Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2015 Apr 1;35(13):5307–5316. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1674-14.2015

Contribution of the Potassium-Chloride Cotransporter KCC2 to the Strength of Inhibition in the Neonatal Rodent Spinal Cord In Vitro

Florian Gackière 1, Laurent Vinay 1,
PMCID: PMC6705413  PMID: 25834055

Abstract

In healthy mature motoneurons (MNs), KCC2 cotransporters maintain the intracellular chloride concentration at low levels, a prerequisite for postsynaptic inhibition mediated by GABA and glycine. KCC2 expression in lumbar MNs is reduced after spinal cord injury (SCI) resulting in a depolarizing shift of the chloride equilibrium potential. Despite modeling studies indicating that such a downregulation of KCC2 function would reduce the strength of postsynaptic inhibition, physiological evidence is still lacking. The present study aimed at investigating the functional impact of a modification of KCC2 function. We focused on a well characterized disynaptic inhibitory pathway responsible for reciprocal inhibition between antagonistic muscles. We performed in vitro extracellular recordings on spinal cords isolated from rodents at the end of the first postnatal week. Genetic reduction of KCC2 expression, pharmacological blockade of KCC2, as well as SCI-induced downregulation of KCC2 all resulted in a reduction of the strength of reciprocal inhibition. We then tried to restore endogenous inhibition after SCI by means of zinc ions that have been shown to boost KCC2 function in other models. Zinc chloride indeed hyperpolarized the chloride equilibrium potential in MNs and increased reciprocal inhibition after neonatal SCI. This study demonstrates that the level of KCC2 function sets the strength of postsynaptic inhibition and suggests that the downregulation of KCC2 after SCI likely contributes to the high occurrence of flexor–extensor cocontractions in SCI patients.

Keywords: chloride homeostasis, KCC2, reciprocal inhibition, spasticity, zinc

Introduction

The proper operation of neuronal networks relies on a balance between excitation and inhibition. A perturbation of this balance in pathological conditions such as after spinal cord injury (SCI) leads to spasticity, a syndrome that is characterized by involuntary muscular spasms, tendon jerks, and an increased muscle resistance to passive stretch (Lance, 1980). Most SCI patients suffer from spasticity 1 year after injury (Crone et al., 2000, 2003; Morita et al., 2001). It is widely accepted that at least two mechanisms in the spinal cord below the lesion contribute to this disorder: an increased excitability of motoneurons (MNs; (Li et al., 2004; Boulenguez and Vinay, 2009) and a reduction of inhibition (known as disinhibition; Boorman et al., 1996; Mazzocchio and Rossi, 1997; Katz, 1999). Disinhibition was recently suggested as being caused by a reduced expression of the potassium (K+)-chloride (Cl) cotransporters KCC2 in the membrane of MNs thereby increasing the intracellular concentration of chloride ions and reversing the effect of IPSPs from hyperpolarization to depolarization (Jean-Xavier et al., 2006; Boulenguez et al., 2010; Bos et al., 2013). For instance, immunohistochemical experiments indeed revealed a reduction of KCC2 expression in MNs 4–6 d after spinal cord transection performed on the day of birth. This was associated with a depolarization of the reversal potential of IPSPs (EIPSPs) in lumbar MNs compared with noninjured animals (Jean-Xavier et al., 2006; Bos et al., 2013).

An important issue is to determine the functional consequences of a downregulation of KCC2 expression that induces a ∼10–15 mV depolarizing shift of EIPSPs in SCI rats (Jean-Xavier et al., 2006; Boulenguez et al., 2010; Bos et al., 2013). In different areas of the CNS including the spinal cord, small depolarizing IPSPs can facilitate action potential firing when they are combined with subthreshold excitatory inputs (Chen et al., 1996; Gao and van den Pol, 2001; Gulledge and Stuart, 2003; Stein and Nicoll, 2003; Jean-Xavier et al., 2007). In addition, modeling studies have suggested that a depolarizing shift of KCC2 increases the cell excitability (Prescott et al., 2006; Doyon et al., 2011). Despite these findings, the functional impact of a disruption of chloride homeostasis on sensory-motor function is still unknown keeping the question of whether SCI-induced KCC2 deficiency is sufficient to cause a motor dysfunction open.

To answer this question, we focused on a well characterized inhibitory reflex pathway through which group Ia afferents provide inhibition of MNs innervating antagonistic muscles (Hultborn and Udo, 1972; Jankowska, 1992). This reciprocal disynaptic inhibition is mediated by glycinergic interneurons (INs) termed Ia inhibitory INs (Curtis et al., 1968; Jankowska and Lindström, 1972) and is functional at birth in mice (Sonner and Ladle, 2013). Using this model in vitro we demonstrate that modulating KCC2 function markedly affects the strength of inhibition.

Materials and Methods

Animals and preparations.

The mammalian KCC2 gene (Slc12a5) generates two alternatively spliced isoforms KCC2a and KCC2b, which differ from each other in their N-terminal part (Uvarov et al., 2007). Some experiments (n = 15) were performed on neonatal transgenic mice with a targeted disruption of KCC2 exon 1b, which lack KCC2b but retain KCC2a mRNA. The KCC2 mouse strain (C57BL/6 genetic background) was provided by E. Delpire (Vanderbilt University School of Medicine, Nashville, TN) (Woo et al., 2002). At the end of the first postnatal week, KCC2−/− (Slc12a5−/−) mice express ∼21% of KCC2 compared with the wild-type KCC2+/+ (Slc12a5+/+) mice (Stil et al., 2011). In addition to these mice (home bred, both sexes), we used neonatal Wistar rats (originally collected from Janvier Laboratories and then home bred; both sexes; n = 89) which were all housed in a temperature-controlled animal-care facility with a 12 h light/dark cycle. The postnatal ages of animals were measured in terms of days after birth. We made all efforts to minimize the number of animals used and animal suffering. All surgical and experimental procedures were in accordance with the French regulations (Ministry of Food, Agriculture and Fisheries; Division of Health and Protection of Animals) and were approved by the local ethical committee (Comité d'éthique en Neurosciences, Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone, Marseille, N°71; A9/01 13).

Genotyping.

Briefly, tail fragments were frozen and kept at −80°C. Purification of DNA was performed after overnight incubation in a lysis buffer. Control and mutant fragments of kcc2 gene were amplified by PCR by using the same forward primer (5′-AGCGTGTGTCCGTGTGCGAGTG-3′) and different reverse primers for the control and the mutant genes (5′-CCGTCCTCGCAGTCCGTCAGGTTG-3′ and 5′-CCAGAGGCCACTTGTGTAGCGC-3′, respectively). Both sets of oligonucleotides generate a 0.2 kb PCR product. Note that researchers were blinded to genotype of the animals throughout in vitro experiments and data analysis.

Surgical procedure: neonatal SCI.

Rats were deeply anesthetized by hypothermia at birth. A dorsal midline skin incision was made over the thoracic vertebra and the overlying fascia and muscles were retracted to expose the dorsal surface of the vertebrae. After a partial laminectomy, the spinal cord was completely transected at the T8 thoracic level with scissors. The lesion cavity was then filled with sterile absorbable local hemostat Surgicoll (Medical Biomaterial Products). The skin incision was closed with sutures (PDSII 6.0, Ethicon; Johnson and Johnson) and covered by Steri-Strips (3M Health Care). The whole surgical procedure took <10 min after anesthesia. Sham-operated rats were treated in the same way except the spinal cord transection. Following surgery, the neonates recovered 45 min in a warm environment maintaining the temperature at 35 ± 1°C. Wounds were then cleaned and rats were kept in a warm environment for 40 min before returning to the nest.

Spinal cord preparations.

Spinal cords isolated from neonatal animals were dissected together with spinal roots. Briefly, after decapitation and evisceration, the spinal cord was exposed by dorsal laminectomy and acute removal of the dura in a cold artificial CSF (ACSF; containing the following (in mm): 130 NaCl, 4 KCl, 3.75 CaCl2, 1.3 MgSO4, 0.58 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, and 10 glucose; all compounds were from Sigma-Aldrich; oxygenated with 95% O2/5% CO2, pH 7.4). By a careful inspection of the lack of continuity between the spinal stumps, we verified the completeness of the spinal cord transection for injured-rats (SCI rats, n = 23). Cord, from sacral segments up to T8, was then removed from the vertebral column together with peripheral roots, notably those from the lumbar L3 and L5 segments that contain the MNs projecting to the knee extensor and flexor muscles, respectively (Nicolopoulos-Stournaras and Iles, 1983). The preparation was then transferred to the recording chamber and allowed to recover for 1 h in oxygenated ACSF equilibrated at room temperature (24 ± 1°C). The isolated spinal cord preparation was pinned down, ventral side up, in sylgard (Dow-Corning)-covered recording chamber, and continuously perfused with the ACSF solution. All ventral (VR) and dorsal (DR) roots were also carefully pinned.

In vitro extracellular recordings.

Extracellular electrophysiological signals of lumbar VR and DR responses were recorded by contact stainless steel electrodes insulated from the bath with Vaseline. Data were acquired through an AC-coupled amplifier (bandwidth: 70–3 kHz) and a Digidata 1440A interface using the Clampex 10.2 software (Molecular Devices).

Evaluation of reciprocal inhibition.

The protocol was adapted from previous reports (Wang et al., 2008; Talpalar et al., 2011; Sonner and Ladle, 2013). Single pulses were delivered to the DR to evoke responses in the homonymous VR. We stimulated at two to three times the threshold (T) intensity that evoked an incoming volley in the DR. The current pulses (0.3 ms duration) required to elicit the maximal response varied among preparations (from 0.2 to 0.6 V for mice; from 0.6 to 1.2 V for rats). Stimulations were delivered every 20 s to avoid fatigue and synaptic depression described previously (Lev-Tov and Pinco, 1992). Test and conditioning stimulations were delivered to the L5 and L3 DRs, respectively. Delays between the two stimulations ranged from 0 to 40 ms. Each delay was tested at least three times in the following way: five controls (test only) followed by five paired (conditioning + test) stimulations. The order in which delays were tested was randomized from series to series to prevent possible order-dependent effects. Data analysis was performed offline and consisted in measuring the peak-to-antipeak amplitude of the L5 monosynaptic reflex (Clampfit 10.2 software). To consider only the monosynaptic component, we restricted measurements to the first 3 ms after the response onset (Kudo and Yamada, 1987). We defined the duration of the inhibition as the intersection of the regression line calculated from data with the x-axis (GraphPad Prism 5.01 software).

Quantification of polysynaptic responses.

We rectified VR signals and measured the areas under the curve from 3 to 100 ms after response onset (Clampfit 10.2 software). The effect of the drug was considered 30–60 min following bath application.

Quantification of rate-dependent depression.

We delivered 15 stimulations at various frequencies (20 s between each series) and we normalized the amplitude of monosynaptic responses (the first three discarded) to the controls evoked at 20 s intervals (mean of 5 pulses). We took into account at least three series of stimulations before and 30–60 min after the onset of drug application.

Intracellular recordings.

After removing the pia, we recorded lumbar MNs intracellularly using glass microelectrodes filled with 2 m K-acetate (70–100 MΩ resistance). We recorded intracellular potentials in the discontinuous current-clamp mode (Axoclamp 2B amplifier; Digidata 1200 interface; pClamp9 software; Molecular Devices). We considered only neurons exhibiting a stable (>15 min) resting membrane potential (Vrest) for analysis. We identified MNs in the L4 or L5 segments by their antidromic response to VR stimulation. We used glass suction electrode to stimulate the ipsilateral ventral funiculus at the L2–L3 level. Such stimulations induced GABAA- and glycine-mediated IPSPs in the presence of DL-2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (DL-APV; 50 μm) and 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (CNQX; 10 μm; Boulenguez et al., 2010; Bos et al., 2013). We recorded IPSPs at various holding potentials (500-ms-long current pulses) and collected at least 20 values for each MN. We measured and plotted amplitudes of IPSPs against holding potentials and obtained EIPSP from the regression line (GraphPad Prism 5.01 software).

Drugs.

We purchased CNQX and APV from Abcam, whereas VU0240551, ZnCl2, strychnine, and picrotoxin were from Sigma-Aldrich. VU0255011 (or ML077) and VU0463271 were generous gifts from Vanderbilt University. VU0240551, VU0255011, and VU0463271 will be referred as “VU compounds” in the following text and figure legends. Strychnine and picrotoxin were prepared in ACSF, whereas VU compounds in DMSO (final concentration of 0.1%). We checked that 0.1% DMSO alone for 2 h has no effect on monosynaptic reflexes and reciprocal inhibition (n = 2). ZnCl2 was prepared in NaH2PO4-free ACSF to avoid precipitation of the metal (Chorin et al., 2011).

Statistical analysis.

All data are reported as mean ± SE. Statistical comparisons between small samples were performed using nonparametric Wilcoxon or Mann–Whitney tests (for comparing two paired or unpaired groups, respectively; GraphPad Prism 5.01 software). Differences were considered to be significant at *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001.

Results

The strength of reciprocal inhibition depends on the level of KCC2 expression in neonatal mice

We started by examining the strength of reciprocal inhibition in KCC2 (Slc12a5) knock-out mice from postnatal day (P)4 to P6. Homozygous KCC2−/− animals retain ∼20% of the KCC2 amounts expressed in the spinal cord of wild-type KCC2+/+ animals (Woo et al., 2002; Stil et al., 2011). An electrical stimulation of the L5 DR evoked a monosynaptic response in the homonymous ipsilateral VR (Fig. 1A1; KCC2+/+ mouse, P6) with no or little response in the L3 VR (Fig. 1A2). A conditioning stimulation was then delivered to the ipsilateral L3 DR, generating a monosynaptic response in L3 VR (Fig. 1A4). Such stimulation attenuated the amplitude of the L5 monosynaptic reflex if delivered within an appropriate time window before the L5 DR stimulus (Fig. 1A3,A5). Different delays between the two stimulations (L3 DR before L5 DR) were tested. The L5 response was reduced in amplitude for intervals in the range of 2–40 ms (Fig. 1B,C), in agreement with earlier observations in newborn mice (Sonner and Ladle, 2013). We then compared the strength of reciprocal inhibition between wild-type KCC2+/+ and KCC2-deficient mice. Figure 1B shows the values obtained for each delay from the two genotypes (KCC2+/+, n = 5; KCC2−/−, n = 10). Significant differences were observed at intervals ranging from 5 to 30 ms (p < 0.05, p < 0.01, p < 0.001, Mann–Whitney test). To further characterize the time course of the inhibition, we fitted individual traces by means of the spline/lowess, cubic option (see Materials and Methods; Fig. 1C). This demonstrated that KCC2+/+ mice displayed two phases of inhibition peaking at interstimulus intervals of 7.3 ms (strength of inhibition: 58.2 ± 6.3%; Fig. 1C,D) and 23.1 ms (49.6 ± 12.7%; Fig. 1C,D), respectively. The two components have different pharmacological profiles, the short-interval inhibition being consistent with the classical glycinergic disynaptic reciprocal inhibition whereas the long-latency component relies mainly on presynaptic inhibition mediated by GABAergic INs (Sonner and Ladle, 2013). In KCC2−/− mice, compared with wild-type animals, the strength of inhibition was markedly reduced to 6.5 ± 3.4% (88.8% reduction; Fig. 1C,D; p < 0.001, Mann–Whitney test) and 14.8 ± 6.3% (70.2% reduction; Fig. 1C,D; p < 0.05, Mann–Whitney test) at short and long intervals, respectively. An inhibition however persisted at the two intervals in KCC2-deficient mice as response amplitudes in the presence and absence of a conditioning stimulus were significantly different (p < 0.05, Wilcoxon test). These results provide the first demonstration in neonatal mice that KCC2 cotransporters are important in regulating the strength of reciprocal inhibition.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

The strength of reciprocal inhibition depends on KCC2 expression in neonatal mice. A, Representative traces of extracellular recordings from L5 and L3 VRs (A1, A3 and A2, A4, respectively) of a P6 KCC2+/+ mouse. Black traces show the responses to the L5 DR stimulation (Test) whose intensity is sufficient to evoke a monosynaptic reflex in L5 VR. Gray traces show the responses when the L5 DR stimulation, at the same intensity, was preceded by a conditioning stimulation of the L3 DR (Test + Conditioning) that is sufficient to evoke a monosynaptic reflex in L3 VR (interval of 10 ms). A5, Enlargement of superimposed monosynaptic responses. B, C, Amplitude of the test response plotted against the interstimulus interval between conditioning and test stimulations. Average normalized strength of inhibition, represented from raw data (B) or after a fitting analysis using spline/lowess, cubic option (C) showing two distinct phases of inhibition. Curves were obtained from 5 KCC2+/+ and 10 KCC2−/− P4–P6 mice with at least three observations at each delay. Note that the discontinuity after 30 ms interval is due to a different number of observations. D, Histograms summarizing the strength of reciprocal inhibition at short (7.3 ms) and long (23.1 ms) interstimulus intervals. Statistical differences between KCC2+/+ and KCC2−/− mice are noted above each delay in B and above short and long delays in C and D (Mann–Whitney test; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001).

Reciprocal inhibition is reduced after neonatal SCI

The next series of experiments aimed at investigating whether the downregulation of KCC2 that is induced by neonatal SCI in rat (Jean-Xavier et al., 2006; Boulenguez et al., 2010) has any functional impact on reciprocal inhibition. However, to the best of our knowledge, the protocol used by Sonner and Ladle (2013) in mice has never been described in neonatal rats. We therefore performed a reference study on the latter model (n = 17; Fig. 2A–C). The time course of inhibition was somewhat different from the one observed in mice as the same fit analysis used above (spline/lowess, cubic; see Materials and Methods) revealed a single phase of inhibition reaching 68.7 ± 3.2% at 10.8 ms (Fig. 2B). The linear regression analysis of the data after the peak of inhibition revealed a duration of inhibition of 50.4 ms in newborn rats (Fig. 2C). We next used pharmacological compounds to specifically block glycine or GABAA receptors. After a control period of at least 1 h, we incubated different spinal cord preparations with strychnine (0.4–1 μm, n = 9) or picrotoxin (20 μm, n = 8). We compared the values calculated for each delay before and after drug application (Fig. 2D,E; p < 0.05, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon test). Bath application of strychnine lead to a facilitation at 5–10 ms (Fig. 2D; 19 ± 12.6% facilitation vs 62.3 ± 6.1% inhibition at 10 ms, n = 9; p < 0.05, Wilcoxon test) and the inhibition was abolished at longer intervals (>15 ms; p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test). Picrotoxin significantly reduced the inhibition at short intervals (10–15 ms; p < 0.05, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon test), such that response amplitudes in the absence and presence of conditioning stimulation were not significantly different (p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test). A trend toward a facilitation (p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test) was observed at longer intervals (>20 ms; Fig. 2E; 24.4 ± 22.3% facilitation compared with 31.9 ± 11% inhibition at 40 ms, n = 6). Together, these results indicate that most of the short- (5–20 ms) and long-latency (>20 ms) components of inhibition at the end of the first postnatal week in rats are mediated by glycinergic and GABAergic transmissions, respectively.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Contribution of GABA and glycine to reciprocal inhibition in neonatal rats. A, B, Amplitude of the test response. Average normalized strength of inhibition, represented from raw data (A) or after a fitting analysis using spline/lowess, cubic option (B), which shows a single phase of inhibition (peaking at 10.8 ms). The curve was obtained from 17 P4–P6 rats with at least three observations at each delay. C, Linear regression analysis of the strength of inhibition at delays larger than 10 ms (r = 0.62). The intersection of the regression line with the x-axis gives the duration of the inhibition. D, E, Amplitude of the test response before (CTL) and 20–45 min after strychnine (D; Stry, 0.4–1 μm; 9 P4–P6 rats) or picrotoxin (E; PTX, 20 μm; 8 P4–P6 rats). At least four observations were made for each delay. Histograms represent the strength of inhibition at short and long interstimulus intervals (10 and 40 ms, respectively) before and after drug application. Statistical differences between CTL and Stry, and between CTL and PTX are noted above delays and histograms in D and E (Wilcoxon test; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01).

To further investigate the involvement of KCC2 in reciprocal inhibition, we incubated spinal cord preparations with VU compounds (25 μm; n = 6, n = 2, and n = 1 for VU0240551, VU0255011, and VU0463271, respectively), described as highly specific KCC2 blockers (Delpire et al., 2009, 2012). The strength of reciprocal inhibition was significantly reduced in the presence of these drugs, compared with control observations made over 1 h (Fig. 3A,B; p < 0.05, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon test). Some inhibition however persisted at interstimulus intervals smaller than 25 ms (p < 0.05; Wilcoxon test). The calculated mean duration of inhibition was also significantly reduced after KCC2 blockade, from 48.8 to 38 ms (Fig. 3C; −22.2%; p < 0.01, linear regression analysis). In support of the results obtained with the genetic model, these data demonstrate that KCC2 function regulates the strength and the duration of reciprocal inhibition in neonatal rodents.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Pharmacological blockade of KCC2 decreases both the strength and the duration of reciprocal inhibition in neonatal rats. A, Amplitude of the test response measured before (CTL) and 30–60 min after application of 25 μm VU compounds. Curves were obtained from nine P4–P6 rats with at least six observations at each delay (6 animals received VU0240551, 2 VU0255011, and 1 VU0463271). B, Histograms represent the strength of inhibition at short and long interstimulus intervals (10 and 30 ms, respectively) from data reported in A. C, Linear regression analysis of the data reported in A (r = 0.68 and r = 0.71, respectively for CTL and VU conditions). Statistical differences between CTL and VU are noted above delays in A, above histograms in B and near regression lines in C (Wilcoxon test and linear regression analysis; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01).

We then explored whether the SCI-induced downregulation of KCC2 affected the reciprocal inhibition using rats that underwent a complete spinal cord transection at birth (SCI, n = 9), compared with untransected rats (Sham, n = 13; Fig. 4). There was no statistical difference concerning the amplitude of the L5 monosynaptic response between these two groups at P4–P6 (p > 0.05, Mann–Whitney test). The strength of inhibition was reduced for all delays longer than 10 ms after SCI, compared with sham animals (Fig. 4A,B; Mann–Whitney test). The conditioning stimulation had no significant inhibitory effect on the test response at intervals longer than 15 ms in SCI animals (p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test). Accordingly, the calculated duration of inhibition was lower in transected rats with a 38.4% reduction from 50.6 in Sham to 31.2 ms in SCI rats (Fig. 4C; p < 0.001, linear regression analysis).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

The strength and the duration of reciprocal inhibition are reduced after neonatal SCI in rats. A, Amplitude of the test response in untransected (Sham; 13 animals with at least 7 observations at each delay) and SCI (9 animals with at least 8 observations at each delay) P4–P6 rats. B, Histograms represent the strength of inhibition at short and long interstimulus intervals (10 and 30 ms, respectively) from data reported in A. C, Linear regression analysis of the data reported in A (r = 0.72 and r = 0.65, respectively for Sham and SCI conditions). Statistical differences between Sham and SCI rats are noted above delays in A, above histograms in B and near regression lines in C (Mann–Whitney test and linear regression analysis; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001).

Zinc upregulates KCC2 function in MNs and strengthens reciprocal inhibition after neonatal SCI

We next sought to test the effect of upregulating KCC2 function on reciprocal inhibition. We used zinc that has been shown to upregulate KCC2 activity (Chorin et al., 2011). The effect of zinc chloride on EIPSP in lumbar MNs in intact P4–P6 rats was examined at two concentrations (1 μm, gray lines, n = 7 and 10 μm, black lines, n = 10; Fig. 5A). ZnCl2 hyperpolarized EIPSP within 15–20 min and measurements were done 30–60 min after application onset. EIPSP was significantly more hyperpolarized when MNs were recorded in the presence of ZnCl2 compared with control conditions (−4.7 mV, n = 17; Fig. 5A; p < 0.001, Wilcoxon test; note that differences were also significant when considering each concentration separately, p < 0.05 and p < 0.01, Wilcoxon test applied for 1 and 10 μm ZnCl2, respectively). ZnCl2 had no significant impact on the resting membrane potential (Vrest, n = 17; Fig. 5B; p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test). Therefore, the driving force increased significantly (−4.8 mV, n = 17; Fig. 5C; p < 0.001, Wilcoxon test). As a result, EIPSP shifted from above to below Vrest. We blocked KCC2 function by VU compounds (n = 6 in total: n = 5 for VU0240551 and n = 1 for VU0463271) which depolarize EIPSP (Fig. 5 compare A,D and C,F; p < 0.01, Mann–Whitney test) without any effect on Vrest (n = 6; Fig. 5E; p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test) confirming previous observations (Bos et al., 2013). Importantly, VU compounds prevented the ZnCl2-induced hyperpolarizing shift of EIPSPs (n = 6; Fig. 5D,F; p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test), suggesting that ZnCl2 modulates chloride homeostasis through an upregulation of KCC2 function.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

ZnCl2 hyperpolarizes EIPSP via KCC2 in neonatal rat MNs. AC, EIPSP (A), Vrest (B), and driving force (EIPSPVrest; C) measured from intact P4–P6 rats before (CTL) and 30–60 min after ZnCl2 (1 μm, gray, n = 7; 10 μm, black, n = 10). Empty symbols indicate mean ± SEM of the pooled data. Note that differences are also significant considering each concentration separately: *1 μm and **10 μm ZnCl2. DF, Pharmacological blockade of KCC2 by VU compounds prevents the hyperpolarizing shift of EIPSP induced by ZnCl2 application in intact P4–P6 rats. EIPSP (D), Vrest (E), and driving force (EIPSPVrest; F) measured 10–30 min after the application of KCC2 blockers (25 μm VU; n = 5 VU0240551 and n = 1 VU0463271) and 30–60 min after 10 μm ZnCl2 (n = 6). GI, EIPSP (G), Vrest (H), and driving force (EIPSPVrest; I) measured from P4–P6 rats that underwent SCI at birth before (SCI) and 30–60 min after ZnCl2 (1 μm, gray, n = 3; 10 μm, black, n = 5). Empty symbols indicate mean ± SEM of the pooled data. Statistical differences are noted below values in A and G, and below histograms in C and I (Wilcoxon test; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001).

We also tested ZnCl2 on MNs recorded from animals that underwent a SCI at birth (1 μm, n = 3, gray lines and 10 μm ZnCl2, n = 5, black lines; Fig. 5G–I). EIPSP was significantly more hyperpolarized when MNs were recorded in the presence of ZnCl2 compared with control conditions (−3.9 mV, n = 8; Fig. 5G; p < 0.01, Wilcoxon test). ZnCl2 had no significant impact on Vrest (n = 8; Fig. 5H; p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test). Therefore, the driving force increased significantly 30–60 min after the onset of ZnCl2 application (−3.7 mV, n = 8; Fig. 5C; p < 0.01, Wilcoxon test), and EIPSP shifted from above to below Vrest.

The amplitude of the monosynaptic response evoked in a VR by supramaximal stimulation of the homonymous ipsilateral DR in vitro decreases when the stimulation occurs repeatedly, and this reduction becomes more important as the frequency of stimulation increases [rate-dependent depression (RDD); Bos et al., 2013]. As the RDD is reduced when KCC2 function is genetically or pharmacologically reduced (Boulenguez et al., 2010; Bos et al., 2013), we hypothesized that hyperpolarizing EIPSP by ZnCl2 would increase the RDD. The amplitude of the monosynaptic response was not affected by ZnCl2 (1 and 10 μm, n = 17; Fig. 6A; p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test). ZnCl2 significantly reduced the amplitude of the monosynaptic response elicited by repetitive stimulation at 0.5 and 1 Hz (n = 17; Fig. 6B; p < 0.05 and p < 0.01, Wilcoxon test). Linear regressions obtained from CTL and ZnCl2 conditions were significantly different (p < 0.05, regression line analysis). Regarding the polysynaptic response, we noticed a significant reduction (−11.1%) after ZnCl2 compared with control (n = 16; Fig. 6C; p < 0.01, Wilcoxon test).

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Effects of ZnCl2 on VR responses to homonymous DR stimulation in the P4–P6 rat spinal cord in vitro. A, Relative amplitudes of the monosynaptic responses before (CTL) and 30–60 min after ZnCl2 (1 and 10 μm, n = 6 and n = 11, respectively; p > 0.05). B, Relative amplitudes of the monosynaptic reflex at different stimulation frequencies before (CTL) and 30–60 min after ZnCl2 (1 and 10 μm, n = 6 and n = 11, respectively). Twelve values (the first three discarded) were averaged for each animal. The RDD was significantly increased by ZnCl2 at 0.5 and 1 Hz. C, Mean areas of the polysynaptic responses before (CTL) and 30–60 min after 1 (n = 5) and 10 μm (n = 10) ZnCl2. Statistical differences between CTL and ZnCl2 are noted above impulse frequencies in B and above histograms in C (Wilcoxon test; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01).

Finally, we investigated whether ZnCl2 application was able to increase reciprocal inhibition in rats in control and pathological conditions (Fig. 7). The strength of reciprocal inhibition recorded from intact rats was quite similar before and after 10 μm ZnCl2, comparing each delay (n = 8; Fig. 7A,B; p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test). Similarly, we did not observe any significant difference regarding the duration of reciprocal inhibition (56.9 ms and 50.2 ms before and after ZnCl2, n = 8; Fig. 7C; p > 0.05, Wilcoxon test). By contrast, ZnCl2 had an effect after neonatal spinal cord transection as it markedly increased reciprocal inhibition. Indeed, the strength (10–20 ms, n = 7; Fig. 7D,E; p < 0.05, Wilcoxon test) and duration (22% gain switching from 31.2 to 38.1 ms, n = 7; Fig. 7F; p < 0.01, regression line analysis) of the inhibition were both significantly increased after 10 μm ZnCl2.

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

ZnCl2 increases both the strength and the duration of reciprocal inhibition only in SCI neonatal rats. A, Amplitude of the normalized test response before (CTL) and 30–60 min after ZnCl2 (10 μm) in untransected P4–P6 rats. Curves were obtained from eight animals with eight observations at each delay. B, Histograms represent the strength of inhibition at short and long interstimulus intervals (10 and 30 ms, respectively). C, Linear regression analysis of the data reported in A (r = 0.83 and r = 0.84, respectively for CTL and ZnCl2 conditions). D, Amplitude of the test response before and 30–60 min after ZnCl2 (10 μm) in SCI P4–P6 rats. Curves were obtained from seven animals with at least five observations at each delay. E, Histograms represent the strength of inhibition at short and long interstimulus intervals (10 and 30 ms, respectively). F, Linear regression analysis of the data reported in D (r = 0.67 and r = 0.75, respectively for SCI and ZnCl2 conditions). Statistical differences between SCI and ZnCl2 conditions are noted above delays in D, above histograms in E and near regression lines in F (Wilcoxon test and linear regression analysis; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01).

Discussion

This study demonstrates that reducing the expression (genetic model) or the functionality (pharmacological blockade) of KCC2 reduces the strength of a well characterized inhibition in the spinal cord. Reciprocally, in pathological conditions such as after SCI, boosting KCC2 function by zinc ions enabled to restore endogenous inhibition.

Reciprocal inhibition in vitro in neonatal rodents

The pathway mediating reciprocal inhibition from muscle spindle afferents (Ia axons) to MNs supplying antagonistic muscles has been well studied in adult cats (Hultborn and Udo, 1972), but little was known until recently about how this disynaptic pathway develops (Wang et al., 2008; Sonner and Ladle, 2013). Using extracellular physiological techniques from hemisected spinal cord preparations isolated from 1-week-old mice, the latter authors described two distinct phases of inhibition at 6.5 and 23 ms with a maximum degree of inhibition of 71 and 52%, respectively. The time course of the inhibition obtained in our study is in accordance with these earlier data (7.3 and 23.1 ms with degrees of inhibition of 58 and 50%, respectively). The experiments performed by Sonner and Laddle (2013) further revealed that the two inhibitory phases were supported by separate pathways that did not mature at the same time. A disynaptic reciprocal glycinergic pathway is responsible for the early phase of inhibition and is functional at birth whereas the longer latency inhibition relies on presynaptic GABAergic inhibition of Ia afferents, that is only weakly observed at P0.

The time course obtained with rats in the present study was quite different from that in mice of the same age. A single phase was indeed observed at ∼11 ms. At first sight, this may suggest that, similar to mice, presynaptic inhibition might be immature during the first postnatal week. This, however, appears unlikely as blocking GABAA-receptors had a marked effect on the late part of the inhibition. It is noteworthy that the effect of pharmacological blockade of glycine and GABAA receptors in rats was not as clear-cut as in mice, in which strychnine and bicuculline affect mainly the early and the late inhibitory phases, respectively. In rats, the two antagonists affected the inhibition throughout its time course, suggesting a corelease of glycine and GABA by the interneurons involved in the disynaptic reciprocal, short-interval, inhibition and a strong contribution of glycine to a polysynaptic postsynaptic inhibition at long delays, that would be superimposed on presynaptic inhibition.

The level of KCC2 function sets the strength of postsynaptic inhibition

The kcc2 gene codes for two isoforms, KCC2a and KCC2b, which differ in their N-termini (Uvarov et al., 2007, 2009). KCC2a contributes 20–50% and 5–10% of total KCC2 mRNA expression in the neonatal and mature mouse brain, respectively, indicating that the expression of the KCC2b isoform markedly increases during development, whereas that of KCC2a remains relatively constant (Uvarov et al., 2007). Mice that lack both KCC2 isoforms die at birth due to severe motor defects, including disrupted respiratory rhythm (Hübner et al., 2001), whereas the mice we used in the present study, with a targeted disruption of the first exon of KCC2b, survived for up to 2 weeks (Woo et al., 2002).

In the present study, the inhibition of the L5 monosynaptic reflex, induced by stimulating the L3 dorsal root, was shown to be significantly reduced in KCC2b KO mice or after blocking KCC2 in neonatal rats. Both the early and late phases of inhibition were affected which is in agreement with a contribution of postsynaptic inhibition to these two phases. Both the genetic and the pharmacological downregulation of KCC2 expression/function lead to a 10–20 mV depolarizing shift of chloride equilibrium potential (ECl) in lumbar MNs (Hübner et al., 2001; Jean-Xavier et al., 2006; Boulenguez et al., 2010; Bos et al., 2013). Depolarizing IPSPs (ECl > VRest) are less able to suppress suprathreshold excitatory events than hyperpolarizing IPSPs, suggesting that the depolarization counteracts and can overcome the shunting effect (Jean-Xavier et al., 2007). Similarly, the duration of inhibition and ECl are negatively correlated, in agreement with the present observations that the duration of inhibition is decreased after reducing KCC2 function/expression. A major conclusion of the simulations we made was that membrane potential changes spread electrotonically as inhibitory synapses are moved away from the soma, whereas the shunting is local (Jean-Xavier et al., 2007). As a consequence, depolarizing IPSPs facilitate action potential generation by subthreshold excitatory events in the late phase of the response. The window of facilitation becomes wider as ECl becomes more depolarized and starts earlier as inhibitory synapses are moved away from the excitatory input. Interestingly, for distal inhibitory inputs, the effect on excitations generated at the soma level, can switch from inhibition (of suprathreshold EPSPs) to facilitation (of subthreshold EPSPs) within a narrow window of ECl values, close to the resting membrane potential. In summary, this study demonstrated that small depolarizing IPSPs can either inhibit or facilitate excitatory inputs, depending on timing and location (van den Pol et al., 1996; Gao et al., 1998; Gulledge and Stuart, 2003; Prescott et al., 2006; Jean-Xavier et al., 2007; Ostroumov et al., 2011). In addition to an effect on the integration of excitations, any alteration of KCC2 function may influence tonically active glycinergic/GABAergic currents in the postsynaptic neuron and thereby change its excitability.

The stronger disinhibition observed after KCC2 knock-down (KCC2−/−mice; Fig. 1B) compared with KCC2 blockade (rats; Fig. 3A) is at first sight surprising. However, at the concentrations of VU compounds used, the blockade of KCC2 is likely partial as suggested by an EIPSP of ∼−60 mV (Bos et al., 2013; the present study). By contrast, the 80% reduction in KCC2 expression in transgenic mice leads to an EIPSP of ∼−49 mV (Stil et al., 2011). The above-mentioned modeling studies showed that such a ∼10 mV difference in the shift of EIPSP has dramatic effects on the strength of postsynaptic inhibition.

Zinc restores endogenous inhibition in pathological conditions

Ipsilateral cocontraction of flexors and extensors is commonly observed in SCI patients (Harkema, 2008). The strength of several well characterized inhibitory mechanisms, such as presynaptic (Katz, 1999), recurrent (Mazzocchio and Rossi, 1997), and reciprocal (Boorman et al., 1996) inhibitions is reduced after SCI. A reciprocal facilitation instead of reciprocal inhibition may even appear (Crone et al., 2003). A reduction in the expression of KCC2 in the plasma membrane of MNs was suggested to be responsible for the reduced inhibition after SCI in adult (Boulenguez et al., 2010) and neonatal (Jean-Xavier et al., 2006; Bos et al., 2013) rats. Therefore, boosting the activity and/or the expression of KCC2 transporters by a pharmacological tool should restore endogenous inhibition.

Extracellular ZnCl2 was recently described as an enhancer of KCC2 activity and surface expression, thereby inducing a hyperpolarizing shift in GABAA reversal potential in rat hippocampal and cortical neurons (Chorin et al., 2011; Saadi et al., 2012). Several studies showed the presence of zinc-enriched neurons in the motor neuron area of rodent spinal cords suggesting a role of their boutons in the regulation of motor functions (Schrøder et al., 2000; Wang et al., 2001). Here, we demonstrate in vitro that ZnCl2 hyperpolarizes EIPSP in lumbar MNs in both control and SCI rats. This effect was prevented by the application of highly specific KCC2 blockers (Delpire et al., 2009, 2012), which is consistent with the requirement of KCC2 for the zinc-induced EIPSP shift.

Because the RDD of the monosynaptic response is markedly reduced when KCC2 is blocked or downregulated in pathological conditions (Boulenguez et al., 2010; Bos et al., 2013), we hypothesized that boosting KCC2 function and or expression by ZnCl2 would affect the RDD. As expected, ZnCl2 increased the RDD in vitro neonatal rats. We next considered the polysynaptic response, which results from a mixture of excitation and inhibition in MNs (Wallis and Wu, 1992; Jiang et al., 1999), and we showed a significant decrease by ZnCl2. Altogether, these results prove that ZnCl2 increases the inhibitory tone in the rat spinal cord. Interestingly, regarding reciprocal inhibition, ZnCl2 had no effect on intact rats but partially restored its strength and duration in SCI rats. Simulations showed that increasing KCC2 activity above normal levels has very little effects on ECl (Prescott et al., 2006; Doyon et al., 2011), and likely on the strength of postsynaptic inhibition. By contrast, in pathological condition associated with a KCC2 deficiency, ZnCl2 was able to increase reciprocal inhibition.

To conclude, the present study demonstrates that KCC2 appears as an attractive target to restore endogenous inhibition after SCI. Zinc ions may be an effective tool to reach such a goal. The positive effects of zinc supplementation on recovery in pathological conditions have been repeatedly demonstrated. For instance, zinc supplementation improves behavioral and cognitive recovery following traumatic brain injury (Cope et al., 2012). Similarly, zinc supplement reduces neurological damage and promotes the recovery of spinal cord function after acute SCI. An interesting potential mechanism involves a positive effect on BDNF synthesis and release (Wang et al., 2011), as BDNF has been shown to increase KCC2 expression after SCI (Boulenguez et al., 2010).

Footnotes

This work was supported by the International Spinal Research Trust and the Agence Nationale pour la Recherche. We thank Cécile Brocard and Sylvie Liabeuf for their technical assistance, and all team members for helpful discussion of the data presented here.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  1. Boorman GI, Lee RG, Becker WJ, Windhorst UR. Impaired “natural reciprocal inhibition” in patients with spasticity due to incomplete spinal cord injury. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996;101:84–92. doi: 10.1016/0924-980X(95)00262-J. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bos R, Sadlaoud K, Boulenguez P, Buttigieg D, Liabeuf S, Brocard C, Haase G, Bras H, Vinay L. Activation of 5-HT2A receptors upregulates the function of the neuronal K-Cl cotransporter KCC2. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:348–353. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1213680110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Boulenguez P, Vinay L. Strategies to restore motor functions after spinal cord injury. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2009;19:587–600. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Boulenguez P, Liabeuf S, Bos R, Bras H, Jean-Xavier C, Brocard C, Stil A, Darbon P, Cattaert D, Delpire E, Marsala M, Vinay L. Down-regulation of the potassium-chloride cotransporter KCC2 contributes to spasticity after spinal cord injury. Nat Med. 2010;16:302–307. doi: 10.1038/nm.2107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chen G, Trombley PQ, van den Pol AN. Excitatory actions of GABA in developing rat hypothalamic neurones. J Physiol. 1996;494:451–464. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chorin E, Vinograd O, Fleidervish I, Gilad D, Herrmann S, Sekler I, Aizenman E, Hershfinkel M. Upregulation of KCC2 activity by zinc-mediated neurotransmission via the mZnR/GPR39 receptor. J Neurosci. 2011;31:12916–12926. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2205-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cope EC, Morris DR, Scrimgeour AG, Levenson CW. Use of zinc as a treatment for traumatic brain injury in the rat: effects on cognitive and behavioral outcomes. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2012;26:907–913. doi: 10.1177/1545968311435337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Crone C, Johnsen LL, Nielsen J. Reciprocal inhibition in hemiplegic patients: a longitudinal study. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;53:187–191. doi: 10.1016/S1567-424X(09)70155-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Crone C, Johnsen LL, Biering-Sørensen F, Nielsen JB. Appearance of reciprocal facilitation of ankle extensors from ankle flexors in patients with stroke or spinal cord injury. Brain. 2003;126:495–507. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Curtis DR, Hösli L, Johnston GA, Johnston IH. The hyperpolarization of spinal motoneurones by glycine and related amino acids. Exp Brain Res. 1968;5:235–258. doi: 10.1007/BF00238666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Delpire E, Days E, Lewis LM, Mi D, Kim K, Lindsley CW, Weaver CD. Small-molecule screen identifies inhibitors of the neuronal K-Cl cotransporter KCC2. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:5383–5388. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812756106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Delpire E, Baranczak A, Waterson AG, Kim K, Kett N, Morrison RD, Daniels JS, Weaver CD, Lindsley CW. Further optimization of the K-Cl cotransporter KCC2 antagonist ML077: development of a highly selective and more potent in vitro probe. Bioorg Med Chem Lett. 2012;22:4532–4535. doi: 10.1016/j.bmcl.2012.05.126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Doyon N, Prescott SA, Castonguay A, Godin AG, Kröger H, De Koninck Y. Efficacy of synaptic inhibition depends on multiple, dynamically interacting mechanisms implicated in chloride homeostasis. PLoS Comput Biol. 2011;7:e1002149. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gao XB, van den Pol AN. GABA, not glutamate, a primary transmitter driving action potentials in developing hypothalamic neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2001;85:425–434. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gao XB, Chen G, van den Pol AN. GABA-dependent firing of glutamate-evoked action potentials at AMPA/kainate receptors in developing hypothalamic neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:716–726. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.2.716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gulledge AT, Stuart GJ. Excitatory actions of GABA in the cortex. Neuron. 2003;37:299–309. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01146-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Harkema SJ. Plasticity of interneuronal networks of the functionally isolated human spinal cord. Brain Res Rev. 2008;57:255–264. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hübner CA, Stein V, Hermans-Borgmeyer I, Meyer T, Ballanyi K, Jentsch TJ. Disruption of KCC2 reveals an essential role of K-Cl cotransport already in early synaptic inhibition. Neuron. 2001;30:515–524. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00297-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hultborn H, Udo M. Convergence of large muscle spindle (Ia) afferents at interneuronal level in the reciprocal Ia inhibitory pathway to motoneurones. Acta Physiol Scand. 1972;84:493–499. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1972.tb05199.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jankowska E. Interneuronal relay in spinal pathways from proprioceptors. Prog Neurobiol. 1992;38:335–378. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(92)90024-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jankowska E, Lindström S. Morphology of interneurones mediating Ia reciprocal inhibition of motoneurones in the spinal cord of the cat. J Physiol. 1972;226:805–823. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1972.sp010011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jean-Xavier C, Pflieger JF, Liabeuf S, Vinay L. Inhibitory postsynaptic potentials in lumbar motoneurons remain depolarizing after neonatal spinal cord transection in the rat. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:2274–2281. doi: 10.1152/jn.00328.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jean-Xavier C, Mentis GZ, O'Donovan MJ, Cattaert D, Vinay L. Dual personality of GABA/glycine-mediated depolarizations in immature spinal cord. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:11477–11482. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704832104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jiang Z, Carlin KP, Brownstone RM. An in vitro functionally mature mouse spinal cord preparation for the study of spinal motor networks. Brain Res. 1999;816:493–499. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)01199-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Katz R. Presynaptic inhibition in humans: a comparison between normal and spastic patients. J Physiol Paris. 1999;93:379–385. doi: 10.1016/S0928-4257(00)80065-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kudo N, Yamada T. Morphological and physiological studies of development of the monosynaptic reflex pathway in the rat lumbar spinal cord. J Physiol. 1987;389:441–459. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lance JW. The control of muscle tone, reflexes, and movement: Robert Wartenberg lecture. Neurology. 1980;30:1303–1313. doi: 10.1212/WNL.30.12.1303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lev-Tov A, Pinco M. In vitro studies of prolonged synaptic depression in the neonatal rat spinal cord. J Physiol. 1992;447:149–169. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1992.sp018996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Li Y, Gorassini MA, Bennett DJ. Role of persistent sodium and calcium currents in motoneuron firing and spasticity in chronic spinal rats. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:767–783. doi: 10.1152/jn.00788.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mazzocchio R, Rossi A. Involvement of spinal recurrent inhibition in spasticity: further insight into the regulation of Renshaw cell activity. Brain. 1997;120:991–1003. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.6.991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Morita H, Crone C, Christenhuis D, Petersen NT, Nielsen JB. Modulation of presynaptic inhibition and disynaptic reciprocal Ia inhibition during voluntary movement in spasticity. Brain. 2001;124:826–837. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.4.826. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Nicolopoulos-Stournaras S, Iles JF. Motor neuron columns in the lumbar spinal cord of the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1983;217:75–85. doi: 10.1002/cne.902170107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ostroumov A, Simonetti M, Nistri A. Cystic fibrosis transmembrane conductance regulator modulates synaptic chloride homeostasis in motoneurons of the rat spinal cord during neonatal development. Dev Neurobiol. 2011;71:253–268. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Prescott SA, Sejnowski TJ, De Koninck Y. Reduction of anion reversal potential subverts the inhibitory control of firing rate in spinal lamina I neurons: towards a biophysical basis for neuropathic pain. Mol Pain. 2006;2:32. doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-2-32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Saadi RA, He K, Hartnett KA, Kandler K, Hershfinkel M, Aizenman E. SNARE-dependent upregulation of potassium chloride co-transporter 2 activity after metabotropic zinc receptor activation in rat cortical neurons in vitro. Neuroscience. 2012;210:38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Schrøder HD, Danscher G, Jo SM, Su H. Zinc-enriched boutons in rat spinal cord. Brain Res. 2000;868:119–122. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02238-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Sonner PM, Ladle DR. Early postnatal development of GABAergic presynaptic inhibition of Ia proprioceptive afferent connections in mouse spinal cord. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109:2118–2128. doi: 10.1152/jn.00783.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Stein V, Nicoll RA. GABA generates excitement. Neuron. 2003;37:375–378. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00056-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Stil A, Jean-Xavier C, Liabeuf S, Brocard C, Delpire E, Vinay L, Viemari JC. Contribution of the potassium-chloride co-transporter KCC2 to the modulation of lumbar spinal networks in mice. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33:1212–1222. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07592.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Talpalar AE, Endo T, Löw P, Borgius L, Hägglund M, Dougherty KJ, Ryge J, Hnasko TS, Kiehn O. Identification of minimal neuronal networks involved in flexor-extensor alternation in the mammalian spinal cord. Neuron. 2011;71:1071–1084. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Uvarov P, Ludwig A, Markkanen M, Pruunsild P, Kaila K, Delpire E, Timmusk T, Rivera C, Airaksinen MS. A novel N-terminal isoform of the neuron-specific K-Cl cotransporter KCC2. J Biol Chem. 2007;282:30570–30576. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M705095200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Uvarov P, Ludwig A, Markkanen M, Soni S, Hübner CA, Rivera C, Airaksinen MS. Coexpression and heteromerization of two neuronal K-Cl cotransporter isoforms in neonatal brain. J Biol Chem. 2009;284:13696–13704. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M807366200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. van den Pol AN, Obrietan K, Chen G. Excitatory actions of GABA after neuronal trauma. J Neurosci. 1996;16:4283–4292. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04283.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Wallis DI, Wu J. FAST and SLOW ipsilateral and contralateral spinal reflexes in the neonate rat are modulated by 5-HT. Gen Pharmacol. 1992;23:1035–1044. doi: 10.1016/0306-3623(92)90283-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wang Y, Me X, Zhang L, Lv G. Supplement moderate zinc as an effective treatment for spinal cord injury. Med Hypotheses. 2011;77:589–590. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2011.06.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wang Z, Danscher G, Mook Jo S, Shi Y, Daa Schrøder H. Retrograde tracing of zinc-enriched (ZEN) neuronal somata in rat spinal cord. Brain Res. 2001;900:80–87. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02261-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wang Z, Li L, Goulding M, Frank E. Early postnatal development of reciprocal Ia inhibition in the murine spinal cord. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100:185–196. doi: 10.1152/jn.90354.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Woo NS, Lu J, England R, McClellan R, Dufour S, Mount DB, Deutch AY, Lovinger DM, Delpire E. Hyperexcitability and epilepsy associated with disruption of the mouse neuronal-specific K-Cl cotransporter gene. Hippocampus. 2002;12:258–268. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES