Abstract
Bipolar disorder (BD) and borderline personality disorder (BPD) are two psychiatric disorders with overlapping features that can be challenging to separate diagnostically. Growing evidence suggests that circadian rhythm disturbances are associated with psychiatric illness, however circadian patterns of behaviour have not been elucidated in BPD or differentiated from BD. This study compared the circadian structure and timing of rest-activity patterns in BPD with BD and healthy volunteers. Participants with BD (N = 31) and BPD (N = 21) and healthy controls (HC, N = 35) wore an actigraph on their non-dominant wrist for 28 day periods as part of the Automated Monitoring of Symptom Severity (AMoSS) study. Non-parametric circadian rhythm analysis of rest-activity patterns and cosinor analysis of distal temperature rhythms were conducted to elucidate circadian function between groups. Covariates controlled for included employment status, BMI and gender. Compared with HC and BD, individuals with BPD showed significantly delayed phase of night-time rest patterns (“L5 onset”) (mean difference = 1:47 h, P < 0.001; mean difference = 1:38 h, P = 0.009, respectively), and relative to HC showed delayed daytime activity onset (“M10 onset”) (mean difference = 2:13 h, P = 0.048) and delayed temperature phase (mean difference = 1:22 h, P = 0.034). These findings suggest that delayed circadian function may be a clinically important phenotype in individuals with BPD. Future work should interrogate the causality of this association and examine interventions which target delayed circadian function in the treatment of BPD.
Subject terms: Psychiatric disorders, Bipolar disorder
Introduction
Disturbed sleep patterns feature prominently in psychiatric disorders1, and a recent focus has been placed on the investigation of sleep related phenotypes to discriminate between psychiatric syndromes2 and chart the progression of symptom severity3. Substantive evidence suggests that bipolar disorder (BD) and borderline personality disorder (BPD) are conditions particularly associated with abnormal sleep characteristics4,5. Both disorders are characterized by recurrent mood instability and impulsivity, which can make clinical differentiation challenging6, however both conditions diverge in terms of treatment approach and prognosis.
Sleep is a complex process involving several neurotransmitter systems and fulfils a wide range of functions. A key component in understanding the interplay between sleep and psychiatric disorder is the circadian timing system, which, independent of sleep, regulates diurnal rhythms of physiology and behaviour, and shapes the phase and structure of the 24-h rest-activity pattern7,8 (to differentiate between ambulatory assessment of activity compared with sleep/wake patterns confirmed by other methods such as polysomnography here we refer to circadian “rest-activity” patterns throughout). Genetic linkage and expression studies of canonical molecular clock elements suggest that circadian rhythm disturbance may be an essential feature of BD9,10 and phenotypic studies of rest-activity patterns and endocrine function in BD have been interpreted as supportive of this hypothesis. Among these findings are an attenuated amplitude and phase delayed rhythm of melatonin secretion11–15, reduced rest-activity rhythm amplitudes and greater rhythm fragmentation2,12,16. While depressive and manic mood states profoundly alter patterns of rest and activity, abnormal features persist in euthymia4. Disturbed patterns of rest and activity are also reported in non-psychiatric groups with marked unstable mood, greater impulsiveness, and in those at higher of risk of BD, and thus such features may be of transdiagnostic relevance17–20.
Despite the large overlap with BD in its core diagnostic symptoms, circadian disturbance in BPD has rarely been investigated. This is despite the greater prevalence of personality disorder (including BPD) among patients presenting with circadian rhythm sleep disorders21 and correlations between symptom severity, suicidal ideation, and non-24-h circadian rhythm period in BPD22. Recently, we further demonstrated desynchrony between the phase of diurnal rhythms of heart rate and parameters reflecting rhythms of activity and estimated sleep in BD and BPD23. Such desynchrony may reflect aberrant phase relationships of internal rhythms driven by the circadian clock, which are hypothesized to be associated with worsened disease state24. Furthermore, that BPD showed greater rhythm desynchronization and greater phase delay than BD, and a clearer association of variable rhythms with unstable mood, was further suggestive of a disturbed circadian component25. However, these data were obtained over short time intervals (several days) and using devices worn on the torso which may interfere with normal sleep.
Wrist-worn actigraphy has been used extensively to monitor rest-activity patterns in healthy populations and patient groups across weeks rather than days, and such patterns are used as standard markers of human circadian function26. We present here rest-activity patterns monitored over 28 day periods in patients with BD, BPD, and healthy controls (HC), with the objective of determining pattern differences between groups. Based on previous actigraphy reports in BD we hypothesized that rest-activity patterns in BD patients would display attenuated amplitude and greater rhythm fragmentation. For BPD we hypothesized evidence of delayed circadian function comporting with heart rate rhythms previously described in these patients. The explicit comparison of BD with BPD was to explore circadian related phenotypes that may be independently germane to each diagnosis. Importantly, features of delayed rest-activity patterns in BPD revealed in the current study suggest a dysfunctional circadian underpinning, amelioration of which may represent a new opportunity for improving treatment.
Methods
Participants
Participants with a diagnosis of BD, BPD, and HC, were recruited from outpatient services and the community, respectively, as part of the Automated Monitoring of Symptom Severity (AMoSS) study27. Recruitment methods involved local advertising and word-of-mouth. A total of 129 participants were enrolled in the study, 113 of whom had 28 day actigraphy recordings. The final sample consisted of data from 87 participants (31 BD; 21 BPD; and 35 HC). Data from other participants were excluded due to actigraph non-compliance (n = 5) or technical problems with recording (n = 5). Sixteen participants were further excluded from analysis due to artefactual phase shifts occurring during daylight savings time transition28,29. Participants were intended to be age matched but this was not the case after aforementioned exclusions (though no significant age differences were detected). Diagnosis and demographic information did not differ between included and excluded participants (see the Supplementary methods). All participants were screened and diagnoses confirmed prior to study admission by an experienced psychiatrist (KEAS) using the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV and the International Personality Disorders Examination (IPDE)30. BD and BPD participants were studied in the mood state that was most normal for them and remained stable throughout the recording interval. For BD, they were not syndromally depressed or (hypo) manic. However, they were symptomatic as the majority of BD patients are during inter-episode phases. For BPD, they were not in crisis but they were symptomatic as is a chronic experience of the condition. Exclusion criteria for BD and BPD were comorbidity of each diagnosis. Exclusion criteria for the control group included: history of neurological disorder or head injury, history of major psychiatric illness, and having a first degree relative with a history of BPD or BD. The study protocol was approved by the NRES Committee East of England—Norfolk (13/EE/0288) and all participants gave written informed consent.
Clinical and self-report assessment
Severity of BPD symptoms was assessed using IPDE scores. Trait impulsivity was assessed among all participants by self-report using the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11)31. The Altman Self-Rating Mania Scale (ASRM)32 and Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (QIDS)33 were completed weekly during the recording interval to assess manic and depressive symptoms respectively. Full descriptions of each questionnaire are contained in the supplementary materials.
Measuring rest-activity and skin temperature rhythms
The structure and timing of daily rest-activity patterns were assessed using wrist-worn GENEActiv Original actigraphs (ActivInsights Ltd, UK) which participants wore continuously for 28 consecutive days. Actigraph sampling frequency was set at 25 Hz to facilitate battery life for the recording period. Data were analysed using the GGIR dedicated package34 for R version 3.4.2 (R Core Team, Vienna). Further details on data processing are contained in the supplementary materials (Supplementary methods; Supplementary Fig. 1).
Non-parametric circadian rhythm analysis (NPCRA) was performed to determine the circadian structure of rest-activity rhythms (intradaily variability, interdaily stability, and relative amplitude) and to examine the average phase and activity levels during the 5 h period of lowest activity (L5) and 10 h period of greatest activity (M10)35. The intradaily variability (IV) describes the within-day consolidation of rest-activity states, where greater values indicate greater rhythm fragmentation (i.e., more transitions between rest and active states); the interdaily stability (IS) describes the consistency of rest-activity patterns between days, where greater values reflect greater stability; and the relative amplitude (RA) describes the relative difference in activity between the most active 10 h period (M10) and least active 5 h period (L5) and is a marker of the amplitude strength. Within subject variability of daily timing (L5, M10) and RA was assessed using the standard deviation of daily measures.
Distal skin temperature measured from the wrist was simultaneously recorded by the actigraph at a resolution of 0.25 °C and exported in 60 s epochs. A phase marker of the temperature rhythm was determined using the centre-of-gravity (CoG) of a single sine wave fit to participant’s average 24-h rhythm using CircWave version 1.4 (Professor Roelof Hut, University of Groningen). This measure indicates the phase of the poikilothermic circadian rhythm, which is inversely associated with core-body temperature and corresponds with the regulation of rest-activity states36 and circadian phase37.
Data analysis
Data were inspected for normal distribution and appropriate parametric or non-parametric tests were performed. Group-wise comparisons were applied to summary NPCRA measures using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) (see expanded description in Supplementary methods and Supplementary Table 1). Covariates inserted were employment status (0 = employed; 1 = unemployed), BMI, and gender, as these showed differences between groups. Follow-up ANOVA main effects adjusted for added covariates are reported with Bonferroni post hoc tests to investigate differences between groups. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 22 (IBM). Due to the paucity of work examining rest-activity characteristics between BD and BPD, sample sizes could not be based on directly comparable experiments. Instead they were chosen as likely to detect worthwhile effect sizes and with the intention to estimate valid power requirements accurately for future studies. All comparisons were two-tailed with significance determined at P < 0.05. To adjust for multiple analyses from follow-up ANOVAs, correction for the false discovery rate (FDR) was applied and P values adjusted for each set.
Results
Sample demographics
Demographic details of study participants are shown in Table 1. Males were less represented in the BPD group and there was a preponderance of unemployed individuals with BPD relative to BD and controls. There were significant group differences in BMI with BD more overweight than controls. Impulsivity, depression and IPDE symptom counts were greater among BD and BPD compared with controls. The proportion of patients taking any psychotropic medication was greater for BD than for BPD; lithium and antipsychotics were common for BD, antidepressants and anxiolytics for BPD.
Table 1.
HC (n = 35) | BD (n = 31) | BPD (n = 21) | Test statistic | P | Post hoc | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender, M:F | 11:24 | 10:21 | 2:19 | χ2 = 4.077 | 0.130 | – |
Age, year (SD) | 39.46 (12.51) | 39.23 (12.24) | 34.14 (10.5) | |||
BMI, mean (SD) | 24.13 (3.98) | 27.74 (4.62) | 27.33 (6.36) | F = 5.288 | 0.007 | BD > HC |
Unemployed, no. (%) | 5 (14) | 2 (7) | 11 (52) | χ2 = 17.558 | <0.001 | BPD > HC, BPD > BD |
Bipolar Diagnosis, BD-I:BD-II | – | 23:8 | – | – | – | – |
BIS-11, mean (SD) | 52.53 (8.74) | 70.07 (12.02) | 75.33 (15.56) | F = 29.324 | <0.001 | BD > HC, BPD > HC |
IPDE, mdn (IQR) | 0 (0.00) | 4 (6.25) | 16 (3.5) | χ2 = 73.15a | <0.001 | BD > HC, BPD > HC, BPD > BD |
ASRM, mdn (IQR) | 0.708 (2.6) | 1.5 (3.6) | 2.0 (3.53) | χ2 = 5.189a | 0.075 | – |
QIDS, mdn (IQR) | 2.0 (2.29) | 7.8 (6.0) | 13.67 (7.23) | χ2 = 28.477a | <0.001 | BD > HC, BPD > HC |
Psychotropic medication use, no. (%) | N/A | 30 (97) | 16 (76) | χ2 = 5.197 | 0.023 | BD > BPD |
Lithium | – | 14 | 0 | |||
Anticonvulsant | – | 6 | 1 | |||
Antipsychotic | – | 22 | 5 | |||
Antidepressant | – | 10 | 16 | |||
Hypnotic | – | 2 | 1 | |||
Anxiolytic | – | 3 | 5 | |||
Season, no. (%) | – | – | – | χ2 = 6.280 | 0.393 | – |
Spring | 8 (23) | 10 (32) | 10 (47) | |||
Summer | 8 (23) | 10 (32) | 4 (19) | |||
Autumn | 9 (26) | 4 (13) | 2 (10) | |||
Winter | 10 (28) | 7 (23) | 5 (24) | |||
Daylight savings time zone, no. (%) | χ2 = 6.280 | 0.922 | – | |||
GMT (+00 h) | 10 (29) | 8 (26) | 5 (24) | |||
BST (+01 h) | 25 (71) | 23 (74) | 16 (76) | |||
Valid actigraphy intervals, days (SD) | 22.26 (3.32) | 22.13 (3.22) | 22.19 (4.3) | F = 0.012 | 0.988 | – |
Weekdays | 15.21 (2.22) | 14.35 (2.01) | 14.81 (2.25) | F = 1.201 | 0.306 | – |
Weekend days | 5.85 (1.1) | 6.16 (0.82) | 6.10 (1.04) | F = 0.850 | 0.431 | – |
Non-wear time, mins (SD) | 263.38 (408.31) | 510 (968.38) | 203.57 (454.94) | F = 1.164b | 0.321 | – |
F statistic denotes univariate ANOVA; χ2 denotes chi-square test
HC healthy control, BD bipolar disorder, BPD borderline personality disorder, ASRM Altman Self-Report Mania Scale, BIS Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, IPDE International Personality Disorder Examination module, QIDS Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, GMT Greenwich Mean Time, BST British Summer Time
aKruskal–Wallis non-parametric ANOVA
bWelch test for inequality of variances
Actigraphy monitoring was well tolerated with 80.5% of included participants wearing devices for the specified 28 day period. Groups did not differ significantly with regard to valid intervals after quality control measures were applied to data, and showed no differences in number of weekdays, weekends, and non-wear time during recording period. The proportion of recordings obtained over seasons and daylight savings adjusted months did not differ between groups.
Assessment of rest-activity and temperature rhythms
Multivariate analysis of variance of summary NPCRA actigraph measures revealed a main effect of diagnosis on the structure of rest-activity rhythms characterized by the variables: IV, IS, and RA (Table 2) (MANOVA; main effect of diagnosis: F6, 162 = 3.04, P = 0.008; main effect adjusting for gender, BMI, and employment status: F6, 156 = 2.45, P = 0.027).
Table 2.
MANOVA | ANOVA | Mean ± SD | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Wilks Λ | F | P | P adj. | η p 2 | F | P Corr | η p 2 | HC (n = 35) | BD (n = 31) | BPD (n = 21) | ||
Rhythm structure | 0.808 | 3.035 | 0.008 | 0.027 | 0.085 | |||||||
Intradaily variability | 4.773 | 0.033 | 0.107 | 1.115 ± 0.244 | 0.906 ± 0.274 | 0.947 ± 0.272 | ||||||
Interdaily stability | 2.31 | 0.12 | 0.055 | 0.336 ± 0.141 | 0.409 ± 0.116 | 0.356 ± 0.144 | ||||||
Relative amplitude | 2.179 | 0.12 | 0.052 | 0.794 ± 0.109 | 0.803 ± 0.083 | 0.733 ± 0.133 | ||||||
Rhythm timing | 0.715 | 4.557 | <0.001 | 0.002 | 0.126 | |||||||
L5 onset | 7.957 | 0.002 | 0.166 | 00:26 ± 01:23 | 00:38 ± 01:20 | 02:13 ± 01:20 | ||||||
M10 onset | 4.374 | 0.024 | 0.099 | 10:26 ± 03:10 | 11:58 ± 03:06 | 12:40 ± 02:50 | ||||||
Temperature CoG | 3.533 | 0.034 | 0.083 | 02:45 ± 01:23 | 02:46 ± 01:41 | 04:06 ± 01:51 | ||||||
Arousal level | 0.917 | 1.814 | 0.129 | 0.241 | 0.034 | |||||||
L5 activity | 1.982 | 0.29 | 0.047 | 7.088 ± 6.261 | 6.090 ± 3.25 | 8.547 ± 6.574 | ||||||
M10 activity | 0.377 | 0.687 | 0.009 | 62.06 ± 23.90 | 57.78 ± 19.63 | 52.79 ± 22.11 | ||||||
Total variability | 0.907 | 1.353 | 0.237 | 0.559 | 0.030 | |||||||
Daily L5 SD | 0.345 | 0.709 | 0.009 | 1.767 ± 2.332 | 1.527 ± 2.03 | 1.866 ± 1.8 | ||||||
Daily M10 SD | 1.737 | 0.549 | 0.042 | 33.23 ± 31.01 | 22.74 ± 16.36 | 21.75 ± 13.13 | ||||||
Daily RA SD | 0.523 | 0.709 | 0.013 | 0.054 ± 0.028 | 0.057 ± 0.035 | 0.068 ± 0.035 |
Padj. denotes MANOVA P values adjusted for gender, BMI, and employment status inserted as covariates. ANOVA main effects are reported adjusted for covariates with P values FDR-adjusted for each ANOVA within each analysis set (PCorr)
Follow-up univariate ANOVAs showed a main effect of diagnosis on IV: F2, 80 = 5.698, P = 0.033. Post hoc tests indicated that HC showed greater IV than BD (P = 0.014) (Fig. 1a). No significant group main effects were detected for IS (Fig. 1b) or RA (Fig. 1c).
Multivariate analysis of the variables describing the timing of the rest-activity rhythm (L5 onset, M10 onset, Temperature CoG) revealed statistically significant effects (MANOVA; main effect of diagnosis: F6, 162 = 4.56, P = 0.0001; main effect adjusting for gender, BMI, and employment status: F6, 150 = 3.62, P = 0.002). Differences were robust across different parameters of rhythm timing determined by univariate ANOVA main group effects (L5 onset: F2, 80 = 7.96, P = 0.002; M10 onset: F2, 80 = 4.37, P = 0.024; Temperature CoG: F2, 78 = 3.53, P = 0.034).
Post hoc tests revealed that BPD had a significantly later time of L5 onset compared with HC (mean difference = 1:47 h, P < 0.001) and BD (mean difference = 1:38 h, P = 0.009) (Fig. 2a). BPD had a significantly later time of M10 onset compared with HC (mean difference = 2:13 h, P = 0.048) as did BD compared with HC (mean difference = 1:31 h, P = 0.047) (Fig. 2b). Distal temperature phase was significantly delayed in BPD compared with HC (mean difference = 1:22, P = 0.034) but not BD (Fig. 2c).
MANOVA results did not indicate a main effect for variables describing the mean levels of motor arousal (L5 activity and 10 activity; main effect of diagnosis: F4, 164 = 1.81, P = 0.129) or the total variability between days captured by the variation in daily values of L5 and M10 activity levels and RAs of each day (main effect of diagnosis: F6, 162 = 1.35, P = 0.237).
The supplementary materials contain a series of sensitivity analyses probing this effect further. All ANOVA results were unchanged after undertaking stratified analyses for gender (see Supplementary Analyses and Supplementary Table 2) and sensitivity analyses further adjusting for the effect of age (Supplementary Table 3), weekend/weekday routines (i.e., social jetlag) (Supplementary Table 4), and ASRM score (Supplementary Table 5). The effect of diagnosis on IV was no longer significant after controlling for medication use (Supplementary Table 6) and QIDS score (Supplementary Table 7).
Representative example actograms displaying 24-h activity patterns are shown in Fig. 3. Examples of individual rest-activity profile plots indicating consolidation of rhythms are shown in Supplementary Fig. 2.
Comparison of mean 24 h profiles
Activity and temperature profiles of BD, BPD, and HC groups were further inspected using mixed repeated measures ANOVAs of hourly means of activity counts and half hourly means of distal temperature recordings. Analysis of activity revealed significant effects of local time (Greenhouse-Geisser corrected F4.6, 365.4 = 213, P < 0.001) and no significant main group effect (F2, 84 = 0.86, P = 0.428), confirming that the mean intensity of motor arousal did not differ between groups. The local time × group interaction was significant (Greenhouse-Geisser corrected F8.7, 365.4 = 2.33, P = 0.016) indicating differences in the timing of activity. Simple comparisons revealed that BPD showed greater levels of activity in the late evening between 22:00 and 01:00 h compared with HC (Fisher’s LSD: P < 0.05) (Fig. 4a). There were no distinguishable differences between the activity rhythms of BD compared with HC or BPD.
Similarly, significant effects of local time were revealed for temperature profiles (Greenhouse-Geisser corrected F4.6, 375.6 = 127.88, P < 0.001) and no significant main group effect was detected (F2, 81 = 0.08, P < 0.92). The local time × group interaction for temperature profile was statistically significant (Greenhouse-Geisser corrected F9.3, 375.6 = 3.41, P < 0.001) indicating that the timing of the daily profile differed between groups. The average temperature profile showed a phase delay of the morning descending limb of distal temperature in the BPD. Simple comparisons most notably revealed that BPD individuals had a slower decrease in temperature suggestive of greater sleep inertia between the 07:00 and 09:30 h compared with HC (Fisher’s LSD: P < 0.05) (Fig. 4b). Comparisons did not reveal overt differences between HC and BD.
Discussion
BD, BPD, and healthy volunteer groups show divergent organization of rest-activity patterns as measured by wrist actigraphy. Most notably we found that BPD patients show phase delayed rest-activity rhythms compared with HC and BD patients, and phase delayed temperature rhythms compared with HC, indicating that delayed circadian rhythmicity may be an important pathophysiological feature of the disorder. We believe this is the first time this phenomenon has been characterized over an adequate period of time.
While an association between circadian disturbance and BPD was proposed some years ago38, the evidence to date has been limited. The first and largest study of 59 patients with mixed diagnoses (the majority with comorbid BPD), conducted over 20 years ago22, focussed on admission for self-harm when patients were depressed and/or in crisis rather than circadian disturbance per se. A systematic review of the literature to 2015 highlights objectively recorded differences between BPD patients and controls, which emphasize shorter sleep duration, and importantly, greater sleep onset latency which may be suggestive of delayed clock function5. However, these measures were ascertained by polysomnography, which involves short duration recordings (i.e., single nights) and therefore do not adequately assess the circadian structure of sleep over multiple days. Of the 32 studies reviewed only two obtained actigraphy records of rest-activity patterns over several days and these involved small samples sizes, were designed to assess treatment efficacy39 or were too short in duration allowing only impressionistic statements about disturbed sleep patterns40. Thus the current study is the first to assess circadian rest-activity patterns in BPD over an adequate time course in an ecologically valid manner. Our activity findings are further supported by significantly delayed temperature rhythms, which also predict circadian phase. These differences were most pronounced during the morning descending portion of the temperature rhythm indicating that greater sleep inertia may be a corresponding feature of BPD pathophysiology as suggested previously40.
According to the two process model of sleep regulation41, findings of delayed phase patterns of activity in BPD may arise as a result of disorganisation of circadian physiology or inadequate entrainment via environmental zeitgebers (i.e., agents, notably light, which signal the relative time of day and synchronize circadian clock phase). Unstable mood and chaotic behaviour are both characteristic of BPD and may result in inadequate zeitgeber exposure and faulty entrainment. If so, the findings could be entirely secondary to psychopathology, or they could form an unstable interaction where circadian phase delay leads to greater mood instability and vice versa. Alternatively or in addition, the clock function of the brain may be disturbed in some way42. Importantly, associations have previously been described between delayed phase indicators and adverse outcomes, such as suicide attempt43 and depressogenic cognitions44. Thus, whatever the mechanism, further work is merited to determine whether delayed rhythms are relevant predictors of core symptom severity in BPD. Pilot studies deploying bright light therapy in BPD have demonstrated the efficacy of light to advance the circadian clock and improve symptoms when combined with antidepressant treatment39,45. Moreover, novel application of chronotherapeutic approaches have been successfully used in other psychiatric illnesses46,47. Thus, stabilization and normalization of circadian function could be a valuable explicit goal for the treatment of BPD.
Abnormality in bipolar patients involved greater IV among HC compared with BD. This pattern diverges from many of the reported studies of circadian function in BD over shorter time intervals, which emphasize rhythm fragmentation and reduced rhythm amplitude. The explanation may be that such disturbances emerge transiently, perhaps dependant on variable mood state48, or experimental method; as the current sample of BD patients was notably stable. Moreover, we found no differences in nocturnal arousal (L5 activity) between groups; this may account for rhythm structure differences seen previously in BD patients and high-risk individuals2,19. Sensitivity analyses indicate that IV differences may be explained by depressive symptoms, similar to the trend observed with sleep duration in remitted BD49. Previous systematic reviews examining actigraphy determined activation (i.e., patterns reflecting activity and energy), found that both euthymic and depressed state BD groups are repeatedly differentiated from HC groups by lower mean activity50,51. Thus, lower IV might emerge as a result of lower daily activation in BD driven by mood and may instead be a consequence of mood symptoms rather than an expression of endogenous circadian rhythm function per se.
The finding of later M10 onset in BD relative to HC may also be indicative of lower energy expenditure early in the day;52 the absence of differences seen in other phase indicators suggests delayed circadian rhythms are not as prominent as those observed in BPD. Phase differences in BD patients have not been consistently replicated among the existing literature with a mix of delayed and advanced patterns reported. Recent studies have suggested that BD patients displaying delayed circadian phase53 and advanced circadian phase54,55 may represent differences in patient profile and the clinical course of BD; the former being associated with younger age, shorter duration of BD, and more frequent depressive mood episodes, while the latter is associated with manic episodes and is more prevalent among suicide attempters with BD. Similarly, patient history and the relative symptom stability in BD in this study compared with others may be a potential source of heterogeneity in results reflecting the rest-activity rhythm structure and phase.
Sensitivity analysis furthermore suggested that the difference in IV is not due to social jetlag. However, prominent bimodal patterns of activity peaks corresponding with typical work start and end times were noted in representative control activity profiles (Supplementary Fig. 2). Thus, the reduced consolidation of rhythms we observe may arise as a result of different behavioural routines in HC compared with patient groups. Alternatively, the difference may be a result of social or functional impairment among patients, or occupational routine and greater caring responsibilities in controls. Finally, as it was difficult in practice to match medication and gender between groups, we accept that these factors could contribute to group-wise differences (e.g., IV differences in BD covaried with medication use; Supplementary Table 6). Thus, increased sleep inertia in BPD could be partially related to the greater use of sedative anxiolytic and antidepressant medicines and/or to differential sleep organization between men and women (there was a higher proportion of women in the BPD group). However, sensitivity and stratification analyses for medication use and gender respectively show that main group differences in circadian parameters indicating rhythm timing remain robust (see supplementary materials).
Strengths and limitations
The current study is one of the longest duration actigraphy studies in BD and BPD patients and to our knowledge the only study longitudinally comparing rest-activity patterns between BD and BPD. Study limitations involve the absence of concurrent sleep monitoring via sleep diaries or polysomnography and the reliance on actigraphy derived indicators of circadian function. As the actigraphy variables measured here are a consequence of exogenous factors, as well as endogenously generated rhythms of the circadian clock, confirmation of the latter is necessary. Parallel to monitoring activity we report identical phase patterns among distal skin temperature profiles. However, due to environmental masking, future work examining physiologic circadian rhythms such as in endocrine (e.g., melatonin) function and clock protein expression is necessary to completely delineate circadian phenotypes between BD and BPD. Other potential limitations relate to the clinical sample; it is difficult to match for comorbidity and we did not explicitly screen for sleep apnoea or other pertinent sleep disorders. Moreover, adequate control for medication effects is challenging due to polypharmacy and the divergent treatment protocols for BD and BPD reflected in the present sample. Unbalanced gender proportions between groups is a further limitation. Future work is necessary to replicate and confirm the associations described and to disentangle which causal elements may be at play.
Conclusion
BD and BPD involve similar core symptoms and both are associated with significant sleep disturbance. Whether circadian dysfunction may underpin BPD had not been appreciated or investigated to the same degree as BD. Our findings show that the rest-activity pattern in BPD is significantly phase delayed across several measures compared with HC and displays significantly later nocturnal activity phase compared with BD. BD showed later phase of diurnal activity relative to HC but disturbance was less pronounced than with BPD. Monitoring of disease phenotypes in this manner may direct future therapeutic strategies. This is especially relevant in BPD given the paucity of pharmacologic and psychotherapeutic treatment options currently recognized.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by the Wellcome Trust through a Centre Grant no. 98,461/Z/12/Z, “The University of Oxford Sleep and Circadian Neuroscience Institute (SCNi)”. This work was also funded by a Wellcome Trust Strategic Award (CONBRIO: Collaborative Oxford Network for Bipolar Research to Improve Outcomes, Reference number 102,616/Z). This research was supported by the National Institute for Health Research Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.
Conflict of interest
G.M.G. is a NIHR Emeritus Senior Investigator, holds a grant from Wellcome Trust, holds shares in P1vital Ltd and has served as consultant, advisor or CME speaker for Allergan, Angelini, Compass pathways, MSD, Lundbeck (/Otsuka or/Takeda), Medscape, Minervra, P1vital Ltd, Pfizer, Servier, Shire, Sun Pharma. A.C.B. receives salaries from P1vital Ltd N.M.McG. and K.E.A.S. declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at (10.1038/s41398-019-0526-2).
References
- 1.Wulff K, Gatti S, Wettstein JG, Foster RG. Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in psychiatric and neurodegenerative disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:589. doi: 10.1038/nrn2868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Faedda GL, et al. Actigraph measures discriminate pediatric bipolar disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and typically-developing controls. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2016;57:706–716. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Van Meter AR, Burke C, Youngstrom EA, Faedda GL, Correll CU. The bipolar prodrome: meta-analysis of symptom prevalence prior to initial or recurrent mood episodes. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2016;55:543–555. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.04.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ng TH, et al. Sleep–wake disturbance in interepisode bipolar disorder and high-risk individuals: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2015;20:46–58. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Winsper C, et al. The sleep phenotype of borderline personality disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav Rev. 2017;73:48–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Saunders KEA, Bilderbeck AC, Price J, Goodwin GM. Distinguishing bipolar disorder from borderline personality disorder: A study of current clinical practice. Eur. Psychiatry. 2015;30:965–974. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Scammell TE, Arrigoni E, Lipton J. Neural circuitry of wakefulness and sleep. Neuron. 2017;93:747–765. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Foster RG, et al. Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in social jetlag and mental illness. Prog. Mol. Biol. Transl. Sci. 2013;119:325–346. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-396971-2.00011-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.McCarthy MJ, Welsh DK. Cellular circadian clocks in mood disorders. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2012;27:339–352. doi: 10.1177/0748730412456367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.McCarthy MJ, et al. Genetic and clinical factors predict lithium’s effects on PER2 gene expression rhythms in cells from bipolar disorder patients. Transl. Psychiatry. 2013;3:e318. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Melo MC, Abreu RL, Neto VBL, de Bruin PF, de Bruin VM. Chronotype and circadian rhythm in bipolar disorder: a systematic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 2017;34:46–58. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bradley AJ, et al. Sleep and circadian rhythm disturbance in bipolar disorder. Psychol. Med. 2017;47:1678–1689. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717000186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kennedy SH, Kutcher SP, Ralevski E, Brown GM. Nocturnal melatonin and 24-hour 6-sulphatoxymelatonin levels in various phases of bipolar affective disorder. Psychiatry Res. 1996;63:219–222. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(96)02910-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Nurnberger JI, et al. Melatonin suppression by light in euthymic bipolar and unipolar patients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 2000;57:572–579. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.6.572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Robillard R, et al. Sleep-wake cycle and melatonin rhythms in adolescents and young adults with mood disorders: comparison of unipolar and bipolar phenotypes. Eur. Psychiatry. 2013;28:412–416. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2013.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Jones SH, Hare DJ, Evershed K. Actigraphic assessment of circadian activity and sleep patterns in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2005;7:176–186. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00187.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bullock B, Murray G. Reduced amplitude of the 24 h activity rhythm: a biomarker of vulnerability to bipolar disorder? Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2014;2:86–96. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Lyall LM, et al. Association of disrupted circadian rhythmicity with mood disorders, subjective wellbeing, and cognitive function: a cross-sectional study of 91 105 participants from the UK Biobank. Psychiatry. 2018;5:507–514. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30139-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rock P, Goodwin G, Harmer C, Wulff K. Daily rest-activity patterns in the bipolar phenotype: a controlled actigraphy study. Chronobiol. Int. 2014;31:290–296. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2013.843542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.McGowan NM, Coogan AC. Sleep and circadian rhythm function and trait impulsivity: an actigraphy study. Psychiatry Res. 2018;268:251–256. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.07.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Dagan Y, Sela H, Omer H, Hallis D, Dar R. High prevalence of personality disorders among circadian rhythm sleep disorders (CRSD) patients. J. Psychosom. Res. 1996;41:357–363. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(96)00165-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Verkes RJ, Kerkhof GA, Beld E, Hengeveld MW, van Kernpen GM. circadian activity rhythms and platelet serotonergic measures in patients with recurrent suicidal behaviour. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1996;93:27–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1996.tb10615.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Carr O, et al. Desynchronization of diurnal rhythms in bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder. Transl. Psychiatry. 2018;8:79. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0125-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Roenneberg T, Merrow M. The circadian clock and human health. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:R432–R443. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Carr O, et al. Variability in phase and amplitude of diurnal rhythms is related to variation of mood in bipolar and borderline personality disorder. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:1649. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-19888-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ancoli-Israel S, et al. The role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep. 2003;26:342–392. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.3.342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Tsanas A, et al. Daily longitudinal self-monitoring of mood variability in bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder. J. Affect Disord. 2016;205:225–233. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kantermann T, Juda M, Merrow M, Roenneberg T. The human circadian clock’s seasonal adjustment is disrupted by daylight saving time. Curr. Biol. 2017;17:1996–2000. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lahti TA, Leppämäki S, Lönnqvist J, Partonen T. Transitions into and out of daylight saving time compromise sleep and the rest-activity cycles. BMC Physiol. 2008;8:3. doi: 10.1186/1472-6793-8-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Loranger AW, et al. The international personalitydisorder examination: The World Health Organization/Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration international pilot study of personality disorders. Arch. Gen. Psych. 1994;51:215–224. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950030051005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Altman EG, Hedeker D, Peterson JL, Davis JM. The Altman self-rating mania scale. Biol. Psychiatry. 1997;42:948–955. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00548-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rush AJ, et al. The 16-item quick inventory of depressive symptomatology (QIDS), clinician rating (QIDS-C), and self-report (QIDS-SR): a psychometric evaluation in patients with chronic major depression. Biol. Psychiatry. 2003;54:573–583. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01866-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.van Hees VT, et al. Separating movement and gravity components in an acceleration signal and implications for the assessment of human daily physical activity. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e61691. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Van Someren EJ, et al. Bright light therapy: improved sensitivity to its effects on rest-activity rhythms in Alzheimer patients by application of nonparametric methods. Chronobiol. Int. 1999;16:505–518. doi: 10.3109/07420529908998724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Krauchi K, Deboer T. The interrelationship between sleep regulation and thermoregulation. Front. Biosci. 2010;15:604–625. doi: 10.2741/3636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bonmati-Carrion MA, et al. Circadian phase asessment by ambulatory monitoring in humans: Correlation with dim light melatonin onset. Chronobiol. Int. 2014;31:37–51. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2013.820740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Fleischer M, Schäfer M, Coogan A, Häßler F, Thome J. Sleep disturbances and circadian CLOCK genes in borderline personality disorder. J. Neural Transm. 2012;119:1105–1110. doi: 10.1007/s00702-012-0860-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bromundt V, et al. Circadian sleep-wake cycles, well-being, and light therapy in borderline personality disorder. J. Personal. Disord. 2013;27:680–696. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2012_26_057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Huỳnh C, Guilé JM, Breton JJ, Godbout R. Sleep-wake patterns of adolescents with borderline personality disorder and bipolar disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2016;47:202–214. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0557-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Borbély AA, Daan S, Wirz‐Justice A, Deboer T. The two‐process model of sleep regulation: a reappraisal. J. Sleep Res. 2016;25:131–143. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Li JZ, et al. Circadian patterns of gene expression in the human brain and disruption in major depressive disorder. PNAS. 2013;110:9950–9955. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305814110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Selvi Y, et al. Chronotype differences in suicidal behavior and impulsivity among suicide attempters. Chronobiol. Int. 2011;28:170–175. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2010.535938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Antypa N, et al. Associations between chronotypes and psychological vulnerability factors of depression. Chronobiol. Int. 2017;34:1125–1135. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2017.1345932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Prasko J, et al. Augmentation of antidepressants with bright light therapy in patients with comorbid depression and borderline personality disorder. Biomed. Pap. Med Fac. Univ. Palacky Olomouc Czech Repub. 2010;154:355–361. doi: 10.5507/bp.2010.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Fargason RE, et al. Correcting delayed circadian phase with bright light therapy predicts improvement in ADHD symptoms: A pilot study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2017;91:105–110. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Henriksen TE, et al. Blue‐blocking glasses as additive treatment for mania: a randomized placebo‐controlled trial. Bipolar Disord. 2016;18:221–232. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Verkooijen S, et al. An actigraphy study investigating sleep in bipolar I patients, unaffected siblings and controls. J. Affect Disord. 2017;208:248–254. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Geoffroy PA, et al. Sleep in patients with remitted bipolar disorders: a meta‐analysis of actigraphy studies. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2015;131:89–99. doi: 10.1111/acps.12367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Scott J, et al. Activation in bipolar disorders: a systematic review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74:189–196. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.De Crescenzo F, Economou A, Sharpley AL, Gormez A, Quested DJ. Actigraphic features of bipolar disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2017;33:58–69. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Faurholt-Jepsen M, et al. Differences in psychomotor activity in patients suffering from unipolar and bipolar affective disorder in the remitted or mild/moderate depressive state. J. Affect Disord. 2012;141:457–463. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Steinan MK, et al. Delayed sleep phase: An important circadian subtype of sleep disturbance in bipolar disorders. J. Affect Disord. 2016;191:156–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Moon JH, et al. Advanced circadian phase in mania and delayed circadian phase in mixed mania and depression returned to normal after treatment of bipolar disorder. EBioMedicine. 2016;11:285–295. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2016.08.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Benard V, et al. Sleep and circadian rhythms as possible trait markers of suicide attempt in bipolar disorders: An actigraphy study. J. Affect Disord. 2019;244:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.09.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.