Skip to main content
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health logoLink to International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health
. 2019 Jul 31;16(15):2725. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16152725

Relevance of Gender and Social Support in Self-Rated Health and Life Satisfaction in Elderly Spanish People

M Pilar Matud 1,*, M Concepción García 1, Demelza Fortes 1
PMCID: PMC6695653  PMID: 31370147

Abstract

Background: Gender and social support are important social determinants of health, but the relevance of such variables in older people’s health has raised less scholarly attention than in younger age groups. This study examines the relevance of gender and social support in the self-rated health and life satisfaction of elderly Spanish people. A cross-sectional study with a sample of 702 men and 754 women aged between 60 and 94 years was conducted. All participants were evaluated through questionnaires that assess gender role traits, social support, and life satisfaction. Results: Men scored higher than women in masculine/instrumental trait and in life satisfaction whereas women scored higher than men in feminine/expressive trait. Results from multiple regression analyses indicated that women and men presenting higher social support had better self-rated health and higher life satisfaction. High scores in masculine/instrumental trait also proved to be an important predictor of men’s and women’s high life satisfaction and of women’s better self-rated health, whereas the high feminine/expressive trait predicted better self-rated health in the men group. A high educational level was associated in the women’s group with better self-rated health and higher life satisfaction. Conclusions: We conclude that gender and social support are important social determinants of health among older people.

Keywords: gender, social support, life satisfaction, older people, social determinants of health

1. Introduction

Human health is related to social factors [1]. The socioeconomic, political, environmental and cultural factors that shape health are known as the social determinants of health, and refer to the conditions in which people are born, grow, live, work, and age [2]. Inequalities in the distribution of power, money, and resources are responsible for health dissimilarities within and among countries [2]. The social risk factors can be at the individual level—a series of attributes such as gender, race, income/wealth and educational attainment determining the position of the individual in hierarchies of power, social status and economic resources—and at the community level, which refers the circumstances people live in and which are often the product of political decisions [1]. As Marmot ([3], p. 88) asserts “People’s lives and health are shaped by the norms, values and structures of society: processes of inclusion, exclusion, vulnerability and disadvantage; the physical environment in which they live and work; and the economic and social support society and government provide”. Moreover, there is a social gradient in health, since people and communities have progressively better health, the better their socioeconomic position/conditions are [3]. Research on the social determinants of health can make out at least two distinct strands. The first one addresses the ‘health gap’ and centers on the importance of structural conditions in people’s live with reference to a range of negative health outcomes. The second strand or ‘social cure’ draws attention to the way in which personal relationships, social networks and social support feed into health outcomes [4].

1.1. Gender and Social Support as Social Determinants of Health

Gender, an important social determinant of health [5,6] describes “the roles, behaviours, activities, attributes and opportunities that any society considers appropriate for boys and girls, and men and women. Gender also refers to relationships between people and can reflect the distribution of power within those relationships” ([5], p. 644). Gender relations of power feature prominently among the most influential of the social determinants of health [7]. Although there is empirical evidence that women and men are similar in most psychological traits and behaviors [8], the majority of societies consider they are different and should occupy different roles; consequently, they are socialized differently according to the sex they are assigned at birth. Sex typing is the process whereby society transmutes males and females into masculine and feminine [9]. Masculinity and femininity designate the features, behaviours, and interests that society considers appropriate for each gender. Masculinity is associated with an instrumental orientation, central to which is agency, which is characterized by focusing on the self, prioritizing independence, and the achievement of personal goals. On the contrary, femininity is linked to an expressive orientation, to which communion, defined as focusing on the others, is central [10,11]. Despite the changes that have taken place in Western societies, which highlight women’s access to higher education and participation in the workforce, gender stereotypes still persist [12]. Gender stereotypes characterize men and women as complementary: women are perceived to be communal but not agentic whereas men are perceived to be agentic but not communal [13]. Such stereotypes do not only describe typical differences between women and men, as they also prescribe what and how they should behave, to the point of influencing on the way women and men define themselves and are treated by others [14].

Research on the relationships between femininity and masculinity-related constructs and health-related outcomes has identified complex relationships [15,16], although the results are influenced by the important change that the masculinity construct has undergone since the 60 s: it evolved from being viewed as a dimension to being considered socially constructed and developing scales to measure its three major and interrelated aspects, masculinity ideologies, conformity to masculine norms, and gender role conflict [17]. Although masculine norms vary by place and time [18], research on conformity to masculine norms has reported that individuals’ conformity to masculine norms is related with a range of negative psychological outcomes and reduced help seeking, although the results may vary depending on the specific dimension of the masculine norm [16]. The masculine/instrumental trait has been found to be more associated with the well-being of men and women than the feminine/expressive trait [19,20,21,22,23]; however, it has also been found that femininity is associated with optimal mental functioning [22,23,24]. It has been suggested that the strict adherence to masculine and feminine roles can limit the range of potential behaviours and choices of women and men [25], which would entail a limitation to the development of those personal characteristics which do not conform to what society considers appropriate to each gender. So, for example, women following the dictates of femininity of empathy and caring for others can prioritize the welfare of others and take care of all domestic tasks and of the care for the elderly and/or sick, thus lacking time for themselves and for activities that benefit their health (i.e., sports); while men following masculinity norms of strength and dominance can perform risky behaviors for their health such as non-help seeking, poor eating habits or alcohol abuse.

Research on the differences between women and men in health has shown complex results. Globally, the average life expectancy of men is lower than that of women [26], although women report poorer health [27] and more distress and chronic conditions than men [22,28]. But gender differences in health may vary according to other variables such as occupational grade, perceived working conditions and orientation to gender roles [29]. Furthermore, it has been found that differences in morbidity and mortality rates between women and men vary across the European Region and are changing in many countries [3].

A frequently used indicator for studying gender differences in health is self-rated health. Self-rated health is a widespread method for assessing health perceptions in populations which has been extensively employed in health research and practice as a marker of general health [30,31]. Self-rated health has proved to be a multidimensional concept that includes, in addition to the subject’s self-assessment of his/her physical health, the extent to which subjects are able to manage themselves (functional dimension), the extent to which they have adapted or their attitudes towards the disease one has (coping dimension) and how one feels (dimension of well-being) [32]. Self-rated health correlates with historical and current hospitalization and diagnosis and with future hospitalizations [33]; it has also been shown that it is an important predictor of mortality and morbidity [33,34,35]. Although several studies have found that women present worse self-rated health than men [36,37], the literature of the gender differences in self-rated health shows that such differences vary depending on other variables such as age and country of residence [30,38]. Worse self-rated health in women with respect to men has also been found in old age [37], but gender differences vary according to age group [38]. Thus, Dahlin et al. [38] found that women rated their health as poorer than men, especially among those aged between 65 and 79, yet gender differences decreased in those aged 80 to 84 years.

The importance of social connections and support for health and well-being is well-documented [39,40,41]. Research indicates that social support and social integration are protective against mortality [42], and high social support has been related to better self-rated health [43,44] and to high levels of life satisfaction around the world [44]. The benefits of social support in health and well-being have also been found in older individuals, “however the mechanism remains poorly understood” ([41], p. 1050).

1.2. Subjective Well-being

In 1948 the World Health Organization (WHO) defined health not merely as the absence of illness or infirmity, but in a broader sense as “a state of complete physical, mental and social well-being” ([45], p. 1); over the last decades the relevance of well-being has been confirmed. This is a complex construct which “refers to optimal psychological functioning and experience” ([46], p. 142). Well-being research distinguishes two broad traditions, the hedonistic and the eudaimonic [46]. In hedonic well-being, the search for pleasure is central, and from this perspective the focus is on subjective well-being, which includes components such as happiness, life satisfaction and positive affect [47]. Health and subjective well-being are closely related and the link could become more important at older ages [48]. Research has reported a close relationship between subjective well-being and physical and psychological health in later life; moreover, subjective well-being is a reliable predictor for mortality among middle-aged and elderly people [49]. Life satisfaction is a key indicator of subjective bell-being [50] as well as an important indicator of quality of life in social gerontology [51]. Subjective well-being is also intimately associated with health and longer survival [48,52,53,54]. In the case of the elderly it has been found that life satisfaction is related to self-rated health, sleep quality and activities of daily living [55,56], to health-related biomarker of inflammation [57] and to lower depression [56,58]. However, the studies on differences in well-being between women and men have not produced conclusive results. In general, no significant differences are found in the mean scores for subjective well-being between women and men, yet women experience positive and negative emotions more frequently and with greater intensity than men [59].

1.3. Aims of the Study and Hypotheses

Although in recent decades the social determinants of health have received considerable attention as a fundamental concept in the field of public health [60], the analyses of health inequalities have been fewer in the case of older people than in younger age, maybe because of the tacit assumption that illness and disability are inevitable in older age [3]. In spite of this, there is evidence that older people experience persistent health inequalities, especially determined by the socioeconomic level, the educational level and gender [3]. Thus, the main aim of the work is to analyze the relevance that two important social determinants of health, gender and social support, have on self-rated health and life satisfaction in elderly Spanish people. A second aim is to know the relevance of the sociodemographic characteristics of age and educational level in the elderly’ self-rated health and life satisfaction.

Based on previous research, it was hypothesized that men will have better self-rated health than women. The second hypothesis predicted that women and men with more educational level will present better self-rated health and greater life satisfaction. The third hypothesis predicted that women and men with higher scores on the masculine/instrumental trait will have better self-rated health and greater life satisfaction. And the fourth hypothesis predicted that women and men with higher scores on social support will report better self-rated health and greater life satisfaction.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The sample consisted of 702 men and 754 women aged between 60 and 94 years, who voluntarily participated in the study, and resided in eleven autonomous communities located in the different geographical areas of Spain. Their mean age (M) was 68 years, and the standard deviation (SD) of 6.52. Most of the sample (94.4%) had at least one child, being the range of the number of children between 0 and 12 (M = 2.51, SD = 1.56). Table 1 presents the main sociodemographic characteristics of women and men. As can be observed, women and men did not differ in age and number of children, but there were statistically significant differences in education level and marital status. Although there was diversity in education, most often they had only elementary studies, which occurred in 44.4% of women and 36.5% of men. Furthermore, not having completed elementary studies was more frequent in women (16.7%) than in men (14.0%). Men would more frequently have a 5-year university degree or a high school degree than women. The analysis of marital status also showed great diversity, although most often they were married or living with a partner, which was more common among men (78.3%) than among women (56.9%). Furthermore, the percentages of widowed women were higher (27.1%) than in the case of men (9.0%).

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of the men and women groups.

Men (n = 702) Women (n = 754) χ2-Value
n % n %
Education
Unfinished elementary studies 98 14.0 126 16.7 46.23 ***
Elementary studies 256 36.5 335 44.4
First grade professional training 33 4.7 41 5.4
High school degree 145 20.7 97 12.9
Second grade professional training 23 3.3 19 2.5
3-year university degree 60 8.5 94 12.5
5-year university degree 87 12.4 42 5.6
Marital Status
Never married 38 5.4 51 6.8 96.85 ***
Married/cohabiting 550 78.3 429 56.9
Separated/divorced 51 7.3 70 9.3
Widowed 63 9.0 204 27.1
M SD M SD t-Value
Age 69.79 6.48 70.17 6.55 −1.10
Number of children 2.42 1.52 2.58 1.60 −1.94

Note: *** p < 0.001.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Dependent Variables: Self-Rated Health and Life Satisfaction

Participants’ self-rated health was assessed by one item asking respondents to rate their overall health at the present time on a five-point scale. The possible choices were: “very good”, “good”, “moderate”, “bad”, “very bad”. Scores were assigned from 0 (for “very bad”) to 4 (for “very good”).

The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) [61] was used to assess life satisfaction. The SWLS is made up of 5 items rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The minimum possible value was 5 and the maximum was 35, and high scores indicated a greater life satisfaction. All items were translated into Spanish and back translated into English by the research team plus two bilingual persons. For the current sample, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.86 in the men’s group and 0.87 in the women’s group.

2.2.2. Independent Variables: Masculine/Instrumental Trait, Feminine/Expressive Trait, Social Support, Age, Educational Level, Number of Children and Marital Status

Masculine/instrumental and feminine/expressive traits were assessed by using The Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) [62]. The BSRI is a self-report inventory that assesses participants’ identification with socially desirable personality traits that are stereotypically associated with women and men. The BSRI contains 60 items consisting of adjectives or short sentences, 20 of which refer to characteristics and traits traditionally regarded as masculine, such as “independent”, “competitive”, “dominant”, which make up the masculine/instrumental scale; 20 characteristics traditionally regarded as feminine, such as “compassionate”, “warm”, “gentle”, which make up the feminine/expressive scale; and 20 items formed by characteristics attributable to both genders, which were not used in the analysis. The response format is a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never or almost never true) to 7 (always or almost always true). The minimum possible value, both in the masculine/instrumental and feminine/expressive scale, was 20 and the maximum 140, and higher scores indicated greater self-identification with the characteristics of the trait. All items were translated into Spanish and back into English by the research team plus two bilingual persons, a native English-speaking professional translator, and a native speaker of Spanish. For the current sample, the Cronbach’s alpha in the men’s group of the 20 items on the masculine/instrumental scale was 0.84, and of the 20 items on the feminine/expressive scale was 0.85. In the women’s group, Cronbach’s alpha on the masculine/instrumental scale was 0.83, and on the feminine/expressive scale it was 0.85.

Social support was assessed by using the Social Support Scale [63]. It consisted of 12 items which measure the availability of social support. For each item, respondents are asked to rate on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (never) to 3 (always) the possibilities of access to other persons who can support and/or help with affective, economic, labor, familiar and advice/guidance needs. Confirmatory factor analyses, with a sample of 3210 people [64], showed that the 12 items were grouped into a factor in the women sample and in the total sample measuring global social support perceived. The minimum possible value was 0 and the maximum 36, and higher scores indicated greater social support perceived. The Cronbach’s alpha for the current sample was 0.91 in the men’s group and 0.92 in the women’s group.

Demographic variables included in the analysis were: participants’ age and number of children, that were treated as continuous variables; educational level, that was approached as an ordinal variable with seven levels, from 1 (for unfinished elementary studies) to 7 (for 5-year university degree); and marital status, that was included in regression analyses as a dummy variable with two levels: one included the never married, separated, divorced, and widowed (reference category, which was coded with 0) and in the other the married or living with a partner, which was coded with 1.

2.3. Procedure

This study forms part of an extensive research on gender and health and was positively evaluated by the Ethics Committee on Animal Research and Well-Being of the University of La Laguna (study approval number 2015-0170). The participants in the study were volunteers, and were not remunerated for their participation. To avoid systematic biases, access to the participants was through several retirees’ association centers of different Spanish localities, as well as by resorting to the social network (usually family and neighbors) of a large number of psychology, nursing, and sociology students from seven Spanish universities trained in administering those tests, who received course credits for that task.

After reported consent was obtained, tests were filled out individually, in some cases self-administered; in the case of participants with low educational level, or those who preferred to be interviewed, the tests were completed during the course of a structured interview carried out by trained university students. There were no significant differences across gender in the method of administering the tests. Test were answered manually on a paper version of the measures. Participants had to fulfill the following requirements: (1) age of 60 or more years; (2) not having cognitive or language problems that limited comprehension of the tests; (3) similar ages in women and men. No names or any other data identifying the participant were used in the tests. We have complied with American Psychological Association ethical standards in the treatment of the sample.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics were computed to describe the demographic characteristics of the participants. Comparisons between men and women were computed performing Pearson’s chi square test in case of categorical variables and by using t-test when they were continuous. The effect size of the mean differences was computed by using the Cohen’s d. The internal consistency reliability for the masculine/instrumental and feminine/expressive traits, social support and life satisfaction were calculated using the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient. Bivariate associations between variables were computed by using the correlation coefficient r of Pearson when they were quantitative variables and Spearman’s Rho when they were ordinal variables. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted to determine the relevance of the sociodemographic variables, the masculine/instrumental and feminine/expressive traits, and the social support in the self-rated health and men’s and women’s life satisfaction. The criterion considered was the score in self-rated health in the first regression analyses and the score in life satisfaction in the second. Age, educational level, number of children and marital status (as a dummy variable) were included in step 1. At step 2, masculinity-instrumental and feminine/expressive scores were entered. In step 3, social support was included. Statistical analyses were carried out using the software IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 22.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Gender Differences in Life Satisfaction, Social Support, Masculine/Instrumental Trait, Feminine/ Expressive Trait, and Self-Rated Health

The 0.6% of men rated their health as very bad, 5.6% as bad, 40.3% as moderate, 42.9% as good and 10.7% as very good. The percentages in women were, respectively, 0.8%, 6.6%, 45.5%, 37.9% and 9.2%. The differences between the percentages were not statistically significant, χ2 (4, N = 1456) = 6.30, p = 0.178. Table 2 show the means, standard deviations and comparisons for men and women in life satisfaction, social support and masculine/instrumental and feminine/expressive traits. Statistically significant differences were found in three of the four variables. Men scored higher than women in masculine/instrumental trait and in life satisfaction, although the effect size for life satisfaction was small. And women scored higher than men in the feminine/expressive trait.

Table 2.

Means (M), standard deviations (SD) and comparisons for men and women for the quantitative study variables.

Men (n = 702) Women (n = 754) t (1454) d-Value
M SD M SD
Life satisfaction 24.79 6.08 23.83 6.84 2.85 ** 0.15
Social support 25.47 7.73 26.10 7.81 −1.57 −0.08
Masculine/instrumental trait 94.80 15.46 83.47 15.89 13.77 *** 0.72
Feminine/expressive trait 92.09 14.05 102.04 13.77 −13.64 *** −0.71

Note: ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001; d-value = Cohen’s d.

3.2. Predictors of Women’s and Men’s Self-Rated Health

Table 3 presents the correlation coefficients between dependent and independent variables calculated independently in the women and the men groups. In both genders, most of the correlation coefficients were statistically significant, although the magnitude of the association was not high. In addition, there were some differences in the correlations in the group of women and in the group of men, which highlighted that there were only statistically significant correlations between educational level and life satisfaction in the women group and between the feminine/expressive trait and self-rated health in the men group. In the latter, the number of children was associated with self-rated health.

Table 3.

Correlations between dependent and independent variables for men and women groups.

Men Women
Life Satisfaction Self-Rated Health $ Life Satisfaction Self-Rated Health $
Age 0.08 * −0.18 *** 0.04 −0.14 ***
Educational level $ 0.03 0.11 ** 0.13 *** 0.25 ***
Number of children 0.07 −0.13 ** −0.06 −0.06
Masculine/instrumental trait 0.30 *** 0.12 ** 0.27 *** 0.23 ***
Feminine/expressive trait 0.20 *** 0.20 *** 0.21 *** 0.01
Social support 0.43 *** 0.17 *** 0.42 *** 0.21 ***

Note: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001; $ = Spearman’s Rho correlation coefficient.

Table 4 presents the standardized regression coefficients (β) with their corresponding t values, and the F and R2 values for the three regression models with the self-rated health as the dependent variable for the men group, and Table 5 for the women group.

Table 4.

Summary of the hierarchical regression with self-rated health as the dependent variable for the men group.

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
β t-Value β t-Value β t-Value
Age −0.15 −3.62 *** −0.13 −3.12 ** −0.14 −3.45 ***
Educational level 0.05 1.17 0.03 0.75 0.03 0.68
Number of children −0.09 −2.11 * −0.09 −2.14 * −0.09 −2.19 *
Married/cohabiting 0.01 0.20 0.00 0.07 −0.02 −0.59
Masculine/instrumental trait 0.08 2.20 * 0.07 1.82
Feminine/expressive trait 0.14 3.79 *** 0.11 2.96 **
Social support 0.14 3.58 ***
R 2 0.05 0.08 0.09
Adjusted R2 0.04 0.07 0.09
R2 Change 0.05 0.03 0.02
F Change 8.19 *** 12.15 *** 12.84 ***
ANOVA (F-value, df) 8.19 (4, 697) *** 9.68 (6, 695) *** 10.28 (7, 694) ***

Note: β = Standardized regression coefficient; R2 = percentage of explained variance; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

Table 5.

Summary of the hierarchical regression with self-rated health as the dependent variable for the women group.

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
β t-Value β t-Value β t-Value
Age −0.08 −2.01 * −0.05 −1.38 −0.08 −2.03 *
Educational level 0.22 5.76 *** 0.18 4.71 *** 0.18 4.78 ***
Number of children −0.01 −0.19 −0.00 −0.10 −0.01 −0.36
Married/cohabiting 0.02 0.50 0.03 0.69 0.01 0.25
Masculine/instrumental trait 0.20 5.41 *** 0.17 4.69 ***
Feminine/expressive trait −0.00 −0.07 −0.03 −0.85
Social support 0.19 5.23 ***
R 2 0.07 0.11 0.14
Adjusted R2 0.06 0.10 0.13
R2 Change 0.07 0.04 0.03
F Change 13.49 *** 15.67 *** 27.32 ***
ANOVA (F-value, df) 13.49 (4, 749) *** 14.57 (6, 747) *** 16.83 (7, 746) ***

Note: β = Standardized regression coefficient; R2 = percentage of explained variance; * p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001.

Results identified that R for regression was significantly different from zero at the end of each step in both men and women groups. The sociodemographic variables entered into step 1 explained 4% of the variance in self-rated health in the men group and 6% in the women group. For men and women too age was statistically significant; in the men group, the number of children was also statistically significant whereas in the women group the statistically significant variable was the educational level. The change in R2 from model 1 to model 2 identified that the masculine/instrumental trait (β = 0.08, p < 0.05) and the feminine/expressive trait (β = 0.14, p < 0.001) played significant roles in men’s self-rated health, whereas in the women group it was the masculine/instrumental trait (β = 0.20, p < 0.001) that proved statistically significant. The addition of social support in Model 3 also yielded a statistically significant increment in R2 in the women and the men groups.

Beta values in Model 3, with all independent variables in the equation, showed that in the men group the significant predictors of self-rated health were social support, age, feminine/expressive trait, and number of children, so men with more social support, less age, higher feminine/expressive trait and lower number of children reported better self-rated health. In the women group, the significant predictors were social support, educational level, masculine/instrumental trait, and age. Women with more social support, higher educational level and masculine/instrumental trait and less age reported better self-rated health.

3.3. Predictors of Women’s and Men’s Life Satisfaction

Table 6 provides the main results of the hierarchical regression with life satisfaction as the dependent variable for the men group, whereas results from Table 7 correspond to the women group.

Table 6.

Summary of the hierarchical regression with life satisfaction as the dependent variable for the men group.

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
β t-Value β t-Value β t-Value
Age 0.08 1.89 0.11 2.90 ** 0.08 2.18 *
Educational level 0.05 1.29 0.01 0.23 0.00 0.01
Number of children 0.04 0.97 0.03 0.68 0.02 0.64
Married/cohabiting 0.05 1.26 0.04 1.16 −0.03 −0.75
Masculine/instrumental trait 0.27 7.33 *** 0.23 6.72 ***
Feminine/expressive trait 0.15 3.94 *** 0.06 1.81
Social support 0.38 10.85 ***
R 2 0.01 0.12 0.25
Adjusted R2 0.01 0.12 0.24
R2 Change 0.01 0.11 0.13
F Change 2.33 43.55 *** 117.68 ***
ANOVA (F-value, df) 2.33 (4, 697) 16.26 (6, 695) *** 33.09 (7, 694) ***

Note: β = Standardized regression coefficient; R2 = percentage of explained variance; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

Table 7.

Summary of the hierarchical regression with life satisfaction as the dependent variable for the women group.

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
β t-Value β t-Value β t-Value
Age 0.13 3.27 0.14 3.77 *** 0.10 2.70 **
Educational level 0.17 4.32 0.12 3.27 ** 0.12 3.49 **
Number of children −0.05 −1.36 −0.04 −1.22 −0.06 −1.85
Married/cohabiting 0.11 3.08 0.12 3.29 ** 0.09 2.59 *
Masculine/instrumental trait 0.23 6.26 *** 0.18 5.09 ***
Feminine/expressive trait 0.14 3.79 *** 0.08 2.45 *
Social support 0.36 10.76 ***
R 2 0.04 0.13 0.24
Adjusted R2 0.04 0.12 0.24
R2 Change 0.04 0.08 0.12
F Change 8.44 *** 35.68 *** 115.84 ***
ANOVA (F-value, df) 8.44 (4, 749) *** 18.04 (6, 747) *** 34.39 (7, 746) ***

Note: β = Standardized regression coefficient; R2 = percentage of explained variance; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

R for regression was significantly different from zero at the end of each step in the women group and only at the end of steps 2 and 3 in the men group. In the women group, after model 1, with age, educational level, number of children and marital status in the equation, R2 = 0.04, p < 0.001. In both genders, the change in R2 from model 1 to model 2 identified that masculine/instrumental and feminine/expressive traits determine significantly men’s and women’s life satisfaction. The addition of social support in model 3 resulted in a statistically significant (p < 0.001) increment in R2 (R2 change = 0.13 in the men group and R2 change = 0.12 in the women group). Beta values in the model 3, with all independent variables in the equation, proved that social support was the variable most associated with men’s and women’s life satisfaction; the second most relevant variable was masculine/instrumental trait. Another statistically significant predictor for both genders was age. Life satisfaction was higher in women and men with high social support, high masculine/instrumental trait and more age. In addition, in the case of women, educational level, marital status, and feminine/expressive trait were also significant predictors. Thus, life satisfaction scored higher in women with higher educational level, high social support, older age, married or cohabiting marital status and with a high score in both the masculine/instrumental and the feminine/expressive trait.

4. Discussion

Although in recent decades the social determinants of health have received considerable attention in the field of public health [60], few studies had focused on the elderly and fewer had approached the relevance of gender for successful ageing [65,66]. Although there is a growing social awareness of the phenomenon called ‘feminization of old age’ “the topic and its psychosocial and health-related consequences have not attracted the scientific interest that they deserve” ([65], p. 208). The main aim of this work was to analyze the relevance that two important social determinants of health, gender and social support, have on self-rated health and life satisfaction in elderly Spanish people. Spain is one of the countries in the world with the highest life expectancy at birth and, together with Japan, they lead a group of 25 Organization of Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) countries with life expectancy over 80 years [67]. As in most countries, life expectancy in Spain is greater in women than in men, with life expectancy at birth of 80.3 years in men and 85.7 in women [26]. Although it is an important achievement, the aging of the population poses important challenges, since it implies greater economic and social needs [68].

The first hypothesis of the study predicting that men would have better self-rated health than women was not supported. Although research has reported that women’s self-rated health was worse than men’s [36,37], gender gaps vary cross-nationally [38,67]. Findings did also reveal that the differences between women and men in self-rated health vary in agreement with other behavioral and demographic variables [30,37] and that female disadvantage disappears when socio-economic and health covariates are considered [69]. Although findings in the present study have provided no statistically significant gender differences in self-rated health, it may be owing to the fact that this research has been done with people who participated voluntarily, so access to part of the sample was through retirees’ association centers; therefore, there may have been a certain bias towards greater participation of people with better health, which may be more common in women than in men. In fact, very few people rated their health as very bad. Older people’s self-rated health has been also associated with demographic variables, as ill health reporting increases with age [3,67]; these results are consistent with those provided by this study where worse self-rated health has been found at older ages, although the strength of the association was greater in men than in women.

The second hypothesis predicting that women and men with a higher educational level would have better self-rated health and greater life satisfaction was only supported in the women sample. In the correlation analysis between dependent and independent variables for the men group, it was found that, although educational level correlated statistically significantly with self-rated health, the magnitude of the association was very low (r = 0.11). In the hierarchical regression analyses the Beta weights for the educational level were not statistically significant in the group of men in any of the three models, which was observed either when the dependent variable was self-reported health or when it was life satisfaction. Although the positive association between education and health and survival is well established, gender could be an important variable in such association [70]. Ross et al. [70] found that education’s beneficial influence on health and on survival were determined by gender but in the opposite ways, since education has a larger effect on women’s self-rated health than on men’s, but a larger effect on men’s mortality due to causes such as lung cancer, stroke, respiratory disease, suicide, homicide, and accidents.

The third hypothesis, predicting that women and men with higher scores on the masculine/instrumental trait would report better self-rated health and greater life satisfaction, was not fully supported. Although women and men scoring higher in the masculine/instrumental trait had higher life satisfaction, in the final model, when social support was introduced, the association between the masculine/instrumental trait and self-rated health only occurred in the case of women. The gender trait that predicted self-rated health in men was the feminine/expressive, with better health in men who scored higher; and the feminine/expressive trait proved to be independent of women’s health. These results underline the relevance of identifying with the characteristics stereotypically associated with gender when addressing health and well-being in the elderly; self-rated health is better in women and men whose self-concept includes the characteristics of the other gender, that is, the masculine/instrumental trait in the case of women and the feminine/expressive in that of men. The underlying reason may be that women who score higher in masculine/instrumental trait are better able to defend their rights and to dedicate more time to themselves and their self-care; whereas men who score higher in feminine/expressive trait are better able to ask for help if case they need, and take better care of their health. It also makes clear how the exclusive identification with the characteristics associated with the same gender, such as independence, dominance, self-sufficiency, or individualism in the case of men, and sensitivity to the needs of others, sympathy, kindness, or warmth in the case of women, is a limitation for health and well-being. The results provided converge and support the literature that states that openness to positive traits of the opposite gender role is an essential trait for successful ageing [66]. Moreover, the research conducted with the general population has found that people whose self-concept includes both instrumental and expressive characteristics report greater self-rated health and well-being [19,22,23,71]. These results could be important for the design of interventions with the elderly, which should include strategies for women to develop characteristics such as independence, self-confidence, individuality, and assertiveness; and for men to develop empathy, sensitivity to the needs of others, or warmth.

The fourth hypothesis predicting that women and men with higher scores on social support would have better self-rated health and greater life satisfaction was supported. These results converge and extend the existing literature regarding the relevance of social support in the elderly’s life satisfaction [55,72,73].

The results of this work show the relevance of gender in the elderly’s health and well-being, although in both genders a high social support is associated with better self-rated health and higher life satisfaction. Age was also relevant in both men and women but its effect was different depending on the indicator of health in question: older age was associated with lower self-rated health—which was greater in the case of men than in women—but with greater life satisfaction. Research carried out in several countries on the relationship between age and life satisfaction does not show consistent results [59], having found that this relationship depends on other variables such as the age group or the country where the study was carried out [59,74]. Literature for most European countries and the USA documents a U-shaped relationship between both variables (life satisfaction decreasing to midlife, to increase subsequently towards retirement), although the literature that claims to find a U shape of life satisfaction over age has been criticized [75]. Although there are important individual differences, it has been found that life satisfaction does not decrease, but rather increases from 40 to 65 years “before declining only close to impending death” ([59], p. 397). Diener et al. [59] affirm that, although it is necessary to increase research in this area, “it is clear that old age is not necessarily a harbinger of unhappiness” (p. 397). The results of the present study on the association between age and life satisfaction converge with this affirmation and expand the empirical evidence that sustains it.

In the present study it has also been found that, while a high educational level is associated with better self-rated health and higher life satisfaction in the women group, this is not the case for the men group. In addition, being married or cohabiting was associated with greater life satisfaction only in the women group. Although the reasons for this are unknown, in interpreting these results it should be taken into account that, in the present sample, there were differences between women and men as for the educational level, which was higher in men than in women, and for the marital status, since being married of cohabiting was more common in men than in women, whereas the percentage of widows was higher than that of widowers. Although the sample of the present study is not random, so that results cannot be generalized to the general population, it is important to bear in mind that these differences between women and men are coherent with the composition of the Spanish population of elderly people, which presents a higher rate of women with lower levels of education than men, and a higher occurrence of widows [76].

Although the results of the present work allow to advance in the knowledge of the social determinants of health and well-being of the elderly, they also present some limitations. The first one is that this is a cross-sectional study, therefore, no cause-effect inferences can be made. Second, the sample, though large and with women and men with different demographic characteristics, is a convenience sample. Third, all participants lived in Spain, which may restrict the generalization of results with respect to other countries. Fourth, the percentage of the variance explained in self-rated health by social support, masculine/instrumental trait, age, and educational level in the case of women, and by social support, age, feminine/expressive trait and number of children in men, was low. Fifth, the measures were administered to some participants by interviewers, in the form of a structured interview, which could have biased some responses. Future research is needed to investigate the causal link between the variables, to assess the generalizability of these results in other countries, as well as to expand knowledge of the variables that determine the self-rated health of elderly women and men.

5. Conclusions

Gender and social support are important social determinants of health among the elderly, as people having greater social support present better self-rated health and greater life satisfaction. In addition, women with higher education have better self-rated health and higher life satisfaction. Identification with traits associated with traditional gender roles has been shown to be an important factor in self-rated health and life satisfaction, and women and men whose self-concept includes both instrumental and expressive characteristics are more likely to have greater health and wellbeing. Against gender stereotypes that characterize women and men as complementary and that prescribe that men should be agentic, independent and instrumental but not communal, warm or expressive, and that women should be communal, warm or expressive but not agentic, independent and instrumental, the results of this work show that both types of characteristics are associated with the health and well-being of both women and men. The results of this study can be useful for the design of policies, programs and strategies aimed at a more successful aging of women and men and for the increase of gender equality.

Author Contributions

M.P.M. designed the research; M.C.G. and D.F. collected the data; the three authors analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness of Spain, grant number PSI2015-65963R, MINECO/FEDER, UE.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  • 1.Ferrer R. Social determinants of health. In: Daaleman T.P., Helton M.R., editors. Chronic Illness Care: Principles and Practice. Springer; Cham, Switzerland: 2018. pp. 435–449. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Commission on the Social Determinants of Health (CSDH) Closing the Gap in a Generation: Health Equity Through Action on the Social Determinants of Health. Final Report of the Commission on Social Determinants of Health. World Health Organization; Geneva, Switzerland: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Marmot M. Review of Social Determinants and the Health Divide in the WHO European Region: Final Report. WHO Regional Office for Europe; Copenhagen, Denmark: 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Haslam S.A., McMahon C., Cruwys T., Haslam C., Jetten J., Steffens N.K. Social cure, what social cure? The propensity to underestimate the importance of social factors for health. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018;198:14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Manandhar M., Hawkes S., Buse K., Nosratid E., Magar V. Gender, health and the 2030 agenda for sustainable development. Bull. World Health Organ. 2018;96:644–653. doi: 10.2471/BLT.18.211607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.World Health Organization . Gender, Women and Primary Health Care Renewal: A Discussion Paper. World Health Organization; Geneva, Switzerland: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Sen G., Östlin P. Gender inequity in health: Why it exists and how we can change it. Glob. Public Health. 2008;3:1–12. doi: 10.1080/17441690801900795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hyde J.S. Gender similarities. In: Cook S.L., Rutherford A., Travis C.B., White J.W., Williams J.W., Wyche K.F., editors. APA Handbook of the Psychology of Women: History, Theory, and Battlegrounds. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC, USA: 2018. pp. 129–143. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bem S.L. Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychol. Rev. 1981;88:354–364. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.354. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bem S.L. The Lenses of Gender. Yale University Press; New Haven, CT, USA: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Helgeson V.S. Gender and personality. In: Mikulincer M., Shaver P., editors. APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology. Volume 4. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC, USA: 2015. pp. 515–534. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Haines E.L., Deaux K., Lofaro N. The times they are a-changing or are they not? A comparison of gender stereotypes, 1983–2014. Psychol. Women Q. 2016;40:353–363. doi: 10.1177/0361684316634081. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kahalon R., Shnabel N., Becker J.C. Positive stereotypes, negative outcomes: Reminders of the positive components of complementary gender stereotypes impair performance in counter-stereotypical tasks. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2018;57:482–502. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ellemers N. Gender stereotypes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2018;69:275–298. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Addis M.E., Reigeluth C.S., Schwab J.R. Social norms, social construction, and the psychology of men and masculinity. In: Wong Y.J., Wester S.R., editors. APA Handbook of Men and Masculinities. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC, USA: 2016. pp. 81–104. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wong Y.J., Ho M.R., Wang S., Miller I.S.K. Meta-analyses of the relationship between conformity to masculine norms and mental health-related outcomes. J. Couns. Psychol. 2017;64:80–93. doi: 10.1037/cou0000176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Levant R.F., Wong Y.J. Introduction: Maturation of the psychology of men and masculinities. In: Levant R.F., Wong Y.J., editors. The Psychology of Men and Masculinities. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC, USA: 2017. pp. 3–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.King K., Schlichthorst M., Reifels L., Keogh L., Spittal M.J., Phelps A., Pirkis J. Impacts of a documentary about masculinity and men’s health. Am. J. Men’s Health. 2018;12:1604–1614. doi: 10.1177/1557988318777927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bassoff E.S., Glass G.V. The relationship between sex roles and mental health: A meta-analysis of twenty six studies. Couns. Psychol. 1982;10:105–112. doi: 10.1177/0011000082104019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.DiDonato M.D., Berenbaum S.A. Predictors and consequences of gender typicality: The mediating role of communality. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2013;42:429–436. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-9955-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Taylor M.C., Hall J.A. Psychological androgyny: Theories, methods, and conclusions. Psychol. Bull. 1982;92:347–366. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.92.2.347. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Matud M.P., Bethencourt J.M., Ibáñez I. Gender differences in psychological distress in Spain. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry. 2015;61:560–568. doi: 10.1177/0020764014564801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Matud M.P. Masculine/instrumental and feminine/expressive traits and health, well-being, and psychological distress in Spanish men. Am. J. Men’s Health. 2019;3 doi: 10.1177/1557988319832749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lefkowitz E.S., Zeldow P.B. Masculinity and femininity predict optimal mental health. A belated test of the androgyny hypothesis. J. Pers. Assess. 2006;87:95–101. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Parent M.C., Moradi B. Confirmatory factor analysis of the conformity to feminine norms inventory and development of an abbreviated version: The CFNI-45. Psychol. Women Q. 2010;34:97–109. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2009.01545.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.WHO World Health Statistics 2018: Monitoring Health for the SDGs, Sustainable Development Goals. [(accessed on 5 February 2019)]; Available online: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/272596/9789241565585-eng.pdf?ua=1.
  • 27.Hosseinpoor A.R., Williams J.S., Amin A., de Carvalho I.A., Beard J., Boerma T., Kowal P., Naidoo N., Chatterji S. Social determinants of self-reported health in women and men: Understanding the role of gender in population health. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e34799. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.McDonough P., Walters P. Gender and health: Reassessing patterns and explanations. Soc. Sci. Med. 2001;52:547–559. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00159-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Emslie C., Hunt K., Macintyre S. Gender differences in minor morbidity among full time employees of a British university. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 1999;53:465–475. doi: 10.1136/jech.53.8.465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.El-Ansari W., Stock C. Gender differences in self-rated health among university students in England, Wales and Northern Ireland: Do confounding variables matter? Glob. J. Health Sci. 2016;8:168–177. doi: 10.5539/gjhs.v8n11p168. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Lidström W.M., Wennberg P., Lundqvist R., Forssén A., Waller G. Time trends of comparative self-rated health in adults aged 25–34 in the Northern Sweden MONICA study, 1990–2014. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0187896. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187896. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Simon J.G., De Boer J.B., Joung I.M.A., Bosma H., Mackenbach J.P. How is your health in general? A qualitative study on self-assessed health. Eur. J. Public Health. 2005;15:200–208. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cki102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Nielsen T.H. The relationship between self-rated health and hospital records. Health Econ. 2016;25:497–512. doi: 10.1002/hec.3167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Barger D., Cribbet M.R., Muldoon M.F. Participant-reported health status predicts cardiovascular and all-cause mortality independent of established and nontraditional biomarkers: Evidence from a representative US sample. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2016;5:e003741. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.116.003741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hu Y., Chen P., Hsu C., Yu H., Chien K., Li C., Hu G. Age and gender differences in the relationship between self-rated health and mortality among middle-aged and elderly people in Taiwan-Results of a national cohort study. Int. J. Gerontol. 2016;10:91–95. doi: 10.1016/j.ijge.2014.08.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bora J.K., Saikia N. Gender Differentials in self-rated health and self-reported disability among adults in India. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0141953. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141953. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Josefsson K., Andersson M., Erikstedt A. Older adults’ self-rated health and differences by age and gender: A quantitative study. Healthy Aging Res. 2016;5:1. doi: 10.1097/01.HXR.0000511864.85548.25. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Dahlin J., Härkönen J. Cross-national differences in the gender gap in subjective health in Europe: Does country-level gender equality matter? Soc. Sci. Med. 2013;98:24–28. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.McKenzie S.K., Collings S., Jenkin G., River J. Masculinity, social connectedness, and mental health: Men’s diverse patterns of practice. Am. J. Men’s Health. 2018;12:1247–1261. doi: 10.1177/1557988318772732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Saikkonen S., Karukivi M., Vahlberg T., Saarijärvi S. Associations of social support and alexithymia with psychological distress in Finnish young adults. Scand. J. Psychol. 2018;59:602–609. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sherman S.M., Cheng Y., Fingerman K.L., Schnyer D. Social support, stress and the aging brain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2016;11:1050–1058. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Holt-Lunstad J., Smith T.B., Layton J.B. Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Med. 2010;7:e1000316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Kumar S., Calvo R., Avendano M., Sivaramakrishnan K., Berkman L.F. Social support, volunteering and health around the world: Cross-national evidence from 139 countries. Soc. Sci. Med. 2012;74:696–706. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Calvo R., Zheng Y., Kumar S., Olgiati A., Berkman L. Well-being and social capital on planet earth: Cross-national evidence from 142 countries. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e42793. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042793. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.World Health Organization Constitution of the World Health Organization. Basic Documents, Forty-Fifth Edition, Supplement, October 2006. [(accessed on 4 February 2019)]; Available online: https://www.who.int/governance/eb/who_constitution_en.pdf 2006.
  • 46.Ryan R.M., Deci E.L. On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2001;52:141–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Diener E. Subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 1984;95:542–575. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Steptoe A., Leigh E.S., Kumari M. Positive affect and distressed affect over the day in older people. Psychol. Aging. 2011;26:956–965. doi: 10.1037/a0023303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Iwasa H., Kawaai C., Gondo Y., Inagaki H., Suzuki T. Subjective well-being as a predictor of all-cause mortality among middle-aged and elderly people living in an urban Japanese community: A seven-year prospective cohort study. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 2006;6:216–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2006.00351.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Erdogan B., Bauer T.N., Truxillo D.T., Mansfield L.R. Whistle while you work: A review of the life satisfaction literature. J. Manag. 2012;38:1038–1083. doi: 10.1177/0149206311429379. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Krause N. Lifetime trauma, emotional support, and life satisfaction among older adults. Gerontologist. 2004;44:615–623. doi: 10.1093/geront/44.5.615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Boehm J.K., Kubzansky L.D. The heart’s content: The association between positive psychological well-being and cardiovascular health. Psychol. Bull. 2012;138:655–691. doi: 10.1037/a0027448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Chida Y., Steptoe A. Positive psychological well-being and mortality: A quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Pychosom. Med. 2008;70:741–756. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818105ba. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Diener E., Chan M.Y. Happy people live longer: Subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being. 2011;3:1–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01045.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Cheung K.C., Chou K. Poverty, deprivation and life satisfaction among Hong Kong older persons. Ageing Soc. 2019;39:703–721. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X17001143. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Noh J., Kim K., Lee J.H., Kim M.H., Kwon Y.D. Relationship of health, sociodemographic, and economic factors and life satisfaction in young-old and old-old elderly: A cross-sectional analysis of data from the Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging. J. Phys. Ther. Sci. 2017;29:1483–1489. doi: 10.1589/jpts.29.1483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ironson G., Banerjee N., Fitch C., Krause N. Positive emotional well-being, health behaviors, and inflammation measured by C-Reactive protein. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018;197:235–243. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.06.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Puvill T., Lindenberg J., de Craen A.J.M., Slaets J.P.J., Westendorp R.G.J. Impact of physical and mental health on life satisfaction in old age: A population based observational study. BMC Geriatr. 2016;16:194. doi: 10.1186/s12877-016-0365-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Diener E., Ryan K. Subjective well-being: A general overview. S. Afr. J. Psychol. 2014;39:391–406. doi: 10.1177/008124630903900402. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Lucyk H., McLaren L. Taking stock of the social determinants of health: A scoping review. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0177306. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Diener E., Emmons R., Larsen R.J., Griffin S. The satisfaction with Life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 1985;49:71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Bem S.L. The measurement of psychological androgyny. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1974;42:152–162. doi: 10.1037/h0036215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Matud M.P. Social Support Scale [Database record] PsycTESTS. 1998 doi: 10.1037/t12441-000. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Matud M.P., Ibáñez I., Bethencourt J.M., Marrero R., Carballeira M. Structural gender differences in perceived social support. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2003;35:1919–1929. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00041-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Perrig-Chiello P., Hutchison S. Health and well-being in old age: The pertinence of a gender mainstreaming approach in research. Gerontology. 2010;56:208–213. doi: 10.1159/000235813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Perrig-Chiello P., Hutchison S. Research on ageing, health and gender: A long and winding road. Reply to Månsdotter’s ‘Further thoughts on gender and lifetime health’. Gerontology. 2010;56:348–350. doi: 10.1159/000266029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.OECD . Health at a Glance 2017: OECD Indicators. OECD Publishing; Paris, France: 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Alvarez-García J., Durán-Sánchez A., Del Río-Rama M.C., García-Vélez D.F. Active ageing: Mapping of scientific coverage. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2018;15:2727. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15122727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Zajacova A., Huzurbazar S., Todd M. Gender and the structure of self-rated health across the adult life span. Soc. Sci. Med. 2017;187:58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.06.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Ross C.E., Masters R.K., Hummer R.K. Education and the gender gaps in health and mortality. Demography. 2012;49:1157–1183. doi: 10.1007/s13524-012-0130-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Vafaei A., Ahmed T., Freire A.N.F., Zunzunegui M.V., Guerra R.O. Depression, sex and gender roles in older adult populations: The International Mobility in Aging Study (IMIAS) PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0146867. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0146867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Liu H., Li S., Xiao Q., Feldman M.W. Social support and psychological well-being under social change in urban and rural China. Soc. Indic. Res. 2014;119:979–996. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0534-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Siedlecki K.L., Salthouse T.A., Oishi S., Jeswani S. The relationship between social support and subjective well-being across age. Soc. Indic. Res. 2014;117:561–576. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0361-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Steptoe A., Deaton A., Stone A. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet. 2015;385:640–648. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61489-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.De Ree J., Alessie R. Life satisfaction and age: Dealing with underidentification in age-period-cohort models. Soc. Sci. Med. 2011;73:177–182. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.IMSERSO and Instituto de la Mujer Informe Sobre Las Mujeres Mayores en España. [Report on elderly women in Spain]. Madrid, Spain, 2011. [(accessed on 19 July 2019)]; Available online: http://www.mscbs.gob.es/novedades/docs/ mujeresBorrador.pdf.

Articles from International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES