Abstract
In contrast to numerous studies of transcallosal communication in visual and somatosensory cortices, the functional properties of interhemispheric connections between auditory cortical fields have not been widely scrutinized. Therefore, the purpose of the present investigation was to measure the magnitude and type (inhibitory/excitatory) of modulatory properties of core auditory fields on contralateral primary auditory cortex (A1) activity. We combined single-unit neuronal recordings with reversible cooling deactivation techniques to measure variations in contralateral A1 response levels during A1, anterior auditory field (AAF), or simultaneous A1 and AAF neuronal discharge suppression epochs in cat auditory cortex. Cortical activity was evoked by presentation of pure tones, noise bursts, and frequency-modulated (FM) sweeps before, during, and after cortical deactivation periods. Comparisons of neuronal response changes before and during neuronal silencing revealed three major findings. First, deactivation of A1 and AAF-induced significant peak response reductions in contralateral A1 activity during simple (tonal) and complex (noise bursts and FM sweeps) acoustic exposure. Second, decreases in A1 neuronal activity appear to be in agreement with anatomical laminar termination patterns emanating from contralateral auditory cortex fields. Third, modulatory properties of core auditory areas lack hemispheric lateralization. These findings demonstrate that during periods of acoustic exposure, callosal projections emanating from core auditory areas modulate A1 neuronal activity via excitatory inputs.
Introduction
Considerable work has been conducted to identify anatomical and functional properties of interhemispheric connections in the visual system (Choudhury et al., 1965; Hubel and Wiesel, 1967; Innocenti, 1980; Silveira et al., 1989; Yinon, 1994; Abel et al., 2000; Rochefort et al., 2007; Schmidt et al., 2010; Genç et al., 2011, for review, see Glickstein and Berlucchi, 2008). Specifically, transcallosal projections connecting visual cortical fields have been implicated in hemifield fusion (Choudhury et al., 1965; Hubel and Wiesel, 1967; Payne, 1990), midline receptive field extension (Antonini et al., 1979, 1983, 1985; Marzi et al., 1982), binocular activation (Berlucchi and Rizzolatti, 1968; Lepore and Guillemot, 1982; Blakemore et al., 1983; Payne et al., 1984), and depth perception (Gardner and Cynader, 1987). Collectively, these studies have greatly advanced our understanding of how we are able to perceive the visual world as a single percept.
In auditory cortex, functional investigations of ipsilateral corticocortical communication have provided evidence of modulatory effects between core and higher order auditory cortical fields (Rauschecker et al., 1997; Carrasco and Lomber, 2009a, b, 2010). Despite the valuable information demonstrated in these studies, to date, the extent, type, and specificity of interhemispheric communication across auditory cortical fields remains vague (Mitani and Shimokouchi, 1985; Kitzes and Doherty, 1994). The conspicuous lack of functional research investigating transcallosal communication in the auditory system is contrasted by well accepted anatomical models of interhemispheric projections across auditory cortical regions (Diamond et al., 1968; Imig and Brugge, 1978; Code and Winer, 1985; Rüttgers et al., 1990; Rouiller et al., 1991; Morel et al., 1993; Lee and Winer, 2008a). Central to the present study are neuroanatomical tracing investigations showing that, in the cat, afferent transcallosal projections to primary auditory cortex (A1) emerge predominantly from contralateral A1 (>50%) and the anterior auditory field (AAF; 10 to <50%) (Lee and Winer, 2008a). Based on these neuroanatomical reports, the present study examines functional characteristics of interhemispheric projections between core auditory areas.
Founded upon models of common information processing across sensory systems (Olshausen and Field, 2004), reports of neuronal response strength decreases in primary cortical fields [visual (Payne et al., 1991); somatosensory (Clarey et al., 1996)] during deactivation of contralateral homologous regions, and known interhemispheric connections in the auditory system (Lee and Winer, 2008a), we hypothesized decreases in response strength of contralateral auditory cortex neurons during suppression of callosal afferent activity. The hypothesis was tested by examining the impact of individual and combined A1 and AAF reversible deactivation on contralateral A1 neuronal response activity in the cat. Statistical analyses revealed decreases in activity across A1 laminae during silencing of contralateral core auditory cortical fields. These findings demonstrate that during acoustic exposure, callosal projections emanating from core auditory areas regulate A1 neuronal responses via excitatory inputs.
Materials and Methods
Overview.
Neuronal responses to acoustic signals were measured in the right (two males and two females) and left (one male and two females) primary auditory cortices of seven adult (>6 M) domestic cats (Felis catus) (Fig. 1A). Procedures were conducted in accord with the National Research Council's Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research (2003), the Canadian Council on Animal Care's Guide to the Care and Use of Experimental Animals (Olfert et al., 1993) and were sanctioned by the University of Western Ontario Animal Use Subcommittee of the University Council on Animal Care. Detailed descriptions of methodological procedures have been published (Carrasco and Lomber, 2009b, 2011).
Cooling loop and head holder implantation procedures.
Approximately 2 weeks before electrophysiological recording, each animal was unilaterally implanted with A1 and AAF cryoloops over the right or left hemisphere (Lomber et al., 1999; Lomber, 1999). Probes of various sizes and shapes were fabricated by shaping loops of 23 gauge stainless steel hypodermic tubing (Lomber et al., 1999). Before surgery, cryoloops were sterilized with ethylene oxide gas. During the 24 h period before cryoloop implantation, cats were fasted and given dexamethasone (1.0 mg/kg, i.m.). Cannulation of the cephalic vein permitted administration of anesthetic and infusion of fluids (2.5% dextrose and half-strength lactated Ringer's solution). Sodium pentobarbital (∼25 mg/kg to effect) was administered intravenously to induce general anesthesia. Animals were then placed in a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments, model 1530) and prepared for surgery using procedures described previously (Lomber et al. 1999). A midline incision was made in the scalp and the temporalis muscles were detached medially and reflected laterally. Craniotomies were made over auditory cortex and the dura was incised and reflected to expose the cerebrum. Location of cortical fields implanted was determined based on anatomical markers (Reale and Imig, 1980; Imaizumi et al., 2004; Carrasco and Lomber, 2009b) (Fig. 1B). A1 cryoloops were ∼6 mm long and extended lengthwise across the middle ectosylvian gyrus, between the dorsal tips of the anterior and posterior ectosylvian sulci (approximately A4–A101; Reale and Imig 1980; Fig. 1). AAF (Knight 1977; Reale and Imig 1980; Phillips and Irvine 1982) cryoloops were ∼6 mm long and were located on the crown of the anterior suprasylvian gyrus between approximately A11 and A17. Appropriate size and shape cryoloops were selected based on cortical topography and field size during implantation procedures (Fig. 1B). In four animals, after cryoloop placement and before incision closure, cortical cooling was induced by pumping chilled methanol through the lumen of the cryoloops and thermal images were acquired to determine the extent of cortical temperature changes (FLIR; model SC325) (Fig. 1C). Copper/constantin micro-thermistors were attached to the union of cryoloops to monitor probe temperature.
Following cryoloop placement, the dura was repositioned or Gelfilm was placed over the exposed cerebrum. With exception of where the cooling tubes exited the skull, the previously removed bone piece was replaced. Skull screws and dental acrylic were applied to secure the cooling loop and bone pieces. Finally, a head holder was attached to the frontal bone using skull screws and dental acrylic. The head holder permitted the animal to be held in the stereotaxic frame during electrophysiological recording without use of ear bars, which allowed the auditory canal to receive unobstructed presentation of acoustic signals. Dermal incisions were closed with 3-0 silk sutures that were removed 7–10 d later. Buprenorphine analgesic (0.01 mg/kg, i.m.) was administered during the first 72 h after awakening. Decreasing doses of dexamethasone were administered over the next week, and fluids (2.5% dextrose and half-strength lactated Ringer's solution, 20 ml/kg, s.c.) were infused as needed. The cats also received systemic antibiotics for 1 week to guard against infection. In all cases, postsurgical recovery was uneventful.
Surgical preparation for electrophysiological recording.
On the day before the commencement of electrophysiological recordings, animals were anesthetized with ketamine (20 mg/kg, i.m.), the cephalic vein was cannulated with an indwelling catheter, and dexamethasone (1.0 mg/kg, i.v.) was administered to reduce the likelihood of edema during surgical procedures. Animals were fasted overnight. The following day, an initial dose of sodium pentobarbital (25 mg/kg, i.v) (Cheung et al., 2001) was used to induce a deep state of anesthesia. Dexamethasone (1.0 mg/kg, i.v.) and atropine (0.03 mg/kg, s.c.) were administered to reduce the risk of brain inflammation and respiratory secretions, respectively. The larynx was anesthetized (Cetacaine; Cetylite Laboratories) and the trachea was intubated. Supplemental doses of sodium pentobarbital were administered as needed to maintain a state of areflexia. Electrocardiogram and blood oxygenation were monitored. Body temperature was maintained at 37°C with a water-filled heating pad (Gaymar, model T/pump). Hydration was achieved via an infusion pump (2.5% dextrose/half-strength lactated Ringer's solution, 4 ml kg−1 h−1, i.v.). The animal was held in the stereotaxic frame with the head holder implanted in the procedure detailed above. A craniotomy was made over areas A1 and AAF, contralateral to the implanted hemisphere. The dura was resected, silicone oil was applied to the exposed tissue to prevent desiccation, and a digital picture was taken to maintain a record of microelectrode recording locations.
Stimulus generation and presentation.
Electrophysiological recordings were completed on a vibration-free table (Technical Manufacturing Corporation, model 63–500), inside an electrically shielded double-walled sound chamber. Acoustic signals were digitally generated with a 24-bit D/A converter at 156 kHz (Tucker-Davis Technologies, model RX6), and delivered in the free-field (Tucker-DavisTechnologies, model FF1) positioned 15 cm from the center of the head and contralateral to the recording hemisphere. Signals were calibrated with a 1/4 inch microphone (Brüel and Kjær, model 4939). Features of acoustic signals used in the present investigation have been described (Carrasco and Lomber, 2011). In brief, three types of acoustic signals were used. (1) Pure tones varying in frequency (250–64,000 Hz in 1/16 octave steps) and amplitude (0 to 75 db SPL in 5 dB steps) were presented in pseudorandom order (25 ms long, 5 ms rise and fall times, cosine squared gated) at a rate of 2 Hz. A1 neuronal responses to 2064 frequency-intensity combinations were used to generate peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) during each cooling condition. Frequency-intensity combinations were delivered once per cooling phase. (2) Twenty-five milliseconds long white noise bursts composed of frequencies ranging from 1–32 kHz (5 ms rise and fall times, cosine squared gated) were presented 600 times per cooling phase at a rate of 2 Hz. (3) Logarithmic frequency modulated (FM) sweeps starting at 1 kHz, 2 kHz, 4 kHz, 8 kHz, 16 kHz, or 32 kHz and moving either up or down from 1 to 5 octaves were presented. Regardless of spectral-frequency distance, each FM sweep was 250 ms in duration. Each range-direction combination was presented 50 times per cooling phase at 2 Hz. Apart from pure tones, signals were calibrated and delivered at 65 dB SPL.
Reversible cooling deactivation.
The cooling method to reversibly deactivate neural tissue is a potent and appropriate technique for examining cerebral interconnections and has a number of highly beneficial and practical features (Lomber, 1999). First, limited regions of the cerebral cortex can be selectively and reversibly deactivated in a controlled and reproducible way. Baseline and experimental measures can be made within minutes of each other (Lomber et al., 1996). Second, repeated cooling induces neither local nor distant degenerations that might compromise conclusions (Yang et al., 2006). Third, as the major effect of cooling is to block synaptic transmission, activity in fibers of passage is not compromised (Jasper et al., 1970; Bénita and Condé, 1972). Overall, the technique induces localized hypothermia in a restricted region of the brain. The locus of the deactivation is kept small by the constant perfusion of warm blood into, and around, the cooled region. The cooling disrupts calcium channel function in the presynaptic terminal and disrupts normal neurotransmitter release (for review, see Brooks, 1983).
Deactivation of A1 and/or AAF was achieved by pumping methanol through Teflon tubing connected to the cryoloops. The tubing was immersed in a methanol dry ice bath that resulted in subzero temperatures. After passing through the cryoloop, the cooled methanol was returned to a reservoir tank (Lomber et al., 1999). Loop temperature was monitored, via a wireless thermometer (Omega, model UWTC-2), from outside the acoustic chamber and controlled within 1°C by altering the rate of methanol flow. The microthermocouple was located at the union of the cryoloop on the surface of the cerebrum. In the mammalian cerebrum, synaptic transmission is blocked at temperatures <20°C (Bénita and Condé, 1972; Adey, 1974). Acoustically evoked activity throughout the full thickness of cortex beneath the cryoloop was eliminated by maintaining the cooling loop at 3°C. This temperature reliably places the 20°C thermocline at the base of layer VI (Lomber and Payne, 2000; Lomber et al., 2007; Palmer et al., 2007; Nakamoto et al., 2008). The effective spread of cooling was restricted to 2 mm, and thus each A1 or AAF cryoloop (3 × 6 mm in dimension), deactivated an estimated volume of 70–75 mm3.
Neuronal activity was recorded during nine cooling phases: (1) A1: warm, AAF: warm; (2) A1: cooling, AAF: warm; (3) A1: cool, AAF: warm; (4) A1: cool, AAF: cooling; (5) A1: cool, AAF: cool; (6) A1: rewarming, AAF: cool; (7) A1: rewarm, AAF: cool; (8) A1: rewarm, AAF: rewarming; and (9) A1: rewarm, AAF: rewarm (Fig. 1D). Each phase of a recording cycle lasted for 5 min (noise exposure), 30 min (FM Sweep), or ∼17 min (pure tones), ensuring ample time for neuronal deactivation and reactivation to occur. Recording cycles commenced with a supplemental dose of sodium pentobarbital to ensure that the highest levels of neuronal suppression produced by anesthesia were acquired during the first (A1: warm, AAF: warm) stage of each cycle, and that possible reductions in response activity induced by cooling conditions could not be attributed to anesthetic levels.
Recording procedures.
Iridium axial array microelectrodes (FHC; model AM-003, 200 μm diameter) with impedances between 1 and 3 MΩ were used to measure extracellular neuronal response activity across cortical laminae using Tucker-Davis Technologies OpenEx software. Each electrode array contained 12 recording sites linearly spaced 150 μm apart (Fig. 2). The distance from the tip of the microelectrode to the first recording site was 0.5 mm, hence, to acquire neuronal responses from all layers of auditory cortex, electrodes were lowered ∼2.4 mm from the cortical surface, or until the deepest and most superficial recording channels in the array exhibited neuronal activity. This determination is supported by histological reports identifying cat A1 cortical thickness to be ∼1900 μm (layer I, ∼150 μm thick; layer II, ∼250 μm thick; layer III, ∼500 μm thick; layer IV, ∼250 μm thick; layer V, ∼400 μm thick; and layer VI <400 μm thick; Winer, 1992. While assessment of variations in response properties across individual cortical layers was intended, uncertainty of exact electrode depth position induced by corticovascular pulsations, dimpling of cortical tissue during electrode penetration, and swelling or depression of brain tissue throughout recording sessions, resulted in intolerable levels of recording depth ambiguity. Consequently, recording locations were classified as coming from superficial (∼150–600 μm, electrodes 9–12), middle (∼750–1200 μm, electrodes 5–8), or deep (∼1350–1800 μm, electrodes 1–4) cortical layers (Fig. 2). Cortical penetrations were limited to the middle ectosylvian gyrus, lateral to the middle suprasylvian sulcus (the dorsal zone of auditory cortex), posterior to the anterior ectosylvian sulcus, and anterior to the posterior ectosylvian sulcus. Neuronal activity was bandpass filtered (500–5,000 Hz), amplified (×10,000), and digitized at 25,000 Hz (Tucker Davis Technologies, model RZ2).
Perfusion and histological processing.
At the conclusion of electrophysiological recordings, animals were deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (40 mg/kg, i.v.). Heparin (an anticoagulant) and 1% sodium nitrite (a vasodilator) were coadministered intravenously. Each animal was perfused through the ascending aorta with saline (1 L), followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (2 L), and finally 10% sucrose. All solutions were buffered at pH 7.4 and infused at a rate of 100 ml/min. The net effect of the procedure was to exsanguinate the cat, a method which complies with the recommendations of the American Veterinary Medical Association Panel on Euthanasia (Beaver et al. 2001), and to prepare scientifically useful tissue. The brain was exposed and the head was placed in a stereotaxic apparatus. To ensure correct orientation for sectioning and inclusion of the entire auditory cortex, the brain was blocked at Horsley and Clarke (1908) coronal level A23, before removal from the cranium. To cryoprotect the tissue, each brain was placed in 30% sucrose until it sunk. Frozen sections were cut (60 μm) in the coronal plane and collected serially. Sections from the first of six series, at 300 μm intervals, were processed for SMI-32 immunoreactivity (Sternberger Monoclonal; Mellott et al., 2010). Adjacent sections were stained for the presence of Nissl bodies (cresyl violet stain) or cytochrome oxidase (Payne and Lomber 1996). SMI-32 staining was used to confirm that electrode penetrations were made into A1. Cresyl violet staining was used to determine the laminar position of the recording sites, and cytochrome oxidase was used to confirm that the tissue beneath the cooling loops was metabolically healthy and functional. Sections were mounted on gelatinized slides, air dried, dehydrated, and coverslipped. Examination of sections for detection of electrode tracks was conducted under a Nikon E600 Eclipse microscope (Nikon Instruments; Fig. 2) equipped with a three-axis computer-controlled stepping motor system (0.1 μm resolution) coupled to a personal computer and color CCD camera (Optronix). High-resolution photomicrographs were acquired using Neurolucida software (Microbrightfield).
In each case, we verified that surgical procedures, cryoloop contact with the cerebrum, and cortical cooling operation did not disrupt the normal structural or functional integrity of cortex (Lomber et al., 1999; Yang et al., 2006). In every instance, cell and myelin stains were rich, and the cyto- and myelo-architecture of AAF or A1 were characteristic of the region investigated, with no signs of pathology, as might be revealed by a marked pale staining of neurons or gliosis or light staining of cytochrome oxidase (Lomber and Payne, 1996).
Confirmation of A1 and AAF deactivation.
Ideally, the locations of AAF and A1 would have been determined before cryoloop placement by performing detailed tonotopic mapping to ascertain the borders of the two areas. However, as this mapping would have severely compromised the integrity of cortex, precise dissociation between damage caused by recording procedures or cooling deactivation would have been unfeasible. The placement of the loops was guided by comparing the results of a number of mapping studies and determining characteristic locations for AAF and A1 based on gyral and sulcal patterns (Fig. 1). Therefore, as techniques to confirm the location of AAF and A1 could not be used to localize the two areas before implantation, we confirmed the location of the two areas during postmortem analysis. To accomplish this, we examined SMI-32 staining patterns to determine the location of areas A1 and AAF and compared them with the thermal images acquired to determine the extent of cortical surface deactivation. In each case we identified that the region of deactivated corresponded quite well with the location of AAF or A1 as determined by postmortem tissue analysis.
Data analysis.
Off-line single-unit sorting was completed using k-mean clustering (Tucker-Davis Technologies OpenSort) with subsequent manual inspection of unit separation. In an effort to lessen possible inaccuracies in response clustering, only one well isolated single-unit was used per recording channel (12 channels per electrode). Neuronal recordings classified as A1 based on tonotopy and response latency were used in data analyses (Merzenich et al., 1973; Knight, 1977; Reale and Imig, 1980; Carrasco and Lomber, 2011). In each phase of a cooling cycle, a PSTHs expressed in spikes per second and binned with a time resolution of 1 ms were constructed using a custom-made program (MATLAB; MathWorks). Peak response levels, defined as the maximum number of spikes per second within a PSTH (bin with highest response level regardless of time of occurrence) were identified and compared across cooling cycle phases. An experienced observer blind to the stimulus conditions evaluated neuronal thresholds and receptive field bandwidths. Neuronal thresholds were defined as the minimum acoustic level (dB SPL) that evoked a neuronal response (i.e., lower tip of receptive field), and bandwidth measures were defined as receptive field widths (5 dB SPL increments) above neuronal threshold level. Comparisons of peak response activity were restricted to recordings where the first (A1: warm, AAF: warm) and last (A1: rewarm, AAF rewarm) phases of a cycle did not vary by more than ±20%. This strict criterion reduced the possibility that changes in neuronal activity across cooling conditions were elicited by neuronal death. Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests were performed to assess standard normal distributions in the dataset. Lack of normality in some conditions impelled nonparametric statistical analyses. Kruskal–Wallis tests (p < 0.05) followed by post hoc Tukey–Kramer corrections were used to determine significant changes across recording conditions.
Results
Overview
The present investigation examined the effects of A1 and/or AAF neuronal deactivation on contralateral A1 activity. In short, neuronal responses to simple and complex stimuli exhibited reductions in strength during cortical deactivation periods across A1 cortical laminae. The results are described in three sections. First, we present evidence of decreases in A1 response activity during epochs of contralateral deactivation. Second, we discuss variations in A1 neuronal activity with respect to cortical laminae. Last, we examine associations between neuronal response modulation and hemispheric-side deactivation.
Effects of A1 and AAF deactivation on contralateral A1 response
Noise bursts
A1 neuronal response activity induced by noise burst exposure was recorded before, during, and after contralateral A1 and/or AAF cooling epochs (n (single units) = 131). A representative example of the effects of cooling deactivation demonstrates decreases in A1 response activity magnitude during individual and combined cooling epochs of contralateral A1 and AAF neurons (Fig. 3). On average, A1 peak response strength measures declined by 21.41% during contralateral A1 deactivation (Fig. 4A). The observed reductions were further extended to 31.78% (p < 0.05) during the simultaneous deactivation of A1 and AAF neurons (Fig. 4B), and measured 17.97% during AAF deactivation alone (Fig. 4C). Decreases in response activity returned to baseline levels (warm phase) during rewarming epochs (Fig. 4D). Neuronal responses observed during phase 9 (rewarm period) emphasize the reversible nature of cooling deactivation. Collectively, these results provide evidence of excitatory influences of A1 and AAF neurons on contralateral A1 response activity to broadband acoustic stimulation (Fig. 4E).
FM sweeps
Response properties of A1 neurons to upward and downward FM sweeps were measured during periods of contralateral core auditory field deactivation. A representative example of modulatory influences of A1 and AAF silencing on contralateral A1 activity are presented in Figure 5. While substantial decreases in response levels were measured during deactivation epochs, FM sweep direction did not influence the type or magnitude of changes observed (upward sweep: n (single units) = 223, downward sweep: n = 230). Comparable to variations measured during noise burst exposure, silencing of contralateral A1 neurons elicited on average a 22.84% (23.88% upward sweep, 21.80% downward sweep, p < 0.05) decline in A1 peak response measures (Fig. 6A), concurrent deactivation of contralateral A1 and AAF neurons resulted in a 32.48% (33.36% upward sweep, 31.60% downward sweep, p < 0.05) decrease in A1 peak activity levels (Fig. 6B), and silencing of AAF neurons alone induced a 27.48% (27.73% upward sweep, 27.23% downward sweep, p < 0.05) decrease in A1 peak activity (Fig. 6C). Response magnitude returned to baseline (warm phase) during rewarmed epochs (Fig. 6D). Together, the observed variations in neuronal activity demonstrate that on average, contralateral deactivation of core auditory fields results in significant reductions of A1 peak activity levels during FM sweeps stimulation regardless of sweep direction (Fig. 6E).
Pure tones
Changes in A1 activity produced by contralateral A1 and/or AAF deactivation were investigated during pure tone exposure. In total, 2064 tones varying in frequency and intensity were presented at each recording location. Consistent with neuronal activity changes observed during noise burst and FM sweep exposure, presentation of tonal stimuli during contralateral cooling resulted in A1 peak activity strength reductions (n (single units) = 174). Figure 7 displays a representative example of A1 response activity changes during epochs of contralateral cooling of core auditory areas. In particular, deactivation of contralateral A1 neurons induced on average an 8.11% decrease in A1 peak response levels (Fig. 8A). The detected decrease was further extended during the combined deactivation of A1 and AAF neurons to 20.73% (p < 0.05) (Fig. 8B), and dropped to 16.86% (p < 0.05) during AAF deactivation alone (Fig. 8C). A1 activity returned to baseline levels (warm phase) during the rewarm phase of the cycle (Fig. 8D). Together, these results demonstrate excitatory influences of contralateral core auditory area neurons on A1 response activity (Fig. 8E).
In addition to variations in response strength, changes in receptive field properties were examined during contralateral deactivation. Specifically, threshold levels and bandwidth measures were compared before, during, and after deactivation epochs. Despite lack of statistical significant changes in receptive field bandwidth measures during cooling periods (Figs. 9, 10) on average, neuronal thresholds increased by 2.64, 6.90 (p < 0.05), and 4.91 (p < 0.05) dB SPL during cooling deactivation periods of contralateral A1, A1 and AAF, and AAF alone, respectively (Fig. 10). Collectively, these results indicate that, while response strength in A1 may be modulated by inputs from contralateral core auditory cortex, some properties of receptive fields may not be modulated by neurons in contralateral hemisphere core auditory fields.
In comparing the results from the three stimulus classes (noise burst, FM sweeps, and pure tones), two general observations can be made. First, the largest decrease in response levels in A1, regardless of stimulus class, was during the simultaneous deactivation of A1 and AAF in the contralateral hemisphere. This reduction was most profound for noise bursts and FM sweeps, where the drop was >30%. Second, for noise bursts and FM sweeps, the magnitude of the reduction in response levels during deactivation of either A1 or AAF was similar. However, this was not the case for pure tone stimuli, where the reduction in A1 response strength during contralateral AAF deactivation was twice that identified during contralateral A1 deactivation. Therefore, the deactivation of contralateral core areas has both general and specific effects on A1 neuronal responses.
Laminar features
Changes in neuronal response strength during contralateral deactivation were examined with respect to A1 laminae. Similar numbers of units were sampled across the 12 recording sites that spanned the full thickness of A1 from the deepest recording site (electrode 1) to the most superficial recording site (electrode 12; Fig. 11A). Decreases in response activity levels across acoustic signals (noise bursts, FM sweeps, and pure tones) permitted the grouping and analysis of laminar response characteristics regardless of stimulus spectral features. Recording were classified as deep (electrode 1–4), mid (electrode 5–8), or superficial (electrode 9–12) laminae. Group analyses revealed reductions in response level of 17.43% in deep, 8.46% in mid, and 11.53% across superficial layers during contralateral A1 deactivation (Fig. 11B). Concurrent A1 and AAF deactivation resulted in a decrease of 26.27% in deep, 20.89% in mid, and 24.42% in superficial layers peak response measures (Fig. 11C). AAF deactivation alone resulted in a reduction of 21.52% in deep, 17.37% in mid, and 15.64% in superficial layers peak response activity (Fig. 11D). Statistical analyses of peak response change across cortical laminae revealed significant differences between deep and mid layers during A1 deactivation (Kruskal–Wallis tests, p < 0.05, followed by post hoc Tukey–Kramer corrections). Furthermore, examination of the relationship between field of deactivation and response change demonstrated that simultaneous deactivation of A1 and AAF neurons results in significantly larger decreases in peak activity across deep, mid, and superficial layers than those measured during individual deactivation of A1 or AAF neurons (Kruskal–Wallis tests, p < 0.05, followed by post hoc Tukey–Kramer corrections (Fig. 12). In contrast, variations between individual deactivation of A1 and AAF neurons was not observed. Overall, decreases in neuronal activity across A1 laminae appear to reflect previously described interhemispheric anatomical connections, with the largest inputs targeting supragranular and infragranular layers and the weakest terminations projecting to the granular cell layer (Kelly and Wong, 1981; Code and Winer, 1986; Aitkin et al., 1988; Fig. 12).
These observations demonstrate that while the principal commissural input to auditory areas emanate from homotopic cortical loci (A1→A1), areas with smaller projections (AAF→A1) can generate comparable changes in response magnitude (Lee and Winer, 2008a). This apparent contradiction between structural and functional characteristics can be explained by known properties of corticocortical connectivity between A1 and AAF neurons. Specifically, it has been demonstrated that AAF deactivation results in significant decreases in response activity across ipsilateral A1 neurons (Carrasco and Lomber, 2009b). Therefore, AAF deactivation can influence response properties of contralateral A1 neurons through direct commissural connections as well as indirect projections via ipsilateral A1. Based on this model, the results of the present investigation are not surprising and corroborate known structural and functional properties of auditory cortex connectivity. In particular, as predicted from connectional reports (Lee and Winer, 2008a, b), the highest influence in A1 neuronal response activity was observed during the combined deactivation of A1 and AAF neurons. The magnitude of these changes was followed by AAF deactivation via direct and indirect connections to A1. Last, deactivation of A1 resulted in the lowest levels of response reduction in contralateral A1 neuronal activity.
Hemispheric lateralization
So far, neuronal recording from both hemispheres have been grouped and analyzed together making the examination of hemispheric dominance unfeasible. Thus, in an effort to measure differences in hemispheric lateralization, recordings were classified as left or right hemisphere, and variances in A1 peak response levels induced by contralateral deactivation were measured. Statistical analyses (Kruskal–Wallis tests, p < 0.05 followed by post hoc Tukey–Kramer corrections) did not reveal significant hemispheric variations during deactivation periods (Fig. 13; left: n = 324, right: n = 434). In particular, deactivation of A1 in the left hemisphere produced average reductions in contralateral A1 activity of 20.92% that were not significantly larger than decreases observed during right hemisphere A1 deactivation on left hemisphere A1 cells (16.81%; Fig. 13). Similarly, concurrent deactivation of left or right hemisphere A1 and AAF elicited comparable reductions in contralateral A1 peak activity (28.29 and 22.93%, respectively; Fig. 13). Last, decreases in peak response activity of right hemisphere A1 neurons (16.52%) during left hemisphere AAF deactivation were not statistically different to left hemisphere A1 response changes (14.59%) during right hemisphere deactivation (Fig. 13). Therefore, core areas of auditory cortex in the left and right hemispheres exert comparable influences on contralateral A1 neurons. These results suggest a symmetrical relationship between auditory areas of the two cerebral hemispheres.
Discussion
Although numerous studies have examined transcallosal communication properties [visual cortex: (Glickstein and Berlucchi, 2008; Innocenti, 1980); somatosensory cortex: (Clarey et al., 1996; Rema and Ebner, 2003; Pluto et al., 2005)], to date, the effects of auditory cortex deactivation on acoustically evoked responses of contralateral cortical regions has not been reported. The present study is the first investigation to demonstrate changes in A1 response characteristics induced by reversible cooling deactivation on contralateral auditory fields. Specifically, we demonstrate that deactivation of core auditory areas (A1 and AAF) reduce acoustically evoked neuronal activity in contralateral A1 neurons.
Comparison across sensory systems
Auditory system
Structural studies of interhemispheric connections in cat auditory cortex have demonstrated that a large number of transcallosal projections emanating from core auditory areas A1 and AAF terminate in contralateral core auditory fields (Lee and Winer, 2008a). Nearly 90% of these projections arise from layers III and V (Ravizza et al., 1976; Kelly and Wong, 1981; Code and Winer, 1985; Games and Winer, 1988; Lee and Winer, 2008a). Auditory transcallosal projections pass through the central body, posterior body, and dorsal splenium of the corpus callosum (Lomber et al., 1994; Clarke et al., 1995) and terminate throughout layers II–VI in the homotopic cortical locus, with the greatest density of terminals found in layers II and III and with the lowest density of terminals in layer IV (Kelly and Wong, 1981; Code and Winer, 1986; Aitkin et al., 1988). In the cat, the sources and terminations of callosally projecting neurons have been related to binaural and frequency maps (Imig and Brugge, 1978; Imig et al., 1986). Similarities of origins and terminations of auditory callosal projections in other species, including rat (Cipolloni and Peters, 1983; Games and Winer, 1988; Rüttgers et al., 1990), hamster (Ravizza et al., 1976), and marmoset (Aitkin et al., 1988) demonstrate consistency in connectivity profiles across animal models.
In contrast to the noticeable abundance of structural information available, few investigations have examined functional features of transcallosal connectivity (Mitani and Shimokouchi, 1985; Kitzes and Doherty, 1994; Bamiou et al., 2007). In a neuroanatomical report by Cipolloni and Peters (1983), it was demonstrated that interhemispheric connections form asymmetric synapses with dendritic spines of pyramidal neurons, suggesting the presence of excitatory projections across hemispheres. The results of Cipolloni and Peters (1983) were validated by an investigation of intracellular activity by Mitani and Shimokouchi (1985), which revealed that stimulation of transcallosal projecting neurons in auditory cortex commonly elicit EPSPs in contralateral auditory cortex. These investigations suggest that deactivation of auditory neurons should result in a reduction of response activity in contralateral auditory neurons as demonstrated in the present investigation. Puzzling, however, are the results of Kitzes and Doherty (1994), in which suppression of neuronal activity was achieved during contralateral electrical stimulation paired with acoustic exposure. The authors of the study proposed that stimulus pulse amplitude parameters could have caused the observed inhibitory effects in the experiment. Thus far, activation (Mitani and Shimokouchi, 1985), deactivation (present study), and neuroanatomical (Cipolloni and Peters, 1983) studies suggest a preponderance of interhemispheric excitatory projections in the auditory system.
Visual system
Considerable work has been conducted to identify anatomical and functional properties of interhemispheric connections in the visual system (Choudhury et al., 1965; Hubel and Wiesel, 1967; Silveira et al., 1989; Yinon, 1994; Abel et al., 2000; Schmidt et al., 2010; Genç et al., 2011; for review, see Glickstein and Berlucchi, 2008). Visual cortex investigations have implicated transcallosal projections in midline fusion (Choudhury et al., 1965; Hubel and Wiesel, 1967; Payne, 1990), midline receptive field extension (Antonini et al., 1979, 1983, 1985; Marzi et al., 1982), binocular activation (Lepore and Guillemot, 1982; Blakemore et al., 1983; Payne et al., 1984), and depth perception (Gardner and Cynader, 1987). While a conspicuous lack of comparable investigations in the auditory system impedes ample scrutiny of functional differences between these two modalities, a study conducted by Payne et al. 1991 is distinctly germane to the discussion of the present results. In the study by Payne et al. 1991 reversibly deactivation of transcallosally projecting neurons from areas 17 and 18 altered response properties of receiving neurons in the opposite hemisphere. Similar to the present investigation decreases in evoked activity during contralateral deactivation epochs were reported. Specifically, 41% of layer II/III, 40% of layer IV, and 95% of layer V/VI neurons investigated showed a decrease of at least 25% in evoked response. While much work in the auditory system is still needed to form models of transcallosal functional properties, similarities in neuronal response variation between visual and auditory systems imply commonalities of interhemispheric functional properties across modalities.
Somatosensory system
Neuroanatomical investigations in somatosensory cortex have reported transcallosal connections between homotopic regions of primary somatosensory area (S1) (Koralek and Killackey, 1990; Carr and Sesack, 1998). These anatomical observations have been functionally examined by measuring changes in S1 response activity during contralateral S1 deactivation (Clarey et al., 1996; Rema and Ebner, 2003; Pluto et al., 2005). In particular, cooling deactivation (Clarey et al., 1996), pharmacological silencing (Rema and Ebner, 2003), and surgical removal (Rema and Ebner, 2003) of S1 has been used to assess functional features of transcallosal projections. Rema and Ebner (2003) demonstrated that pharmacological deactivation or surgical removal of the vibrissa representation of SI barrel field cortex in adult rats can induce reductions in response levels of contralateral SI activity to somatic stimulation (layers II/III, 50%; IV, 56%; V, 66% reduction). Similarly, cooling deactivation of primary somatosensory area (3b) in adult macaque monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) and little red flying foxes (Pteropus scapulatus) resulted in neuronal response activity suppression in the contralateral hemisphere (Clarey et al., 1996). Collectively, the findings of Clarey et al. (1996) and Rema and Ebner (2003) substantiate the results of the present investigation in the somatosensory system.
Laminar and hemispheric specificity
Laminar
In the present investigation, decreases in peak response strength were revealed across all cortical laminae. Despite this homogeneous effect, differences in response change levels were observed between laminar groups. Specifically, in all conditions investigated a trend of magnitude changes was observed to be lower in middle layers than deep layers. Similar differences were found between middle and superficial laminae during A1 and simultaneous A1 and AAF deactivation (Fig. 12). This trend in response change demonstrates that while contralateral deactivation can decrease response levels of A1 neurons across all laminae, neurons in deep and superficial laminae are more receptive to contralateral deactivation than neurons in middle layers. This result corroborates previously described anatomical connections where the largest contralateral input terminates in supragranular and infragranular layers and the weakest terminations project to the granular cells (Kelly and Wong, 1981; Code and Winer, 1986; Aitkin et al., 1988).
Hemispheric
In contrast to known origins and terminations of transcallosal pathways, little is known about how these profiles vary between hemispheres. Nonetheless, numerous studies have demonstrated differences in activation levels across hemispheres during various cognitive tasks (for review, see Samara and Tsangaris, 2011). Based on these reports, the present study was designed to investigate the possibility of asymmetries in neuronal response modulation between A1 fields across hemispheres. Our statistical analyses did not reveal significant differences between left and right hemisphere modulatory properties. Presently, the lack of anatomical information about differences in transcallosal connections between auditory cortical fields impedes comparisons of the present results with structural characteristics. Future experiments will be required to describe the putative anatomical basis of hemispheric asymmetries/symmetries in cat auditory cortex.
Other considerations
Converging inputs
Throughout the manuscript, reductions in neuronal response strength have been imputed to transcallosal projections; however, a less parsimonious explanation might attribute variations in contralateral neuronal response to an alternate pathway of auditory converging inputs. In particular, A1 efferent projections to cochlear nucleus (CN) neurons (Weedman and Ryugo, 1996) and projections from CN neurons to the contralateral superior olivary complex (SOC) could account for the observed phenomena (Adams and Warr, 1976; Hackney, 1987; Thompson and Thompson, 1991; Schofield and Cant, 1996; Alibardi, 2000; Arnott et al., 2004). In this alternative model, deactivation of A1 neurons elicits variations in ipsilateral CN activity that subsequently alters response characteristics of contralateral SOC neurons, consequently inducing neuronal response vicissitudes in (contralateral) A1 via feedforward connections. This alternative model, while still interhemispheric, does not rely on the corpus callosum, and is supported by reports of changes in midbrain neuronal activity during cortical deactivation (Nakamoto et al., 2008). Future experiments should aim at exploring the participation, or lack thereof, of alternative communicative pathways between auditory cortical fields.
Anesthesia
It is crucial to acknowledge the inability to extend the present results to awake-behaving states; however, comparisons across studies suggest plausible consistencies between awake and anesthetized states. In particular, similarities in neuronal response inhibition during contralateral deactivation has been reported in awake (Rema and Ebner, 2003), as well as anesthetized states induced by halothane (Payne et al., 1991), ketamine (Clarey et al., 1996), and pentobarbital (present results). Furthermore, neuroanatomical and intracellular recordings support a model of response suppression during contralateral deactivation (Cipolloni and Peters, 1983; Mitani and Shimokouchi, 1985).
Footnotes
This work was supported by grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, and the Canada Foundation for Innovation. We thank Pam Nixon and Amee McMillan for technical and surgical assistance during this study. Stereotaxic coordinates are provided using the Horsley and Clarke (1908) system as described by Reinoso-Suárez (1961).
References
- Abel PL, O'Brien BJ, Olavarria JF. Organization of callosal linkages in visual area V2 of macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol. 2000;428:278–293. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20001211)428:2<278::aid-cne7>3.0.co;2-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adams JC, Warr WB. Origins of axons in the cat's acoustic striae determined by injection of horseradish peroxidase into severed tracts. J Comp Neurol. 1976;170:107–121. doi: 10.1002/cne.901700108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adey WR. Biophysical and metabolic bases of cooling effects on cortical membrane potentials in the cat. Exp Neurol. 1974;42:113–140. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(74)90011-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aitkin LM, Kudo M, Irvine DR. Connections of the primary auditory cortex in the common marmoset, Callithrix jacchus jacchus. J Comp Neurol. 1988;269:235–248. doi: 10.1002/cne.902690208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alibardi L. Cytology, synaptology and immunocytochemistry of commissural neurons and their putative axonal terminals in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of the rat. Ann Anat. 2000;182:207–220. doi: 10.1016/S0940-9602(00)80023-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antonini A, Berlucchi G, Marzi CA, Sprague JM. Importance of corpus callosum for visual receptive fields of single neurons in cat superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol. 1979;42:137–152. doi: 10.1152/jn.1979.42.1.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antonini A, Berlucchi G, Lepore F. Physiological organization of callosal connections of a visual lateral suprasylvian cortical area in the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1983;49:902–921. doi: 10.1152/jn.1983.49.4.902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antonini A, Di Stefano M, Minciacchi D, Tassinari G. Interhemispheric influences on area 19 of the cat. Exp Brain Res. 1985;59:171–184. doi: 10.1007/BF00237677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnott RH, Wallace MN, Shackleton TM, Palmer AR. Onset neurones in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus project to the dorsal cochlear nucleus. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2004;5:153–170. doi: 10.1007/s10162-003-4036-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bamiou DE, Sisodiya S, Musiek FE, Luxon LM. The role of the interhemispheric pathway in hearing. Brain Res Rev. 2007;56:170–182. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beaver BV, Reed W, Leary S, McKiernan B, Bain F, Schultz R, Bennett BT, Pascoe P, Schull E, Cork LC, Francis-Floyd R, Amass KD, Johnson RJ, Schmidt RH, Underwood W, Thorton GW, Kohn B. 2000 Report of the American Veterinary Medical Association Panel of Euthanasia. J Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2001;218:669–696. [Google Scholar]
- Bénita M, Condé H. Effects of local cooling upon conduction and synaptic transmission. Brain Res. 1972;36:133–151. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90771-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlucchi G, Rizzolatti G. Binocularly driven neurons in visual cortex of split-chiasm cats. Science. 1968;159:308–310. doi: 10.1126/science.159.3812.308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakemore C, Diao YC, Pu ML, Wang YK, Xiao YM. Possible functions of the interhemispheric connexions between visual cortical areas in the cat. J Physiol. 1983;337:331–349. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks VB. Study of brain function by local, reversible cooling. Rev Physiol Biochem Pharmacol. 1983;95:1–109. [Google Scholar]
- Carr DB, Sesack SR. Callosal terminals in the rat prefrontal cortex: synaptic targets and association with GABA-immunoreactive structures. Synapse. 1998;29:193–205. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199807)29:3<193::AID-SYN1>3.0.CO;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrasco A, Lomber SG. Evidence for hierarchical processing in cat auditory cortex: nonreciprocal influence of primary auditory cortex on the posterior auditory field. J Neurosci. 2009a;29:14323–14333. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2905-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrasco A, Lomber SG. Differential modulatory influences between primary auditory cortex and the anterior auditory field. J Neurosci. 2009b;29:8350–8362. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6001-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrasco A, Lomber SG. Reciprocal modulatory influences between tonotopic and nontonotopic cortical fields in the cat. J Neurosci. 2010;30:1476–1487. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5708-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrasco A, Lomber SG. Neuronal activation times to simple, complex, and natural sounds in cat primary and nonprimary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1166–1178. doi: 10.1152/jn.00940.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chafee MV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Inactivation of parietal and prefrontal cortex reveals interdependence of neural activity during memory-guided saccades. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:1550–1566. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.3.1550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheung SW, Nagarajan SS, Bedenbaugh PH, Schreiner CE, Wang X, Wong A. Auditory cortical neuron response differences under isoflurane versus pentobarbital anesthesia. Hear Res. 2001;156:115–127. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5955(01)00272-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choudhury BP, Whitteridge D, Wilson ME. The function of the callosal connections of the visual cortex. Q J Exp Physiol Cogn Med Sci. 1965;50:214–219. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.1965.sp001783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cipolloni PB, Peters A. The termination of callosal fibres in the auditory cortex of the rat. A combined Golgi–electron microscope and degeneration study. J Neurocytol. 1983;12:713–726. doi: 10.1007/BF01258146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarey JC, Tweedale R, Calford MB. Interhemispheric modulation of somatosensory receptive fields: evidence for plasticity in primary somatosensory cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6:196–206. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke S, de Ribaupierre F, Bajo VM, Rouiller EM, Kraftsik R. The auditory pathway in cat corpus callosum. Exp Brain Res. 1995;104:534–540. doi: 10.1007/BF00231988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Code RA, Winer JA. Commissural neurons in layer III of cat primary auditory cortex (AI): pyramidal and non-pyramidal cell input. J Comp Neurol. 1985;242:485–510. doi: 10.1002/cne.902420404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Code RA, Winer JA. Columnar organization and reciprocity of commissural connections in cat primary auditory cortex (AI) Hear Res. 1986;23:205–222. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(86)90110-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond IT, Jones EG, Powell TP. Interhemispheric fiber connections of the auditory cortex of the cat. Brain Res. 1968;11:177–193. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(68)90080-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Games KD, Winer JA. Layer V in rat auditory cortex: projections to the inferior colliculus and contralateral cortex. Hear Res. 1988;34:1–25. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(88)90047-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner JC, Cynader MS. Mechanisms for binocular depth sensitivity along the vertical meridian of the visual field. Brain Res. 1987;413:60–74. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90154-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Genç E, Bergmann J, Tong F, Blake R, Singer W, Kohler A. Callosal connections of primary visual cortex predict the spatial spreading of binocular rivalry across the visual hemifields. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;5:161. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glickstein M, Berlucchi G. Classical disconnection studies of the corpus callosum. Cortex. 2008;44:914–927. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackney CM. Anatomical features of the auditory pathway from cochlea to cortex. Br Med Bull. 1987;43:780–801. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a072218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horsley V, Clarke RH. The structure and functions of the cerebellum examined by a new method. Brain. 1908;31:45–124. [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Cortical and callosal connections concerned with the vertical meridian of visual fields in the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1967;30:1561–1573. doi: 10.1152/jn.1967.30.6.1561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imaizumi K, Priebe NJ, Crum PA, Bedenbaugh PH, Cheung SW, Schreiner CE. Modular functional organization of cat anterior auditory field. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92:444–457. doi: 10.1152/jn.01173.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imig TJ, Brugge JF. Sources and terminations of callosal axons related to binaural and frequency maps in primary auditory cortex of the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1978;182:637–660. doi: 10.1002/cne.901820406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imig TJ, Reale RA, Brugge JF, Morel A, Adrian H. Topography of cortico-cortical connections related to tonotopic and binaural maps of cat auditory cortex. In: Leporé F, Ptito M, Jasper HH, editors. Two hemispheres-one brain: functions of the corpus callosum. New York: Liss; 1986. pp. 103–115. [Google Scholar]
- Innocenti GM. The primary visual pathway through the corpus callosum: morphological and functional aspects in the cat. Arch Ital Biol. 1980;118:124–188. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jasper HH, Shacter DG, Montplaisir J. The effect of local cooling upon spontaneous and evoked electrical activity of cerebral cortex. Can J Physiol Pharmacol. 1970;48:640–652. doi: 10.1139/y70-094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly JP, Wong D. Laminar connections of the cat's auditory cortex. Brain Res. 1981;212:1–15. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90027-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kitzes LM, Doherty D. Influence of callosal activity on units in the auditory cortex of ferret (Mustela putorius) J Neurophysiol. 1994;71:1740–1751. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.71.5.1740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knight PL. Representation of the cochlea within the anterior auditory field (AAF) of the cat. Brain Res. 1977;130:447–467. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90108-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koralek KA, Killackey HP. Callosal projections in rat somatosensory cortex are altered by early removal of afferent input. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1990;87:1396–1400. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.4.1396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee CC, Winer JA. Connections of cat auditory cortex: III. Corticocortical system. J Comp Neurol. 2008a;507:1920–1943. doi: 10.1002/cne.21613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee CC, Winer JA. Connections of cat auditory cortex: II. Commissural system. J Comp Neurol. 2008b;507:1901–1919. doi: 10.1002/cne.21614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lepore F, Guillemot JP. Visual receptive field properties of cells innervated through the corpus callosum in the cat. Exp Brain Res. 1982;46:413–424. doi: 10.1007/BF00238636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomber SG. The advantages and limitations of permanent or reversible deactivation techniques in the assessment of neural function. J Neurosci Methods. 1999;86:109–117. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(98)00160-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomber SG, Payne BR. Removal of two halves restores the whole: reversal of visual hemineglect during bilateral cortical or collicular inactivation in the cat. Vis Neurosci. 1996;13:1143–1156. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800007781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomber SG, Payne BR. Translaminar differentiation of visually guided behaviors revealed by restricted cerebral cooling deactivation. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:1066–1077. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.11.1066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomber SG, Cornwell P, Sun JS, MacNeil MA, Payne BR. Reversible inactivation of visual processing operations in middle suprasylvian cortex of the behaving cat. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1994;91:2999–3003. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.8.2999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomber SG, Payne BR, Horel JA. The cryoloop: an adaptable reversible cooling deactivation method for behavioral or electrophysiological assessment of neural function. J Neurosci Methods. 1999;86:179–194. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(98)00165-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomber SG, Malhotra S, Hall AJ. Functional specialization in non-primary auditory cortex of the cat: areal and laminar contributions to sound localization. Hear Res. 2007;229:31–45. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2007.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marzi CA, Antonini A, Di Stefano M, Legg CR. The contribution of the corpus callosum to receptive fields in the lateral suprasylvian visual areas of the cat. Behav Brain Res. 1982;4:155–176. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(82)90070-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mellott JG, Van der Gucht E, Lee CC, Carrasco A, Winer JA, Lomber SG. Areas of the cat auditory cortex as defined by neurofilament proteins expressing SMI-32. Hear Res. 2010;267:119–136. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merzenich MM, Knight PL, Roth GL. Cochleotopic organization of primary auditory cortex in the cat. Brain Res. 1973;63:343–346. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90101-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitani A, Shimokouchi M. Neuronal connections in the primary auditory cortex: an electrophysiological study in the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1985;235:417–429. doi: 10.1002/cne.902350402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morel A, Garraghty PE, Kaas JH. Tonotopic organization, architectonic fields, and connections of auditory cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 1993;335:437–459. doi: 10.1002/cne.903350312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamoto KT, Jones SJ, Palmer AR. Descending projections from auditory cortex modulate sensitivity in the midbrain to cues for spatial position. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99:2347–2356. doi: 10.1152/jn.01326.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olfert ED, Cross BM, McWilliam AA. Guide to the care and use of experimental animals. Ottawa, Ontario: Canadian Council on Animal Care; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Olshausen BA, Field DJ. Sparse coding of sensory inputs. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:481–487. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer AR, Hall DA, Sumner C, Barrett DJ, Jones S, Nakamoto K, Moore DR. Some investigations into non-passive listening. Hear Res. 2007;229:148–157. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2006.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne BR. Function of the corpus callosum in the representation of the visual field in cat visual cortex. Vis Neurosci. 1990;5:205–211. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800000225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne BR, Lomber SG. Age dependent modification of cytochrome oxidase activity in the cat dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus following removal of primary visual cortex. Vis Neurosci. 1996;13:805–816. doi: 10.1017/s095252380000907x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne BR, Pearson HE, Berman N. Role of corpus callosum in functional organization of cat striate cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1984;52:570–594. doi: 10.1152/jn.1984.52.3.570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne BR, Siwek DF, Lomber SG. Complex transcallosal interactions in visual cortex. Vis Neurosci. 1991;6:283–289. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800006283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips DP, Irvine DR. Properties of single neurons in the anterior auditory field (AAF) of cat cerebral cortex. Brain Res. 1982;248:237–244. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90581-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pluto CP, Chiaia NL, Rhoades RW, Lane RD. Reducing contralateral SI activity reveals hindlimb receptive fields in the SI forelimb-stump representation of neonatally amputated rats. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:1727–1732. doi: 10.1152/jn.00228.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauschecker JP, Tian B, Pons T, Mishkin M. Serial and parallel processing in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1997;382:89–103. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ravizza RJ, Straw RB, Long PD. Laminar origin of efferent projections from auditory cortex in the golden Syrian hamster. Brain Res. 1976;114:497–500. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90971-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reale RA, Imig TJ. Tonotopic organization in auditory cortex of the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1980;192:265–291. doi: 10.1002/cne.901920207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rema V, Ebner FF. Lesions of mature barrel field cortex interfere with sensory processing and plasticity in connected areas of the contralateral hemisphere. J Neurosci. 2003;23:10378–10387. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-32-10378.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinoso-Suàrez F. Topographical atlas of the cat brain for experimental-physiological research [in German] Darmstadt, Federal Republic of Germany: Merck; 1961. [Google Scholar]
- Rochefort NL, Buzás P, Kisvárday ZF, Eysel UT, Milleret C. Layout of transcallosal activity in cat visual cortex revealed by optical imaging. Neuroimage. 2007;36:804–821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rouiller EM, Simm GM, Villa AE, de Ribaupierre Y, de Ribaupierre F. Auditory corticocortical interconnections in the cat: evidence for parallel and hierarchical arrangement of the auditory cortical areas. Exp Brain Res. 1991;86:483–505. doi: 10.1007/BF00230523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rüttgers K, Aschoff A, Friauf E. Commissural connections between the auditory cortices of the rat. Brain Res. 1990;509:71–79. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90310-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samara A, Tsangaris GT. Brain asymmetry: both sides of the story. Expert Rev Proteomics. 2011;8:693–703. doi: 10.1586/epr.11.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt KE, Lomber SG, Innocenti GM. Specificity of neuronal responses in primary visual cortex is modulated by interhemispheric corticocortical input. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:2776–2786. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schofield BR, Cant NB. Origins and targets of commissural connections between the cochlear nuclei in guinea pigs. J Comp Neurol. 1996;375:128–146. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19961104)375:1<128::AID-CNE8>3.0.CO;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silveira LC, Heywood CA, Cowey A. Direct and transcallosal contribution to the cortical visual evoked response in rats. Behav Brain Res. 1989;31:291–294. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(89)90012-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson AM, Thompson GC. Projections from the posteroventral cochlear nucleus to the superior olivary complex in guinea pig: light and EM observations with the PHA-L method. J Comp Neurol. 1991;311:495–508. doi: 10.1002/cne.903110405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weedman DL, Ryugo DK. Pyramidal cells in primary auditory cortex project to cochlear nucleus in rat. Brain Res. 1996;706:97–102. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01201-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winer JA. The functional architecture of the medial geniculate body and primary auditory cortex. In: Webster DB, Popper AN, Fay RR, editors. The mammalian auditory pathway: neuroanatomy. New York: Springer; 1992. pp. 222–409. [Google Scholar]
- Yang XF, Kennedy BR, Lomber SG, Schmidt RE, Rothman SM. Cooling produces minimal neuropathology in neocortex and hippocampus. Neurobiol Dis. 2006;23:637–643. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2006.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yinon U. Visual hemispheric dominance induced in split brain cats during development: a model of deficient interhemispheric transfer derived from physiological evidence in single visual cortex cells. Behav Brain Res. 1994;64:97–110. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(94)90122-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]