Abstract
Rationale
Methamphetamine (MA) use is steadily increasing and thus constitutes a major public health concern. Women seem to be particularly vulnerable to developing MA use disorder, as they initiate use at a younger age and transition more quickly to problematic use. Initial drug responses may predict subsequent use, but little information exists on potential gender differences in the acute effects of MA prior to dependence.
Objective
We examined gender differences in the acute effects of MA on subjective mood and reward-related behavior in healthy, non-dependent humans.
Methods
Men (n = 44) and women (n = 29) completed 4 sessions in which they received placebo or MA under double-blind conditions twice each. During peak drug effect, participants completed the monetary incentive delay task to assess reaction times to cues signaling potential monetary losses or gains, in an effort to determine if MA would potentiate reward-motivated behavior. Cardiovascular and subjective drug effects were assessed throughout sessions.
Results
Overall, participants responded more quickly to cues predicting incentivized trials, particularly large-magnitude incentives, than to cues predicting no incentive. MA produced faster reaction times in women, but not in men. MA produced typical stimulant-like subjective and cardiovascular effects in all participants, but subjective ratings of vigor and (reduced) sedation were greater in women than in men.
Conclusions
Women appear to be more sensitive to the psychomotor-related behavioral and subjective effects of MA. These findings provide initial insight into gender differences in acute effects of MA that may contribute to gender differences in problematic MA use.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (10.1007/s00213-019-05276-2) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Keywords: Methamphetamine, Monetary incentive delay, Gender differences, Sex differences, Subjective effects, Psychomotor activation
Introduction
Methamphetamine (MA) is the second most commonly used illicit drug, behind marijuana, and MA use disorder accounts for the majority of persons entering treatment for drug use globally (DEA 2015; UNODC 2015). As such, understanding factors that contribute to MA use disorder is of high societal importance, particularly due to the host of negative consequences associated with prolonged use (Baicy and London 2007; Brecht et al. 2004). Initial investigations into the antecedents and consequences of substance use disorders, as well as many preclinical research efforts, have primarily focused on male users (for review, see Fattore et al. 2008). However, accumulating evidence suggests that, as compared with men, female stimulant drug users may be more vulnerable to developing problematic drug use patterns (Fattore et al. 2008). Specifically in regard to MA, women consume more drug than men, transition from recreational use to dependence more quickly, and are significantly younger than their male counterparts (Brecht et al. 2004; Hser et al. 2005; Rawson et al. 2005). While it is not yet evident why women are more susceptible to problematic MA use, one possible factor is their initial response to the drug, as initial responses to drugs appear to be predictive of future use across a variety of drug classes (de Wit and Phillips 2012). Therefore, understanding differences in how men and women acutely respond to MA may help to clarify why women are more vulnerable to developing MA-related problems.
Initial use of stimulant drugs such as MA is typically characterized by positive subjective effects, including increased energy, a sense of well-being, and euphoria (Cho 1990). For many drugs of abuse, including stimulants, initial positive subjective responses are associated with a greater likelihood of continued drug use (de Wit and Phillips 2012). Retrospective accounts suggest that positive responses to stimulant drugs during initial use predict a shorter latency to subsequent use (Davidson et al. 1993) and an increased risk of developing a substance use disorder (Lambert et al. 2006). Prospective accounts originating from controlled laboratory settings provide additional support. Non-dependent humans who report more pleasant, stimulant-like effects following amphetamine administration in the laboratory are more likely to take the drug again when given the opportunity in subsequent experimental sessions (Chait 1993; de Wit et al. 1986; Johanson and Uhlenhuth 1980). As such, understanding potential gender differences in the subjective effects of MA, prior to the development of dependence, may elucidate factors that contribute to differing rates of problematic use.
Variations in responses to acute doses of stimulant drugs may also be detected with objective, behavioral measures. Female rodents are more sensitive to the psychomotor effects of cocaine, amphetamine, and MA (Becker 1999; Hu and Becker 2003; Milesi-Hallé et al. 2007; Ohia-Nwoko et al. 2017; Schindler et al. 2002). While female rodents are also more sensitive to the reinforcing effects of cocaine (Lynch and Carroll 1999, 2000), this sex difference appears to be less evident with amphetamine and MA (Mattei and Carlini 1996; Schindler et al. 2002; Stöhr et al. 1998). In human laboratory studies, stimulant drugs also improve psychomotor performance (de Wit et al. 2002; Johnson et al. 2000), but there are few reports on the potential contribution of gender differences to these effects in controlled laboratory settings (White et al. 2002). However, women who use MA recreationally report taking the drug primarily to increase efficiency and productivity (Dluzen and Liu 2008). Thus, in women, sensitivity to the psychomotor-activating effects of MA combined with a desire to use the drug to enhance behavior may result in a higher likelihood of continued drug use.
In humans, acute administration of stimulant drugs increases extracellular dopamine (DA) within reward-related neurocircuitry, including the ventral striatum (Buckholtz et al. 2010; Leyton 2002). Anticipation of non-drug (e.g., monetary) reward activates these same areas (Haber and Knutson 2010; Knutson et al. 2001b). Moreover, greater striatal activation during anticipation of reward is associated with more positive subjective response to a stimulant drug in non-dependent humans (Crane et al. 2018). Thus, stimulant drugs may further potentiate responses to rewards, presumably through effects on DA neurotransmission. This effect may be especially pronounced in individuals more sensitive to the effects of the drug. Consistent with this, preclinical evidence shows that amphetamine infusion into the nucleus accumbens increases the incentive value of a non-drug reward, such that animals will work more for a sucrose reward under the influence of amphetamine (Wyvell and Berridge 2000).
In the present study, we aimed to explore how MA influences behavioral motivation in response to reward-related cues in healthy men and women, and whether those more sensitive to the behavioral effects of MA would also be more sensitive to the subjective drug effects. Participants completed four drug administration sessions in which they received methamphetamine (MA, 2 sessions) or placebo (PBO, 2 sessions) in alternating order. Subjective mood and drug responses, as well as cardiovascular drug effects, were assessed at regular intervals. During peak drug effect (30 min post-drug administration), participants completed a modified version of the monetary incentive delay (MID) task. We predicted that incentivized cues, particularly those of large magnitude, would elicit the fastest reaction times. We hypothesized that women would be more sensitive to the behavioral and subjective effects than men and that reward-related cues would act synergistically with drug effects to potentiate cue-elicited behavior.
Materials and methods
Study design
Participants (n = 73, 29 women) completed four sessions in which they received either methamphetamine (MA, 20 mg oral) or placebo under double-blind conditions as part of a larger study investigating conditioned drug responses (Mayo and de Wit 2015). Both drug and placebo were administered twice in alternating order (MA, PBO, MA, PBO or PBO, MA, PBO, MA), and the drug order (i.e., MA first or PBO first) was counterbalanced across participants. During each 4-h-long session, participants completed a modified version of the MID task during the window of estimated peak drug effect (30 min post-administration; see Fig. 1). Cardiovascular and subjective measures regarding drug effects and mood were obtained at multiple time points through all sessions.
Participants
Healthy men and women aged 18–35 were recruited from the community via posters, newspaper and online advertisements, and word of mouth referral. Demographic information regarding participants is reported in Table 1. Inclusion criteria were as follows: body mass index (BMI) between 19 and 26 kg/m2, completed high school education, fluency in English, resting blood pressure (BP) < 140/90 mmHg, current alcohol consumption < 4 standard drinks/day, and caffeinated beverage consumption < 4 units per day. Exclusion criteria were as follows: current major Axis I DSM-IV disorder (APA 2000), including current or past alcohol or drug dependence, or psychotic disorder; diagnosis of a mood disorder within the past year; ongoing treatment with psychoactive medication; cardiovascular illness; past or present medical conditions contraindicating methamphetamine use; shift work; pregnancy, nursing, or plans to become pregnant. Women not on hormonal birth control were scheduled for testing sessions only during the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle, as subjective drug effects are dampened during the luteal phase (White et al. 2002).
Table 1.
N (% of total) | |||
Total sample | Men | Women | |
Gender | 3 (44 M/29 W) | 44 (60%) | 29 (40%) |
Race | |||
Caucasian | 44 (60%) | 23 (52%) | 21 (72%) |
African American | 11 (15%) | 8 (18%) | 3 (10%) |
Asian | 11 (15%) | 8 (18%) | 3 (10%) |
Other | 7 (10%) | 5 (11%) | 2 (7%) |
Mean (± SD) | |||
Age (years) | 24.8 (3.48) | 25.4 (3.86) | 23.7 (2.52) |
Education (years) | 15.7 (1.70) | 15.6 (1.70) | 15.8 (1.72) |
BMI | 22.6 (1.86) | 22.9 (1.78) | 22.1 (1.93) |
Body weight | 70.90 (9.14) | 74.9 (7.98) | 64.7 (7.24) |
Current drug use | Mean (± SD) | ||
Caffeine/day | 2.01 (3.66) | 2.28 (4.76) | 1.60 (0.66) |
Cigarettes/day | 3.64 (5.33) | 4.07 (6.13) | 2.57 (2.89) |
Drinks/week | 7.04 (5.30) | 6.97 (5.46) | 7.14 (5.16) |
Marijuana/month | 9.56 (13.5) | 11.9 (15.0) | 2.50 (1.29) |
Lifetime drug use | Ever used (% of total) | ||
Marijuana | 55 (75%) | 31 (70%) | 25 (86%) |
Opiates | 29 (40%) | 15 (34%) | 14 (48%) |
Stimulants | 23 (32%) | 14 (32%) | 9 (31%) |
Hallucinogens | 20 (27%) | 14 (32%) | 6 (21%) |
MDMA | 15 (21%) | 8 (18%) | 7 (24%) |
Current drug use includes values only for those who regularly use; cigarettes per day for those who have used in the past month (N = 14; 10 M, 4 W); marijuana per month for those who have used in the past month (N = 16; 12 M, 4 W)
Screening included a physical examination and electrocardiogram. Participants received instructions regarding upcoming sessions and completed an abbreviated practice version of the MID task. They also read and signed the consent form which outlined the study procedures and drugs they may receive along with their effects. To prevent expectancy effects, participants were informed that they could be given placebo, a stimulant, or a sedative drug, and they were told that the purpose of the study was to investigate the effects of drugs on mood and task performance. Following completion of the study, participants were fully debriefed. The study took place at the Human Behavioral Pharmacology Laboratory at the University of Chicago Hospital and was approved by the University of Chicago Biological Science Division Institutional Review Board. Participants were paid for their participation. The present analysis was conducted with the 73 individuals (44 men, 29 women) for whom complete MID data were available from the primary study (Mayo and de Wit 2015).
Drug
MA was administered orally. The dose used (20 mg oral; Desoxyn, Lundbeck) is safe and produces robust subjective effects (Cook et al. 1992; Martin et al. 1971; Mayo et al. 2013; Söderpalm et al. 2003). To speed up the absorption time, tablets were crushed and administered in 10 ml sugar-free syrup (Ora-Sweet, Paddock Laboratories, Minneapolis, MN) (Mayo et al. 2013; Söderpalm et al. 2003). Placebo solutions consisted of 10 ml Ora-Sweet alone and were indistinguishable from active drug by appearance or taste.
Monetary incentive delay task
The MID task (Knutson et al. 2000, 2001a, b) was used to assess behavioral motivation (i.e., reaction time) to cues signaling potential monetary gains and losses or no monetary incentive. The task consisted of 40 trials each lasting up to 12 s. A trial consisted of a 1.5-s presentation of a cue (gain, loss, or no incentive), followed by a fixation crosshair of a variable duration (1.3–4.0 s). A white target box was then displayed for 0.5 s, signaling subjects to press the response button. After another variable interval (1.3–4.0 s), subjects were given feedback for 1.5 s about the amount of money they had won or lost during that trial, as well as their cumulative total. Trials were separated by 1.3–4.0 s. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible by pressing a button every time the white target box appeared. Cues signaled potential reward (denoted by circles), loss prevention (denoted by squares), or no monetary outcome (denoted by triangles). In addition to the type of cue (win, loss, or no incentive), we also varied the magnitude of the cue. Potential reward trials included trials in which a low reward amount ($0.25; denoted by a circle with 1 horizontal line) or a high reward amount ($1.00; denoted by a circle with 3 horizontal lines) could be at stake, whereas potential loss prevention trials included trials in which a low amount ($0.25; denoted by a square with 1 horizontal line) or a high amount ($1.00; denoted by a square with 3 horizontal lines) could be lost. “Hit” trials in which the participant responded during the 0.5s target box presentation resulted in monetary gain (gain trials) or prevention of monetary loss (loss trials). If the participant responded too slow (e.g. over 0.5s), “miss” trials resulted in no gain (gain trials) or monetary loss (loss trials). Trial types were pseudorandomly ordered within each session, and the task lasted approximately 8 min. To ensure active participation during the task, participants were informed prior to the start of the task that they would receive earnings from the task in cash at the end of the session.
Subjective and physiological measures
During each of the sessions, participants completed self-report computer-based standardized subjective mood and drug effects questionnaires at − 15, + 15, + 30, + 70, + 115, and + 200 min relative to drug administration. Measures included Profile of Mood States (McNair et al. 1971), Addiction Research Center Inventory (Haertzen 1966), and the Drug Effects Questionnaire (Johanson and Uhlenhuth 1980). We also assessed blood pressure and heart rate using portable monitors (Omron, Lake Forest, IL).
Session procedures
Prior to the potential drug administration sessions, participants were instructed to consume their normal amount of caffeine/nicotine before sessions but to abstain from using prescription and over-the-counter drugs 24 h before and 6 h after each session, other recreational drugs 2 days before and 6 h after sessions, and marijuana 7 days before and 6 h after each session and to not operate any machinery or vehicles 6 h following each session. Compliance measures were obtained before all sessions and included a self-report questionnaire, a breath alcohol level test (Alco-Sensor III, Intoximeters, St. Louis, MO), a urine drug test (ToxCup, Branan Medical Corporation, Irvine, CA), and, for women, a pregnancy test (AimStick PBD, hCG professional, Craig Medical Distribution, Vista, CA).
Sessions were conducted from 0900 to 1300 h, with at least 48 h between sessions. After compliance tests (BAL, urine toxicology, and pregnancy), predrug measures were obtained and subjects received the drug. Study rooms were comfortably furnished with a couch, a desk and chair, a computer (for tasks and questionnaires), a television, a video player, and magazines. When participants were not completing tasks or questionnaires, they were allowed to relax, watch selected movies, or read, but were not permitted to sleep, work, or study. Cellphones and internet access were not allowed in order to minimize emotional contact outside the testing environment. The MID task was performed between time points + 30 and + 70 min after study drug administration. Participants were paid earnings from the behavioral tasks at the end of each session. They were allowed to leave once subjective and cardiovascular drug effects subsided.
Statistics
We compared behavioral measures (reaction time, hit rate) separately for the two MA sessions and the two PBO sessions. Responses during these two sets of sessions did not differ, and so responses were averaged for the two drug sessions, as well as the two placebo sessions (Mayo and de Wit 2015). Demographic factors (age, gender, body weight, drug use history, etc.) were entered as covariates or between-subject factors in all analyses and only included in the final model when significant. We found gender to be a significant between-subject factor influencing reaction time and thus included it in subsequent analyses. We verified that the weight body mass index (BMI) was not a significant covariate (see Supplemental Results).
Subjective and cardiovascular effects
We created peak change scores from the six time points (relative to administration of drug, − 15, + 15, + 30, + 70, + 115, and + 200) to represent the magnitude of drug effects. Effects of MA vs PBO self-report questionnaires were assessed using the number of scales (ARCI—6, POMS—8, DEQ—5) and the treatments (MA, PBO) as within-subject factors in a RM-ANOVA with gender as a between-subject factor. We expected a main effect of scale, as each scale measures a relatively distinct subjective effect, and thus focused on scale × gender interactions. Significant interactions were followed up using Benjamini-Hochberg (B-H) post hoc tests with a false discovery rate of 0.10. Uncorrected p values are provided, and those that do not remain significant following B-H correction are noted. Cardiovascular effects, blood pressure, and heart rate were treated similarly. Blood pressure was represented as mean arterial pressure, calculated as follows: MAP = (systolic BP + 2 × diastolic BP) ∕ 3. In addition, although only 73 (44 men, 29 women) participants had complete behavioral data, we had complete subjective and cardiovascular data from 90 participants (50 men, 40 women). Thus, we validated our subjective and cardiovascular findings in the current 73-participant sample with the larger 90-participant sample (see Supplemental Materials).
MID task
Using mixed-effects analyses (also called multilevel analyses or hierarchical regression) (Blackwell et al. 2006; Hox 2010), we assessed gender differences in the influence of MA on reward processing modeled by the MID task. Mixed-effects analyses account for the nested structure of the data, where each participant contributed data in both PBO and MA sessions. They furthermore allow us to model between-subjects variation by including random effects, and assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity of residuals are relaxed.
We tested a two-level model, with drug and MID trial type nested within participants and gender as a level 2 factor. MID trial type (high gain, low gain, no incentive, low loss, high loss), drug (MA, PBO), and gender were included as fixed factors. For the MID trial type, the reference trial was defined as the no-incentive trial, comparing low and high magnitudes of gain and loss trials with those without an incentive. Interaction effects between all factors were included. Since our primary interest was to assess interaction effects between gender and drug, but not trial type, we dropped non-significant interaction effects including the trial type to make the model more parsimonious.
Significant interaction effects were followed up by estimating marginal means and their 95% confidence intervals. Furthermore, responses on the vigor subscale of the POMS and the PCAG (sedative-like) subscale of the ARCI, completed during both PBO and MA sessions, were included as fixed covariates as significant drug × gender interaction effects on these scales were found in the data at hand. However, the inclusion of the subjective data did not alter the results and was therefore excluded from further consideration. A random intercept was included to account for individual variability (see Supplementary Fig. 1 for a visual representation of variation in RT) as well as a random slope to assess variability in individual drug response. Coefficients (β) and their standard errors (SE) are reported, and degrees of freedom were calculated using the Satterthwaite method. All statistical analyses were carried out with IBM SPSS Statistics version 25.
Subjective and cardiovascular measures
Subjective mood and drug effect ratings and cardiovascular effects are summarized in Table 2, in which we denote significant main effects of drug and drug × gender interactions (see also Fig. 1). Below, we focus on significant scale × gender interactions. More extensive statistical results regarding expected main effects can be found in supplemental materials.
Table 2.
Overall (N = 73) | Men (N = 44) | Women (N = 29) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PBO | MA | PBO | MA | PBO | MA | |
POMS | ||||||
Friendly | − 1.79 (3.49) | 1.40 (4.74)*# | − 1.20 (2.92) | 0.94 (4.50) | − 2.67 (4.11) | 2.10 (5.09) |
Anxious | − 0.12 (1.58) | 0.64 (2.86) | − 0.08 (1.64) | 0.84 (2.73) | − 0.19 (1.50) | 0.34 (3.07) |
Elation | − 1.12 (2.32) | 2.35 (3.63)* | − 0.69 (2.45) | 2.11 (3.42) | − 1.76 (1.98) | 2.71 (3.96) |
Anger | − 0.29 (1.90) | − 0.36 (1.41) | − 0.46 (2.04) | − 0.14 (1.40) | − 0.67 (1.62) | − 0.71 (1.37) |
Fatigue | 1.01 (2.70) | − 0.99 (2.39)* | 0.49 (2.76) | − 0.95 (2.27) | 1.81 (2.44) | − 1.05 (2.62) |
Depression | − 0.38 (1.82) | − 0.51 (2.23) | − 0.41 (2.06) | − 0.16 (1.92) | − 0.33 (1.43) | − 1.05 (2.59) |
Confusion | 0.40 (2.02) | − 0.14 (2.01) | 0.05 (2.04) | − 0.15 (1.92) | 0.93 (1.89) | − 0.14 (1.85) |
Vigor | − 2.01 (3.28) | 2.70 (5.25)*# | − 1.43 (3.52) | 1.77 (4.52) | − 2.90 (2.70) | 4.10 (6.00) |
ARCI | ||||||
A | − 0.12 (1.33) | 3.30 (2.89)* | 0.02 (1.49) | 3.03 (3.08) | − 0.33 (1.02) | 3.71 (2.56) |
MBG | − 0.18 (1.74) | 5.27 (4.38)* | − 0.07 (2.11) | 4.94 (4.68) | − 0.36 (0.92) | 5.78 (3.89) |
LSD | 0.31 (1.35) | 1.05 (1.91)* | 0.33 (1.44) | 1.20 (1.92) | 0.28 (1.25) | 0.83 (1.89) |
BG | − 1.45 (1.64) | 2.03 (3.00)* | − 1.06 (1.72) | 1.98 (3.06) | − 2.03 (1.34) | 2.12 (2.95) |
PCAG | 2.41 (2.83) | − 0.08 (3.22)*# | 1.84 (2.99) | 0.25 (3.41) | 3.28 (2.36) | − 0.57 (2.90) |
M | 0.90 (1.15) | 3.69 (2.16)* | 0.92 (1.21) | 3.63 (2.36) | 0.86 (1.05) | 3.79 (1.83) |
DEQ | ||||||
Feel drug | 17.6 (15.7) | 47.2 (23.4)* | 18.9 (17.0) | 46.5 (24.5) | 15.7 (13.8) | 48.2 (20.3) |
Like effects | 20.3 (21.2) | 62.0 (22.8)* | 24.3 (23.4) | 62.5 (29.2) | 14.2 (15.8) | 61.3 (28.5) |
Dislike effects | 23.3 (22.7) | 25.4 (21.1) | 23.9 (23.9) | 24.3 (21.9) | 22.6 (21.1) | 27.1 (20.0) |
Feel high | 10.5 (13.7) | 34.7 (25.0)* | 11.6 (15.7) | 32.7 (25.7) | 8.74 (10.1) | 37.7 (25.7) |
Want more | 16.6 (21.4) | 58.8 (30.7)* | 20.0 (24.1) | 60.9 (31.6) | 11.5 (15.5) | 55.6 (29.6) |
Cardiovascular | ||||||
Heart rate | − 7.87 (9.84) | 4.49 (12.1)* | − 8.44 (7.58) | 3.45 (11.7) | − 7.03 (12.5) | 6.02 (12.6) |
Blood pressure | − 4.14 (7.04) | 11.2 (10.1)* | − 5.48 (5.44) | 9.65 (11.4) | − 2.06 (8.69) | 13.7 (7.11) |
Values represent peak change scores from baseline at each session ± standard deviation. POMS, Profile of Mood States; ARCI, Addiction Research Center Inventory; A, amphetamine scale; MGB, morphine-Benzedrine group scale; LSD, lysergic acid diethylamide scale; BG, Benzedrine group scale; PCAG, pentobarbital, chlorpromazine, and alcohol group scale; M, marijuana scale; DEQ, Drug Effects Questionnaire. Blood pressure calculated as mean arterial pressure ((systolic BP + 2 × diastolic BP) ∕ 3). *p < 0.05, effect of drug; #p < 0.05, drug × gender interaction. Bold values are significant at corrected p < 0.05; italicized values are significant at uncorrected p < 0.05
POMS
There was a significant drug × gender interaction for the vigor scale such that women reported a greater increase in vigor than men (p = 0.009; Fig. 1a). There was also a trend towards a drug × gender interaction for the friendliness scale (p = 0.041), again with women reporting a greater increase than men, but this did not remain significant after correction for multiple comparisons. These effects were not influenced by body weight (vigor, p = 0.98; friendly, p = 0.48) or BMI (vigor, p = 0.27; friendly, p = 0.41). Across both genders, MA produced the expected stimulant-like effects (main effect of drug (F(1,71) = 57.6), p < 0.001), including ratings of friendliness (p < 0.001), elation (p < 0.001), and vigor (p < 0.001), and reduced fatigue (p < 0.001).
ARCI
There was a significant drug × gender interaction on the ARCI PCAG (pentobarbital, chlorpromazine, and alcohol group; sedative-like effects) scale such that women reported a greater reduction in sedative effects than men (p = 0.011; Fig. 1b) following drug administration. This effect was not influenced by body weight (p = 0.91) or BMI (p = 0.55). In both genders, MA increased the following scales (main effect of drug (F(1,71) = 116), p < 0.001): A (p < 0.001), MBG (p < 0.001), BG (p < 0.001), M (p < 0.001), and LSD (p = 0.014; no longer significant when correcting for multiple comparisons).
DEQ
There was no effect of gender on any DEQ scale. MA increased DEQ ratings (main effect of drug (F(1,71) = 197), p < 0.001) of “feel drug” (p < 0.001), “like effects” (p < 0.001), “feel high” (p < 0.001), and “want more” (p < 0.001).
Cardiovascular effects
MA increased heart rate (F(1,72) = 56.4, p < 0.001), but there was no effect of gender and no drug × gender interaction. MA also increased blood pressure (MAP; F(1,71) = 140, p < 0.001). There was a significant effect of gender (F(1,71) = 5.01, p = 0.028) such that men had lower mean arterial pressure across both sessions, but this was unaffected by drug (drug × gender: p = 0.82).
MID task
Results are presented in Tables 3 and 4 and Fig. 2. The mean reaction time across all participants, trial types, and drug conditions was 268.8 ms. The low loss trial did not significantly differ from the no-incentive trial. Participants reacted slower to the low-gain cue, but significantly faster to the high-incentive cues compared with no incentive. Men and women significantly differed in mean reaction times regardless of drug administration, with women having slower reaction times than men. MA did not have a main effect on reaction time, but significantly decreased reaction times in women only (Table 4).
Table 3.
β | SE | t (df) | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 268.82 | 4.79 | 56.15 (110) | <0.001 |
Gender | 24.09 | 9.04 | 3.34 (90.37) | 0.001 |
Drug | − 1.98 | 2.99 | − 0.655 (73) | 0.515 |
Trial type | ||||
Low loss | − 3.49 | 2.34 | − 1.49 (584) | 0.137 |
High loss | − 15.48 | 2.34 | − 6.62 (584) | <0.001 |
Low gain | 7.78 | 2.34 | 3.33 (584) | 0.001 |
High gain | − 16.23 | 2.34 | − 6.94 (584) | <0.001 |
Drug × gender | − 12.82 | 4.74 | − 2.7 (73) | 0.009 |
Italicized values are significant at p < 0.05
Table 4.
Men | Women | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Estimated marginal mean | 95% confidence interval | Estimated marginal mean | 95% confidence interval | |
PBO | 263.3 | 254.3–272.4 | 287.4 | 276.3–298.6 |
MA | 261.4 | 252.3–270.4 | 272.7 | 261.5–283.8 |
PBO, placebo; MA, methamphetamine
Estimated marginal means and 95% confidence intervals around them were calculated to further understand the interaction effect between drug and gender. These results indicate that women respond significantly faster on MA compared with PBO, but there is no difference in men (Fig. 2).
Discussion
In this study, we assessed the effect of MA on cue-elicited behavior using the MID task in healthy, non-dependent men and women. Overall, cues signaling potential monetary losses or gains, particularly those signaling high-magnitude incentives, elicited the fastest reaction times. We found that MA had a robust effect on behavior in women, producing faster reaction times across all trial types, but there was no effect of MA on behavior in men. MA produced the typical profile of subjective effects across both genders. However, we found that MA resulted in greater ratings of vigor (POMS) and reduced sedation (ARCI—PCAG scale) to a larger degree in women as compared with men. We did not find gender differences in the cardiovascular effects of MA, nor were the gender effects influenced by body weight or BMI, suggesting that the results presented are likely not attributable solely to straightforward pharmacokinetic differences. Thus, we find gender differences in the effects of MA that are consistent across behavioral and subjective measurements and as such highlight novel gender differences in the acute effects of MA that may contribute to reported gender differences in MA use and abuse.
Overall, we find that MA administration produces faster reaction times in women, but does not significantly affect behavior in men. This is in line with preclinical research suggesting that female rodents are more sensitive to the psychomotor-activating effects of MA than males (Milesi-Hallé et al. 2007; Ohia-Nwoko et al. 2017; Schindler et al. 2002). Moreover, we find that MA increased ratings of vigor (POMS) and reduced ratings of sedation (ARCI—PCAG) to a greater extent in women than in men. Our mixed-effects model did not find a significant interaction between behavior and subjective measures, suggesting that these behavioral and subjective effects may be reflective of a shared underlying construct. As a result, women show heightened sensitivity to the psychomotor-related effects of MA, regardless of whether these effects are assessed subjectively or behaviorally. This corroborates previous reports of enhanced sensitivity to MA, but not amphetamine, in healthy women (White et al. 2018).
It should be noted that we did see significant differences between genders at the placebo session, such that men responded faster to all cues in the absence of MA. Consequently, it is possible that we did not see an effect of drug in men due to a ceiling effect of them responding so quickly during the placebo session. However, the fact that high-magnitude incentives were able to elicit faster reaction times than low-magnitude incentives suggests that men were indeed capable of speedier reaction times, but that MA itself was not sufficient to reduce reaction times in these individuals. This is further supported by previous studies suggesting that monetary incentives elicit faster reaction times in men than in women (Spreckelmeyer et al. 2009), suggesting that the differences in reaction times during the placebo condition are indeed representative of a reliable gender difference in cue-elicited behavior.
We used the MID task to assess whether MA would influence behavioral responses to reward-related cues. Neuroimaging studies using the MID task have shown that anticipation of monetary gains or losses elicits activation of the ventral striatum that scales with reward magnitude (Knutson et al. 2000, 2001a, b). Moreover, multimodal brain imaging studies suggest that DA release in these areas is correlated with reward anticipatory activation (Weiland et al. 2014). Stimulant drugs also enhance striatal DA release in humans (Buckholtz et al. 2010; Oswald et al. 2007). Thus, we predicted that the dopaminergic effects of the reward cues and MA administration may act synergistically to potentiate behavioral reactions. However, we instead found that MA either increased reaction times in response to all cues (e.g., in women) or had no effect on response to any cues (e.g., in men), regardless of whether the cue signaled potential monetary loss and gain or no incentive. Thus, we failed to find a facilitation of reward-elicited behavior by MA.
It is possible that our slight modifications of the task may have influenced our findings. The more commonly used fMRI version of the MID has a titrated target presentation time to ensure that only 2/3 of trials are successfully completed (Knutson et al. 2001a). In our modified version of the task, the target presentation was fixed, and as a result, participants responded accurately at a much higher rate (approximately 98%). Thus, the reward-associated cues might have elicited greater incentivized behavior if the rewards were less frequent. However, others (Hasler et al. 2009) have assessed behavioral responses in the MID following catecholamine depletion, which reduces the amount of available dopamine, norepinephrine, and epinephrine. These authors report that catecholamine depletion in women with remitted major depression shows deficits in reward-related behavior. However, consistent with our results, catecholamine depletion produced slower reaction times across all trial types, not only incentivized trials. This study used the more standard version of the MID task, in which target presentation times are based on individual reaction times, and still found that depleting catecholamines influenced behavior across all trial types. As such, it is unlikely that our results are a consequence of these minor task modifications. Moreover, it lends further support to our findings, as modulation of dopamine in the opposite direction (i.e., a reduction via catecholamine depletion) produced slower reaction times. Interestingly, in both cases, these effects are reported only in women.
Women and men who regularly consume MA report different reasons for drug consumption. Although both genders report that availability of the drug is the primary reason for use, women report that the second most common reason is to increase energy and productivity, while men report curiosity as a reason for using (Cretzmeyer et al. 2003). Here, we find that in a fairly stimulant-naïve population (approximately 30% with previous use), women report greater increases in the psychomotor-related effects of MA. Thus, in the early stages of MA use, women may be more sensitive to MA’s ability to enhance behavioral output. Moreover, they may be more likely to consume MA again due to these psychomotor-related effects. These effects may contribute to the faster transition from initial to problematic MA use that has been reported in women (Dluzen and Liu 2008).
Mechanistic differences in the pharmacodynamics of individual stimulant drugs likely contribute to differences in their use and abuse potential. There is some evidence that MA is more effective than amphetamine in releasing serotonin and that MA is more toxic to serotonergic than to dopaminergic systems (Kuczenski et al. 1995; Sabol et al. 1995). Moreover, extensive sexual dimorphism exists in the serotonergic system between males and females (Zhang et al. 1999), suggesting that these serotonergic effects may influence males and females differently. Accordingly, women more frequently report using MA to escape emotional problems and cope with mood (Cretzmeyer et al. 2003; Semple et al. 2004) and have higher rates of comorbid depression (Hser et al. 2005). In the analysis of our larger data set (N = 90; 50 men, 40 women), we find that women report more positive mood-related subjective effects following MA administration, including increased ratings of friendliness, elation, and reduced depressive ratings. These effects did not reach statistical significance in the smaller sample, though the directionality of effects was in concordance. While preliminary, this may serve as initial evidence supporting the hypothesis that MA use in women may be related to the positive effect on subjective mood. Subsequent studies may provide further insight by including women with a greater range of depressive symptomatology to determine if the positive effects of MA are even more pronounced in those with greater mood deficits.
Previous studies have highlighted differences in subjective response to stimulant drugs throughout the menstrual cycle (White et al. 2002). Women in the current study who were not on hormonal birth control were only tested during the follicular phase, as subjective responses during this phase do not differ from men. This somewhat limits the interpretation of our effects to this menstrual cycle phase. Studies assessing effects of stimulant drugs on the menstrual cycle have primarily involved cocaine or amphetamine (Lukas et al. 1996; White et al. 2002). Future work may determine how similar the effects of MA are to those of amphetamine across the menstrual cycle and if behavioral effects are consistent across the menstrual cycle phase. These data may potentially highlight time periods in which women may be particularly resilient, or more vulnerable, to MA abuse or attempt at MA treatment.
A major strength of this study is the large sample size. However, this strength is somewhat limited by the fact that we only used a single dose of MA. It is not known if these effects would be apparent in higher or lower doses. In addition, we did not dose based on body weight but instead used a standard dose (20 mg), comparable with previous studies (Ballard et al. 2015; Mayo et al. 2013), though the effects reported here were not driven by differences in body weight. Moreover, genetic variation at the cytochrome P450 isozyme CYP2D6 can influence the metabolism of methamphetamine, and we did not control for this, nor do we know if this influences metabolism to different degrees in men and women (De La Torre et al. 2012). Finally, our sample included healthy young adults who may not be representative of those most vulnerable to MA use in naturalistic, real-world settings. For instance, we used oral dosing, whereas most MA users transition to smoked or intravenous administration (Brecht et al. 2004). The route of drug administration of stimulant drugs can contribute to abuse potential (Lile et al. 2011). Thus, one should take caution when extrapolating the effects reported in healthy adults to dependent MA users.
In summary, the current study highlights novel gender differences in the behavioral and subjective effects of MA in healthy, non-dependent humans. Our results suggest that women are particularly sensitive to the psychomotor-activating effects of MA during acute administration, as assessed via self-report and behavior. Heightened sensitivity to these behavioral and subjective effects in women may, in combination with other factors, promote future drug use. This may be particularly true for women who consume MA to enhance productivity and efficiency and eventually contribute to the increased MA-related negative consequences experienced by women compared with men.
Electronic supplementary material
Funding information
This work was supported by NIDA grant DA02812 (HdW). At the time of data collection, LMM was supported by DA32015 (Diversity Supplement).
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
This article belongs to a Special Issue on Translational Computational Psychopharmacology.
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- APA (2000) DSM-IV-TR: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Retrieved from https://www.alibris.com/DSM-IV-TR-Diagnostic-and-Statistical-Manual-of-Mental-Disorders-American-Psychiatric-Association/book/14916076
- Baicy K, London ED. Corticolimbic dysregulation and chronic methamphetamine abuse. Addiction. 2007;102(Suppl 1):5–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01777.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballard ME, Weafer J, Gallo DA, Wit H d. Effects of acute methamphetamine on emotional memory formation in humans: encoding vs consolidation. PLoS One. 2015;10(2):e0117062. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker JB. Gender differences in dopaminergic function in striatum and nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1999;64(4):803–812. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00168-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackwell E, de Leon CFM, Miller GE. Applying mixed regression models to the analysis of repeated-measures data in psychosomatic medicine. Psychosom Med. 2006;68(6):870–878. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000239144.91689.ca. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brecht M-L, O’Brien A, von Mayrhauser C, Anglin MD. Methamphetamine use behaviors and gender differences. Addict Behav. 2004;29(1):89–106. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(03)00082-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckholtz JW, Treadway MT, Cowan RL, Woodward ND, Li R, Ansari MS, Baldwin RM, Schwartzman AN, Shelby ES, Smith CE, Kessler RM, Zald DH. Dopaminergic network differences in human impulsivity. Science. 2010;329(5991):532–532. doi: 10.1126/science.1185778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chait LD. Factors influencing the reinforcing and subjective effects of d-amphetamine in humans. Behav Pharmacol. 1993;4(3):191–199. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho AK. Ice: a new dosage form of an old drug. Science. 1990;249(4969):631–634. doi: 10.1126/science.249.4969.631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook CE, Jeffcoat AR, Sadler BM, Hill JM, Voyksner RD, Pugh DE, … Perez-Reyes M (1992) Pharmacokinetics of oral methamphetamine and effects of repeated daily dosing in humans. Drug Metab Dispos 20(6):856–862 [PubMed]
- Crane NA, Gorka SM, Weafer J, Langenecker SA, de Wit H, Phan KL. Neural activation to monetary reward is associated with amphetamine reward sensitivity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43(8):1738–1744. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0042-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cretzmeyer M, Sarrazin MV, Huber DL, Block RI, Hall JA. Treatment of methamphetamine abuse: research findings and clinical directions. J Subst Abus Treat. 2003;24(3):267–277. doi: 10.1016/s0740-5472(03)00028-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson ES, Finch JF, Schenk S. Variability in subjective responses to cocaine: initial experiences of college students. Addict Behav. 1993;18(4):445–453. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(93)90062-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De La Torre R, Yubero-Lahoz S, Pardo-Lozano R, Farré M. MDMA, methamphetamine, and CYP2D6 pharmacogenetics: what is clinically relevant? Front Genet. 2012;3:235. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2012.00235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, Phillips TJ. Do initial responses to drugs predict future use or abuse? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(6):1565–1576. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, Uhlenhuth EH, Johanson CE. Individual differences in the reinforcing and subjective effects of amphetamine and diazepam. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1986;16(4):341–360. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(86)90068-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, Enggasser JL, Richards JB. Acute administration of d-amphetamine decreases impulsivity in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27(5):813–825. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00343-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DEA (2015) National Forensic Laboratory Information System: 2014 Annual Report. In: Department of Justice (Ed) Drug Enforcement Administration, Springfield, VA
- Dluzen DE, Liu B. Gender differences in methamphetamine use and responses: a review. Gender Med. 2008;5(1):24–35. doi: 10.1016/s1550-8579(08)80005-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fattore L, Altea S, Fratta W. Sex differences in drug addiction: a review of animal and human studies. Women’s Health. 2008;4(1):51–65. doi: 10.2217/17455057.4.1.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haber SN, Knutson B. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haertzen CA. Development of scales based on patterns of drug effects, using the Addiction Research Center Inventory (ARCI) Psychol Rep. 1966;18(1):163–194. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1966.18.1.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasler G, Luckenbaugh DA, Snow J, Meyers N, Waldeck T, Geraci M, Roiser J, Knutson B, Charney DS, Drevets WC. Reward processing after catecholamine depletion in unmedicated, remitted subjects with major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(3):201–205. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hox JJ. Multilevel analysis: techniques and applications. 2. New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Hser Y-I, Evans E, Huang Y-C. Treatment outcomes among women and men methamphetamine abusers in California. J Subst Abus Treat. 2005;28(1):77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2004.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu M, Becker JB. Effects of sex and estrogen on behavioral sensitization to cocaine in rats. J Neurosci. 2003;23(2):693–699. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-02-00693.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johanson CE, Uhlenhuth EH. Drug preference and mood in humans: d-amphetamine. Psychopharmacology. 1980;71(3):275–279. doi: 10.1007/BF00433062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson BA, Ait-Daoud N, Wells LT. Effects of isradipine, a dihydropyridine-class calcium channel antagonist, on D-methamphetamine-induced cognitive and physiological changes in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22(5):504–512. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00116-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Westdorp A, Kaiser E, Hommer D. FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. NeuroImage. 2000;12(1):20–27. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Fong GW, Adams CM, Varner JL, Hommer D. Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI. Neuroreport. 2001;12(17):3683–3687. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200112040-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Adams CM, Fong GW, Hommer D. Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci. 2001;21(16):RC159–RC159. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-j0002.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuczenski R, Segal D, Cho A, Melega W. Hippocampus norepinephrine, caudate dopamine and serotonin, and behavioral responses to the stereoisomers of amphetamine and methamphetamine. J Neurosci. 1995;15(2):1308–1317. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-02-01308.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambert NM, McLeod M, Schenk S. Subjective responses to initial experience with cocaine: an exploration of the incentive-sensitization theory of drug abuse. Addiction. 2006;101(5):713–725. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01408.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leyton M. Amphetamine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine, drug wanting, and novelty seeking a PET/[11C]raclopride study in healthy men. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27(6):1027–1035. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00366-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lile JA, Babalonis S, Emurian C, Martin CA, Wermeling DP, Kelly TH. Comparison of the behavioral and cardiovascular effects of intranasal and oral d-amphetamine in healthy human subjects. J Clin Pharmacol. 2011;51(6):888–898. doi: 10.1177/0091270010375956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukas SE, Sholar M, Lundahl LH, Lamas X, Kouri E, Wines JD, Kragie L, Mendelson JH. Sex differences in plasma cocaine levels and subjective effects after acute cocaine administration in human volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 1996;125(4):346–354. doi: 10.1007/BF02246017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch WJ, Carroll ME. Sex differences in the acquisition of intravenously self-administered cocaine and heroin in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1999;144(1):77–82. doi: 10.1007/s002130050979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch WJ, Carroll ME. Reinstatement of cocaine self-administration in rats: sex differences. Psychopharmacology. 2000;148(2):196–200. doi: 10.1007/s002130050042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin WR, Sloan JW, Sapira JD, Jasinski DR. Physiologic, subjective, and behavioral effects of amphetamine, methamphetamine, ephedrine, phenmetrazine, and methylphenidate in man. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1971;12(2):245–258. doi: 10.1002/cpt1971122part1245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mattei R, Carlini EA. A comparative study of the anorectic and behavioral effects of fenproporex on male and female rats. Braz J Med Biol Res. 1996;29(8):1025–1030. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayo LM, de Wit H. Acquisition of responses to a methamphetamine-associated cue in healthy humans: self-report, behavioral, and psychophysiological measures. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(7):1734–1741. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayo LM, Fraser D, Childs E, Momenan R, Hommer DW, de Wit H, Heilig M. Conditioned preference to a methamphetamine-associated contextual cue in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38(6):921–929. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNair DM, Droppleman LF, Lorr M (1971) Profile of Mood States (POMS). Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
- Milesi-Hallé A, McMillan DE, Laurenzana EM, Byrnes-Blake KA, Owens SM. Sex differences in (+)-amphetamine- and (+)-methamphetamine-induced behavioral response in male and female Sprague–Dawley rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86(1):140–149. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohia-Nwoko O, Haile CN, Kosten TA. Sex differences in the acute locomotor response to methamphetamine in BALB/c mice. Behav Brain Res. 2017;327:94–97. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.03.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oswald LM, Wong DF, Zhou Y, Kumar A, Brasic J, Alexander M, Ye W, Kuwabara H, Hilton J, Wand GS. Impulsivity and chronic stress are associated with amphetamine-induced striatal dopamine release. NeuroImage. 2007;36(1):153–166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rawson RA, Gonzales R, Obert JL, McCann MJ, Brethen P. Methamphetamine use among treatment-seeking adolescents in Southern California: participant characteristics and treatment response. J Subst Abus Treat. 2005;29(2):67–74. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabol KE, Richards JB, Layton K, Seiden LS. Amphetamine analogs have differential effects on DRL 36-s schedule performance. Psychopharmacology. 1995;121(1):57–65. doi: 10.1007/BF02245591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schindler CW, Bross JG, Thorndike EB. Gender differences in the behavioral effects of methamphetamine. Eur J Pharmacol. 2002;442(3):231–235. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(02)01550-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semple SJ, Grant I, Patterson TL. Female methamphetamine users: social characteristics and sexual risk behavior. Women Health. 2004;40(3):35–50. doi: 10.1300/j013v40n03_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Söderpalm A, Nikolayev L, de Wit H. Effects of stress on responses to methamphetamine in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2003;170(2):188–199. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1536-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreckelmeyer KN, Krach S, Kohls G, Rademacher L, Irmak A, Konrad K, Kircher T, Gründer G. Anticipation of monetary and social reward differently activates mesolimbic brain structures in men and women. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009;4(2):158–165. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stöhr T, Schulte Wermeling D, Weiner I, Feldon J. Rat strain differences in open-field behavior and the locomotor stimulating and rewarding effects of amphetamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1998;59(4):813–818. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(97)00542-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- UNODC . World Drug Report. Vienna: United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Weiland BJ, Heitzeg MM, Zald D, Cummiford C, Love T, Zucker RA, Zubieta J-K. Relationship between impulsivity, prefrontal anticipatory activation, and striatal dopamine release during rewarded task performance. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2014;223(3):244–252. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.05.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White TL, Justice AJH, de Wit H. Differential subjective effects of d-amphetamine by gender, hormone levels and menstrual cycle phase. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;73(4):729–741. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00818-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White TL, Monnig MA, Walsh EG, Nitenson AZ, Harris AD, Cohen RA, Porges EC, Woods AJ, Lamb DG, Boyd CA, Fekir S, (2018) Psychostimulant drug effects on glutamate, Glx, and creatine in the anterior cingulate cortex and subjective response in healthy humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 43 (7):1498-1509 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Wyvell CL, Berridge KC. Intra-accumbens amphetamine increases the conditioned incentive salience of sucrose reward: enhancement of reward “wanting” without enhanced “liking” or response reinforcement. J Neurosci. 2000;20(21):8122–8130. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-21-08122.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang L, Ma W, Barker JL, Rubinow DR. Sex differences in expression of serotonin receptors (subtypes 1A and 2A) in rat brain: a possible role of testosterone. Neuroscience. 1999;94(1):251–259. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00234-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.