Abstract
Media is so pervasive that it should no longer be considered a nuisance variable that could affect development but rather, a fundamental part of the context in which development occurs. Despite the rapid growth in access to digital media, there is a scarcity of research examining changes in the family media ecology and the subsequent effects of early media exposure on socio-cognitive development. Early research erroneously focused on the absolute amount of exposure by the child. Current research now considers the entire household, including both intentional exposure to child-directed content and unintended exposure to background media sources. Such research demonstrates that young children can and do learn from well-designed media particularly when they engage with others during digital play.
Parents, educators, and policy makers often have polarized responses regarding the adoption of digital devices by young children, either acting with extreme concern or overly optimistic enthusiasm (Lauricella, Blackwell, & Wartella, 2017). Media evokes such polarized responses because it is challenging for caregivers to navigate a rapidly changing digital world. This review will begin with an overview of the digital landscape, adopting an ecological perspective that includes media use by children, media use by parents, and joint media engagement between parents and children. This overview is followed by a discussion of current directions needed to address critical questions surrounding the consequences of growing up in the digital age.
What is the digital landscape like for very young children?
Digital media availability has surged over the past decade fueled in part by the introduction of the iPhone in 2007 and the iPad in 2010. It pervades daily activities – from driving in the car to eating a meal with family at a restaurant (McDaniel & Radesky, 2017). Most early screen time (72%) is spent viewing televised content but is no longer limited to the family TV set. Rather televised media is viewed on multiple mobile devices and via multiple forms of content delivery (e.g., streaming video, cable, YouTube). For young children, media usage is governed by parental decisions. The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommends that parents limit exposure to most types of screen time before 18 months of age (AAP, 2016). Parents seldom heed these recommendations and introduce infants to the digital world (Barr & Linebarger, 2017). In the U.S. for example, infants under 2 years are exposed to ~1 hour of media per day, 2 -to 4-year-olds are exposed to ~2 hours per day and almost half (46%) of all children less than 2 years old have used a mobile device (Rideout, 2017). These estimates are based on parental reports of intentional direct exposure to media and do not include incidental background media exposure.
More recently researchers have considered the family media ecology focusing on how media is used by all members of the household and whether these media patterns promote or interfere with early learning (Barr & Linebarger, 2017). The term “technoference” describes instances where technology interrupts interpersonal interactions and may disrupt the young child’s ability to regulate their attention or emotions (McDaniel & Radesky, 2017). The most extensive research on technoference has measured the effects of background television, defined as television that is not intended for children that are on in the background while young children are engaged in other activities. In the United States the average daily background television exposure for toddlers is 5.5 hours (Lapierre, Piotrowski, & Linebarger, 2012). Further, 42% of parents in the US report the TV is on “always” or “most of the time” in their home, whether anyone is watching or not (Rideout, 2017). During this programming, infants and toddlers typically attend to the television only 5% of the time probably because the content is mostly incomprehensible (Schmidt, Pempek, Kirkorian, Lund, & Anderson, 2008). But young children frequently orient to the screen for short periods prompted by television sound effects and then quickly look away. Parents may believe that, because their infants are not “watching” background television, their infants are not being affected by it. However, background TV decreases play episode duration, play quality and complexity (Schmidt, et al., 2008). It also decreases parent-child interactional quality; parents are slower to respond to bids for attention and respond in a more passive manner (Kirkorian, Pempek, Murphy, Schmidt, and Anderson, 2009). Exposure to background TV is associated with a negative effect on children’s language development, cognitive development, and executive functioning skills (Barr, Lauricella, Zack & Calvert, 2010; Linebarger, Barr, Lapierre, and Piotrowki, 2014; Wright et al., 2001; Zimmerman & Christakis, 2007).
Parental cell phone usage can also be problematic; when checking cell phones, parents’ faces typically have no expression, which may be perceived by young children as a “still face,” to which children respond aversively (Adamson & Frick, 2003). This behavior appears to interrupt children’s learning. Reed, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff (2017) asked mothers to teach their 2-year-olds two novel words. Mothers received a call that interrupted them while teaching one of the words, but not the other word. Children were significantly more likely to learn the uninterrupted word than the interrupted word. The result remained despite the child hearing the novel word the same number of times in both conditions.
Technoference may also occur when parents use mobile devices to distract or calm their young children, and parents are more likely to use this strategy when their children have more difficult temperaments (Radesky, Peacock-Chambers, Zuckerman & Silverstein, 2016). The authors caution, it is not possible to know whether parents with more difficult children use mobile devices more for calming, whether parents who felt more overwhelmed used mobile devices, or if mobile devices were likely to result in more socioemotional difficulties. The latter explanation is least likely given that earlier socioemotional difficulties predict later television viewing habits and not vice versa. The authors argued that frequent use of mobile devices for self-regulation may result in the development of fewer other regulatory strategies by parents and children. Overall, parents are likely to be unaware of the impact of technoference on their children’s play, learning, and emotion regulation.
In contrast, exposure to educational television programming is associated with better cognitive outcomes, particularly for low resourced families (Barr et al, 2010; Linebarger et al., 2014; Wright et al., 2001). In low-resourced families, educational media (e.g., television, apps, e-books) may be providing cognitive stimulation to children, which may not have the same impact in higher-resourced families (Linebarger et al., 2014). In sum, it is critical to not only examine the quantity of media consumed but also consider the content and context of early childhood media exposure (Barr & Linebarger, 2017).
Learning from media
Infants and toddlers do learn from television and tablets (Barr, 2013). Infants as young as 6 months old can imitate simple actions they see on TV up to 24 hours later (Barr, Muentener & Garcia, 2007); and by 18 months, toddlers can remember brief sequences that they saw on TV for 2 weeks. By 2 years old, they can remember these sequences for 1 month (Brito, Barr, McIntyre & Simcock, 2012). There is however a transfer deficit in learning from television and tablets. It is easier for young children to learn from real-life interactions with people and objects, compared with information delivered via a screen. The transfer deficit can result in a 50% decrement in learning (Barr, 2013). This reduced learning is evident in many domains, including imitation, language learning, and object retrieval tasks (Barr, 2013). For example, 1-year-olds imitate significantly less following a televised demonstration than they learn from a live demonstration of the same actions (Barr & Hayne, 1999). Similarly, when 2 year-old children are told via a pre-recorded video where to find an attractive toy hidden in the room, they are typically unable to locate the toy, even though children are perfectly capable of doing so when given the same information in person (Troseth, Saylor & Archer, 2006). Children under the age of three years are also capable of learning from interactive touchscreen tablets, but they still experience a transfer deficit (Zack, Barr, Gerhardstein, Dickerson, & Meltzoff, 2009; Moser, Zimmermann, Dickerson, Grenell, Barr, & Gerhardstein, 2015). The extent of the deficit depends upon task complexity, memory load, and the number of repetitions of content (Barr, 2013). There are a number of potential explanations for the transfer deficit, including immature memory flexibility in young children (i.e., the inability to retrieve information after a change in cues; Barr, 2013), poor symbolic understanding of the connection between 2D and 3D sources (Troseth, 2010), as well as, a lack of social contingency in 2D sources (Troseth et al., 2006).
Ameliorating the transfer deficit
Media are tools that require children to learn new affordances that often do not equally apply in the 3D world. Therefore, young children require scaffolded experiences to learn new affordances and to transfer media content to the real world.
Tablet interactivity:
Tablets provide a new affordance, allowing children to easily interact directly with the device. The addition of an interactive component largely enhances transfer of learning likely due to the addition of retrieval cues (Kirkorian, 2018). For example, in one study, 2-year-olds who engaged with interactive videos on touchscreen tablets demonstrated increased word learning as compared to toddlers who viewed non-interactive videos on tablets (Kirkorian, Choi & Pempek, 2016). But the effectiveness of the interactivity depended on the child’s age: at 2 years of age, children benefited most when they were directed by the app to interact with specific information on the screen, whereas older 2.5-year-olds did better when they could choose for themselves where to interact on the screen. Daily experiences also contribute to learning from media. Kirkorian and Choi (2017) reported that the more time toddlers had spent the previous day engaged in interactive media activities, the more they learned from both interactive tablets and videos.
Videochat.
Research show that the transfer deficit can be ameliorated by using socially contingent video calls instead of pre-recorded video for imitation tasks (Myers LeWitt, Gallo, & Masell, 2017), object search tasks (Troseth, et al., 2006), and language learning tasks (Roseberry Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff, 2014). For example, 2-year-olds learned new verbs via video chat interactions as well as they did in traditional face-to-face interactions (Roseberry et al., 2014). This may be because social contingency focuses attention, is socially rewarding and facilitates symbolic understanding. Video chat may also present some new cognitive challenges for young children as well: there may be audio or video delays; there is no physical contact with their social partner; and due to the location of the web cameras, eye contact is often misaligned (McClure & Barr, 2017).
Joint media engagement (JME).
Learning from television (Strouse & Troseth 2014), tablets (Zack & Barr, 2016) and videochat (Myers, Crawford, Murphy, Aka-Ezoua, & Felix, 2018) can be maximized via JME which occurs when people interact around media together to scaffold learning. Only when a parent provided tailored verbal scaffolding to the child did toddlers learn a word from video (Strouse & Troseth 2014). High-quality parent JME also increased 15-month-old infants’ transfer of learning from the touchscreen to an object in the physical world (Zack & Barr, 2016). Myers and colleagues (2018) reported that when the caregiver in the room was responsive, 2-year-olds remained engaged longer (e.g., looked, vocalized, and imitated more), and learned more during a video chat interaction than when the caregiver was unresponsive. Effective JME strategies, such as verbal descriptions of key features, structuring of the task, and encouragement are the same ones that parents typically use when reading picture books to their young children (Fidler, Barr & Zack, 2010; McClure & Barr, 2017).
In summary, because media content is a symbolic representation of information existing in the real world, children need guidance to learn the relation between images and real objects. Children rapidly master these skills under responsive and supportive conditions, and such mastery may be important for school readiness and academic achievement.
Current directions
Media now comprise a significant part of daily experiences for young children. There are significant gaps in the literature, including lack of research on neural mechanisms underlying learning from media, poor assessment of family media ecology, and lack of longitudinal research on the effects of media exposure across diverse populations.
Little is known about how the brain processes different forms of media during early childhood. A comprehensive series of studies examining the development of attention to media using heart rate measurement during early childhood illustrates the utility of adding biological measures (Richards, 2010). This elegant series of studies showed how attention, indexed via heart rate deceleration patterns, changed as a function of age and stimulus complexity of the media. Richards (2010) concluded that the comprehension of content involved active processing rather than a passive process. Basic audio-visual processing mechanisms have been examined using functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), a non-invasive neuroimaging technique that measures cortical hemodynamic activity via infrared light absorption changes that index changes in blood oxygenation (Aslin, 2012). Early confirmatory studies documented cortical responding to audio, visual, and audiovisual stimuli in the predicted regions of temporal, occipital, and temporal-occipital activity respectively (e.g. Hespos, Ferry, Cannistraci, Gore, Park, 2009). The advantages of fNIRS for conducting studies of learning from media in young children include tolerance of head motion, no noise, and no physical constraints. Currently, there are very few theories of developmental cognitive neuroscience that consider media. Well-designed cognitive neuroscientific experiments will be important for theory building because they will be able to evaluate how the brain processes different media content and to determine the neural resources needed to transfer learning.
More precise measurement of the family ecology of early media exposure is needed in order to predict the long-term effects of media exposure on child outcomes. Currently, there is no standardized, systematic, scalable, and cost-effective tool that comprehensively and accurately captures child and household media exposure, as well as the social context surrounding that exposure. Taking a synergistic science approach a group of researchers is developing a more comprehensive assessment of family media exposure (Barr, et al., 2018) that will include parental report of household patterns, accompanied by detailed online time use diaries and data collected via passive sensing applications. Time diaries are more accurate than surveys for measuring blocks of time to estimate TV viewing, video game play, and other activities. They can account for displacement and reduce social desirability because all activities have to be included, and detailed content and context information is collected (Vandewater & Lee, 2009). Parent report might be less reliable for mobile device use which tend to occur in shorter and more frequent bursts of activity (Goedhart, Kromhout, Wiart & Vermeulen, 2015). Passive sensing applications detect media usage on mobile devices eliminating parents’ recall bias, establishing more accurate usage estimates. Finally, there are a number of wearable devices that could be added to track physiological responses, such as heart rate variability and locomotion. New assessments also need to capture emerging technologies like videochat, virtual reality, and intelligent agents.
Limited longitudinal assessment of the long-term outcomes associated with media exposure exist (for an exception see Wright et al., 2001). Unfortunately, ongoing large-scale longitudinal studies have not routinely included media assessment, despite the fact that media are embedded in children’s daily lives. Furthermore, there is a lack of multi-nation reports on media usage. Reports almost exclusively originate within the United States. Over the past decade, mobile technology has been rapidly adopted across the globe. Although very limited in number, studies suggest that quantity of media exposure are similar cross-culturally, particularly when examining exposure via mobile devices, (e.g., in Singapore Goh et al., 2016).
In sum, new studies of any domain of development need to consider the family media environment. These studies should include comprehensive assessment of family media ecology and well-designed experiments which include neural imaging and physiological responses along with robust behavioral measures of learning. Data collection from multiple countries is needed to document both commonalities and cultural differences in the content and context of early media usage across the globe. In addition, inclusion of participants across the socioeconomic spectrum is necessary to build a complete picture of the effects of media on child outcomes. Within this framework, designs that pay close attention to fundamental developmental and learning principles will allow us to address the consequences of growing up in the digital age despite an ever evolving digital landscape.
Conclusion
The content and context of early media exposure is likely to shape developmental trajectories (Barr & Linebarger, 2017). Research demonstrates positive associations between joint media engagement of age-appropriate well-designed media content and child outcomes and negative associations between technoference and child outcomes. In this time of unprecedented technology expansion, researchers need better tools to track family media ecology and child responses and longitudinal approaches examine how developmental trajectories of media exposure affect child outcomes.
Acknowledgements
Thank you to a grant from Children and Screens: Institute of Digital Media and Child Development.
References
- Adamson L, and Frick J (2003). The Still Face: A History of a Shared Experimental Paradigm Infancy, 4, 451–473 DOI: 10.1207/S15327078IN0404_01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Aslin RN (2012). Questioning the questions that have been asked about the infant brain using near-infrared spectroscopy. Cognitive neuropsychology, 29, 7–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Academy of Pediatrics. Council on communications and media (2016). Reid Chassiakos Y, Radesky J, Christakis D, et al. Children and Adolescents and Digital Media. Pediatrics,138, e20162593. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr R (2013). Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 205–210. DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12041. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Barr R & Hayne H (1999). Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. Child Development, 70, 1067–1081. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8624.00079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr R, Lauricella A, Zack E, & Calvert SL (2010). The relation between infant exposure to television and executive functioning, cognitive skills, and school readiness at age four. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 56, 21–48. [Google Scholar]
- Barr R, Muentener P, & Garcia A (2007). Age-related changes in deferred imitation from television by 6- to 18-month-olds. Developmental Science, 15, 812–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr R, Kirkorian H, Nichols D, Radesky J, Coyne S, Stockdale L, Ball R, Blanchfield O, Chang Y, Epstein M & the CAFE Consortiuum Key investigators (October, 2018). Growing up in the digital age: Developing the CAFE assessment tool. Presented at the Digital Media and the Minds Second National Congress. Cold Springs Habor, NY. [Google Scholar]
- Brito N, Barr R, McIntyre P, & Simcock G (2012). Long-term transfer of learning from books and video during toddlerhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 108–119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr R, Kirkorian H, Nichols H, Radesky J, Coyne S, Stockdale L, Ball R, Blanchfield O, Chang Y, Epstein M and CAFE consortium members. (2018). Growing Up in the Digital Age: Developing the CAFE Assessment. Paper to be presented at the Child and Screens Institute Conference. Cold Springs Habor, NY. [Google Scholar]
- Fidler A, Zack E, & Barr R (2010). Television viewing patterns in 6- to 18-month-olds: The role of caregiver-infant interactional quality. Infancy, 15, 176–196. Doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00013.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goedhart G, Kromhout H, Wiart J, & Vermeulen R (2015). Validating self-reported mobile phone use in adults using a newly developed smartphone application. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 72, 812–818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goh SN, Teh LH, Tay WR, Anantharaman S, van Dam RM, Tan CS, Chua HL, Wong PG & Müller-Riemenschneider F (2016). Sociodemographic, home environment and parental influences on total and device-specific screen viewing in children aged 2 years and below: an observational study. BMJ Open, 6 (1) e009113; DOI: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hespos SJ, Ferry AL, Cannistraci C, Gore J, Park S (2009). Optical imaging on human infants. In Roe AW (Ed.) Imaging the brain with optical methods (pp. 159–176).Springer: New York. [Google Scholar]
- Kirkorian HL (2018). When and How Do Interactive Digital Media Help Children Connect What They See On and Off the Screen? Child Development Perspectives, 10.1111/cdep.12290 [DOI]
- Kirkorian HL, & Choi K (2017). Associations Between Toddlers’ Naturalistic Media Experience and Observed Learning from Screens. Infancy, 22, 271–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkorian HL, Choi K, & Pempek TA (2016). Toddlers’ word learning from contingent and noncontingent video on touchscreens. Child Development, 87, 405–413. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12508 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkorian HL, Pempek TA, Murphy LA, Schmidt ME, and Anderson DR (2009). The impact of background television on parent-child interaction. Child Development, 80, 1350–1359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lauricella AR, Blackwell CK, & Wartella E (2017). The “New” technology environment: The role of content and context on learning and development from mobile media. In Barr R, and Linebarger DN (Eds.), The Effect of Content and Context on Learning and Development (pp. 1–24). New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Lapierre MA, Piotrowski JT, and Linebarger DL (2012). Background television in the homes of US children. Pediatrics, 130(5), 839–846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linebarger DL, Barr R, Lapierre MA, & Piotrowski JT (2014). Associations between parenting, media use, cumulative risk, and children’s executive functioning. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 35, 367–377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClure E and Barr R (2017). Building Family Relationships from a Distance: Supporting Connections with Babies and Toddlers Using Video and Video Chat. In Barr R, and Linebarger DN (Eds.). Media Exposure During Infancy and Early Childhood: The Effect of Content and Context on Learning and Development (pp. 227–248). New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- McDaniel BT and Radesky JS (2017), Technoference: Parent Distraction With Technology and Associations With Child Behavior Problems. Child Development doi: 10.1111/cdev.12822 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Myers LJ, LeWitt RB, Gallo RE and Maselli NM (2017), Baby FaceTime: can toddlers learn from online video chat?. Developmental Science, 20: n/a, e12430. doi: 10.1111/desc.12430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers LJ, Crawford E, Murphy C, Aka-Ezoua E & Felix C (2018). Eyes in the room trump eyes on the screen: effects of a responsive co-viewer on toddlers’ responses to and learning from video chat, Journal of Children and Media, doi: 10.1080/17482798.2018.1425889 [DOI]
- Moser A, Zimmermann L, Dickerson K, Grenell A, Barr R & Gerhardstein P (2015). They can interact but can they learn? Toddlers’ transfer learning from touchscreens and television. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 137, 137–155. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radesky JS, Peacock-Chambers E, Zuckerman B & Silverstein M (2016). Use of Mobile Technology to Calm Upset Children Associations with Social-Emotional Development. JAMA Pediatrics, 170, 397–399. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.4260 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed J, Hirsh-Pasek K and Golinkoff R (2017). Learning on hold: Cell phones sidetrack parent-child interactions. Developmental Psychology, 53, 1428–1436. doi: 10.1037/dev0000292. Epub 2017 Jun 26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards JE (2010). The development of attention to simple and complex visual stimuli in infants: Behavioral and psychophysiological measures. Developmental Review, 30, 203–219. 10.1016/j.dr.2010.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rideout V (2017). The Common Sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media. [Google Scholar]
- Roseberry S, Hirsh-Pasek K,& Golinkoff RM (2014). Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development 85, 956–70. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt ME, Pempek TA, Kirkorian HL, Lund AF, & Anderson DR (2008). The effects of background television on the toy play behavior of very young children. Child Development, 79, 1137–1151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strouse GA and Troseth GL, (2014). Supporting toddlers’ transfer of word learning from video. Cognitive Development, 30, 47–64. DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.01.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Troseth GL (2010). Is it life or is it Memorex? Video as a representation of reality. Developmental Review, 30, 155–175. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.03.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Troseth GL, Saylor MM, and Archer AH (2006). Young children’s use of video as a source of socially relevant information. Child Development, 77, 786–799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandewater EA, and Lee SJ (2009). Measuring Children’s Media Use in the Digital Age: Issues and Challenges. The American Behavioral Scientist, 52, 1152–1176. 10.1177/0002764209331539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright JC, Huston AC, Murphy KC, Peters M St., Piñon M, Scantlin R, and Kotler J (2001). The relations of early television viewing to school readiness and vocabulary of children from low-income families: The Early Window Project. Child Development, 72, 1347–1366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zack E, and Barr R (2016). The Role of Interactional Quality in Learning from Touch Screens during Infancy: Context Matters. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1264 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01264 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zack E, Barr R, Gerhardstein P, Dickerson K and Meltzoff AN (2009). Infant imitation from television using novel touch-screen technology. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 13–26. Doi: 10.1348/026151008X334700 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman FJ, & Christakis DA (2007). Associations between content types of early media exposure and subsequent attentional problems. Pediatrics, 120, 986–992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Recommended readings
- 1.Barr R & Linebarger DN (Eds. 2017). Media Exposure During Infancy and Early Childhood: The Effect of Content and Context on Learning and Development. Springer, New York, NY. [Google Scholar]; This paper provides full discussion of early exposure to media for readers who wish to learn more about the content and context of early media exposure.
- 2.Schmidt ME, Pempek TA, Kirkorian HL, Lund AF, & Anderson DR (2008). The effects of background television on the toy play behavior of very young children. Child Development, 79, 1137–1151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; A representative study that illustrates original research about background media.
- 3.Wright JC, Huston AC, Murphy KC, Peters M St., Piñon M, Scantlin R, and Kotler J (2001). The relations of early television viewing to school readiness and vocabulary of children from low-income families: The Early Window Project. Child Development, 72, 1347–1366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; A historical classic; one of the first papers to raise attention about long-term effects of the media content on cognitive outcomes for low-resourced families.
- 4.Barr R & Hayne H (1999). Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. Child Development, 70, 1067–1081. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8624.00079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; A good illustration of learning from television and the transfer deficit.
- 5.Kirkorian HL (2018). When and How Do Interactive Digital Media Help Children Connect What They See On and Off the Screen? Child Development Perspectives, 10.1111/cdep.12290 [DOI]; A clearly written, user-friendly, and relatively comprehensive review for readers who wish to expand their knowledge on interactive media.
- 6.Rusnak S & Barr R (accepted). Infant Learning and Memory from Technology. In Catherine Tamis-LeMonda & Jeffry Lockman (Eds.). Handbook of Infant Development, Cambridge University Press Cambridge, UK. [Google Scholar]; This chapter provides a comprehensive review of the nascent field of neural imaging and media, as well as, an overview of recent findings in the field of infants and the media.