). Urbanization as a major cause of biotic homogenization. Biological Conservation, 127, 247–260. 10.1016/j.biocon.2005.09.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. McLaughlin, M. E. , Janousek, W. M. , McCarty, J. P. , & Wolfenbarger, L. L. (2014). Effects of urbanization on site occupancy and density of grassland birds in tallgrass prairie fragments. Journal of Field Ornithology, 85, 258–273. 10.1111/jofo.12066 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Murray, M. H. , Kidd, A. D. , Curry, S. E. , Hepinstall‐Cymerman, J. , Yabsley, M. J. , Adams, H. C. , … Hernandez, S. M. (2018). From wetland specialist to hand‐fed generalist: Shifts in diet and condition with provisioning for a recently urbanized wading bird. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 373, 20170100 10.1098/rstb.2017.0100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Nicolakakis, N. , Sol, D. , & Lefebvre, L. (2003). Behavioural flexibility predicts species richness in birds, but not extinction risk. Animal Behavior, 65, 445–452. 10.1006/anbe.2003.2085 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Penick, C. A. , Savage, A. M. , & Dunn, R. R. (2015). Stable isotopes reveal links between human food inputs and urban ant diets. Proceedings of the Royal Society B‐Biological Sciences, 282, 20142608 10.1098/rspb.2014.2608 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Peters, K. J. , Myers, S. A. , Dudaniec, R. Y. , O'Connor, J. A. , & Kleindorfer, S. (2017). Females drive asymmetrical introgression from rare to common species in Darwin's tree finches. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 30, 1940–1952. 10.1111/jeb.13167 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Phillips, R. B. , Wiedenfeld, D. A. , & Snell, H. L. (2012). Current status of alien vertebrates in the Galápagos Islands: Invasion history, distribution, and potential impacts. Biological Invasions, 14, 461–480. 10.1007/s10530-011-0090-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Pianka, E. R. (1973). The structure of lizard communities. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 4, 53–74. 10.1146/annurev.es.04.110173.000413 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  78. Podos, J. (2001). Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal structure in Darwin's finches. Nature, 409, 185–188. 10.1038/35051570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. R Core Team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  80. Rentería, J. L. , Gardener, M. R. , Panetta, F. D. , Atkinson, R. , & Crawley, M. J. (2012). Possible impacts of the invasive plant Rubus niveus on the native vegetation of the Scalesia forest in the Galapagos Islands. PLoS One, 7, e48106 10.1371/journal.pone.0048106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Rhymer, J. M. , & Simberloff, D. (1996). Extinction by hybridization and introgression. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 27, 83–109. 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.27.1.83 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Roughgarden, J. (1976). Resource partitioning among competing species‐a coevolutionary approach. Theoretical Population Biology, 9, 388–424. 10.1016/0040-5809(76)90054-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Salmo, P. , Nilsson, J. F. , Nord, A. , Bensch, S. , & Isaksson, C. (2016). Urban environment shortens telomere length in nestling great tits, Parus major . Biology Letters, 12, 254–260. 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0155 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Schluter, D. (2000). The ecology of adaptive radiation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  85. Schluter, D. , & Grant, P. R. (1984). Determinants of morphological patterns in communities of Darwin's finches. American Naturalist, 123, 175–196. 10.1086/284196 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  86. Schochat, E. , Warren, P. S. , Faeth, S. H. , McIntyre, N. E. , & Hope, D. (2006). From patterns to emerging process in mechanistic urban ecology. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 21, 186–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Schoener, T. W. (1968). The Anolis lizards of Bimini: Resource partitioning in a complex fauna. Ecology, 49, 704–726. 10.2307/1935534 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  88. Schoener, T. W. (1974). Resource partitioning in ecological communities. Science, 185, 27–39. 10.1126/science.185.4145.27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Seehausen, O. , Takimoto, G. , Roy, D. , & Jokela, J. (2008). Speciation reversal and biodiversity dynamics with hybridization in changing environments. Molecular Ecology, 17, 30–44. 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2007.03529.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Seto, K. C. , Sánchez‐Rodríguez, R. , & Fragkias, M. (2010). The new geography of contemporary urbanization and the environment. Annual Review of Environment and Resources, 35, 167–194. 10.1146/annurev-environ-100809-125336 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Shochat, E. , Lerman, S. B. , Katti, M. , & Lewis, D. B. (2004). Linking optimal foraging behavior to bird community structure in an urban desert landscape: Field experiments with artificial food patches. American Naturalist, 164, 232–243. 10.1086/422222 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Simpson, G. (1953). Major features of evolution. New York, NY: Columbia University Press. [Google Scholar]
  93. Slabbekoorn, H. , & Peet, M. (2003). Birds sing at a higher pitch in urban noise. Nature, 424, 267 10.1038/424267a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Smith, T. B. , & Bernatchez, L. (2008). Evolutionary change in human‐altered environments. Molecular Ecology, 17, 1–8. 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2007.03607.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Smith, J. N. M. , Grant, P. R. , Grant, B. R. , Abbott, I. J. , & Abbott, L. K. (1978). Seasonal variation in feeding habits of Darwin's Ground finches. Ecology, 59, 1137–1150. [Google Scholar]
  96. Sol, D. , González‐Lagos, C. , Moreira, D. , Maspons, J. , & Lapiedra, O. (2014). Urbanisation tolerance and the loss of avian diversity. Ecology Letters, 17, 942–950. 10.1111/ele.12297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Sol, D. , Griffin, A. S. , Bartomeus, I. , & Boyce, H. (2011). Exploring or avoiding novel food resources? the novelty conflict in an invasive bird. PLoS One, 6, e19535 10.1371/journal.pone.0019535 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Sol, D. , Lapiedra, O. , & González‐Lagos, C. (2013). Behavioural adjustments for a life in the city. Animal Behavior, 85, 1101–1112. 10.1016/J.ANBEHAV.2013.01.023 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Sol, D. , Lefebvre, L. , & Rodríguez‐Teijeiro, J. D. (2005). Brain size, innovative propensity and migratory behaviour in temperate Palaearctic birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B‐Biological Sciences, 272, 1433–1441. 10.1098/rspb.2005.3099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Sol, D. , Timmermans, S. , & Lefebvre, L. (2002). Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds. Animal Behavior, 63, 495–502. 10.1006/ANBE.2001.1953 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  101. Sulloway, F. J. , & Kleindorfer, S. (2013). Adaptive divergence in Darwin's small ground finch (Geospiza fuliginosa): Divergent selection along a cline. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 110, 45–59. 10.1111/bij.12108 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  102. Svensson, E. , & Calsbeek, R. (2012). The adaptive landscape in evolutionary biology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  103. Taylor, E. B. , Boughman, J. W. , Groenenboom, M. , Sniatynski, M. , Schluter, D. , & Gow, J. l. (2006). Speciation in reverse: Morphological and genetic evidence of the collapse of a three‐spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) species pair. Molecular Ecology, 15, 343–355. 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2005.02794.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Tebbich, S. , Sterelny, K. , & Teschke, I. (2010). The tale of the finch: Adaptive radiation and behavioural flexibility. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365, 1099–1109. 10.1098/rstb.2009.0291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Tilman, D. (1982). Resource competition and community structure. Monographs in Population Biology, 17, 1–296. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Trueman, M. , Atkinson, R. , Guézou, A. , & Wurm, P. (2010). Residence time and human‐mediated propagule pressure at work in the alien flora of Galapagos. Biological Invasions, 12, 3949–3960. 10.1007/s10530-010-9822-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  107. Watson, J. , Trueman, M. , Tufet, M. , Henderson, S. , & Atkinson, R. (2010). Mapping terrestrial anthropogenic degradation on the inhabited islands of the Galapagos Archipelago. Oryx, 44, 79 10.1017/S0030605309990226 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  108. Wiggins, I. L. , & Porter, D. M. (1971). Flora of the Galapagos Islands. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  109. Wigginton, N. S. , Fahrenkamp‐Uppenbrink, J. , Wible, B. , & Malakoff, D. (2016). Cities are the future. Science, 352, 904–905. 10.1126/science.352.6288.904 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Winchell, K. M. , Reynolds, R. G. , Prado‐Irwin, S. R. , Puente‐Rolón, A. R. , & Revell, L. J. (2016). Phenotypic shifts in urban areas in the tropical lizard Anolis cristatellus . Evolution, 70, 1009–1022. 10.1111/evo.12925 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Wright, T. , Eberhard, J. , Hobson, E. , Avery, M. l. , & Russello, M. A. (2010). Behavioral flexibility and species invasions: The adaptive flexibility hypothesis. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 22, 393–404. 10.1080/03949370.2010.505580 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  112. Zhang, J. (2016). spaa: SPecies Association Analysis. R package version 0.2.2. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=spaa

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

 

 

 

Data Availability Statement

Data are available in the Supporting Information.


Articles from Evolutionary Applications are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES