Abstract
Presynaptic short-term plasticity (STP) dynamically modulates synaptic strength in a reversible manner on a timescale of milliseconds to minutes. For low basal vesicular release probability (prob0), four components of enhancement, F1 and F2 facilitation, augmentation (A), and potentiation (P), increase synaptic strength during repetitive nerve activity. For release rates that exceed the rate of replenishment of the readily releasable pool (RRP) of synaptic vesicles, depression of synaptic strength, observed as a rundown of postsynaptic potential amplitudes, can also develop. To understand the relationship between enhancement and depression at the frog (Rana pipiens) neuromuscular synapse, data obtained over a wide range of prob0 using patterned stimulation are analyzed with a hybrid model to reveal the components of STP. We find that F1, F2, A, P, and depletion of the RRP all contribute to STP during repetitive nerve activity at low prob0. As prob0 is increased by raising Cao2+ (extracellular Ca2+), specific components of enhancement no longer contribute, with first P, then A, and then F2 becoming undetectable, even though F1 continues to enhance release. For levels of prob0 that lead to appreciable depression, only F1 and depletion of the RRP contribute to STP during rundown, and for low stimulation rates, F2 can also contribute. These observations place prob0-dependent limitations on which components of enhancement contribute to STP and suggest some fundamental mechanistic differences among the components. The presented model can serve as a tool to readily characterize the components of STP over wide ranges of prob0.
Introduction
Chemical synapses are major sites of information transfer between neurons and from neurons to effectors, such as muscle cells. A characteristic property of such synapses is that the amount of transmitter released from nerve terminals by each action potential can change dramatically during a train of impulses (Cowan et al., 2001; Atwood and Karunanithi, 2002; Zucker and Regehr, 2002; Neher and Sakaba, 2008). These changes, referred to as short-term synaptic plasticity (STP), have dramatic reversible effects on synaptic strength (Liley and North, 1953; Zengel et al., 1980; Magleby and Zengel, 1982; Dittman et al., 2000; Wesseling and Lo, 2002; Abbott and Regehr, 2004; Pan and Zucker, 2009; Kandaswamy et al., 2010; Müller et al., 2010).
Components of STP that increase transmitter release (enhancement) include first (F1) and second (F2) components of facilitation with time constants of 50 and 300 ms (Mallart and Martin, 1967; Magleby, 1973a; Zengel and Magleby, 1982; Goda and Stevens, 1994; Bennett et al., 2007), augmentation (A) with a time constant of 4–7 s (Magleby and Zengel, 1976a; Zengel and Magleby, 1982; Stevens and Wesseling, 1999), and potentiation (P) with a time constant of tens of seconds to minutes (Liley and North, 1953; Magleby, 1973b). For low basal vesicular release probability, prob0, all four enhancement processes, F1, F2, A, and P, make major contributions to STP during repetitive nerve activity (Magleby and Zengel, 1982). In contrast, at normal prob0, depression, typically indicated by a rundown of postsynaptic potential amplitudes during a train of impulses, can develop depending on the specific synapse and the stimulation rate (Liley and North, 1953; Mallart and Martin, 1968; Magleby, 1973b; Wu and Betz, 1998; Dittman et al., 2000; Wesseling and Lo, 2002; Stevens and Williams, 2007). The contributions of F1, F2, A, and P to STP during depression are less clear, as previous studies suggest that some of the components of enhancement may no longer contribute for normal prob0 (Dittman et al., 2000; Kandaswamy et al., 2010; Müller et al., 2010).
To explore this question, we examine the interaction of the components of enhancement with depression over a wide range of prob0 to change the level of depression. Experiments are performed on the neuromuscular synapse where the components of enhancement and depression were initially characterized (Liley and North, 1953; Takeuchi, 1958; Mallart and Martin, 1967; Magleby and Zengel, 1976a, 1982) and where depletion and recovery of synaptic vesicles is well described (Richards et al., 2003; Rizzoli and Betz, 2005). To reveal the components of STP during repetitive stimulation, a quantitative model that accounts for transmitter release at low quantal content (Magleby and Zengel, 1982) is expanded to incorporate depletion and replenishment of synaptic vesicles, and patterned stimulation is used to facilitate identification of the various components. We find that the specific components of enhancement contributing to STP during repetitive stimulation depend on prob0, with first P, then A, and then F2 no longer contributing as prob0 is increased from low to normal by increasing extracellular Ca2+ (Cao2+), whereas F1 always contributes.
Materials and Methods
Animals, solutions, and surface recording of end plate potentials
End plate potential (EPP) amplitude is used to track STP, as under the conditions of our experiments EPP amplitude is proportional to the number of synaptic vesicles releasing their transmitter after each nerve impulse (Zengel and Sosa, 1994; Zengel and Magleby, 1981). EPP amplitudes were recorded from the sartorius nerve–muscle preparation of northern grass frogs (Rana pipiens) of either sex with a surface electrode, as described previously (Magleby, 1973a). Animal protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Miami Miller School of Medicine and are in accordance with National Research Council Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. The preparation was bathed in modified Ringer's solution containing the following (in mm): 115 NaCl, 2 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 5 HEPES, 5 glucose, and 0.03 choline chloride. pH was adjusted to 7.3. Experiments were performed within the physiological temperature range for frogs (18–21°C), as temperature alters STP (Zengel et al., 1980; Klyachko and Stevens, 2006). The extracellular concentration of Ca2+ was reduced or raised, as indicated, to decrease or increase prob0, respectively. Tubocurare was added at concentrations of ∼2–5 μm to reduce EPP amplitudes well below threshold for the generation of action potentials in the muscle fibers.
A surface electrode was used to record the summed response of EPPs from a large number of end plates, each with thousands of active zones, thereby greatly reducing the measured stochastic variation in response. To further reduce variation, EPP amplitudes from three to five trains obtained from the same end plate region of a single muscle were averaged. Such surface recording gives a good measure of average intracellular response (Magleby, 1973a). Under the conditions of our experiments, where EPP amplitudes are typically a few millivolts or less because of low quantal content or the presence of curare, nonlinear summation of EPP amplitudes (McLachlan and Martin, 1981) is minimal and quantal size (miniature EPP amplitude) and postsynaptic sensitivity remain constant during a stimulation train (Magleby and Pallotta, 1981; Zengel and Sosa, 1994). Hence, observed changes in EPP amplitudes (synaptic strength) and STP in our experiments are presynaptic in origin, arising from a change in the number of vesicles whose contents are released by each nerve impulse. Examples of surface recorded EPPs have been presented previously (Magleby, 1973a; Magleby and Zengel, 1976a). The basal (control) EPP amplitude was obtained from the average of five EPPs delivered one every 5 or 10 s before the train. Rest periods between trains of impulses were typically 6 min.
Estimating the contributions of F1, F2, A, P, and depression to STP
A hybrid model was used to quantify the contributions of F1, F2, A, and P during repetitive stimulation. The model is an extension of the quantal hypothesis proposed by del Castillo and Katz (1954) that often serves as a starting point for more extensive models of transmitter release (Varela et al., 1997; Wu and Borst, 1999; Dittman et al., 2000; Zucker and Regehr, 2002; Trommershäuser et al., 2003; Kalkstein and Magleby, 2004; Pan and Zucker, 2009; Kandaswamy et al., 2010) as follows:
where EPPt is the number of quanta (vesicles) released by a nerve impulse to generate an EPP at time t during a train, RRPt is the number of vesicles in the readily releasable pool (RRP) available for release at time t, and probt is the vesicular release probability given by the fraction of vesicles in the RRP that are released by a nerve impulse at time t. EPP0, prob0, and RRP0 indicate the values of these parameters in the fully rested state at time 0 before the start of the train. At the frog neuromuscular synapse, RRP0 is ∼10,000 vesicles (Rizzoli and Betz, 2005), and a single action potential in the rested state for conditions of normal quantal content (2 mm Ca2+) releases ∼200 vesicles from the RRP (Heuser et al., 1979; Van der Kloot and Molgó, 1994), giving a prob0 of ∼0.02. Vesicles released from the RRP are assumed to be replenished from a recycling pool (RP) with a content of ∼75,000 vesicles (Rizzoli and Betz, 2005). There is an additional relatively fixed reserve pool of ∼400,000 vesicles, which is not included in the considered models. With ∼300 active zones and ∼40 vesicles per active zone, ∼12,000 vesicles appear docked at each frog neuromuscular synapse, similar to the size of the RRP (Rizzoli and Betz, 2005).
Enhancement.
The model of Magleby and Zengel (1982) is based on the accumulation and decay of release enhancing factors for F1, F2, A, and P, and accounts for STP at low quantal content in the absence of apparent depression. Their model is expanded to include depletion of the RRP, as follows:
where EPPt/EPP0 is the ratio of the number of vesicles released by a nerve impulse at time t during the train divided by the number of vesicles released by a nerve impulse in the basal condition. Under the recording conditions of our experiments EPP amplitude is proportional to the number of quanta released (see above). EPPt/EPP0 thus gives a normalized measure of the changes in transmitter release during the train. F1t, F2t, At, and Pt are the magnitudes of the first and second components of facilitation, augmentation, and potentiation, respectively, at time t immediately before each nerve impulse. For Equation 2, enhancement arises from increases in F1, F2, A, and P, and depression from the loss of vesicles from the RRP, although depression may be more complex than simple depletion of synaptic vesicles (Zucker and Regehr, 2002; Sippy et al., 2003; Xu et al., 2007; Young and Neher, 2009; Neher, 2010).
The magnitude of each enhancement component is given by the fractional increase in transmitter release when all the other components are zero and there is no depression, D. For example, when only F1 is present, then its magnitude is given by the following:
with similar definitions for F2, A, and P.
The equations describing the buildup and decay of F1, F2, A, and P during and after repetitive stimulation under conditions of low quantal content have been presented previously (Magleby and Zengel, 1982; Zengel and Magleby, 1982). Briefly, F1, F2, A, and P arise from the underlying factors, F1*, F2*, A*, and P*. Each nerve impulse adds an increment of these factors, f1*, f2*, a*, and p*, during repetitive stimulation. The growth in the magnitude of A can increasingly accelerate during a train of impulses (Magleby and Zengel, 1976a) and has been modeled as an increase in the effective increments of a* added by each impulse during the train as follows:
where a*0 is the increment of A* added by the first impulse in the train (in the rested state), a* is the increment added for impulse number N during the train, and Z is a parameter with a value ≥1 that determines the rate of increase in a* with each successive impulse (Zengel and Magleby, 1982). If Z = 1, there is no increase in a*.
F1*, F2*, and A* decay exponentially between nerve impulses with characteristic time constants, τF1*, τF2*, and τA*. For example, the decay of F1* is given by the following:
where F1*t is the magnitude of F1* at time t during the train, and F1*t+Δt is the magnitude of F1* Δt after time t. The change in F1* during the train is given by the following:
where f1* is the increment of F1* added by each nerve impulse, kF1* is the rate constant (given by 1/τF1*) for removal (or deactivation) of F1* from its site of action, and J(t) is an impulse function with a value of 1 at the time of each nerve impulse and 0 at all other times. Equations 5 and 6 with appropriate substitutions for the different components also apply for F2*, A*, and P*, with the exception that the decay of P depends on the magnitude of P (Magleby and Zengel, 1975, 1982) as follows:
where τP0 is the time constant for the decay of P after the first impulse in the train, P(t) is the magnitude of P at time t during the train, and B is the parameter that sets the increase in τP as P increases during the train. Mechanisms that could increase the time constant of decay of potentiation have been considered previously (Magleby and Zengel, 1976c; Tank and Zucker, 1997; García-Chacón et al., 2006). Mean estimates of B in this paper were ∼23, which could lead to a 15% increase in τP by the end of the trains, depending on the magnitude of P.
The observed potentiation P is not directly proportional to the underlying factor P*, but appears to saturate, as follows:
where G sets the level of saturation (Magleby and Zengel, 1982).
Depression.
In addition to enhancement of transmitter release, reflected as an increase in the probability of release caused by increases in one or more of the components F1, F2, A, and P, repetitive stimulation can also lead to a depression of transmitter release. In the model we use to study STP, depression arises from a depletion of vesicles from the RRP during stimulation. The RRP is then replenished from a recycling pool (RP). The formulation used for depletion is similar to previous studies (Schikorski and Stevens, 1997, 2001; Wu and Betz, 1998; Richards et al., 2003; Kalkstein and Magleby, 2004; Rizzoli and Betz, 2005; Neher and Sakaba, 2008; Pan and Zucker, 2009; Kandaswamy et al., 2010). Depletion occurs because vesicles are released from the RRP faster than they are replaced as follows:
where d(RRPt)/dt is the rate of change in the number of vesicles in the RRP, JtEPP0(EPPt/EPP0) is the number of vesicles released by a nerve impulse at time t, where Jt equals 1 at the time of each nerve impulse and zero at all other times, EPP0 is the quantal content of the first EPP in the train, EPPt/EPP0 is given by Equation 2, RRP0 − RRPt is the number of vesicles in the RRP that are depleted, RPt/RP0 is the fractional fullness of the RP, and 1/τRRP is the rate constant for replenishment of the RRP from the RP. Movement of vesicles from the RRP to the RP is not included, as this process is slow (Murthy and Stevens, 1999) compared with the durations of the stimulation trains.
In a similar manner, the change in the number of vesicles in the RP with time, d(RP)/dt, is given by the following:
where 1/τRP is the rate constant for refilling the RP.
The predicted EPP amplitudes during a train are calculated from the numerical solution of Equations 2–8 for enhancement together with Equations 9 and 10 for synaptic vesicle turnover and depression. Estimates of the parameters in this study are those that give the least-squares error [i.e., the least sum of ((predicted − observed)/predicted)2) for each EPP amplitude during the train, determined using an iterative approach to search for the parameter values that minimized the error using a program written in the laboratory (EPP37S54)]. If the search routine reduced the magnitude of a component of enhancement or depression to negligible values, then the component no longer contributed to STP, becoming undetected. RRP0 was fixed at 10,000 synaptic vesicles (Rizzoli and Betz, 2005). For data obtained at low prob0, depletion of the RP was small so that the RP0 and τRP were poorly defined, with their values having little effect on the goodness of fit as long as RP0 > 20,000 and τRP > 10 s.
The key to revealing the faster components of STP during repetitive stimulation and the interaction between these components and depression was to apply step changes in stimulation rate throughout the train (Magleby, 1973a; Magleby and Zengel, 1982; Wesseling and Lo, 2002; Müller et al., 2010). Whereas application of natural stimulation patterns serves the same purpose while allowing the critical decoding of natural synaptic input (Dittman et al., 2000; Hermann et al., 2009; Kandaswamy et al., 2010), such irregular stimulation does not readily allow direct visual observation of the components and their time course, as is the case when step changes in stimulation rate are applied systematically.
Testing additional mechanisms.
The data and analysis presented in this paper are for replenishment of the RP from unspecified sources, as described by Equation 10. We also examined a model in which the number of vesicles available to refill the RP depends on the number of vesicles incorporated into the presynaptic membrane that have not yet been retrieved and are available for recycling, such that Equation 10 is replaced by the following:
We found that the ability to describe the data was essentially the same assuming either replenishment versus strict recycling for the examined stimulation patterns. As an additional test, the data were fitted with various models with three compartments for vesicle recycling instead of two. If a component of enhancement was found to be negligible with the two-compartment model, its contribution with the three-compartment models was also negligible (data not shown).
In addition to Equation 2, we also examined the ability of Equation 12 to describe STP. This equation is consistent with the idea that the mechanisms of the components of enhancement are fully independent of the other components:
We found that Equation 12 gave descriptions of STP almost identical with those of Equation 2.
We left n a free parameter in Equations 2 and 12 to maximize the possibility of finding the facilitation components, as there is typically a power relationship for facilitation (Zengel and Magleby, 1982; Zucker and Regehr, 2002). As will be shown in the results, estimates of n were ∼1.5 for low and intermediate prob0, and ∼0.8 for normal to elevated prob0, indicating possible cooperativity for lower prob0 and possible saturation for higher prob0. Nevertheless, STP could be described about equally well using a linear relationship by setting n = 1.0, consistent with previous observations of difficulty in distinguishing the power for facilitation models (Zengel and Magleby, 1982; Dittman et al., 2000; Kandaswamy et al., 2010).
Kandaswamy et al. (2010) described F1 and F2 and A with a saturation formulation such that for F1 (in our terminology) is given as follows:
where F1 is the observed facilitation, F1* is an exponentially decaying factor that gives rise to F1, and η is a saturation factor that relates F1* to F1. The same form of saturation applied to F2 and A in their study. Kandaswamy et al. (2010) set θ = 1.0, as higher values led to no significant improvement, and found η = 1.2 for F1* and F2* and 0.59 for A. To test whether their formulation for saturating facilitation could be applied to the neuromuscular synapse, we substituted Equation 13 for F1 and F2 with θ = 1.0 into Equation 12, and set n = 1.0 in Equation 12, and fit the data. Estimated values of η in Equation 13 ranged from 0.02 to 0.13, consistent with limited saturation at the neuromuscular synapse (see above). The description of STP with the Kandaswamy et al. (2010) formulation for saturation of facilitation was little changed from the description with our formulation of facilitation. We did not examine a saturating A because in our formulation of STP the increment of A* added by each impulse was found to increase during the trains (Eq. 4), consistent with previous studies (Zengel and Magleby, 1982). P in our formulation was saturating (Eq. 8), also consistent with previous studies (Magleby and Zengel, 1982).
Results
STP is highly dependent on prob0
To examine the relationship between the components of enhancement and depression, STP was studied over a wide range of prob0, obtained by changing external Ca2+. Prob0 is the probability that a synaptic vesicle in the RRP releases its contents into the synaptic cleft after a nerve impulse is delivered under basal (resting) conditions when the components of STP are at their baseline values (i.e., the probability of vesicular release for the first impulse in a train of impulses). The nerve leading to the presynaptic nerve endings was stimulated at 33/s, with a dropped or added impulse every 20 stimuli to test for facilitation. Figures 1B, 2B, and 3B, which plot normalized EPP amplitudes during the trains, show that STP is profoundly different for low prob0 (Fig. 1, 0.2 mm Ca2+), intermediate prob0 (Fig. 2, 1.0 mm Ca2+), and normal prob0 (Fig. 3, 2.0 mm Ca2+).
During the 12 s train of patterned stimulation at low prob0, EPP mean amplitudes increased 24-fold above the first (control) EPP in the train (Fig. 1B), reflecting the buildup of F1, F2, A, and P during the train (Magleby and Zengel, 1982). At the low prob0 used for this experiment, the stepwise increase in EPP amplitudes after each added impulse and rapid decay back to the mean response (Fig. 1A) mainly reflects F1 and F2 facilitation added by the extra impulse (Magleby, 1973a; Magleby and Zengel, 1982; Zengel and Magleby, 1982). Similarly, the step decrease in EPP amplitudes after each dropped impulse and return to the mean response mainly reflects the absence of the increments of F1 and F2 that would have been added by the dropped impulse. It is the faster time constants of decay of F1 (∼50 ms) and F2 (∼300 ms) compared with the slower time constants of decay of A (∼6 s) and P (tens of seconds to minutes) and the larger increments of F1 (∼0.8) and F2 (∼0.15) added by each impulse when compared with the smaller increments of A (∼0.005) and P (∼0.01) that enables the detection of F1 and F2 with dropped and added impulses when these components are superimposed on A and P during repetitive stimulation at low prob0 (Magleby, 1973a; Magleby and Zengel, 1982).
In contrast to the 24-fold increase in mean EPP amplitude at low prob0 (Fig. 1B), at intermediate release probability mean EPP amplitudes increase 3.6-fold during the train (Fig. 2B), and at normal release probability EPP amplitudes increase 1.5-fold during the first few impulses and then run down to 30% of the control level by the end of the 12 s train (Fig. 3B). Despite the marked differences in the mean responses during the trains, two consistent features are present at all release probabilities: EPP amplitudes increase rapidly at the very start of the trains and there are step changes in EPP amplitudes with rapid return after the dropped and added impulses (Figs. 1A,B, 2A,B, 3A,B). At low prob0, these rapid changes in STP arise predominantly from F1 and F2, as discussed above. The presence of similar features at intermediate and normal prob0 suggests that F1 and possibly also F2 also contribute to STP at intermediate and normal prob0. In the following sections, the contributions of F1, F2, A, and P, and the depletion of the RRP to STP during repetitive stimulation are determined by fitting the data with the model presented in Materials and Methods. Dissecting out the components of STP during repetitive stimulation requires a model because of the complex relationship between the various components and transmitter release (see Materials and Methods).
F1, F2, A, P, and partial depletion of the RRP all contribute to STP at low prob0
Estimates of F1, F2, A, P, RRP, and RP during the train at low prob0 are shown in Figure 1E–G. F1 and F2 rapidly increase at the start of the train to near steady-state levels after the first 5 and 30 impulses, respectively, and then undergo step decreases and increases (with rapid return) from these levels after each dropped and added impulse during the train (Fig. 1F). The components of A and P increase throughout the train (Fig. 1G), and the RRP and RP decrease during the train, becoming depleted by 37 and 23%, respectively, because of depletion of vesicles from each pool (Fig. 1E). The product of RRPt and probt (Eq. 1) predicts the observed STP during the train (Fig. 1A,B,D). Hence, F1, F2, A, P, and partial depletion of the RRP all contribute to STP at low quantal content. The partial depletion of the RRP did not give rise to observable depression during the train because the increases in F1 and F2 at the start of the train and the continuing increase in A and P during the train more than compensated for the decreasing RRP during the train. Thus, even though obvious depression is not observed during repetitive stimulation at low prob0, partial depletion of the RRP contributes to STP by making the transmitter release less than what would be expected from enhancement processes alone. Mean parameters for three experiments like that in Figure 1 are presented in Table 1.
Table 1.
Parameters and datab | Prob0a |
||
---|---|---|---|
Low (0.0002–0.0009) | Intermediate (0.0011–0.0095) | Normal to elevated (0.017–0.043) | |
EEP0 | 5.5 ± 2.1 | 41 ± 15 | 270 ± 31 |
f1 * | 0.74 ± 0.34 | 0.79 ± 0.11 | 1.15 ± 0.25 |
τF1* (ms) | 39 ± 5 | 40 ± 3 | 47 ± 4 |
f2* | 0.13 ± 0.04 | 0.11 ± 0.03 | 0.0 |
τF2* (ms) | 275 ± 36 | 190 ± 57 | NAc |
n | 1.5 ± 0.1 | 1.4 ± 0.1 | 0.81 ± 0.08 |
a*0 | 0.0037 ± 0.0017 | 0.0021 ± 0.0008 | 0.0 |
τA* (s) | 6.0 ± 1.2 | 4.9 ± 1.4 | NA |
Z | 1.0045 ± 0.0048 | 1.0024 ± 0.0010 | NA |
A(400)b | 2.27 ± 1.21 | 0.67 ± 0.30 | 0.0 |
p* | 0.012 ± 0.003 | 0.004 ± 0.002 | 0.0 |
τP0 (s) | 20 ± 1 | 38 ± 14 | NA |
B | 24.6 ± 13.6 | 21.5 ± 9.3 | NA |
G | 4.8 ± 1.7 | 1.76 ± 0.45 | NA |
P(400)b | 1.46 ± 0.63 | 0.35 ± 0.14 | 0.0 |
RRP0d | 10,000 | 10,000 | 10,000 |
τRRP (s) | 3.2 ± 1.2 | 2.8 ± 1.8 | 1.1 ± 0.1 |
RP0 | 92,800 ± 53,900 | 36,800 ± 8,000 | 34,800 ± 2,100 |
τRP (s) | 236 ± 142 | 28 ± 15 | 17 ± 2 |
aRange of prob0 grouped into low, intermediate, and normal to elevated classes obtained with 0.14–0.3, 0.2–1.0, and 2.0–4.0 mm Ca2+o, respectively, for three, six, and eight different nerve–muscle preparations. Prob0, EPP0, and other parameters were obtained by fitting with Equations 2–10 for data obtained with the stimulation pattern used for Figures 1–3. Parameters are defined in Materials and Methods where the equations are presented. Values are mean ± SEM.
bA(400) and P(400) are the magnitudes of the observed augmentation and potentiation at 400 impulses of stimulation.
cNA indicates not applicable as the component was not detected.
dRRP0 was fixed at 10,000 vesicles (Rizzoli and Betz, 2005). One atypical experiment at elevated prob0 was excluded from the table because it had a P(400) = 0.31 compared with 0.0 for the other seven experiments of the same type. This excluded experiment is included in Figure 8.
Figure 1C shows why a partial depletion of the RRP develops during repetitive stimulation at low prob0 even though prob0 in this experiment is <1% of prob0 at normal release probability. Although the first EPP in the train at low prob0 releases only 1.5 vesicles on average, after 400 impulses the release per impulse has increased to >36 vesicles, so that the cumulative release during the train sums to ∼9000 vesicles, which is sufficient to partially deplete the RRP even though vesicles are being mobilized from the RP to the RRP.
The model used to describe STP in the present study is the same as the one used by Magleby and Zengel (1982) to describe STP at low release probability, but with the added feature that depletion of the RRP can occur. Magleby and Zengel (1982) did not include depletion in their studies, assuming that there would be little depletion at the low quantal contents used in their study. We fit the two sets of data in the study by Magleby and Zengel (1982), their Figure 6, with the expanded model to allow possible depletion and found a small (8%) depletion of the RRP at the end of their 200 impulse trains and also a small increase in enhancement to compensate for the depletion (results not shown), consistent with both the assumption in Magleby and Zengel (1982) of limited depletion and the findings in Figure 1 that a small partial depletion of the RRP contributes to STP at low prob0.
Components of STP during repetitive stimulation at intermediate prob0
For the experiment shown in Figure 2 at intermediate prob0, F1, F2, A, P, and depletion of the RRP contribute to STP (Fig. 2E–G), and the product of the RRPt and probt predicts the observed STP during the train (Fig. 2A,B,D). The 53% depletion of the RRP and 25% depletion of the RP during the train at intermediate prob0 (Fig. 2E) is greater than at low release prob0 (Fig. 1E), and the buildup of A and P during the train is considerably less at intermediate prob0 (Fig. 2G, note change in scale) than at low prob0 (Fig. 1G). It is the increased depletion of the RRP and the decreased buildup of A and P that limits the mean increase in EPP amplitudes during the train to 3.6-fold at intermediate prob0 (Fig. 2B) compared with 24-fold (Fig. 1B) at low prob0 (Fig. 1). No obvious depression is observed at intermediate prob0 (Fig. 2B) because the increases in F1 and F2 (Fig. 2F) and the smaller but continuing increase in A and P during the train (Fig. 2G) are still sufficient to compensate for the larger depletion of the RRP. Mean parameters for six experiments like that in Figure 2 are summarized in Table 1.
In experiments of this type, differences in prob0 among different frogs for the same 1.0 mm Cao2+ could lead to differences in the contribution of P during the train. As will be shown in a later section, when EPP0 for the first EPP in the train was <40 vesicles, as was the case for Figure 2, P was present during the train, and when it was >40 vesicles, P was typically absent. Hence, at intermediate prob0, F1, F2, A, and partial depletion of the RRP contribute to STP, and in some experiments at the lower range of intermediate prob0, P also contributes.
As indicated by the changes in EPP amplitudes in Figure 2, A and B, there is a period of time after a dropped impulse when fewer vesicles are released. This is the case because the dropped impulse itself does not release vesicles and the decreased facilitation after the dropped impulse leads to fewer vesicles released for the next seven to eight impulses. Inversely, more vesicles are released for a period of time when an impulse is added because the added impulse itself releases additional vesicles and because facilitation from the added impulse leads to more vesicles released for the next seven to eight impulses. These decreases and increases in vesicles released during the train lead to increases and decreases in the size of the RRP during the train (Fig. 2E), which then lead to small increases and decreases in mean EPP amplitudes during the train after facilitation approaches steady-state levels (marginally visible in Fig. 2A,B,D, but will be more apparent in later figures with different stimulation patterns). Such changes in EPP amplitudes after changes in stimulation rate have been exploited to estimate the fullness of the RRP (Wesseling and Lo, 2002).
F1 and marked depletion of the RRP can account for STP during rundown at normal prob0
At normal prob0 with associated rundown of EPP amplitudes during the train (Fig. 3B), only F1 and marked depletion of the RRP contribute to STP (Fig. 3E,F). F2, A, and P were not detected (Fig. 3F,G), so that the product of RRPt and probt, where probt included only F1, was sufficient to predict the observed STP during the train (Fig. 3A,B,D). The 85% depletion of the RRP and 60% depletion of the RP during the rundown at normal prob0 (Fig. 3E) can be compared with the 53 and 25% depletion at intermediate prob0 (Fig. 2E) and the 37 and 23% depletion at low prob0 (Fig. 1E). After the 1.5-fold increase in EPP amplitudes at the start of the train in Figure 3B, the rundown of the EPP amplitudes tracked the depletion of the RRP (Fig. 3E) because there was no buildup of F2, A, and P during the train to counter the decrease in the RRP. That F1 still contributes during the train can be seen by the response to the dropped and added impulses. Mean parameters for eight experiments like that in Figure 3 with marked depression are presented in Table 1 (listed under normal to elevated prob0). Consistent with the absence of F2 at normal prob0, Mallart and Martin (1968) did not observe F2 after five impulses of high frequency stimulation.
Imposing F2, A, and P at normal prob0 gives a response inconsistent with the data, indicating that these processes do not contribute to STP during rundown
The question arises as to whether F2, A, and P were not detected during rundown because they were missing or because the fitting program could not find them. If F2, A, and P were contributing to STP but not detected, then it should be possible to describe the data by imposing F2, A, and P and refitting. When this was done, the mean response during the rundown was approximated (Fig. 4B,D), but the predicted responses after dropped and added impulses were incorrect, being opposite to what was observed (Fig. 4A,B,D). Imposing F2, A, and P in various combinations and with various magnitudes also did not describe the data (data not shown), and when F2, A, and P were given initial values and then allowed to freely change during fitting, their values went to zero (Fig. 3F,G). Hence, F1 and depletion of the RRP were sufficient to account for STP during the rundown in Figure 3, with no evidence that F2, A, and P contribute.
As additional support for why F2, A, and P do not contribute to STP during the rundown, it is worthwhile to consider why imposing these components and refitting gave such a poor description of the response after dropped and added impulses. Imposing F2, A, and P led to large progressive increases in probt during the train. Therefore, to describe the rundown with this large imposed increase in probt, the program optimized the parameters to severely deplete the RRP (Fig. 4E) so that the product of probt × RRPt during the rundown would remain the same as in the absence of imposed F2, A, and P. The responses after dropped and added impulses were not predicted when the RRP was severely depleted in the presence of imposed F2, A, and P for the following reason. During the slow phase of the rundown, most of the vesicles released by each nerve impulse are replaced from the RP during the interval before the next impulse. When an impulse is dropped, the transmitter that would have been released by the dropped impulse is not released so that the RRP is increased by that amount of transmitter, compared with if the impulse were not dropped. For example, when F2, A, and P were imposed, the RRP was 94% depleted at 5.4 s so that the dropped impulse led to a large ∼16% fractional increase in the RRP (Fig. 4E, determined from numerical data). The consequence of such a large fractional increase in the RRP is that the predicted EPP amplitudes increase after the dropped impulse (Fig. 4A, red circles, ∼5.4 s) in contrast to the experimentally observed decrease (Fig. 4A, black circles), even though facilitation decreases because of the dropped impulse.
Conversely, with imposed F2, A, and P, the fractional decrease in the size of the RRP for an added impulse was much greater (∼18%) than without imposed F2, A, and P (∼3%), so that the predicted EPP amplitudes decreased (Fig. 4A, red circles, ∼6 s) in contrast to the experimentally observed increase (Fig. 4A, black circles), even though facilitation increased because of the added impulse. Hence, F2, A, and P do not contribute to STP during rundown at normal prob0, because if they did, the response to patterned stimulation would be opposite to what was observed.
Absence of F2, A, and P during rundown at normal prob0 using a stimulation pattern that amplifies the contributions of these components
As an additional test for F2, A, and P during rundown at normal prob0, we applied stimulation that amplifies the contributions of the components of enhancement by applying 40 impulses at 40/s alternating with 20 impulses at 20/s (Fig. 5). With such a pattern, the changes in F2, A, and P should be approximately twice as great at the higher than the lower stimulation rate. With this amplifying stimulation pattern, F1 and depletion of the RRP were still sufficient to account for STP during the train, as F2, A, and P were not detected (Fig. 5). As was the case for Figure 4, imposing F2, A, and P gave far worse descriptions of the data (Fig. 6).
For low stimulation rates at normal prob0, F2, in addition to F1 and depletion of the RRP, can also contribute to STP
Because of the large fractional release of transmitter at normal prob0, it is possible that the limited time for refilling release sites between nerve impulses might contribute in some way to the loss of F2, A, and P at normal prob0 for the high average stimulation rates (≥30/s) used in Figures 3 and 5. To explore this possibility, we applied stimulation patterns with a low average stimulation rate of 5/s to increase the average time between nerve impulses (Fig. 7A,B, black open circles). For these slow stimulation rates at normal prob0, F1 (blue) and now also F2 (green) contributed to enhancement during the rundown (Fig. 7F), compared with no contribution from F2 at higher stimulation rates (Figs. 3F, 5F). As was the case at higher stimulation rates for normal prob0, there was no contribution from A and P at the slower stimulation rates at normal prob0 (data not shown). Hence, for low stimulation rates at normal prob0, F1, F2, and depletion of the RRP are sufficient to account for STP (Fig. 7A,B,D,F).
As an additional test of the contribution of F2 to STP at the low stimulation rates during rundown, F2 was fixed to zero and the data refitted. The responses to the step changes in stimulation rate were no longer predicted (Fig. 7A, blue stars; compare G with B, D), consistent with F2 contributing to STP during rundown for slower stimulation rates at normal prob0. Transmitter output for low-frequency simulation is maintained by recycling rather than mobilization from the reserve pool (Richards et al., 2003), which might contribute to differences in F2 at different stimulation rates. This variable contribution of F2 to STP at normal prob0, depending on the stimulation rate, adds another complexity to STP.
Solving for EPP0 and RRP0
When solving for the components of STP, fixing the size of RPP0 to values in the literature gave a calculated value for EPP0 in Equation 1. For example, setting RRP0 to 10,000 vesicles (Rizzoli and Betz, 2005) for the experiments in Figures 1–3 for low, intermediate, and normal prob0 gave calculated values for EPP0 of 1.5, 38, and 176 vesicles, respectively (and see Table 1), consistent with expected experimental values for the levels of extracellular Ca2+ used in these experiments (Dodge and Rahamimoff, 1967; Magleby and Pallotta, 1981; Rizzoli and Betz, 2005). Prob0 for these same experiments would then be 0.00015, 0.0038, and 0.018, given by EPP0/RRP0. Alternatively, fixing the value of EPP0 in normal Ca2+ to the previously calculated value of 176 vesicles (found in Fig. 3), setting new starting parameters, and refitting with RRP0 as a free parameter gave estimates of RRP0 within 0.5% of 10,000 synaptic vesicles. Hence, given either EPP0 or RRP0, the unspecified parameter can be determined together with EPP amplitudes during a train expressed as the number of released vesicles. In the absence of a specified value for either EPP0 or RRP0, RRP0 can be set to 1.0 and the equations then give normalized EPP amplitude (normalized synaptic strength) with the fitted value of EPP0 giving prob0. In terms of the equations, it is the ratio of EPP0/RRP0 that determines STP by setting prob0. Prob0 then determines the depletion rate of the RRP and the components of enhancement that contribute during the train, as shown in the next section.
The components of enhancement contributing to STP during repetitive stimulation depend on prob0
Twenty-three additional experiments for a variety of stimulation patterns over a wide range of prob0, including three experiments with 4 mm Cao2+, were performed to estimate the components of STP during repetitive stimulation. The observations in these additional experiments are consistent with those presented in previous sections. RRP0 was fixed at 10,000 vesicles (Rizzoli and Betz, 2005) and EPP0 was determined by fitting the trains. Prob0 was then calculated from EPP0/RRP0. Figure 8 summarizes the results. The increments of F1 and F2 added by each impulse, f1* and f2*, are plotted against prob0 in Figures 8, A and B. The magnitudes of A and P and the percentage of the RRP remaining just before impulse 400 in the train are also plotted against prob0 in Figure 8C–E, respectively. EPP0 is also plotted in Figure 8D and applies to all parts of the figure.
Figure 8 shows that F1 is a major contributor to STP for all examined levels of prob0. In contrast, the contributions of F2, A, and, P decrease as prob0 increases, with the contributions of P, A, and F2 becoming negligible when prob0 is greater than ∼0.005, 0.01, and 0.015, respectively, or when EPP0 is greater than ∼50, 100, and 150 vesicles, respectively. Hence, as prob0 is increased, the three slower components of enhancement disappear in order of longer to shorter time constants of decay. (The information in Fig. 8 and Table 1 could be used to set parameters to estimate STP for a given prob0 using Eqs. 2–10.)
Whereas changes in Cao2+ were used to change synapses from highly facilitating to highly depressing in the experiments reported in this paper, the data have been presented in terms of prob0 for the following reasons: (1) consistent with the findings in Figures 1–8, synaptic strength (which is related to prob0) has been associated with determining whether synapses are facilitating or depressing (Mallart and Martin, 1968; Magleby, 1973b; Atwood and Karunanithi, 2002; Zucker and Regehr, 2002); (2) the STP response is not always strictly tied to Cao2+, as the STP responses for neuromuscular synapses from different frogs can differ somewhat for the same Cao2+; and (3) neuromuscular synapses can become more facilitating during extended durations of experimentation, even though Cao2+ is kept constant (Magleby and Zengel, 1976b). Variability in STP properties at the same Cao2+ for calyx synapses from different rats has previously been observed (Weis et al., 1999; Hennig et al., 2008).
Presenting the data in terms of prob0 rather than Cao2+ removed much of the synaptic variability and has the advantage that, in terms of the examined models, prob0 is a major determinant of the initial rate of depletion of the RRP, giving immediate insight into the expected type of response. Nevertheless, it was Cao2+ that was altered to change the type of synaptic response. Thus, the results in Figures 1–3 are the same for low (0.2 mm), intermediate (1.0 mm), and normal (2.0 mm) Ca2+ as for low (0.00015), intermediate (0.00384), and normal (0.0176) prob0. When the data from multiple experiments were grouped in terms of low (0.14–0.2 mm), intermediate (1.0 mm), and normal to high (2–4 mm) Cao2+, the parameter values and trends were very similar to those in Table 1 for low, intermediate, and normal to elevated prob0, but with more variability in some of the parameters (data not shown). Thus, our findings in terms of the components contributing to enhancement are essentially the same for changes in either Cao2+ or prob0. This is not surprising, as changing Cao2+ would change both Ca2+ entry and residual Ca2+, which has been associated with the components of enhancement (see Discussion).
Whereas Figure 8 describes the relationships between prob0 and the various enhancement components of STP, such a relationship is unlikely to apply to all synapses or for all experimental conditions, because prob0 can be uncoupled from STP. For example, Young and Neher (2009) have shown that overexpressing mutated synaptotagmin leaves intrinsic Ca2+ sensitivity intact while loosening the tight coupling between Ca2+ influx, release, and STP, most likely by interfering with the positioning of vesicles with respect to Ca2+ channels. Additional support for possible uncoupling comes from the observations of Sippy et al. (2003), who found that a depressing synapse can be converted into a facilitating synapse without changing basal synaptic strength by increasing the level of neuronal calcium sensor-1, effectively uncoupling prob0 from STP. Changes in Ca2+ currents or changes in increments in intracellular Ca2+ with each nerve impulse during trains (Borst and Sakmann, 1998; Narita et al., 2000; von Gersdorff and Borst, 2002; Xu et al., 2007) might also be expected to partially uncouple prob0 from STP.
Discussion
Summary of findings
Presynaptic STP encompasses a broad range of responses, from highly facilitating synapses to deeply depressing synapses (Cowan et al., 2001; Zucker and Regehr, 2002). Whereas all four components of enhancement, F1, F2, A, and P, contribute to STP at a highly facilitating neuromuscular synapse induced by low Cao2+ (Magleby and Zengel, 1982), the contributions of these components to STP is less clear for moderately facilitating to strongly depressing synapses. To address this question, we explored which components contribute to STP at the neuromuscular synapse as it was progressively transformed from a highly facilitating synapse to a deeply depressing synapse by changing prob0 over a 280-fold range, from very low (0.00015) to above normal (0.043), by changing Cao2+ in the range of 0.14–4.0 mm.
We found that F1, F2, A, P, and partial depletion of the RRP all contribute to STP during repetitive nerve activity at low prob0 (Figs. 1, 8). Interestingly, significant depletion of the RRP can occur at low prob0 because the robust buildup of enhancement during the trains greatly increases release (Fig. 1). As prob0 was increased by raising Cao2+, the contributions of the slower components of enhancement to STP progressively decreased to negligible levels, with first P, then A, and then F2 no longer contributing (Figs. 2–6, 8). For levels of prob0 that led to appreciable rundown during repetitive stimulation, only F1 and depletion of the RRP were needed to account for STP (Figs. 3–6, 8), with F2 also contributing during rundown for low stimulation rates ≤5/s (Fig. 7). Despite the essentially unconstrained parameters during the fitting, the properties of the detected components were consistent with those in the literature obtained by other methods, lending support to the approach and analysis.
Our expectation when starting this study was that the kinetic model combined with step changes in stimulation rate would reveal all four components of enhancement for the explored levels of depression, provided that the RRP contained excess synaptic vesicles. The presence of F1 throughout the rundowns indicates that excess vesicles were immediately available for release. Indirect support for excess vesicles also comes from the observation that the calculated RRP contained vesicles in large excess over the number released by any individual nerve impulse during the rundown. Thus, the absence of F2, A, and P during rundown at normal prob0 did not arise from a lack of vesicles available for release.
Imposing slower enhancement components for those experiments where the slower components were undetected and then refitting gave predictions of STP that were inconsistent with the experimental data (Figs. 4, 6). Hence, slow components that were not detected were not contributing to STP, because if they were, it would not have been possible to describe STP (Figs. 4, 6).
Limitations of analysis
The models described by Equations 1–12 were suitable to test for the components of STP during repetitive stimulation under the conditions of our experiments, and provided excellent descriptions of the dynamics of STP over wide ranges of prob0. Nevertheless, the equations are not meant as a comprehensive description of transmitter release, because the long-term recovery processes of some of the component parameters are not known and, consequently, not included. Related to this limitation, the models would not be able to predict the delayed onset of P after extensive stimulation at normal prob0 (Magleby, 1973b) that gives rise to posttetanic potentiation, because a recovery process for undetected P is not incorporated. Furthermore, the recycling of synaptic vesicles is more complicated than included in the model (Stevens and Wesseling, 1998; Wang and Kaczmarek, 1998; Wesseling and Lo, 2002; Richards et al., 2003; Sudhöf, 2004; Schweizer and Ryan, 2006; Denker and Rizzoli, 2010), and additional factors that can contribute to STP are not included (Zucker and Regehr, 2002; Neher and Sakaba, 2008; Todd et al., 2010).
The mechanism of the Ca2+-dependent release process is not part of the model, but replaced by an impulse function that places few restrictions on fundamental release mechanisms. Such mechanisms have been considered by others (Bennett et al., 2004; Neher and Sakaba, 2008; Pan and Zucker, 2009; Parnas and Parnas, 2010). To fully understand STP, it will be necessary to extend physical models based on underlying cellular physiology and structure, such as those by Bennett et al. (2007) and Pan and Zucker (2009) to account for all observed STP phenomena. Descriptive models such as Equations 2–13 are useful in that they provide dynamic descriptions of STP and provide a tool to study underlying physical processes.
Comparison with previous work
Our observations that F1, F2, A, and P components of enhancement all contribute to STP for a low prob0 highly facilitating amphibian synapse are consistent with highly facilitating invertebrate synapses (Kamiya and Zucker, 1994) and mammalian ganglion synapses (Zengel et al., 1980). In addition, our studies suggest that some depletion of the RRP is also likely to contribute to STP even at highly facilitating synapses (Fig. 1).
For intermediate prob0 leading to moderately facilitating synapses, release increases severalfold at the start of a train and then changes by only limited amounts during the train (Fig. 2). Our observations that F1, F2, and A (and also P for the lower range of intermediate prob0) together with a sequential two pool depletion and replacement of vesicles can account for STP at such moderately facilitating synapses (Fig. 2) are consistent with previous descriptions of STP at moderately facilitating excitatory hippocampal synapses (Kandaswamy et al., 2010) and also at moderately facilitating cutaneous pectorus neuromuscular synapses (Kalkstein and Magleby, 2004).
Our observations that F1 and depression, but not F2, A, and P, contribute to STP for highly depressing synapses with marked rundown because of normal or elevated prob0 (Figs. 3–6) are consistent with previous observations from depressing calyx synapses (Müller et al., 2010) and visual cortex layer 2/3 synapses (Varela et al., 1997), where STP arises mainly from a fast component of facilitation and one or two components of depression. Calyx synapses can also exhibit fast and slow phases of rundown (Hennig et al., 2008), as is the case for the neuromuscular synapse (Fig. 3). Hence, the model we present for STP has major features in common with STP at calyx and visual cortex layer 2/3 synapses. Nevertheless, depending on the stimulation pattern, STP at calyx may be simpler (Hermann et al., 2009) or more complicated (Hennig et al., 2008) than a fast component of facilitation and one or two components of depression.
Dittman et al. (2000) accounted for STP at three different CNS synapses ranging from highly facilitating to depressing with a general model that included depression and a fast component of facilitation, with Ca2+-dependent refilling of empty release sites. Whereas our findings are consistent with theirs for depressing synapses, their model had fewer components of enhancement for highly facilitating and moderately facilitating synapses than either our model or the model of Kandaswamy et al. (2010). This difference may reflect that their trains were generally brief so that the slower components of enhancement, if present, would have made only small contributions.
That F1, F2, A, and P components of miniature EPP frequency (Zengel and Magleby, 1981) are still observed in the presence of depression (Zengel and Sosa, 1994) suggests that the presumed Ca2+-dependent driving processes for the enhancement components (Kamiya and Zucker, 1994; Tank and Zucker, 1997; Suzuki et al., 2000; Zucker and Regehr, 2002; Kalkstein and Magleby, 2004; García-Chacón et al., 2006) are still present during marked rundown, but uncoupled from evoked release so that F2, A, and P of evoked release are not detected. The reason for this uncoupling during marked depression is not known but may be related to the fact that spontaneous and evoked release may have differences in location and/or molecular components (Washbourne et al., 2002; Zucker and Regehr, 2002; Maximov and Südhof, 2005; Wadel et al., 2007; Young and Neher, 2009; Yoshihara et al., 2010). Our observations that F1 contributes to STP under conditions where F2, A, and P are undetected (Figs. 3–8) supports previous studies suggesting that the mechanism for F1 has some differences from F2, A, and P (Delaney and Tank, 1994; Kamiya and Zucker, 1994; Neher and Sakaba, 2008).
Significance
Facilitating synapses typically have lower prob0 than depressing synapses (Zucker and Regehr, 2002) and Figures 1–8. Our systematic study characterizes yet another mechanistic difference. At low prob0, the four enhancement processes F1, F2, A, and P are robust, overcoming the partial depletion of the RRP, leading to a facilitating synapse. In contrast, as prob0 increases, the contributions of the three slower enhancement components F2, A, and P progressively decrease so there is little enhancement (except from F1) to compensate for the increased depletion of the RRP, leading to rundown. Thus, prob0-dependent enhancement adds additional complexity to STP that will need to be taken into account. On this basis, experimental procedures that alter prob0 might also be expected to alter F2, A, and P, even if they do not directly act on these components.
Footnotes
This work was supported in part by a grant from the Muscular Dystrophy Association (K.L.M.) and an American Heart Association fellowship (A.M.H.).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Abbott LF, Regehr WG. Synaptic computation. Nature. 2004;431:796–803. doi: 10.1038/nature03010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atwood HL, Karunanithi S. Diversification of synaptic strength: presynaptic elements. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:497–516. doi: 10.1038/nrn876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett MR, Farnell L, Gibson WG. The facilitated probability of quantal secretion within an array of calcium channels of an active zone at the amphibian neuromuscular junction. Biophys J. 2004;86:2674–2690. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(04)74323-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett MR, Farnell L, Gibson WG, Dickens P. Mechanisms of calcium sequestration during facilitation at active zones of an amphibian neuromuscular junction. J Theor Biol. 2007;247:230–241. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2007.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borst JG, Sakmann B. Facilitation of presynaptic calcium currents in the rat brainstem. J Physiol. 1998;513:149–155. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.149by.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan C, Südhof TC, Stevens CF, editors. Synapses. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins UP; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Delaney KR, Tank DW. A quantitative measurement of the dependence of short-term synaptic enhancement on presynaptic residual calcium. J Neurosci. 1994;14:5885–5902. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-10-05885.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- del Castillo J, Katz B. Quantal components of endplate potentials. J Physiol. 1954;257:560–573. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1954.sp005129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denker A, Rizzoli SO. Synaptic vesicles pools: an update. Front Synaptic Neurosci. 2010;2:135. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dittman JS, Kreitzer AC, Regehr WG. Interplay between facilitation, depression, and residual calcium at three presynaptic terminals. J Neurosci. 2000;20:1374–1385. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01374.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge FA, Jr, Rahamimoff R. Co-operative action of calcium ions in transmitter release at the neuromuscular junction. J Physiol. 1967;193:419–432. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- García-Chacón LE, Nguyen KT, David G, Barrett EF. Extrusion of Ca2+ from mouse motor terminal mitochondria via a Na+-Ca2+ exchanger increases post-tetanic evoked release. J Physiol. 2006;574:663–675. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.110841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goda Y, Stevens CF. Two components of transmitter release at a central synapse. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1994;91:12942–12946. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.26.12942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hennig MH, Postlethwaite M, Forsythe ID, Graham BP. Interactions between multiple sources of short-term plasticity during evoked and spontaneous activity at the rat calyx of Held. J Physiol. 2008;586:3129–3146. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.152124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hermann J, Grothe B, Klug A. Modeling short-term synaptic plasticity at the calyx of Held using in vivo-like stimulation patterns. J Neurophysiol. 2009;101:20–30. doi: 10.1152/jn.90243.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heuser JE, Reese TS, Dennis MJ, Jan Y, Jan L, Evans L. Synaptic vesicle exocytosis captured by quick freezing and correlated with quantal transmitter release. J Cell Biol. 1979;81:275–300. doi: 10.1083/jcb.81.2.275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalkstein JM, Magleby KL. Augmentation increases vesicle release probability in the presence of masking depression at the frog neuromuscular junction. J Neurosci. 2004;24:11391–11403. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2756-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamiya H, Zucker RS. Residual Ca2+ and short-term synaptic plasticity. Nature. 1994;371:603–606. doi: 10.1038/371603a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandaswamy U, Deng PY, Stevens CF, Klyachko VA. The role of presynaptic dynamics in processing of natural spike trains in hippocampal synapses. J Neurosci. 2010;30:15904–15914. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4050-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klyachko VA, Stevens CF. Temperature-dependent shift of balance among the components of short-term plasticity in hippocampal synapses. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6945–6957. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1382-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liley AW, North KA. An electrical investigation of effects of repetitive stimulation on mammalian neuromuscular junction. J Neurophysiol. 1953;16:509–527. doi: 10.1152/jn.1953.16.5.509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magleby KL. The effect of repetitive stimulation on facilitation of transmitter release at the frog neuromuscular junction. J Physiol. 1973a;234:327–352. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magleby KL. The effect of tetanic and post-tetanic potentiation on facilitation of transmitter release at the frog neuromuscular junction. J Physiol. 1973b;234:353–371. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magleby KL, Pallotta BS. A study of desensitization of acetylcholine receptors using nerve-released transmitter in the frog. J Physiol. 1981;316:225–250. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magleby KL, Zengel JE. A dual effect of repetitive stimulation on post-tetanic potentiation of transmitter release at the frog neuromuscular junction. J Physiol. 1975;245:163–182. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp010839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magleby KL, Zengel JE. Augmentation: a process that acts to increase transmitter release at the frog neuromuscular junction. J Physiol. 1976a;257:449–470. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magleby KL, Zengel JE. Long term changes in augmentation, potentiation, and depression of transmitter release as a function of repeated synaptic activity at the frog neuromuscular junction. J Physiol. 1976b;257:471–494. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magleby DL, Zengel JE. Stimulation-induced factors which affect augmentation and potentiation of transmitter release at the neuromuscular junction. J Physiol. 1976c;260:687–717. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magleby KL, Zengel JE. A quantitative description of stimulation-induced changes in transmitter release at the frog neuromuscular junction. J Gen Physiol. 1982;80:613–638. doi: 10.1085/jgp.80.4.613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mallart A, Martin AR. Two components of facilitation at the neuromuscular junction of the frog. J Physiol. 1967;193:679–694. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mallart A, Martin AR. The relation between quantum content and facilitation at the neuromuscular junction of the frog. J Physiol. 1968;196:593–604. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maximov A, Südhof TC. Autonomous function of synaptotagmin 1 in triggering synchronous release independent of asynchronous release. Neuron. 2005;48:547–554. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLachlan EM, Martin AR. Non-linear summation of end-plate potentials in the frog and mouse. J Physiol. 1981;311:307–324. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller M, Goutman JD, Kochubey O, Schneggenburger R. Interaction between facilitation and depression at a large CNS synapse reveals mechanisms of short-term plasticity. J Neurosci. 2010;30:2007–2016. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4378-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murthy VN, Stevens CF. Reversal of synaptic vesicle docking at central synapses. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:503–507. doi: 10.1038/9149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narita K, Akita T, Hachisuka J, Huang S, Ochi K, Kuba K. Functional coupling of Ca2+ channels to ryanodine receptors at presynaptic terminals. J Gen Physiol. 2000;115:519–532. doi: 10.1085/jgp.115.4.519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neher E. What is rate-limiting during sustained synaptic activity: vesicle supply or the availability of release sites. Front Synaptic Neurosci. 2010;2:144. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neher E, Sakaba T. Multiple roles of calcium ions in the regulation of neurotransmitter release. Neuron. 2008;59:861–872. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pan B, Zucker RS. A general model of synaptic transmission and short-term plasticity. Neuron. 2009;62:539–554. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parnas I, Parnas H. Control of neurotransmitter release: from Ca2+ to voltage dependent G-protein coupled receptors. Pflugers Arch. 2010;460:975–990. doi: 10.1007/s00424-010-0872-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards DA, Guatimosim C, Rizzoli SO, Betz WJ. Synaptic vesicle pools at the frog neuromuscular junction. Neuron. 2003;39:529–541. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00405-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzoli SO, Betz WJ. Synaptic vesicle pools. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:57–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn1583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schikorski T, Stevens CF. Quantitative ultrastructural analysis of hippocampal excitatory synapses. J Neurosci. 1997;17:5858–5867. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-15-05858.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schikorski T, Stevens CF. Morphological correlates of functionally defined synaptic vesicle populations. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:391–395. doi: 10.1038/86042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweizer FE, Ryan TA. The synaptic vesicle: cycle of exocytosis and endocytosis. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:298–304. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sippy T, Cruz-Martín A, Jeromin A, Schweizer FE. Acute changes in short-term plasticity at synapses with elevated levels of neuronal calcium sensor-1. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:1031–1038. doi: 10.1038/nn1117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens CF, Wesseling JF. Activity-dependent modulation of the rate at which synaptic vesicles become available to undergo exocytosis. Neuron. 1998;21:415–424. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80550-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens CF, Wesseling JF. Augmentation is a potentiation of the exocytotic process. Neuron. 1999;22:139–146. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80685-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens CF, Williams JH. Discharge of the readily releasable pool with action potentials at hippocampal synapses. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:3221–3229. doi: 10.1152/jn.00857.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sudhöf TC. The synaptic vesicle cycle. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:509–547. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki S, Osanai M, Murase M, Suzuki N, Ito K, Shirasaki T, Narita K, Ohnuma K, Kuba K, Kijima H. Ca2+ dynamics at the frog motor nerve terminal. Pflugers Arch. 2000;440:351–365. doi: 10.1007/s004240000278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeuchi A. The long-lasting depression in neuromuscular transmission of the frog. Jpn J Physiol. 1958;8:102–113. doi: 10.2170/jjphysiol.8.102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tank Y, Zucker RS. Mitochondrial involvement in post-tetanic potentiation of synaptic transmission. Neuron. 1997;18:483–491. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81248-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd KJ, Darabid H, Robitaille R. Perisynaptic glia discriminate patterns of motor nerve activity and influence plasticity at the neuromuscular junction. J Neurosci. 2010;30:11870–11882. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3165-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trommershäuser J, Schneggenburger R, Zippelius A, Neher E. Heterogeneous presynaptic release probabilities: functional relevance for short-term plasticity. Biophys J. 2003;84:1563–1579. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(03)74967-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Kloot W, Molgó J. Quantal acetylcholine release at the vertebrate neuromuscular junction. Physiol Rev. 1994;74:899–991. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1994.74.4.899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varela JA, Sen K, Gibson J, Fost J, Abbott LF, Nelson SB. A quantitative description of short-term plasticity at excitatory synapses in layer 2/3 of rat primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1997;17:7926–7940. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-20-07926.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Gersdorff H, Borst JG. Short-term plasticity at the calyx of Held. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:53–64. doi: 10.1038/nrn705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wadel K, Neher E, Sakaba T. The coupling between synaptic vesicles and Ca2+ channels determines fast neurotransmitter release. Neuron. 2007;53:563–575. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang LY, Kaczmarek LK. High-frequency firing helps replenish the readily releasable pool of synaptic vesicles. Nature. 1998;394:384–388. doi: 10.1038/28645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Washbourne P, Thompson PM, Carta M, Costa ET, Mathews JR, Lopez-Benditó G, Molnár Z, Becher MW, Valenzuela CF, Partridge LD, Wilson MC. Genetic ablation of the t-SNARE SNAP-25 distinguishes mechanisms of neuroexocytosis. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:19–26. doi: 10.1038/nn783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weis S, Schneggenburger R, Neher E. Properties of a model of Ca++-dependent vesicle pool dynamics and short term synaptic depression. Biophys J. 1999;77:2418–2429. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(99)77079-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wesseling JF, Lo DC. Limit on the role of activity in controlling the release-ready supply of synaptic vesicles. J Neurosci. 2002;22:9708–9720. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-22-09708.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu LG, Betz WJ. Kinetics of synaptic depression and vesicle recycling after tetanic stimulation of frog motor nerve terminals. Biophys J. 1998;74:3003–3009. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(98)78007-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu LG, Borst JG. The reduced release probability of releasable vesicles during recovery from short-term synaptic depression. Neuron. 1999;23:821–832. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)80039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu J, He L, Wu LG. Role of Ca2+ channels in short-term synaptic plasticity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17:352–359. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoshihara M, Guan Z, Littleton JT. Differential regulation of synchronous versus asynchronous neurotransmitter release by the C2 domains of synaptotagmin 1. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:14869–14874. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000606107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young SM, Jr, Neher E. Synaptotagmin has an essential function in synaptic vesicle positioning for synchronous release in addition to its role as a calcium sensor. Neuron. 2009;63:482–496. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zengel JE, Magleby KL. Changes in miniature endplate potential frequency during repetitive nerve stimulation in the presence of Ca2+, Ba2+, and Sr2+ at the frog neuromuscular junction. J Gen Physiol. 1981;77:503–529. doi: 10.1085/jgp.77.5.503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zengel JE, Magleby KL. Augmentation and facilitation of transmitter release. A quantitative description at the frog neuromuscular junction. J Gen Physiol. 1982;80:583–611. doi: 10.1085/jgp.80.4.583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zengel JE, Sosa MA. Changes in MEPP frequency during depression of evoked release at the frog neuromuscular junction. J Physiol. 1994;477:267–277. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1994.sp020189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zengel JE, Magleby KL, Horn JP, McAfee DA, Yarowsky PJ. Facilitation, augmentation, and potentiation of synaptic transmission at the superior cervical ganglion of the rabbit. J Gen Physiol. 1980;76:213–231. doi: 10.1085/jgp.76.2.213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zucker RS, Regehr WG. Short-term synaptic plasticity. Annu Rev Physiol. 2002;64:355–405. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.64.092501.114547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]